Relationship Between Social Axioms and Subjective Well-Being: The Role of Self-Regulation
Relationship Between Social Axioms and Subjective Well-Being: The Role of Self-Regulation
Relationship Between Social Axioms and Subjective Well-Being: The Role of Self-Regulation
1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
CHIN-MING HUI
Northwestern University
MICHAEL HARRIS BOND
Hong Kong Polytechnic University
professional help; Kuo, Kwantes, Towson, & Nanson, 2006) and to use
coercive tactics in influencing other individuals (Bond et al., 2004; Fu et
al., 2004). Again, this pattern of findings suggests that social axioms serve
as knowledge about which activities are instrumental in attaining
preferred goals (Leung et al., 2002).
The study served two major purposes in bridging between social
axioms and self-regulation. First, we wanted to empirically establish the
unexplored relationship between social axioms and self-regulatory orien-
tations. Second, we wanted to synthesize the tripartite relationships
among social axioms, well-being, and self-regulation. To do so, we
proposed to examine whether self-regulatory orientations mediated the
effect of social axioms on well-being.
In this study, we examined the relationship between two factors of
social axioms, namely reward for application and social cynicism, and
dispositional self-regulatory orientations. Social cynicism represents a
negative view about the consequences of interactions with other people,
groups, and social institutions. Reward for application represents the
strength of judged contingencies between any person’s investment of
resources and gains arising from these investments. These two axiom
dimensions were chosen because past studies have indicated that, among
other axioms, they were consistently more predictive of self-reported
behavioral preferences, such as conflict resolution strategies and coping
behaviors (Bond et al., 2004), and negotiation behaviors (Fu et al., 2004).
These associations imply a relatively stronger link between these two
axiom dimensions and self-regulatory orientations (for a comprehensive
review, see C. M. Hui & Hui, 2009).
In general, reward for application is related to adaptive psychological
adjustment, including more prosocial behaviors and persistent goal-pursuit
activities, while social cynicism is related to psychosocial impairment,
such as low life satisfaction and elevated death anxiety (C. M. Hui & Hui,
2009). We sought to expand our understanding of these psychological
outcomes by considering their underlying self-regulatory mechanisms.
We considered a number of self-regulatory constructs, each of which has
been consistently related to subjective well-being in the literature.
2009; Kwantes & Karam, 2009). We argue that this tendency may stem
from the pessimistic expectancy about the likely yield from social
exchanges.
Mindfulness. Mindfulness refers to “an enhanced attention to and
awareness of current experience or present reality” (Brown & Ryan, 2003,
p. 822). It facilitates one’s openness and receptiveness to conscious expe-
riences with ongoing events in the given moment (Martin, 1997). Mind-
fulness is beneficial to one’s psychological functioning because it
increases one’s conscious awareness of choices and opportunities that are
congruent with one’s own needs and values. Accordingly, mindfulness
cultivates psychological well-being by promoting realization of one’s
values, basic needs, and interests (Brown & Ryan, 2003).
We hypothesized that social cynicism is related to a lower level of
mindfulness. Indeed, cynical individuals are usually preoccupied by nega-
tive thoughts, as described above. Due to such an elevated, security-ori-
ented concern, those endorsing socially cynical beliefs may be more
defensive and narrow minded (Forster & Higgins, 2005) and hence less
open to disconfirming information (Hart et al., 2009). Consistent with
these speculations, social cynicism has been related to cognitive inflexi-
bility (Singelis et al., 2003) and need for cognitive closure (Guan & Bond,
2008). Hence, social cynicism may be associated with reduced openness
to one’s here-and-now experiences.
Approach and avoidance motivations. Human behaviors are governed
by basic principles of approach and avoidance motivations (e.g., Carver,
Sutton, & Scheier, 2000; Gray, 1981). Approach motivation is presumed
to regulate one’s attainment of rewards and nonpunishment, while avoid-
ance motivation regulates focus on the prevention of nonreward and
punishment. This basic dichotomy of motivations has been identified as
having important consequences for positive and negative emotional expe-
riences (Cacioppo, Gardner, & Bernston, 1997) as well as influencing
behavioral outcomes, like decision making (Frank & Brandstatter, 2002).
We hypothesized that reward for application is related to approach
motivation. Based on its name and content, reward for application focuses
on the contingency of rewards to investments in exchanges with the social
system. Hence, reward for application should be at least moderately
related to approach motivation, which is responsible for such reward-
related incentive. Indeed, previous research has indirectly supported this
notion. For instance, reward for application is related to extraversion, high
arousal, and positive emotions, which are psychological phenomena
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
Method
Participants
A total of 111 Chinese university students (52 male and 59 female; age
M = 20.50, SD = 1.20) participated in this study. They were recruited
at a university campus in Hong Kong by using flyers and mass
e-mailing. Participants were asked to fill out a battery of questionnaires
measuring social axioms, self-regulation, and subjective well-being, as
described below.
Measurements
Social axioms. The Social Axioms Survey (Leung et al., 2002), consisting
of 60 items with a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disbelieve and 5
= strongly believe), was administered. The social axioms survey consists
of items tapping the five factors, namely social cynicism (e.g., “Powerful
people tend to exploit others”), reward for application (e.g., “One will
succeed if he/she really tries”), social complexity (e.g., “Human behaviour
changes with the social context”), fate control (e.g., “All things in the
universe have been determined”), and religiosity (e.g., “Belief in a reli-
gion makes people good citizens”).
Optimism. The Life Orientation Test (LOT; Scheier & Carver, 1992)
was used in the study. We adopted the Chinese version of the LOT for the
current study (J. C. L. Lai & Yue, 2000). The scale consists of three posi-
tive items (e.g., “I’m always optimistic about my future”) and three nega-
tive items (e.g., “I hardly ever expect things to go my way”), each
measured with a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 =
strongly agree). Though some studies have advocated using two subfac-
tors based on the positively and negatively worded items (J. C. L. Lai,
1994), the two factors did not differentially relate to the predictors in our
study. Thus, for simplicity of presentation, we aggregated all items to
calculate a global optimism score.
Mindfulness. The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS;
Brown & Ryan, 2003) was employed. The scale consists of 15 items (e.g.,
“I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until
some time later”), with a 6-point Likert-type scale (1 = almost always, 6 =
almost never).
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
Results
Relating Social Axioms With Self-Regulatory Orientations
Descriptive statistics and correlational analyses are presented in Table 1.1
As shown in Table 1, the pattern of correlations is consistent with our
predictions. Reward for application was positively related to optimism, r
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
= .24, p < .05, approach motivation, r = .31, p < .01, and locomotion
mode, r = .48, p < .001. On the other hand, social cynicism was nega-
tively related to optimism, r = –.33, p < .01, and mindfulness, r = –.32, p
< .01, but positively related to avoidance motivation, r = .27, p < .01, and
assessment mode, r = .29, p < .01. Hence, the first two hypotheses were
supported. In addition, consistent with our prediction, among the five
axiom dimensions, only social cynicism and reward for application were
related to variables of self-regulation.
We were also interested in the strength of relationships between the
two factors of social axioms and the self-regulatory orientations. To
achieve this goal, we did additional analyses to assess how much variance
in reward for application and social cynicism could be accounted for by
the six self-regulatory orientations employed in this study. The data
showed that the self-regulatory orientations accounted for 32% and 22%
of the total variance in reward for application and social cynicism, respec-
tively. The degree to which the two axioms are related to the self-regula-
tory orientations is at least as strong as their correlations with personality
variables (Chen, Fok, et al., 2006).
Variable M SD α 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
7. Mindfulness 3.76 0.56 .80 .12 –.32** –.10 .16 .05 .26** —
8. Approach motivation 2.93 0.30 .72 .31** .18 .03 .06 .13 .05 –.27** —
9. Avoidance motivation 2.91 0.42 .69 –.17 .27** .07 .14 .05 –.44*** –.26** .24* —
10. Locomotion mode 4.19 0.50 .71 .48*** –.08 –.14 .09 .10 .24* .08 .25** –.07 —
11. Assessment mode 3.83 0.57 .73 .13 .29** –.11 .04 –.03 –.26** –.19* .25** .22* .23* —
12. Life satisfaction 4.28 1.07 .85 .12 –.38*** –.03 –.07 .08 .55*** .32** –.07 –.35*** .18 –.38*** —
13. Positive affect 2.96 0.70 .89 .40*** –.06 –.04 .15 .02 .38*** .11 .23* –.16 .58*** .16 .31** —
Published by the Chinese University Press
14. Negative affect 2.31 0.78 .91 .00 .42*** .14 .01 .19* –.37*** –.38*** .15 .39*** .02 .46*** –.41*** .04 —
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Discussion
The study aimed at enriching our understanding of the link between social
axioms and well-being by considering the perspective of self-regulation
theory (Carver et al., 2000; Higgins & Scholer, 2008). The results in
general suggested that two factors of social axioms, namely social cyni-
cism and reward for application, are related to this selected set of self-reg-
ulatory orientations. Moreover, as indicated by regression analyses, the
relationship between the axioms and self-regulatory orientations was at
least as strong as the correlations between social axioms and other trans-
situational, dispositional variables, such as values (Bond et al., 2004) and
various comprehensive measures of personality (Chen, Fok, et al., 2006).
In addition, the study also demonstrated that it is fruitful to study the rela-
tionship between social axioms and self-regulation by illustrating that
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
Table 3. Estimating mediated effects with bootstrapping in the multiple mediator model
Social cynicism → Life Reward for application → Social cynicism →
satisfaction positive affect Negative affect
Bootstrap Bootstrap Bootstrap
estimate estimate estimate
Indirect M SE 95% bias- M SE 95% bias- M SE 95% bias-
effects corrected CI corrected CI corrected CI
Overall –.58 .16 (–.93, –.29)** .60 .14 ( .35, .90)** .37 .12 ( .16, .65)**
Optimism –.27 .11 (–.54, –.10)** .12 .06 ( .02, .29)* .06 .07 (–.08, .21)
Mindfulness –.09 .08 (–.25, .02) .01 .03 (–.03, .09) .11 .06 ( .02, .25)*
Approach .01 .03 (–.06, .09) .04 .06 (–.06, .19) –.02 .03 (–.11, .02)
motivation
Avoidance –.05 .07 (–.24, .05) .01 .03 (–.04, .10) .08 .06 ( .01, .24)
motivation
Locomotion –.02 .04 (–.16, .02) .39 .11 ( .20, .63)** .01 .02 (–.07, .02)
mode
Assessment –.16 .10 (–.42, –.03)* .04 .03 (–.01, .13) .02 .08 ( .03, .34)**
mode
CI = confidence interval.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
Carver & White, 1994; Gable & Poore, 2008; Higgins et al., 2003). The
pattern of results was consistent with findings showing that reward for
application is related to intention and effort exertion on striving (Liem et
al., 2009; Zhou et al., 2009), persistence and active problem solving in the
face of difficulties (Bond et al., 2004; Kurman, 2004), and better coping
and adjustment in intercultural exchanges (Safdar, Lewis, & Daneshpour,
2006). As optimism and locomotion are both beneficial to psychosocial
adjustment, the study also suggested that reward for application shapes
positive affect through its influence on these two variables.
Social Cynicism
In contrast, social cynicism is related to pessimism, mindlessness, avoid-
ance motivation, and use of an assessment regulatory mode. The correla-
tions between social cynicism and these constructs support the notion that
social cynicism reflects one’s vigilance in scanning the social world for
deception and avoiding potential interpersonal risks (Leung & Bond,
2004). The avoidance and assessment orientations have been proposed to
elevate sensitivity and evaluative responsiveness to dangers dormant in
social situations (Higgins et al., 2003; Lang, 1995). In addition, relevant
pessimistic beliefs are also useful for self-regulation, as negative thoughts
can fuel and sustain avoidance motivation (Puca & Schmalt, 2001; Tamir,
2005). Consistent with this reasoning, empirical data have shown that
social cynicism facilitates better adaptation in novel situations, such as
new cultural settings (Kurman & Ronen Eilon, 2004).
However, endorsement of social cynicism is not without its cost.
Social cynicism has frequently been associated with poor psychological
health (Chen, Cheung, et al., 2006; J. Lai et al., 2007). This may be due
to the fact that cynical individuals tend to chronically use self-regulatory
strategies that compromise their psychological health. Indeed, research
has found that poor well-being stems from pessimism (Scheier &
Carver, 1987), mindlessness (Brown & Ryan, 2003), and use of an
assessment mode (Kruglanski et al., 2000). Moreover, our results indi-
cated that social cynicism reduces life satisfaction through mediation of
the assessment mode and lack of optimism and increases negative affect
through mediation of the assessment mode and lack of mindfulness.
Hence, maladaptive self-regulation may be a reason why social cynics
have poor well-being.
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
Note
1. Though independence should be expected based on previous findings (Carver
& White, 1994; Kruglanski et al., 2000), we found mildly positive correla-
tions between approach and avoidance motivations (r = .24, p < .05) and
between locomotion and assessment modes (r = .23, p < .05). We considered
these correlations spurious.
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
References
Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being: Amer-
ica’s perception of life quality. New York: Plenum.
Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2007). Self-regulation and
the executive function: The self as controlling agent. In A. W. Kruglanski & E.
T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed.,
pp. 516–539). New York: Guilford.
Bond, M. H. (2009). Believing in beliefs: A scientific but personal quest. In K.
Leung & M. H. Bond (Eds.), Psychological aspects of social axioms (pp.
319–342). New York: Springer.
Bond, M. H., Leung, K., Au, A., Tong, K. K., & Chemonges Nielson, Z. (2004).
Combining social axioms with values in predicting social behaviours. Euro-
pean Journal of Personality, 18, 177–191.
Boski, P., Bilas-Henne, M., & Wieckowska, J. (2009). Cynicism in love and in
politics. In K. Leung & M. H. Bond (Eds.), Psychological aspects of social
axioms (pp. 239–267). New York: Springer.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindful-
ness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 84, 822–848.
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Bernston, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar
conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative space.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3–25.
Carver, C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M. F., Robinson, D. S.,
et al. (1993). How coping mediates the effect of optimism on distress: A
study of women with early stage breast cancer. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 65, 375–390.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual
framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology. Psycholog-
ical Bulletin, 92, 111–135.
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Fulford, D. (2008). Self-regulatory processes,
stress, and coping. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.),
Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 725–742). New
York: Guilford.
Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and
personality: Emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26, 741–751.
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activa-
tion, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/
BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.
Cavallo, J. V., Fitzsimons, G. M., & Holmes, J. G. (2010). When self-protection
overreaches: Relationship-specific threat activates domain-general avoidance
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
Higgins, E. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Pierro, A. (2003). Regulatory mode: Loco-
motion and assessment as distinct orientations. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 293–344). San
Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Higgins, E. T., & Scholer, A. A. (2008). When is personality revealed? In O. P.
John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory
and research (3rd ed., pp. 182–207). New York: Guilford.
Hui, C. M. (2005). Can two viewpoints determine your affectivities? Self and
social views as mood regulators. Unpublished ’bachelor’s thesis, Chinese
University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.
Hui, C. M., & Hui, N. H.-H. (2009). The mileage from social axioms: Learning
from the past and looking forward. In K. Leung & M. H. Bond (Eds.),
Psychological aspects of social axioms (pp. 13–30). New York: Springer.
Hui, V. K.-Y., Bond, M. H., & Ng, T. S. W. (2007). General beliefs about the
world as defensive mechanisms against death anxiety. Omega: Journal of
Death and Dying, 54, 199–214.
Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61,
651–670.
Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.
Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (2004). The psychology of worldviews. Review of General
Psychology, 8, 3–58.
Kruglanski, A. W., Thompson, E. P., Higgins, E. T., Atash, M. N., Pierro, A.,
Shah, J. Y., et al. (2000). To “do the right thing” or to “just do it”: Locomo-
tion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory imperatives. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 793–815.
Kumashiro, M., Rusbult, C. E., Finkenauer, C., & Stocker, S. L. (2007). To
think or to do: The impact of assessment and locomotion orientation on the
Michelangelo phenomenon. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
24, 591–611.
Kuo, B. C. H., Kwantes, C. T., Towson, S., & Nanson, K. M. (2006). Social
beliefs as determinants of attitudes toward seeking professional psychological
help among ethnically diverse university students. Canadian Journal of
Counselling, 40, 224–241.
Kurman, J. (2004, August). The dimension of reward for application. Paper
presented at the 17th Congress of International Association of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, Xi’an, China.
Kurman, J., & Ronen Eilon, C. (2004). Lack of knowledge of a culture’s social
axioms and adaptation difficulties among immigrants. Journal of Cross-Cul-
tural Psychology, 35, 192–208.
Kwan, V. S. Y., Bond, M. H., & Singelis, T. M. (1997). Pancultural explana-
tions for life satisfaction: Adding relationship harmony to self-esteem.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1038–1051.
Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, Vol. 11, No. 1 (2010)
Published by the Chinese University Press
社會通則與主觀幸福感的關係:自我調節所扮演的角色
許展明
美國西北大學
彭邁克
香港理工大學
摘要
本研究探討社會通則與四種自我調節的不同取向的關係。該四種取向分別
為樂觀、心智專注、自我調節模式,及趨近和迴避動機。我們主要針對兩
種不同的社會通則:憤世嫉俗觀及付出有回報觀。研究結果顯示憤世嫉俗
觀及付出有回報觀與自我調節的取向有關聯。中介作用分析顯示憤世嫉俗
觀跟非適應性自我調節取向有關,並因此對主觀幸福感帶來負面影響。但
是付出有回報觀跟適應性自我調節取向有關,並因此對主觀幸福感帶來正
面影響。我們就這些結果對人際關係的策略性取向及心理健康的啟示意義
進行了討論。
關鍵字:社會信念、幸福感、自我調節
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.