Segmentational Approaches of Atonal Musi PDF

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 75
At a glance
Powered by AI
The document discusses various theories of segmentation of atonal music and compares perceptual and mathematical approaches. It also develops a method for segmenting polyphonic atonal music.

The thesis employs Dora Hanninen’s analytical framework of segmentation to identify segmentational boundaries in works by Webern and Schoenberg in order to support published analyses.

The author briefly compares Lefkowitz and Taavola’s mathematical theory to James Tenney and Larry Polansky’s perception-based theory, which is rooted in visual Gestalt perception and provides the foundation for Dora A. Hanninen’s segmentation theory.

SEGMENTATIONAL APPROACHES OF ATONAL MUSIC: A STUDY BASED ON A

GENERAL THEORY OF SEGMENTATION FOR MUSIC ANALYSIS

Stefanie Acevedo

A Thesis

Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green


State University in partial fulfillment of
the requirements for the degree of

MASTER OF MUSIC IN MUSIC THEORY

December, 2010

Committee:

Per Broman, Advisor

Gene Trantham

 
© 2010

Stefanie Acevedo

All Rights Reserved


    iii

ABSTRACT

Per Broman, Advisor

The complexity of atonal musical structures has led theorists to offer varying analyses of

atonal works. This ambiguity stems from the intricacies of human perception: Is it

possible to state a definitive analysis when perceptions differ? In order to justify a

segmentation, the analyst must provide supporting evidence in the music. Due to the

wide range of perception, this evidence yields analyses that are more or less

persuasive, but neither correct nor incorrect. David S. Lefkowitz and Kristin Taavola,

however, propose a mathematical model that defines a correct segmentation.

This thesis briefly compares Lefkowitz and Taavola’s mathematical theory to

James Tenney and Larry Polansky’s perception-based theory. Tenney and Polansky’s

theory is rooted in visual Gestalt perception and provides the foundation for Dora A.

Hanninen’s segmentation theory. I then employ Hanninen’s analytical framework to

identify segmentational boundaries that support published analyses of two atonal works:

the fourth of Anton Webern’s Fünf Sätze, Op. 5 and an excerpt from Arnold

Schoenberg’s Klavierstücke, Op. 11, No. 1. I apply two of Hanninen’s three

segmentational criteria: the sonic, which refers to acoustical properties, and the

contextual, which refers to categorizations, such as set-classes.

Lefkowitz and Taavola note that Tenney and Polansky’s theory cannot be applied

to polyphony. Although Tenney and Polansky concede this point, Hanninen encourages

the use of her theory for polyphonic segmentation. She does not, however, provide a

method for addressing polyphony. Thus, I combine aspects from Lefkowitz and
    iv

Taavola’s simultaneous analysis with Hanninen’s theory in order to formulate a basic

method for segmenting polyphonic music.

I find that sonic and contextual criteria in the music strongly support the analyses

by George Perle, Allen Forte, Gary Wittlich, and Charles Burkhart. Due to the emphasis

of set-class theory for atonal analysis, there is an inherent reliance on contextual

criteria; however, sonic criteria also reinforce their segmentations and sometimes may

even support their contextual criteria in places lacking local sonic criteria. Thus, the

musical structures strongly support the segmentations, validating the diversity of

analyses and suggesting that atonal music can legitimately be heard in different ways.
    v

To my father

It's only when you grow up, and step back from him, or leave him for your own career
and your own home—it's only then that you can measure his greatness and fully
appreciate it.
-Margaret Truman
    vi

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

First and foremost, I would like to thank my family. Their love, encouragement, and

support are, without a doubt, the most important part of my life. I thank my father, to

whom I dedicate this thesis, for his struggle and never-ceasing quest to see me

succeed. ¡Te quiero, papi! Jacqueline, you are not only like a mother but a best friend;

thanks for your love, your support, and your zest. My aunts, uncles, and cousins—you

provide never-ending love, merriment, and inspiration; thank you. And to the elders of

the family: Nona and Abuelito—both of you have gone through this life giving it all

you’ve got, never giving up, and seeing your families grow into a diverse group of

individuals that love and care for each other no matter what. I can only aspire to achieve

what you have.

I want to thank my friends for their inspiration and endless support. I would

especially like to thank Kristopher, Nick, Aleks, Dennis, Heather, and Zoey for not only

thinking with me, but also enduring endless weeks of stress and harsh treatment!

Ultimately, I would not have gotten where I am without the aid of great teachers

and mentors. Dr. Per Broman has been a wonderful advisor, encouraging me and

devoting endless amounts of time toward helping me with my endeavors. I thank Dr.

Gene Trantham for putting time into this project despite the hardships in his life; he has

great dedication and has been a great influence. Dr. Robert Fallon, thanks for all of your

help throughout my masters career, especially with that abstract magic! Both Dr. Nora

Engebretsen and Dr. William Lake have not only taught me how to be a better scholar,

but have guided me through difficult times and shown me how to persevere through it
    vii

all. I love you both very much. Dr. Christopher Alan Williams and Dr. Smith Alexander

Reed—two great friends, teachers, and role models—thanks for all the late-night chat

sessions, coffee breaks, office hours, and unending support and encouragement. You

two are my rock.

There are many other people in my life that I could not include here. However,

you know who you are and I thank you from the bottom of my heart for being a part of

my quest.
    viii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

I. INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND ............................................................... 1

What is Segmentation? ................................................................................. 2

A General Theory of Segmentation .............................................................. 7

Overview of the Theory’s Framework ................................................ 7

Two Differing Theories ....................................................................... 11

Polyphonic Analysis………….. ........................................................... 16

II. ANALYSIS OF KLAVIERSTÜCKE, OP. 11, NO. 1 .............................................. 18

Overview ..... ................................................................................................. 18

Measures 1 – 3 ............................................................................................. 19

Measures 4 – 8 ............................................................................................. 26

III. ANALYSIS OF FÜNF SÄTZE, OP. 5, NO. 4....................................................... 33

Overview ................................................................................................. 33

Section A, Measures 1 – 6 ............................................................................ 34

Section B, Measures 7 – 11 .......................................................................... 44

Section A’, Measures 11 – 13 ....................................................................... 47

The Seven-Note Motive ................................................................................ 49


    ix

IV. CONCLUSION.. ................................................................................................. 54

Discussion ................................................................................................. 54

Further Research .......................................................................................... 57

Closing Thoughts .......................................................................................... 58

BIBLIOGRAPHY .... ................................................................................................. 59

APPENDIX: SCORES.............................................................................................. 61
    x

LIST OF TABLES/FIGURES

Table Page

1 Criteria Employed in Analysis ....................................................................... 9

Figure Page

1 Visual Example of Chunking ......................................................................... 3

2 A. Model Using Tenney and Polansky’s Theory of Segmentation ................ 5

B. Difference Between Proximity and Similarity ............................................ 6

3 A. An S1-pitch Genosegment ........................................................................... 10

B. A Phenosegment Arising from a Combination of Genosegments ............ 10

4 A. Lefkowitz/Taavola Segmentation Based on Rhythmic Domain ................ 14

B. Parallel Segmentation Based on Pitch Domain ........................................ 14

5 A. SINGLE S1-pitch Genosegments Forming a SIMUL Genosegment............ 16

B. SIMUL Genosegments Forming a SIMUL Phenosegment ....................... 16

(Figures for Schoenberg Op. 11, No. 1)

6 SINGLE Genosegment/Phenosegment Analysis, mm. 1-3 .......................... 20

7 SIMUL Analysis, mm. 1-3 ............................................................................. 21

8 Perle’s Segmentation, mm. 1-3..................................................................... 21

9 Pitch Contour Analysis of Melodic Line, mm. 1-3 ......................................... 22


    xi

10 A. Forte’s Hexachord Segmentation, mm. 1-3 .............................................. 23

B. Support for CSC 6-Z44, mm. 1-3................................................................... 23

11 A. Wittlich’s Trichord Segmentation, mm. 1-3 ............................................... 24

B. Wittlich’s Tetrachord Segmentation, mm. 1-3........................................... 24

12 A. SINGLE Analysis of Top Lines, mm. 4-8 .................................................. 27

B. SINGLE Analysis of Bottom Lines, mm. 4-8 ............................................. 27

13 A. SIMUL Analysis of Four Parts, mm. 4-8 ................................................... 28

B. S1-rest Phenosegments from SIMUL Analysis of Left Hand, mm. 4-8........ 28

C. S1-attack Genosegments from SIMUL Analysis, mm. 4-8 ........................... 28

14 Forte’s Segmentation, mm. 4-8..................................................................... 30

15 Cadential Contextual Criteria, mm. 4-8 ......................................................... 30

16 Wittlich’s Segmentations, mm. 4-5................................................................ 31

(Figures for Webern Op. 5, No. 4)

17 SINGLE Analysis of Violin I, mm. 1-6 ........................................................... 35

18 SINGLE Analysis of Violin II, mm. 1-6 .......................................................... 36

19 SINGLE Analysis of Viola, mm. 1-6 .............................................................. 36

20 A. SINGLE Analysis of Cello, mm. 1-6.......................................................... 37


    xii

B. S1-pitch Analysis of Cello, mm. 3-4 ............................................................. 37

21 SIMUL Phenosegment from Selected Genosegments, mm. 1-6 .................. 38

22 A. Perle’s Segmentation, mm. 3-5 and Support............................................ 40

B. Phenosegment Support for E/F-sharp Dyad in the Viola, mm. 1-4 .......... 40

23 A. Burkhart’s Segmentation, mm. 1-3 and Supporting Criteria ..................... 41

B. Burkhart’s Segmentation, mm. 3-6 ........................................................... 41

24 A. Straus’s Segmentation, mm. 1-2 .............................................................. 43

B. Straus’s Segmentation, mm. 4-5 .............................................................. 43

25 S1-rest Genosegments, mm. 7-9 ..................................................................... 44

26 Perle’s Segmentation, mm. 7-9..................................................................... 45

27 Straus’s Segmentation of the Viola Ostinato, mm. 7-9 and Support............. 46

28 Phenosegments and Genosegments, mm. 11-13 ........................................ 47

29 Comparison of Burkhart and Straus Segmentations, m. 12.......................... 48

30 Appearances of the Seven-Note Motive, mm. 6, 10, 12-13 .......................... 50

31 A. Perle’s Segmentation of the First Seven-Note Motive, m. 6 ..................... 51

B. Perle’s Segmentation of the Second Seven-Note Motive, m. 10.............. 51

32 A. Burkhart’s Segmentation of the Seven-Note Motive, m. 6........................ 52


    xiii

B. Burkhart’s Segmentation of the Second Seven-Note Motive, mm. 10-11 52

33 CSC 8-09 Segmentation, mm. 12-13 ................................................................ 53


1

I. INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND

Allen Forte, in his preface to The Structure of Atonal Music, describes atonal music as

“complicated music [that] has not been well understood.”1 In the forty years since the

first publication of Forte’s groundbreaking book, much work has been done in the

development of atonal theory; no longer can it be said that atonal music is too complex

or completely misunderstood. The theoretical field has developed widely and many

offshoots of atonal theory have led to the development of models and methodologies to

describe not only how this type of music is written and structured, but also how it is

perceived and understood.

Many atonal theories rely on segmentational approaches, a method of breaking

musical structures into groups or segments, which Dora Hanninen defines as “the basis

for subsequent musical organization and interpretation.”2 The different theoretical tools

that have emerged have led to multiple segmentations, or interpretations, of the same

musical work. As Christopher Hasty states in his article “Segmentation and Process in

Post-Tonal Music,” “ambiguity…is an extremely important aspect of [modern] music.”

Different analyses, then, do not detract from the credibility of the music, but instead add

to the profundity and importance of the works.3

This thesis examines different analyses of two atonal works, Fünf Sätze, Op. 5,

No. 4 by Anton Webern and an excerpt from Arnold Schoenberg’s Klavierstücke, Op.

11, No. 1, and defines the most musically apparent boundaries that may have impacted

1
Allen Forte, The Structure of Atonal Music (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1973), ix.
2
Dora Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments: A General Theory of Segmentation for Music
Analysis,” Journal of Music Theory 45, no. 2 (Autumn, 2001): 345.
3
Christopher Hasty, “Segmentation and Process in Post-Tonal Music,” Music Theory Spectrum 3
(Spring, 1981): 59.
2

the analysts’ segmentations. The multiple segmentations serve to not only authenticate

Hasty’s ideas about ambiguity, but also lead to a more complete and musically evident

interpretation of each musical work.

What Is Segmentation?

Many theories of segmentation have been developed, some involving human perception

principles. The main purpose for an interdisciplinary approach (involving perception

studies) is to decipher how the brain perceives music, and subsequently, segments the

information. Two main theories are discussed here: David S. Lefkowitz and Kristin

Taavola’s segmentation in music,4 which focuses on developing a mathematical model

of musical perception, and James Tenney and Larry Polansky’s temporal Gestalt

perception, which has a basis in visual Gestalt perception and serves as the foundation

for Dora Hanninen’s theory.5 However, a brief overview of the technicalities of

segmentation applies before imparting on a discussion of the individual theories.

In their introduction to “Segmentation in Music: Generalizing a Piece-Sensitive

Approach,” David S. Lefkowitz and Kristin Taavola state that musical analysis relies on

segmentation:

Segmentation—the process of parsing a composition into meaningful parts—lies


at the heart of many music-theoretic activities. Given the fact that the very word
“analysis” means the division of the whole into its constituent parts, segmentation
is intrinsic implicitly or explicitly—to many analytic endeavors.6

Typing the search string “music segmentation” into any library database brings up

hundreds of articles, books, and other resources dwelling on what should appear to be

4
David S. Lefkowitz and Kristin Taavola, “Segmentation in Music: Generalizing a Piece-Sensitive
Approach,” Journal of Music Theory 44, no. 1 (Spring, 2000): 171-229.
5
Employed for analysis in this thesis and discussed in a later section.
6
Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 171.
3

the simple concept of dissecting a musical passage. And it might, indeed, be a simple

concept were it not for the intricate workings of human perception and its implications

on what is heard and, thus, how it leads to segmentation by different individuals.

Since human-brain capacitance is limited, large amounts of information, whether

visual, auditory, or other, must be condensed into smaller units. For example, a ten-

digit phone number is a string of numbers too long for one to easily parse visually and

thus, is usually divided into three separate groups, making it easier to discern (Figure

1). This principle of chunking is also true for music, or any type of aural perception.

Figure 1: Visual Example of Chunking

A ten-digit phone number without grouping:


8003932893

A ten-digit phone number grouped into three


chunks: 800-393-2893

The latter is not only easier to parse visually, but


also easier to remember.

While listening to a musical passage (the musical stream), one subconsciously

chunks the stream of sound into separate entities. These entities can either be defined

as segments or sub-segments, depending on the level of hierarchy.7 According to Dora

Hanninen, segments are groupings of musical events that are bounded by musical

borders based on changes in musical criteria;8 James Tenney and Larry Polansky

define these changes as temporal separation and parametric dissimilarity.9 Thus, a

parameter is established and a method of measurement is used to describe the

7
Hanninen’s definition depends on whether the group in question involves a phenosegment or
not. Others’ vary. David S. Lefkowitz and Kristin Taavola say, “There is a roughly ideal number of [three
to five] notes that combine to form a segmentational group;” The number is drawn from human perception
studies. These ideas vary due to the theorists’ respective hierarchical definitions of segments and their
sub-components. Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 181.
8
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 426.
9
James Tenney and Larry Polansky, “Temporal Gestalt Perception in Music,” Journal of Music
Theory 24, no. 2 (Autumn 1980): 208.
4

differences between musical events that create differences or “disjunctions.” So, for

example, if pitch were the parameter, a method of measurement may be the intervallic

distance between pitches. Thus, if the musical example consisted of a string of major

seconds and then a perfect fourth, the disjunction would occur when that different

measurement (the perfect fourth) occurred.

Due to the many similarities between visual and aural phenomena, and the

perception of these, some music theorists have drawn parallels between visual Gestalt

theory and music perception theory.10 James Tenney and Larry Polansky, in their article

“Temporal Gestalt Perception in Music,” use Gestalt theory principles to develop an

algorithm for music segmentation. They define three musical differences that can be

perceived between temporal gestalt units:11 state (the mean value of a unit’s measured

parameter), shape (the contour of a parameter across time), and structure (the relation

of the unit to others across hierarchical levels). At the lowest hierarchical level, or what

Tenney and Polansky call the “element-level,” it is hard to discern differences between

the temporal gestalt units’ shape and structure (due to the lack of hierarchy and

extended time). Thus, they argue that state is the only quality of a unit that pervades

through all hierarchical levels, and is the essential difference that drives segmentation.12

Tenney and Polansky’s algorithm for segmentation, derived from their theories

on disjunctions, is defined as:

10
For more information on Gestalt Theory, please visit the website for the Society of Gestalt
Theory and Its Applications. The International Society for Gestalt Theory and its Applications, Gestalt
Theory and its Applications, http://www.gestalttheory.net.
11
A temporal gestalt-unit is defined as “time-spans” perceived by the listener. These can include
anything from a motive to a movement of a piece, depending on hierarchical structures. In either case,
these are largely determined by perception and the smallest temporal gestalt-unit can be equated to
Lefkowitz’s and Taavola’s event. Tenney, “Gestalt Perception,” 217.
12
Tenney, “Gestalt Perception,” 214-216.
5

A new [temporal gestalt-unit] at the next higher level will be initiated in perception
whenever a [temporal gestalt-unit] occurs whose disjunction (with respect to the
previous [temporal gestalt-unit] at the same hierarchical level) is greater than
those immediately preceding and following it.13

Events in the same hierarchical stream of musical information will only be segmented if

the measurement of a musical parameter is greater from the one before and after it.

Dora Hanninen uses their method and further defines it in a footnote:

Starting with the first note, consider each pair of adjacent notes in turn. For the
magnitudes of any three adjacent notes, x, y, and z with respect to the sonic
dimension in question, determine the (non-directed) intervals |y - x| and |z – y|. If
|z – y| < |y - x|, then place a segment boundary between x and y. Note that the
algorithm always considers and compares the size of the following interval before
placing any boundary…14

Thus, looking at the melody in figure 2a, a segmentation, according to the pitch

parameter, would be appropriate after pitch C.

Figure 2a: Model Using Tenney and Polansky’s Theory of


Segmentation (Also employed by Dora Hanninen)

Adjacent Differences (based on Adjacent comparisons:


scalar steps): A, B, C: |C – B| > |B – A|
|B – A| = 1 B, C, D: |D – C| > |C – B|
|C – B| = 2 C, D, E: |E – D| < |D – C|
|D – C| = 4
|E – D| = 1 Boundary between C and D.

There are drawbacks to Tenney and Polansky’s theory.15 Conflicting

segmentations may occur when two separate parameters create boundaries at different

points. For example, the pitch parameter, as stated above, could remain the same as in

figure 2a, but a rhythm parameter may imply a different segmentation. Also, Tenney and

13
Ibid., 217.
14
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 426.
15
Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 173.
6

Polansky’s theory is designed for the segmentation of monophonic music, an issue that

will be discussed in depth later.

Figure 2b: Difference Between Proximity and Similarity

*Tenney/Polansky: Compares the notes, thus focusing on the top row of differences (Is the interval
between A and B larger than the distance between B and C? If so, there is no disjunction.)

*Lefkowitz/Taavola: Compares the intervals, thus focusing on the bottom row of differences (Is the
difference between intervals the same? If yes, there is no disjunction).

Results will sometimes give the same segmentation, but the NOMENCLATURE is different.

As an alternative to Tenney and Polansky’s theory, David S. Lefkowitz and

Kristin Taavola define segmentation through an algorithm in which discontinuation is a

“change in the rate of change.” They argue that this change in definition allows for

“preference for similarity…versus proximity.” In other words, Tenney and Polansky

focus on the similarity of the difference between parameters, in comparison to Lefkowitz

and Taavola, who focus on the proximity between parameters. Thus, given a parameter

of pitch, Lefkowitz and Taavola’s algorithm performs operations on the similarity of the

intervals between the pitches, while Tenney and Polansky’s method compares the

proximity of the pitches (Figure 2b).16

Essentially, both theories agree that disjunctions, or discontinuations, lead to

chunking or segmentation of a musical passage. The definition of these disjunctions,

however, is a main topic of discussion: what characteristics lead to the perception of

them, how are they measured, how do different musical styles lead to their
16
Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 177.
7

interpretation? All of these and many other questions naturally influence the “method”

chosen for segmentation and each method’s inherent problems and implications.

A General Theory of Segmentation

As mentioned before, segmentation theory has been developed for many years and

there have been many postulated methods. There is, however, a general lack of

codification of segmentation procedures and theories. Two theories have been touched

upon thus far, leaving out the fundamental theory employed in this thesis: Dora

Hanninen’s theory of segmentation.17 James Tenney & Larry Polansky’s theory is the

foundation of Hanninen’s, the use of which will be justified in this section.

Overview of the Theory’s Framework18

Dora Hanninen’s theory of musical segmentation develops a nomenclature that focuses

on the possibilities of not only multiple analyses, but, in contrast to Lefkowitz and

Taavola, also emphasizes the importance of the music by stating that segmentations

must be audible.19 The theory focuses on the interaction of three main criteria (sonic,

contextual, and structural) and how these affect groupings that enable the emergence of

segments based on perceptual and theoretical precepts.20

Musical segments are, in essence, groups of musical material that are isolated

from other groups according to boundaries. These groups, according to Hanninen, are

17
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 345-433.
18
This section includes a very concise overview of Dora Hanninen’s theory of segmentation,
which will be employed for analysis. Please refer to the article for more detail and specific analytical
illustrations. Ibid., 345-433.
19
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 413.
20
There are three criteria involved in Hanninen’s theory, as opposed to the four used by
Lefkowitz and Taavola. Hanninen points this out as another flaw in their theory: the four domains do not
amply represent the “fourteen dimensions listed.” Ibid., 423.
8

supported by musical criteria in three ways: instantiation, coincidence, and realization. A

single criterion that delineates a musical group (that is, a “one-to-one mapping”) is

called instantiation; two or more criteria that identify the same musical group (two-to-one

mapping) are called coincidence. In turn, realization is a type of coincidence, in which

one criterion is structural and the other contextual.21

The sonic criteria are acoustical properties such as pitch, timbre, rhythm, attack,

dynamic, and so forth. Each sonic parameter can be considered a characteristic of a

note, with magnitude, and exists within one dimension. There are two subtypes of sonic

criteria (S1 and S2); S1 defines criteria that are adjacent in sequential time and S2

defines criteria that may or may not be temporally adjacent.22 Contextual criteria rely on

interactions between musical events, and thus are also perceptually grounded. These

criteria define musical aspects such as pitch contour, set-classes, interval classes, and

other such aspects determined by “basic concepts in music theory…generally

understood and used as observation language.”23 Finally, structural criteria depend on

theoretical bases and relate groupings to an established “orienting theory.”24 Thus, a

structural criterion would describe aspects such as how a V-I motion may relate to the

fundamental structure in Schenkerian analysis, or how a set of pitches is part of a row in

Serialism.

21
Ibid., 357-358.
22
“Music is represented as a string of temporally adjacent events; the analyst then identifies and
compares intervals in pitch, attack-points, dynamics, or some other sonic dimension formed by pairs of
temporally adjacent tones…The resulting segmentation is a series of disjunct time-slices. I group all such
segmentation criteria based on temporal adjacency under the heading subtype 1.”; “Sonic criteria can
also be predicated on adjacency in linear dimensions other than time, such as pitch, duration and
dynamics. I call these sonic subtype 2.” Ibid., 360-361.
23
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 364.
24
Ibid., 375.
9

Table 1: Criteria Employed in Analysis


(Source: Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 362, 366-367.)
S Criterion Description and Comments C Criterion Description and Comments Example
S1-attack Simultaneity; share an attack point Ccseg Pitch Contour Ccseg <0132>
S1-pitch Proximity in pitch interval between Cic Interval Class Cic 5
two temporally adjacent notes
S1-duration Proximity in beats or seconds of CSC Set-Class CSC 3-4
“real time”
S1-rest Rest marks boundary between two Cip Pitch interval; ordering of pitch Cip 8; Cip
otherwise temporally adjacent interval as distances; ordering <81>; Cip <+8, -
events of directed pitch intervals 1>
S2-duration Proximity in duration; events may
be temporally adjacent or non-
adjacent
25
S1-tone Measures differences in
timbre/articulation.

A simple labeling system is used to describe each type of criteria by category

and descriptor. The letters S, C, and T are used to denote sonic, contextual, and

structural criteria respectively. Each label, in turn, also contains a subscript with extra

information. In the case of sonic criteria, the subscript contains not only subtype one or

two, but also a descriptor such as adjacency, attack, pitch, rest, dynamics, etc. (i.e. S1-

pitch or S2-dynamics). Similarly, the contextual criteria will denote a subtype (such as set

class) as well as a descriptor of said subtype (i.e. Cpc R<9A10> where PC stands for pitch-

class set, R for retrograde).26 Structural criteria are comparably shown, however, due to

the atonal focus of this thesis, will not be discussed in further detail.27

Dora Hanninen establishes a multitude of sonic and contextual subtypes, a

limited number of which will be employed for analysis (Table 1). Following her theory,

the criteria will be used to delineate genosegments based on parameter measurements.

For example, using the pitch sonic criteria (S1-pitch), a musical line will be analyzed for

boundaries based on the size of intervallic distances between the pitches (Figure 3a).

25
Criterion not described by Hanninen.
26
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 359-387.
27
Since there is no definitive orienting theory used to describe the musical works analyzed in this
thesis (i.e. serialism, reductive analysis, etc.), structural criteria will not be used for analysis.
10

Using the formula |z – y| < |y - x| (where each set of three notes is labeled respectively;

see page 13), a boundary marks a segmentation (denoted by brackets). This

fundamental grouping of notes is called a genosegment and is supported by one

criterion (in this case, S1-pitch). A phenosegment (denoted by a caesura), in turn, arises

from the combination of one or more genosegments (Figure 3b).

Figure 3a: An S1-pitch Genosegment


(Musical Example Drawn from Schoenberg Op. 11, No.1)

Figure 3b: A Phenosegment Arising from a Combination of Genosegments

*Since there are no common boundaries between the genosegments, except the beginning and end, the
phenosegment arising from all three genosegments includes the entire musical example.

Two Differing Theories

It could be argued that Dora Hanninen’s segmentation theory emphasizes the analyst’s

individuality and perception. Each theorist hears different hierarchies and groupings,

suggesting different analyses about a work and, thus, leading to segmentations that

may differ from each other. Hanninen’s theory is a tool that can lead to a better

understanding of how these segmentations have been reached, serving to describe

segmentations theoretically, instead of arbitrarily, per se.

It is important to note that the purpose of employing the theory is not to define

what segmentations are correct and which are wrong, nor to explicitly define a routine
11

for musical segmentation.28 If used for analysis after the fact, as in this thesis,

Hanninen’s theory can aid the understanding of why certain segmentations were

chosen since individual perception defines how one chunks musical segments. Thus,

the criteria and processes described are only suggestions of how one might go about

segmenting a work.

Due to the emphasis on individuality, Hanninen’s theory differs substantially from

Lefkowitz and Taavola’s theory, which weighs the strength of musical segments.29 This

huge disconnect between the two theories leads to an interesting quandary: if

segmentation depends on individual perception, how is it possible to compare the

strengths of said divisions and determine weaker or stronger segmentations?

Hearing is subjective and perception even more so. As stated by Fred Lerdahl

and Ray Jackendoff in their influential work A Generative Theory of Tonal Music,

“Rarely do two people hear a given piece in precisely the same way or with the same

degree of richness.”30 Thus, an experienced listener31 is more likely to hear

segmentations based on atonal sets than, say, a college music student specializing in

18th-century music. Yet, as musicians, these two very different individuals can agree on

certain criteria, such as dynamics or intervallic distances, that help to define

28
“The theory is not a methodology (although it might be used as one), nor is it prescriptive.
Rather, it is a flexible, neutral, conceptual framework and language—both terminology and notation—that
analysts can use to identify and grasp sub rosa aspects of [segmentation.]” Hanninen, “Orientations,
Criteria, Segments,” 346.
29
“The overall aim of the methodology is a musically-responsive weighting of different
parameters’ effect on perceptual grouping.” This, inherently, leads to the weighting of segments produced
by the selection of stronger parameters. Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 172.
30
Fred Lerdahl and Ray Jackendoff, A Generative Theory of Tonal Music (Cambridge, MA: The
MIT Press, 1983), 3.
31
“The ‘experienced listener’ is meant as an idealization…Occasionally we will refer to the
intuitions of a less sophisticated listener, who uses the same principles as the experienced listener in
organizing his hearing of music, but in a more limited way. In dealing with especially complex artistic
issues, we will sometimes elevate the experienced listener to the status of the ‘perfect’ listener—that
privileged being whom the great composers and theorists presumably aspire to address.” Lerdahl,
Generative Theory, 3.
12

segmentations based on theoretically sound boundaries and measurements. Unlike

Dora Hanninen’s theory, Lefkowitz and Taavola’s work ensures optimal chunks for

segmentation that are carefully figured through mathematical formulas focusing on a

perfect ratio of discontinuities and events within different specified domains or musical

parameters.32 This is a methodology that leads to supposedly ideal segmentations.

However sound, those mathematical models do not guarantee that the human brain will

be satisfied with the derived divisions.

Chunking provides some justification for Lefkowitz and Taavola’s mathematical

models: “Based upon...research into studies of psychological perception, we have

determined that [the] ideal number [of events within a chunked entity] is between three

to five.”33 The mathematical model takes into account this formula and derives the

strongest possible boundaries within each domain (or a combination of domains), thus

creating the aforementioned segmentations. These may be technically correct,

according to statistical testing and mathematical formulas, but serve only as that, a

model for the perfect segmentation of the musical passage. They cannot accurately

portray how one (or many) would hear these musical ideas.

Dora Hanninen’s theory corroborates Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s argument that a

musical theory should describe multiple ways of organization to appease the possibility

of different hearings.34 Taavola and Lefkowitz, in turn, while proposing that different

32
The domains defined are pitch, articulation, timbre, and rhythm. Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in
Music,” 175-182. Hanninen has issues with these as they are limiting “fourteen dimensions” into four
domains. Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 423.
33
Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 181.
34
“A theory of a musical idiom should be concerned above all with those musical judgments for
which there is substantial interpersonal agreement. But it also should characterize situations in which
there are alternative interpretations, and it should have the scope to permit discussion of the relative
merits of variant readings.” Lerdahl, Generative Theory, 3.
13

methods of segmentation are viable, contradict the idea through the use of an exact

mathematical approach to describe an outcome of subjective perception. They state:

In sum, this is a theory of segmentational processing, not a theory of music. The


results of the segmentational system are not, therefore, “answers” in and of
themselves to traditional questions about the structure and content of a piece of
music—that is, they do not necessarily reveal the underlying unities or
hierarchies.35

Thus, one could employ this theory when attempting to understand segmentational

processes but not necessarily to concretely segment a piece of music. The theory

certainly should not be used to comprehend the meaning of said segments within a

musical structure.

Aside from the philosophical and perceptual issues with Lefkowitz and Taavola’s

methods, there is an inherent flaw in their basic definition of disjunctions. This

fundamental issue leads to contradictions within the algorithm, which may or may not be

augmented in computational practice.36 Either way, the theory defines a discontinuity at

the point where the rate of change changes. Lefkowitz and Taavola employ multiple

examples, including one that focuses on rhythm (Figure 4a):

Thus, the change-in-rate-of-change approach enables one to segment the


passage in [Figure 4a] as shown. For the first four notes in [Figure 4a], the rate of
change within the Rhythm Domain is minus-one-eighth, while for the next six
notes the rate of change is zero.

Taking into account that the distance between each subsequent note is shorter by one

eighth-note each time (thus having the same rate-of-change), a discontinuity occurs

after the fourth note, at which point the rate of change becomes three eighths. Now,

consider a similar scenario employing the pitch domain: figure 4b features the exact

35
Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 220.
36
The depth and strength of the flaw has not been examined within the algorithm or mathematical
computations, only within the definition of the discontinuities.
14

same rate-of-change as figure 4a, yet the parameter is pitch. Following the similar

schematic for segmentation, a segment would arise after the fourth note (between the A

and B-flat). However, looking at the melodic line, it seems logical that there should be

no segmentation between the B-flat and A due to their intervallic proximity, especially

considering the surrounding intervals.

Figure 4a: Lefkowitz/Taavola Segmentation Based on Rhythmic


Domain (Source: Lefkowitz, “Segmentations in Music,” 178.)

Figure 4b: Parallel Segmentation Based on Pitch Domain

The complex issues described above limit the use of Lefkowitz and Taavola’s

theory. Yet polyphonic music still poses a problem for the use of Dora Hanninen’s

theory. Unlike Lefkowitz and Taavola’s method, Tenney and Polansky admit that their

theory should only be used on monophonic music.37 Hanninen, however, implies that

the method can be used for other textures; she mentions drawbacks to theories limited

to homophonic music38 and labels criteria that allow simultaneities.39 Still, there is no

analysis of polyphonic music in Hanninen’s article and no mention of the flaw with

37
Tenney, “Gestalt Perception,” 212.
38
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 352.
39
See the definition of S1-attack. Ibid., 362.
15

Tenney and Polansky’s method, which is the fundamental algorithm used for

Hanninen’s theory.

A wise word of advice from Lefkowitz and Taavola now applies: “It is important to

understand that we need not be slaves to any theory or system.”40 Their theory provides

a method for dealing with polyphonic music, which can be employed using Hanninen’s

criteria. A work can be heard as a single line or multiple lines at once (and everything in

between). It is important to consider an analysis of multiple ways of hearing, thus

isolating different domains/criteria into single lines (what Lefkowitz calls the SINGLE-line

listening) and a composite whole (the SIMUL).41 Therefore, a combination of both

theories can be employed to describe a very limited polyphonic texture of music, a

process that will be described in the following sections.

Polyphonic Analysis

It is impossible to predict the multiple ways in which one may hear polyphony. However,

it is possible to gain an understanding of how multiple parts interact by performing a

polyphonic analysis, like that of Lefkowitz and Taavola’s theory, using sonic and

contextual criteria.42 They perform a simultaneous analysis, which they call a SIMUL

analysis, by isolating a specific parameter (for example, timbre) and analyzing the

interaction of the multiple lines as a whole.43 This concept can be used in combination

with Hanninen’s theory. Since a genosegment is defined as the grouping based on one

40
Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 219.
41
Ibid., 208.
42
It must be clear that this is only a slight approximation. The multitudes of ways in which musical
lines can interact to form a polyphonic texture are innumerable and cannot be fully understood with this
method.
43
Lefkowitz, “Segmentation in Music,” 208.
16

specific criterion, it could be stipulated that the grouping of multiple genosegments for

individual lines can be combined to create a single genosegment for a multiple-voice

example (the individual-voice genosegments will be called SINGLE genosegments;

Figure 5a). In turn, these SIMUL genosegments can be combined across multiple

criteria in order to create phenosegments for multiple voices (Figure 5b).

Figure 5a: SINGLE S1-pitch Genosegments Forming a SIMUL Genosegment

Figure 5b: SIMUL Genosegments Forming a SIMUL Phenosegment

*SIMUL S1-rest derivation not shown

The combination of these two theories could be considered controversial.

However, it is imperative that some sense of polyphonic analysis be used. Most music

is polyphonic, and thus a monophonic analysis is extremely limited in order to define the

perception of segmentation. The methods employed here for polyphonic study are very

limited; this is only a demonstration of the potential arising from the fusion of two diverse

methods for analysis.

The following section describes analyses for sections of two works. The analyses

contain similar segmentations, but sometimes deviate in detail or underlying structures.


17

A survey of main sonic criteria precedes the discussions of three analyses, which, in

turn, highlight some of the contextual criteria emergent in the passage.


18

II. ANALYSIS OF KLAVIERSTÜCKE, OP. 11, NO. 1

Overview

As Allen Forte states in his article “The Magical Kaleidoscope: Schoenberg’s First

Atonal Masterwork, Opus 11, Number 1,” the piece has been analyzed from multiple

perspectives and has given theorists many hardships, contradicting Schoenberg’s belief

that the piece was one that was easily understood.44 Various approaches have led to a

multitude of different analyses, relying on tonality and atonality alike.45 However, the

focus will be on atonality in this thesis. Some of the theorists who have endeavored to

analyze the work include Allen Forte, George Perle, and Gary Wittlich, three analyses

that will be discussed by focusing on coincident excerpts.

Opus 11, No. 1 begins with a passage that introduces the fundamental material

of the piece. Perle describes the first three measures by placing emphasis on the

trichords,46 while Forte focuses on hexachordal divisions.47 Wittlich brings both ideas

into play and describes the trichords as the building materials of the hexachords.48

44
Allen Forte, “The Magical Kaleidoscope: Schoenberg’s First Atonal Masterwork, Opus 11,
Number 1,” Journal of the Arnold Schoenberg Institute 5, no. 2 (November 1981): 127-129.
45
“There is a long history of published analyses of this work, beginning soon after the score was
published, in October, 1910…Many of them attempt to place the music in some kind of tonal framework.”
Ibid., 129.
46
George Perle, Serial Composition (Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, 1991): 10-15.
47
And as such, this thesis will focus on segmentations of hexachordal divisions, and will only
touch on divisions of dyads, trichords, tetrachords, and pentachords as mentioned in Forte’s article. Forte,
“Magical Kaleidoscope,” 127-129.
48
Gary Wittlich, “Interval Set Structure in Schoenberg’s Op. 11 No. 1,” Perspectives of New
Music 13, no. 1 (Autumn-Winter, 1974): 42.
19

Measures 1 – 3

A rest on the downbeat of the fourth measure sectionalizes the beginning of the piece.

This sudden repose brings attention to the importance of the opening statement in the

first three measures. This opening material is clearly divided into two main parts: a

melodic segment in the right-hand and a harmonic segment comprised of two chords on

the second beats of mm. 2-3. Gary Wittlich describes each of the two parts as

autonomous hexachords, each playing a fundamental part in the fabric of the work.49

Similarly, Forte also describes these two hexachords, along with a multitude of others,

which are supported by both contextual and sonic criteria.

Focusing on a monophonic (or SINGLE) analysis of the sonic criteria for mm. 1-

3, it is easy to differentiate between four voices, following a standard S-A-T-B voice-

leading structure in which the soprano is an accompanied melody. The main sonic

genosegments appearing in this passage are S1-pitch, S1-duration, and S1-rest (Figure 6).

Dynamics do not exert segmenting power, and due to the short length of the passage,

S2 criteria do not seem to play a large part either (S2-duration criteria will be discussed in a

polyphonic analysis).

The soprano line is fairly conjunct and thus can be described by the connection

of mainly two S1-pitch genosegments.50 The only segmentation apparent occurs at

measure two, after which the segment continues far beyond the reach of measure three

(due to the sameness of the intervals between measures 4-8). The S1-duration and S1-rest

criteria, however, end at the rest (m. 4) due to their emphasis on musical time. A strong

49
Wittlich, “Set Structure,” 42.
50
Due to the focus on atonality, interval size is evaluated by interval-class 1 increments. The first
pitch is technically its own genosegment due to the size of the following interval, thus creating three total
genosegments. However, the majority of the melody is included in the two genosegments and very limited
in segmentation.
20

phenosegment appears from the melodic line due to the interaction between all three of

the aforementioned criteria subtypes and their coincidental segmenting points at

measures 1 and 4.

In a similar fashion to the melodic line in the excerpt, the subservient harmony

lines feature genosegments described by the subtypes for rest, duration, and pitch.

Taken individually, each harmony voice (alto/tenor/bass) contains an apparent

phenosegment supported by S1-rest and S1-duration. However, due to the simultaneity of

the harmony (chordal texture) and the repetition of said harmony (mm. 2 and 3), there is

also a subcriterion of S2-duration that also supports the aforementioned phenosegment

(Figure 6).

Figure 6: SINGLE Genosegment/Phenosegment Analysis, mm. 1-3


21

Taking a SIMUL segmentation approach, it is necessary to differentiate between

each criteria subtype while combining the individual voice parts. As a whole, it is

apparent that a phenosegment is mainly supported by the harmonic voices (alto-tenor-

bass; Figure 7). This is in part due to the strong attack of the left-hand chords and the

prevalence of the S2-duration criteria, which emphasizes a symmetrical structure.

Unfortunately, however, the SIMUL segmentation does not support the right-hand

melody and thus detracts from that segmentation.

Figure 7: SIMUL Analysis, mm. 1-3

*Genosegments denote supporting voices in parentheses (A = Alto, T = Tenor, B = Bass)

Figure 8: Perle’s Segmentation, mm. 1-3


(Source: Perle, Serial Composition, 11.)
22

George Perle, in Serial Composition and Atonality, describes four trichords,

which he calls cells, in the first three measures of Op. 11, No. 1 (Figure 8).51 These

trichords can be by the contextual criterion CSC 3-3. Gary Wittlich, in his own analysis,

denotes trichords as being “the most common structures of the piece,” specially the 3-3

trichord.52

Figure 9: Pitch Contour Analysis of Melodic Line, mm. 1-3

The only sonic criteria that appears to support Perle’s segmentations are SIMUL

S2-duration and S1-rest for the harmonic trichord appearing in measure 3. The other

trichords (in measures 1 and 2), however, are not readily apparent from the sonic

genosegments of the section. The first trichord segmentation (appearing in the right-

hand melody mm. 1-2) is supported by a contour contextual criterion. A split in the

melodic line is not apparent in the single sonic criterion for said line, yet there appears

to be a sequential-type contour structure (Ccseg<210>; Figure 9). This separation

delineates a contextual split in the top voice, which leads to the emergence of the

Perle’s trichord in mm. 1-2 (if the repeated B in the alto line is omitted). While other

supporting contextual criteria are limited, a strong case is made for the trichord

segmentations based on the overall structural prominence in the piece, as discussed by

both Perle and Wittlich.53

51
Perle, Serial Composition, 11.
52
Wittlich, “Set Structure,” 42.
53
Perle, Serial Composition, 10-15; Wittlich, “Set Structure,” 41-42.
23

Figure 10a: Forte’s Hexachord Segmentation, mm. 1-3


(Source: Forte, “Magical Kaleidoscope,” 139.)

Figure 10b: Support for CSC 6-Z44, mm. 1-3

In contrast to Perle, Allen Forte segments the first three measures into four

separate hexachords: CSC 6-Z10, CSC 6-Z44, CSC 6-21, and CSC 6-16 (Figure 10a).54 Sonically,

the most readily apparent hexachords are CSC 6-16 and CSC 6-Z10 due to the strong

stratification of the left and right hands into melody and accompaniment chords. He

describes these two hexachords as the thematic components of the work. The two

segmentations are supported by the SINGLE-line analyses of sonic criteria (Figure 6),

while CSC 6-16 emerges through the phenosegment established by the SIMUL sonic

criteria S1-pitch and S2-duration (Figure 7). Two contextual criteria help to establish the

CSC 6-Z44 hexachord: motion by Cip <+4> in the bass staff and Ccseg <210> in the melody

(Figure 10b). The chordal nature of the alto, tenor, and bass lines discourage any

attempt to divide the three into separate groups, yet the similar movement that exists

only between the tenor and bass voices supports a decision to segment the voices

54
Forte, “Magical Kaleidoscope,” 139.
24

separately. The phenosegment that would justify the segmentation with CSC 6-21 is only

supported by the S1-pitch criteria. CSC 6-21 contains the strong melodic line of the right

hand and the alto and tenor voices, an odd segmentation on the basis of sonic criteria.

Thus, the SINGLE criteria of S1-rest and S1-duration for the bottom voices lead to a

phenosegment, but exclude the bass voice; the strong correlations between the three

harmony voices could challenge this segmentation.

Figure 11a: Wittlich’s Trichord Segmentation, mm. 1-3


(Source: Wittlich, “Set Structure,” 42.)

*Unless otherwise noted, segmentations are CSC 3-3.


Figure 11b: Wittlich’s Tetrachord Segmentation, mm. 1-3
(Source: Wittlich, “Set Structure,” 42.)

Anachronistically speaking, one could say that Gary Wittlich’s analysis in “Interval

Set Structure in Schoenberg’s Op. 11, No. 1” builds on the analyses by Allen Forte and

George Perle. Wittlich discusses the prominent segmentations from both theorists’

analyses, including the importance of trichords and two main hexachordal figures in
25

mm. 1-3. Wittlich’s segmentations into trichords are the same as Perle’s except for an

extra segmentation of a CSC 3-5, which creates a second trichord segmentation of the

right-hand melody (Figure 11a); this segmentation is also supported by the

aforementioned Ccseg<210> criterion that supports the first trichord division of the line

stipulated by Perle.

Wittlich also discusses fundamental tetrachord segmentations that are supported

by trichord subsets and, in turn, support hexachord segments. These tetrachords

emerge from criteria CSC 4-2, CSC 4-5, and CSC 4-19 (Figure 11b). The melodic S1-pitch criteria

supports one of the melodic line tetrachords (CSC 4-2) while no apparent sonic criteria

supports the similar overlapping mate. Two tetrachords form symmetrical

segmentations in measure 2 and measure 3; these include the chord structures and the

downbeat pitch of the melody (apparent from CSC 4-5 and CSC 4-19). Because of the attack

offset between melody and harmony, a SIMUL phenosegment does not emerge from

S1-attack. However, due to the simultaneity, harmony is a strong parameter for the

contextual parameters. This quality, on the other hand, hurts the emergence of Wittlich’s

last tetrachord segment (m. 3) since the bass voice is omitted from the segment,

despite the support from the SINGLE S1-pitch criteria. Either way, Wittlich argues for the

importance of these tetrachords and trichords due to their relation to his hexachordal

segments,55 narrowing the “supersets of practically all the smaller sets of the piece” into

two of Forte’s same hexachordal segmentations (delineated by CSC 6-Z10 and CSC 6-16).

As previously mentioned, these two hexachords are the most apparent due to strong

55
See Table 2. Inclusion of the Primary Tetrachords and Hexachords in Wittlich, “Set Structure,”
44.
26

sonic support from criteria and the inherent homophonic texture, as well as the strong

contextual support emerging from Wittlich’s inclusions.

Measures 4 – 8

After a short three-measure introduction, the texture of the movement becomes more

contrapuntal, weaving an intricate web of segment juxtapositions. There is still a distinct

four-part texture as in the prior three measures. As before, the four distinct parts (S-A-T-

B) can be analyzed for individual segments, and combined for a SIMUL approach.

The structure shows that measures four and five are reiterated in measures five

and six yet the attacks of each voice are offset by an eighth-rest. Despite this change,

the segmentations apparent in the first appearance of the musical passage still hold for

the repetition, the only difference being a change in genosegment support. Each

individual line is a repeats its own respective motive. The soprano line is a repetition of

the E and G, which leads to a S1-pitch genosegment that pervades throughout most of the

four measures, except for a disjunction in measure seven due to the following material

(in measure 9). Thus, the more influential genosegments of the soprano line (that is,

those which combine to create a phenosegment) are the S1-rest and S1-duration that break

the line into three separate segments (Figure 12a). The alto line, which begins with an

attack concurrent with the soprano line, is a string of seconds that essentially

embellishes the top line.56 Except for an eighth-note neighbor, the alto line would be

equal in rhythm to the soprano. The neighbor resolution (Bb to B) is highlighted in the

disjunction apparent from the S1-dur and S1-pitch criteria that brings forth a genosegment

56
However, the alto line will diverge from the soprano by an eighth rest, as previously mentioned.
27

beginning in the second note of the motive. Thus, the phenosegment that appears is

supported by these two criteria, diverging from the S1-rest genosegment.

Figure 12a: SINGLE Analysis of Top Lines, mm. 4-8

Figure 12b: SINGLE Analysis of Bottom Lines, mm. 4-8

*Due to the pitch proximity of the pitches in m. 7, these are considered part of the tenor line despite stem
directions.

Like the soprano/alto pairing, the tenor and bass voices in these measures also

appear coupled. The tenor line could be described as an embellished arpeggiation of a

D Major chord in which the fifth is further elaborated through a string of minor seconds

in order to resolve to a B (a case which can be made due to the alto sonority held during

the cadence). The bass merely serves as a pedal tone to the figure. The SINGLE

analysis of the tenor is very straight forward in that a phenosegment arises from S1-pitch,

S1-duration, and S1-rest; there is a strong appearance of a disjunction between iterations of

the tenor motive. This is further amplified by an S1-rest genosegment in the bass (Figure

12b). Since the bass line is only one pitch with the same rhythmic duration, no

segments are supported by S1-duration or S1-pitch. A slight departure from the motivic line

occurs in measure seven, two pitches which can be considered neighboring


28

embellishments to the tenor line, an explanation supported by the S1 criteria. This is

perhaps due to a lack of rhythmic importance in support; the rhythmic simultaneity

dissolves so that by measure 7, the top voice and bottom voice motives are no longer

juxtaposed, allowing for the addition of the embellishment to the bottom voice.

Figure 13a: SIMUL Analysis of Four Parts, mm. 4-8

This figure shows the genosegments for the four individual lines (top=soprano, bottom=bass). Notice the
lack of bracket-endpoint overlap, which prevents the emergence of four-part genosegments.

Figure 13b: S1-rest Phenosegments from SIMUL Analysis of Left Hand, mm. 4-8

Figure 13c: S1-attack Genosegments from SIMUL Analysis, mm. 4-8

*Text in parentheses denotes which voices support the genosegment (S = Soprano, A = Alto, T = Tenor,
B = Bass).

A SIMUL analysis of each individual subcriterion for all four parts does not lead to

prominent genosegments (Figure 13a). This is probably due to the strong pairing of the
29

right and left hand voices and variation of attacks in the reiterations. Thus, the analysis

must also support the structure of the musical passage, leading to a SIMUL analysis of

the two top voices and the two bottom voices individually along with a combination of

the four voices. This analysis shows support for an S1-rest phenosegment in the tenor

and bass voice (Figure 13b). Also, due to the consequent attack points, the S1-attack

criterion contributes to the emergence of a phenosegment supporting segmentations

arising from pairing of voices (Figure 13c). Despite the lack of S1-attack between left and

right hands, though, there could be a case made for an S1-rest phenosegment arising for

all four voices because of the rests in the lower voices and the ambiguity for the

beginning of the S1-rest criterion (see dotted phenosegment lines in Figure 13a). This

phenosegment, however, would encompass all four measures.

Allen Forte describes this passage in two sections, measures 4 to 6 (containing a

repetition of the material) and measures 7 to 8. There is a division between the top

voices, which contains an iteration of the criterion CSC 5-Z38, and the lower parts, which

introduce the hexachord CSC 6-Z39 (Figure 14). This division into top and bottom voices is

not only supported by the aforementioned sonic criteria, but also by further contextual

criteria as explained by Forte.57 The rhythmic offset in the repetitions does not alter the

support for the individual contextual criteria, mainly due to the support from other

contextual criteria. For example, the prominence of CSC 5-Z38 is not only supported by the

same hexachord heard as the attack on the third beat of measure 4 (and thus S1-attack),

but also by the relationship to another structural hexachord, CSC 6-Z42, which is

introduced in measure 7. Also, Forte describes the bottom voices and their contextual

criteria as inclusive: a pentachord appearing in the tenor voice (CSC 5-Z37), which is
57
Forte, “Magical Kaleidoscope,” 140-141.
30

included in a hexachord with the bass pitch (CSC 6-Z39).58 These contextual criteria are

supported by the tenor S1-pitch/S1-rest/S1-duration genosegments and the bass/tenor S1-rest

phenosegment, respectively.

Figure 14: Forte’s Segmentation, mm. 4-8


(Source: Forte, “Magical Kaleidoscope,” 139.)

George Perle’s analysis describes the importance of the new passage for the

introduction of a second theme. The tenor voice’s prominent line returns throughout the

work and is derived from Perle’s basic cell CSC 3-3 and CSC 3-12. He states that these

ideas return throughout in the development and even later in the introduction as

permutations.59 The aforementioned hypothetical phenosegment between all four voices

(arising from the ambiguity of S1-rest) could show enough support for the importance of

the tenor line as a melody with cadential-harmony accompaniment.

Figure 15: Cadential Contextual Criteria, mm. 4-8

Perle further describes that the predominant factors of the musical structure (of

said second theme) are the contextual criteria Cic 3 and Cic 4 (Figure 15). These interval

58
Forte, “Magical Kaleidoscope,” 140-141.
59
Perle, Serial Composition, 14.
31

classes, which occur as cadential harmonies, are derived from the relations of the

pitches in the basic cell CSC 3-3.60 Likewise, Allen Forte also emphasizes the importance

of the CSC 3-3 as a cadential component of measure 5.61 These contextual correlations

are visually obvious, especially when naming the trichord as a component of CSC 5-Z38.

Sonically, the harmonic implications are supported at measure 4 and 6 through S1-attack

in the right hand. The rhythmic disparity, however, later causes an interaction between

the sonic criterion and the contextual; the criterion S1-attack supports CSC 3-3 at measure 4

and 6, a criterion that later heralds the sustained CSC 3-3 at measure 8 without the need

of a sonic criterion. This strong correlation leads to a prominent segmentation (as

proposed by Forte and, indirectly, by Perle).

Figure 16: Wittlich’s Segmentations, mm. 4-5


(Source: Wittlich, “Set Structure,” 45.)

Gary Wittlich compares two methods of analysis for the four-measure excerpt.

He recognizes the basic cell emphasis as described by Perle and gives his own

interpretations, which give prominence to the criteria CSC 3-3, CSC 3-1, and CSC 3-8. Wittlich

describes a trichord segmentation that is reminiscent of Perle’s in that it contains the

CSC 3-3 segmentation on the third beat of measure four, but also emphasizes a CSC 3-3

prior to that (Figure 16). To support said segmentations, Wittlich argues that Cp [78E] is
60
Perle, Serial Composition, 13.
61
Forte, “Magical Kaleidoscope,”141.
32

emphasized due to the prominence at the onset of the piece, a similar argument to

Perle’s.

Wittlich’s segmentations focus on sets of trichords, which lead to tetrachordal

and hexachordal inclusions not mentioned in the text of his article, are themselves

contextual criteria that support the segmentations.62 The sets are derived from similar

trichords employed by Perle, as mentioned before, and are also subsets to Forte’s

hexachords. Therefore, some of the similar sonic criteria support Wittlich’s analysis. The

division of top and lower voice aid in the division of segments by trichords that include

only soprano/alto or tenor/bass voices. The S1-rest for the soprano and alto helps to

differentiate the trichord on the second beat of measure 4 from the trichord including the

bass and tenor of the bottom voice. S1-rest for the alto is also support for the trichord

emphasizing the lone alto line. Due to the hierarchical nature of the phenosegments, it

was possible to state that S1-pitch supported a segmentation of the tenor line as a whole

melodic swoop, but the actual genosegments of said criterion provide support for the

analysis given by Wittlich. This same tenor line is described to be part of the hexachord

CSC 6-Z39, which is related to both CSC 6-16 and CSC 6-Z10.63 The segmentation of the line

into two hexachords is the same as Forte’s analysis and thus similarly supported.

62
Wittlich, “Set Structure,” 44-45.
63
Ibid.
33

III. ANALYSIS OF FÜNF SÄTZE, OP. 5, NO. 4

Overview

A relatively short piece, characteristic of Anton Webern’s compositional output, the

fourth movement of Op. 5 has been a favorite for analysis.64 Analyses by three theorists

are discussed in depth here: George Perle Charles Burkhart, and Joseph Straus. All

analyses agree that the piece consists of a three-part form. However, there is a bit of

contention as to where the exact boundaries lie. Burkhart believes that the sections are

demarcated by a seven-note motive (at mm. 6, 10, 13).65 Perle, however, states that the

second section occurs at measures seven through nine.66 Straus is a bit more

ambiguous, claiming that measures seven through nine are clearly a contrasting

section, yet later stating that the seven-note figure at measure ten ends the second

section.67 Taking into account the expression markings of the piece and the seemingly

punctual purpose of the seven-note motive, three sections emerge: section A (mm. 1-6),

section B (mm. 7-10) and section C (mm. 11-13). The following discussion will focus on

each of these parts as individual excerpts to facilitate examination, with a separate

section focusing on the segmentation of seven-note motive.

The orchestration of the movement allows for a clearer SINGLE analysis. Yet,

there are some areas that feature double stops and other such non-monophonic playing

64
Charles Burkhart, “The Symmetrical Source of Webern’s Opus 5, No. 4,” The Music Forum 5
(1980): 317.
65
“To me this little piece has long been a tantalizing puzzle, particularly so the three transposition
of what I call the ‘7-note figure’—that is, the rising unaccompanied figure…that demarcates the end of
each of the piece’s three sections.” Charles Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 318-319.
66
Perle, Serial Composition, 16.
67
Joseph N. Straus, Introduction to Post-Tonal Theory (Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall,
2005), 119.
34

abilities. Such figures will be analyzed to portray the aural qualities of the piece, more

than the pitches or similar attributes which appear on the written paper.

The abilities of the instruments also allow for a variety of timbral interchanges.

Dora Hanninen does not define a quality for timbre or articulation due to their

multidimensionality.68 However, an extra subcriterion will be employed, the S1-tone, which

will be, for the purposes of this thesis, a one-dimensional approach to timbre. As

suggested by Hanninen, only change will be evaluated.69 Thus, whenever a change of

bowing or sonority occurs (i.e. harmonics/arco/sul ponticello/pizzicato/tremolo), a

genosegment will arise in the S1-tone criterion.

Section A, Measures 1 – 6

The first two measures of the piece serve as an introduction to the movement, in which

the violins introduce the fundamental pitch-sets of the musical material employed

throughout.70 The rest of the section is categorized by contrapuntal interplay featuring

overlapping of the established sets.71 A slight pairing of voices begins the piece, in

which the violin one and two enter with tremolos and the viola and cello play an

accompanying role. This is quickly dissolved at measure three, during which the voices

contrapuntally share motives. The change of texture supports the division of a two-

measure quasi-introduction.

68
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 362.
69
“One interim solution is to treat timbre and articulation as nominal spaces. Musicians have long
done this in practice, registering simply the presence or absence of change: change indicates disjunction
and marks a segment boundary, whether in S1-timbre, S2-timbre, S1-articulation,
S2-articulation.” Ibid, 363.
70
Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 319; Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 120.
71
Perle, Serial Composition, 16.
35

Following a SINGLE S1-pitch analysis, it is difficult to categorize the opening

tremolos of the violins. The analyses put forth by Perle, Straus, and Burkhart pay close

attention to the pitch material of the figures. However, it is difficult to aurally discern the

exact pitch material of these ornamentations, and thus, it is a prerogative to label these

as non-pitched figures. The pizzicato in the first violin also poses an issue as to what

combination of pitch material is heard, and thus is left out of the S1-pitch analysis (as will

the last pizzicato figure of the viola part).

Figure 17: SINGLE Analysis of Violin I, mm. 1-6

A SINGLE analysis of the violin one part supports a division of the section into

three phenosegments (Figure 17). Two are strongly supported by S1-pitch, S1-duration, and

S1-rest (in mm. 3 and 4), while S1-tone also supports the first (mm. 3). A third

phenosegment arises at the end of measure four, but continues through to the next

section of the piece. However, the material after the section break does not contain

anything aside from rests, so it could be said that the phenosegment indeed ends at the

end of the section. Thus, the phenosegment contributes to the delineation of formal

structure. The violin two parts also supports this delineation, mainly due to the seven-

note motive.
36

Figure 18: SINGLE Analysis of Violin II, mm. 1-6

A phenosegment boundary arises in the violin two part at measure three,

supported by S1-pitch, S1-duration, S1-rest, and S1-tone. This new phenosegment, however,

ends immediately before the seven-note figure in measure five as supported by S1-pitch,

S1-duration, and S1-rest.The figure could be called its own phenosegment, as a boundary

arises immediately before the beginning of violin-two material in section B. However,

this boundary before section B is only supported by S1-rest and S1-tone (Figure 18).

Figure 19: SINGLE Analysis of Viola, mm. 1-6

The viola part contains two very strong phenosegments, supported by all

evaluated criteria (Figure 19). Once again, the second phenosegment lingers into

section B, but only containing rest material before the onset of viola ostinato in measure

7. The musical material of the viola is not as varied as that of the violins, as the
37

instrument only plays for pitches in the span of six measures. Thus, the main breaking

segmentation points are characterized by its steep change in register and its change to

am Steg (in measure 4).

Figure 20a: SINGLE Analysis of Cello, mm. 1-6

Figure 20b: S1-pitch Analysis of Cello, mm. 3-4

The two bottom staves show the division of the cello


part into two separate lines. Thus, a nesting of S1-pitch
occurs in the original line (top).

The cello part contains perhaps the most ambiguous material of the section. The

strongest phenosegments would only arise from support by three criteria at any one

time (Figure 20a). The double stop at measure four adds to this ambiguity: the addition

of the extra note juxtaposes boundary notes, in essence abolishing said disjunction.

Thus, if the G were placed linearly next to the C-sharp, a genosegment boundary would
38

occur. However, this slight temporal modification liquefies the segmentation, creating a

type of S1-pitch elision (Figure 20b).

Despite a general agreement between genosegments for the individual parts, a

SIMUL analysis of the four instrumental parts does not show a large amount of

consensus. Generally, the S1-tone criterion is highly inconsequential in the scope of the

SIMUL analysis. A phenosegment does seem to have a bit of support at the end of

measure four, where the violin one and viola parts interact to form a disjunction in the

S1-pitch, S1-duration, and S1-rest criteria at measure four, along with some participation from

the cello in the S1-pitch criteria (Figure 21).

Figure 21: SIMUL Phenosegment from Selected Genosegments, mm. 1-6

George Perle’s analysis of the movement reveals a strong influence of the

tremolo pitch material. His ideas stem from the transposition of an intervallic cell. The

fundamental material is a semitone dyad transposed by a tritone, thus creating criteria

CSC 4-8 and CSC 4-9. Perle pays close attention to the iterations of these sets as they

appear at measure three (Figure 22a)—CCS 4-8[E045] and CCS 4-9[E056]. These sets contain
39

three common tones, excluding pitch classes E and F-sharp, a dyad that appears as the

highest pitches of the opening tremolos.72 This segmentation is not supported by the

aforementioned analysis due to the aural qualities of the tremolo. However, a later

melodic iteration of the dyad in the viola (measures two and three) is highly supported

by genosegments arising from the SINGLE S1-pitch/rest/duration criteria (Figure 22b). The

dyad also appears at the end of the section (mm. 5-6) in the violin one and cello parts,

two iterations that are minimally supported by the respective S1-pitch criterion.

Perle alludes to segmentations of CSC 4-8 and CSC 4-9 throughout measures three

to six, yet only clearly describes five iterations (Figure 22a).73 The SINGLE analysis

supports iterations occurring in violin one (Violin one S1-pitch/duration/rest, m. 3) and violin

two (Violin two S1-pitch/duration/rest, m. 4). The cello figure (mm. 4-5) is not strongly

supported, however, as the phenosegment arising from any SINGLE criteria extends to

measure six. The CSC 4-8 occurring in measure three is also not supported by any

SINGLE criteria, but the S1-attack criterion supports the inclusion of the viola pitch on the

upbeat of beat two. The more interesting support, however, occurs at the downbeat of

measure four where the all of the parts sound simultaneously. The SINGLE criteria for

the second violin, which are not included in the segmentation, are actually the support

as the melodic line genosegments detract from the inclusion. Once again, like in the

cello part in measure two, a quasi-elision has occurred and thus, aids the support for

the harmonic segmentation.

72
Perle, Serial Composition, 16.
73
Ibid.
40

Figure 22a: Perle’s Segmentation, mm. 3-5 and Support


(Segmentation Source: Perle, Serial Composition, 16.)

*Violin 1 = V1, Violin 2 = V2, Viola = Va, Cello = C


Figure 22b: Phenosegment Support for E/F-sharp Dyad in the Viola, mm. 1-4

Charles Burkhart’s analysis has many similarities to Perle’s. He establishes the

importance of the CSC 4-9 and CSC 4-8 criteria, but also connects them to larger over-

arching concepts, including a CSC 8-9 criterion and the multiple ways in which the eight-

note set is realized in order to create symmetry. Burkhart describes the opening tremolo

measures as segments including a CSC 4-9, CSC 4-8 and CSC 4-16 (Figure 23a).74 The

genosegments created by S1-attack, S1-tone, S1-duration, and S1-rest show great support for the

74
Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 319-324.
41

harmonic iterations of CSC 4-9 and CSC 4-8 in the violins. The CSC 4-16, occurring in the violin

one part, is well supported by the same melodic criterion.

Figure 23a: Burkhart’s Segmentation, mm. 1-3 and Supporting Criteria


(Segmentation Source: Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 319-321.)

Figure 23b: Burkhart’s Segmentation, mm. 3-6


(Source: Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 321.)
42

Just like Perle, Burkhart describes the importance of the dyad occurring in the

top register of violin one, as well the polyphonic interplay of CSC 4-9 and CSC 4-8 in

measures three through six. Burkhart, however, delineates the tetrachords, seven in

total, four of which are identical to Perle’s segmentations (Figure 23b).75 The remaining

segmentations are a little difficult to show support for using sonic criteria. Some of the

segmentation can be supported in individual parts but not through the four parts. For

example, the violin and cello parts in the CSC 4-9 segment of measure 5 are supported by

S1-pitch, yet the viola part has no support for said segmentation. In this context, it is easier

to explain the support through the inherent contextual criteria of the segmentation.

It is logical that Joseph Straus’s analysis would build on previous analyses. While

the overarching theories digress (Perle’s basic cell versus Burkhart’s symmetry), the

three segmentations studied increasingly build on each other and share many ideas.

Straus’s analysis is a bit more pedagogical (due to its inclusion in an atonal theory

textbook) yet it contains a very intricate and well-defined web of segmentations.

Straus, like the other analysts, emphasizes the sets that underlie the first

measures of the movement. However, he also makes mention of a CSC 5-19 appearing in

measure two, a figure which includes the cello’s E-flat pedal (Figure 24a). Straus

defines this set due to its complementary nature against CSC 7-19, the fundamental set of

the seven-note motive.76 The S1-duration for the cello voice is the only sonic supporter of

said segmentation, but the relationship between the aforementioned contextual criteria

is strong.

75
Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 319-324.
76
Later discussed in detail. Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 122.
43

Figure 24a: Straus’s Segmentation, mm. 1-2


(Source: Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 120-122.)

Figure 24b: Straus’s Segmentation, mm. 4-5


(Source: Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 124.)

Straus also makes mention of trichord segmentations, which have generally been

ignored by Perle and Burkhart. CSC 3-4 and CSC 3-5 are both subsets of CSC 4-8 and CSC 4-9

and play an important role in contrapuntal interplay of measures four and five, or the

canon section, as Straus nicknames it (Figure 24b).77 Melodically, Straus identifies CSC

3-4 in the violin one part, which is strongly supported by S1-pitch, and the cello part, which

contains limited support by the same. Harmonically, the segmentations are also

77
Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 124-125.
44

apparent on every beat of measure five, interchanging between CSC 3-4 and CSC 3-5.78

However, only two of these harmonic segmentations are truly supported by any of the

sonic criteria analyzed: the pickup to measure five, the upbeat of the first beat, and the

second beat (all supported by S1-attack).

Section B, Measures 7 – 11

The texture of the piece once again changes in measure seven as the three lower parts

take subservient roles and the first violin takes on the melody. The section ends with a

seven-note punctuation in measure ten, which also heralds a tempo change on the

downbeat of measure eleven (the beginning of the next section).

Figure 25: S1-rest Genosegments, mm. 7-9

*The genosegments are arranged by part (Violin I = top; Cello = bottom). Notice how the section is
marked by rests on both sides, thus creating a segment despite how the genosegments line up.

Due to the shorter length of this section, it is not necessary to divide it into parts

like the first section, and no strong phenosegments appear that would indicate

otherwise. However, it is imperative to point out that a strong S1-rest offsets section B

78
Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 124.
45

from the rest of the movement (Figure 25). The viola sets up an ostinato that is strongly

supported by the SINGLE analysis criteria S1-rest, S1-tone, and S1-duration. To create

harmonic interest, the cello and second violin add pedals, individual lines which are

supported by the cello’s S1-rest/pitch/tone and the violin’s S1-rest. No strong SIMUL

phenosegments exist, however, most likely due to the offsetting of the parts’ attacks.

The violin one contains the melodic interest, and contains a highly sectionalized line

according to S1-pitch/rest/duration.

Figure 26: Perle’s Segmentation, mm. 7-9


(Source: Perle, Serial Composition, 16-18.)

George Perle focuses on the ostinato and the relationships between the voices.

He denotes a segmentation that emphasizes the criterion CSC 4-16 emerging from the

combination of the pitches for the three accompaniment voices (Figure 26).79 It could be

said that the delay of the first violin causes the accompaniment to be more audible and

therefore a segmentation would be supported. In this case, the most prominent sonic

criteria would be the S1-rest, despite no support for a true phenosegment. Perle also

points out a prominent dyad (E/F-sharp), which appears in the cello and viola (as a G-

79
Perle, Serial Composition, 18.
46

flat).80 Due to the high harmonic range of the cello, these two pitches sound next to

each other in range. A segmentation is supported by the S1-attack criterion due to the

simultaneity of the cello’s attack and the first G-flat of the ostinato. As the viola ostinato

dies out (in m. 9), the violins and cello come to rest on CSC 3-4, establishing the

foundation for a CSC 4-8 that arises with the last pitch played by the viola.81 There are no

strong sonic criteria to support this segment, and thus the support must rely on the

contextual criteria and the simultaneity.

Figure 27: Straus’s Segmentation of the Viola Ostinato, mm. 7-9 and Support
(Segmentation Source: Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 121.)

Burkhart’s analysis of these measures is highly focused on proving his theory

behind symmetrical analysis. Thus, his segmentations are limited to the CSC 4-16 as

proposed by Perle and a discussion of their relation to the original CSC 4-16 appearing in

the first violin in measures one and two.82 Similarly, Straus focuses on the

accompaniment’s emergent CSC 4-16 set.83 However, he also mentions the subset

produced by the viola line alone, a CSC 3-12. This new, completely distinct subset is used

for contrast.84 The viola’s S1-pitch criterion shows many divisions during this ostinato,

each of which supports the subset as it matches the three-note division pattern

established by the viola (Figure 27).

80
Perle, Serial Composition, 16.
81
Ibid., 18.
82
Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 325-327.
83
Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 121.
84
Ibid., 119.
47

Section A’, Measures 11 – 13

The third section of this movement does not contain material that is musically similar to

that of the first section. As Straus mentions, though, the final section still sounds very

similar to the beginning.85 This is due to, as Straus states, a similar pizzicato figure to

that of measure two and the stark contrast of sets employed in section B. Thus, a return

to the initial sets creates a sense of closure.

Figure 28: Phenosegments and Genosegments, mm. 11-13

*Violin I = V1, Violin II = V2, Viola = Va, Cello = C

The last three measures of the movement contain genosegments that support a

stark segmentation into two phenosegments, a disjunction that occurs before the

pizzicato at measure twelve (Figure 28). The first violin line does not contain the

pizzicato or any material after the second beat of measure twelve, and thus is not

included in the segmentation. The SINGLE analysis shows a strong support for the

85
Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 122.
48

phenosegment in the other lines. The viola and cello lines contain no material after the

pizzicato, yet they both show S1-tone/rest support for a phenosegment break before it (the

viola also includes a S1-pitch criterion). The only voice that contains music afterward, the

violin two splits the pizzicato into a phenosegment that is supported through

S1-pitch/tone/rest. Due to the strong, and similar, phenosegments arising from the SINGLE

analysis, the SIMUL analysis shows the same support stemming from S1-rest/tone/pitch.

Figure 29: Comparison of Burkhart and Straus Segmentations, m. 12


(Sources: Burkhart, “Symmetry Sources,” 329; Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 122.)

George Perle does discuss the final section in depth. He mainly focuses on the

seven-note motive and its relation to the material in section A.86 Burkhart, on the other

hand, brings the piece full-circle by discussing the return of CSC 4-9 and the transposition

of this set that contributes to his symmetrical theory.87 As he states, the pizzicato chord

nearing the end (at measure twelve) is one of the most structurally important moments

of the movement, a statement that mirrors Straus’ comments about its formal

86
Perle, Serial Composition, 17-18.
87
Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 327-330.
49

implications. As Straus segments the chord, it harmonically forms a CSC 4-9 criterion.88

Burkhart segments this harmonic criterion, but also combines it with other pitch classes

from the sonorities that appear earlier in the measure (Figure 29).89 By itself, the

pizzicato chord is most strongly supported through S1-attack. However, the segmentation

including the other pitches is a bit more ambiguous and the only strong support appears

to be the contextual criterion. Burkhart also mentions a CSC 4-28 set that is more relevant

to the discussion of the seven-note motive.90

The Seven-Note Motive

The main emphasis on the fourth movement of Webern’s Op. 5 is placed on a seven-

note motive that appears three times during the work (Figure 30). The analysis of the

motive has led to the discussion of its influence on the form of the movement and the

sets employed.

The figure itself is comprised of CSC 7-19 and it is generally permutated through

transposition and rhythmic change. A SINGLE analysis supports a phenosegment of the

motive during all three appearances. The first, in measure six, is supported in the

second violin by S1-pitch/duration/rest and the second appearance, in measure ten, is

supported by S1-duration/rest/tone. The last iteration, however, is the only one that is fully

supported by all four analyzed SINGLE sonic criteria (S1-pitch/tone/rest/duration).

88
Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 122.
89
Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 329.
90
Ibid., 330.
50

Figure 30: Appearances of the Seven-Note Motive, mm. 6, 10, 12-13.

George Perle’s discussion of the seven-note motive focuses on the inherent

relationship stemming from the basic cell described above. Thus the first iteration of the

motive includes the exact initial four-note CSC 4-16 in the first violin part.91 Not only that,

but the two notes of semitone dyad (E/F-sharp) are also adjacent (Figure 31a). No

91
Perle, Serial Composition, 16.
51

sonic criterion supports this segmentation of the motive into a tetrachord and a trichord,

yet the dyad segmentation is supported by S1-pitch. The contextual relations also serve

as support for both the segmentation of the motive and the segmentation occurring in

measure one. The second iteration of the motive contains the CSC 4-16 set in the middle

(the third through sixth notes; Figure 31b).92 Again, there is no strong support for a

segmentation that would divide the motive into a dyad, a tetrachord, and a dyad. Yet,

once again the appearance of the dyad is supported by a genosegment in the S1-pitch

criterion of the viola. No direct CSC 4-16 from the beginning of the movement appears in

the last appearance of the motive and thus a segmentation is mostly dependent on the

phenosegment that groups the septachord together (violin two, S1-pitch/duration/rest/tone).

Figure 31a: Perle’s Segmentation of the First Figure 31b: Perle’s Segmentation of the Second
Seven-Note Motive, m. 6 Seven-Note Motive, m. 10
(Source: Perle, Serial Composition, 16.) (Source: Perle, Serial Composition, 18).

Charles Burkhart’s analysis of the seven-note motive is similar to Perle’s but is

also dependent on the appearance of incomplete CSC 4-28 criteria that relates to an

inventory of pitch classes for the movement. Thus, he argues that the seven-note

motives are really based on eight-note groups, an explanation he uses to prove his

points about symmetry.93 The contextual criteria, CSC 4-28, however, is not present in the

92
Perle, Serial Composition, 16.
93
Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 322.
52

iteration of the motives as Burkhart segments them, so it does not work to support said

segmentation (Figure 32a). On the other hand, a CSC 3-10 is apparent, and thus can be

supported by the contextual criteria.

Figure 32a: Burkhart’s Segmentation of Figure 32b: Burkhart’s Segmentation of the Second
the Seven-Note Motive, m. 6 Seven-Note Motive, mm. 10-11
(Source: Burkhart, “Symmetrical (Source: Burkhart, “Symmetrical Sources,” 325-327.)
Sources,” 322.)

Notice that the last segment is a trichord,


which Burkhart labels as an incomplete CSC 4-
28. Here, it is labeled as the appropriate
trichord set.

Like Perle’s analysis, Burkhart’s segmentation of the first iteration of the seven-

note motive focuses on a tetrachord (CSC 4-16) followed by a trichord (CSC 4-28). The

groupings of the last two appearances of the motive, however, vary from Perle’s:

Burkhart follows the pattern of tetrachord and trichord to segment the motives into a CSC

4-16 and CSC 4-28 once again (or CSC 3-10 for the purposes of this analysis).94 The sonic

criteria still, however, do not support these segmentations. There is a stronger case

made for the contextual criteria and its repetitive nature in the three motives, however,

along with the aforementioned segmentation that lingers into measure eleven (Figure

32b). The beginning of each part (except the second violin) re-states pitch-classes from

94
Burkhart, “Symmetrical Source,” 326.
53

the end of the seven-note motive. Despite no sonic-criteria support, the contextual

criterion does uphold the segmentation.

Straus follows Perle’s segmentations of the seven-note motives.95 However, he

also makes two very important observations, which may add answers to the questions

raised by the movement’s musical structure. As mentioned before, measure two

contains a segmentation of a CCS 5-19, which is the complement to the seven-note

motive’s CCS 7-19.96 The segmentation is mainly supported by S1-rest criteria. There is also

a complementary set to CSC 4-9 in the last two measures of the movement (CSC 8-9;

Figure 33).97 This segmentation contains the pizzicato chord and the seven-note motive,

so as mentioned before, the sonic criteria support is very strong (Figure 28).

Figure 33: CSC 8-09 Segmentation, mm. 12-13


(Source: Straus, Post-Tonal Theory, 122.)

95
Straus, Post-tonal Theory, 119-124.
96
Ibid., 122-123.
97
Ibid.
54

IV. CONCLUSION

Discussion

This study served two main purposes: to describe the contextual and sonic criteria

apparent in the musical examples, and to describe how the criteria may have supported

the segmentations by several theorists. The analyses were, indeed, limited and

sampled a very small part of the large body of work that has been implemented on

segmentation analysis. No single thesis would be able to discern all of the intricate inner

workings of each excerpt. However, it is interesting to note that, however cursory and

contextually based the analyses were, each theorist’s analysis was supported by a

significant amount of both contextual and sonic criteria.

Some of the analyses included individual segmentations that were wholly

dependent on contextual criteria, for example, Burkhart’s and Straus’s analyses of the

canonic section in measures four through six of Webern’s Op. 5, No. 4. It is possible

that the highly trained theorist did indeed hear these segmentations. Other explanations

are also probable: these types of segmentations rely on the existence of a similar

contextual criterion for support. In that case, prior contextual criteria were introduced in

the first measures of the piece and were supported by sonic criteria. Thus, in essence,

the aural cues that influenced the initial segment may indirectly influence the perception

of the latter contextual criteria; the contextual criteria that seem to have no sonic

support are subconsciously recalled in the mind through recurring aural cues.
55

Dora Hanninen believes that there should be a minimum of sonic support for

segmentations since “we must be able to hear them as musical units.”98 It is arguable

that the amount of sonic support needed to justify an analysis depends on the purpose

of said analysis. However, whether the theorist decides to employ an aural perspective

or not, music is an auditory experience. Despite the amount of emphasis placed on

contextual criteria in order to define groups of musical material, this thesis shows that

sonic criteria serve as the majority of support for the segmentations. Also, the

contextual criteria employed could also be aural cues for certain trained listeners as

corroborated by Hanninen.99 Whichever criteria serves as aural support, it is apparent

that the theorists are, in fact, processing their segmentations musically, however

subconscious their musical judgment and whatever their analytical aims may be.

Sonic and contextual criteria form an intricate web of possible segmentations as

expounded by Hanninen,100 and supported by the analyses of the two pieces in this

thesis. In more than one instance, sonic criteria alone did not describe the support for

the contextual criteria. For example, in the analysis for measures four through six of

Schoenberg’s Op. 11, No. 1, the segmentation CSC 3-3 was originally supported by sonic

criteria. However, as the motives were rhythmically rearranged, the sonic criteria failed

to support later segmentations of said contextual criterion. The earlier sonic support,

however, solidified that segment in the mind, and thus when it reappeared in a different

manner, the contextual criterion was the one that supported the segmentation.

98
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 413.
99
“Sonic and contextual criteria identify features of a sound-surface (sonic disjunctions and
contextual associations) that may be grasped by basic perceptual and cognitive faculties.” Ibid., 357.
100
Ibid., 413.
56

It is, of course, possible to segment depending solely on either contextual or

sonic criteria. Theories such as Allen Forte’s Structure of Atonal Music rely on

contextual criteria; thus, an exclusive sonic-criteria-based analysis would only hinder the

contributions made by such theories. Going further, Hanninen describes contextual

criteria as sometimes bolstering or even displacing sonic criteria.101 However, an only

contextual-criteria based analysis would detract from the musical context. Sure, an

experienced listener may be able to hear sets and retrogrades of those sets, yet the

average listener may not. Just like when analyzing tonal or any other type of music, it is

important to employ both sonic and contextual criteria, as indirectly done by Straus,

Wittlich, Forte, Perle, and Burkhart, in order to better understand the depth of the

musical structure.

Despite their subconscious musical thinking, the aforementioned theorists lacked

a precise and common language to communicate their musical findings. All of them

were able to relay their findings, yet some of the writing described the segmentations as

if they were self-evident; take, for example, a passage from Perle’s discussion of

Webern’s Op. 5, No. 4: “At the same time the figure as a whole is a linearization of

elements comprised within the initial transpositions of x and y, from the conclusion of

bar 3 through the beginning of bar 4.”102 Of course, Perle wrote his analyses before the

development of a common segmentation language,103 yet, despite the theorists’ ability

to discuss their contextual criteria findings, it seemed difficult to communicate the ways

in which the music portrayed their analyses.

101
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 413.
102
Perle, Serial Composition, 16.
103
For example, Allen Forte’s The Structure of Atonal Music.
57

Further Research

It is without question that much work still remains to be done in segmentation theory.

This thesis works to describe the criteria that may have influenced segmentations of

Schoenberg’s Op. 11, No. 1 and Webern’s Op. 5, No. 4. Although only hinted, this study

proposes the development of a polyphonic method that would work with Dora

Hanninen’s theory. However, this method is still far from perfect and much still needs to

be done in order to make it adaptable for more complex polyphonic textures.

As discussed in the introduction, there are many doubts about how humans

perceive polyphony. It is difficult even for a computer program to describe the exact

segmentations and combinations of sounds that are extracted from a musical stream.104

Even so, who is to say that the rules of perception for monophonic music are fully

understood? This thesis has combined two theories in order to arrive at some

understanding of polyphonic ideas: Hanninen’s methods for criteria analysis and

Lefkowitz and Taavola’s imperfect SINGLE versus SIMUL methodologies. The result is

not precise, but it is an important step in a very intricate and winding path toward the

understanding of a widely used musical cognitive function.

Dora Hanninen defines subcriteria that give insights to the potential for analysis

of polyphonic music. As employed in this thesis, S1-attack is a fundamental sonic criterion

that defines simultaneity of attacks. The development of subcriteria to define other

polyphonic attributes is required for further work. For example, throughout the analysis,

there was a need for describing simultaneity of notes, despite a lack of concurrent

104
Lefkowitz and Taavola attempt to do this with a SIMUL analysis, yet the matching of voices
and mathematical operations to decipher differences in pitch height and other such criteria do not break
the code of human polyphonic perception.
58

attacks (for example, to define harmonies). This lack of subcriteria is, thus, detrimental

to a full polyphonic analysis.

Closing Thoughts

Segmentation is an inherent practice needed for musical perception. The process of

analytical segmentation is an attempt to understand the cognitive workings of the

musical human mind. Thus, the methods of analysis should include cognitive roots in

order to truly grasp the musical understanding. As Dora Hanninen states in her writing,

it is imperative for theorists to be aware of the underpinnings that may influence their

analyses in order to achieve a more fruitful musical experience as it extends not only to

listening, but also performance, music instruction, and other musical endeavors.105

105
Hanninen, “Orientations, Criteria, Segments,” 347.
59

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Baars, Bernard J. and Nicole M. Gage, eds. Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness.
London: Academic Press, 2007.

Beach, David W. “Pitch Structure and the Analytic Process in Atonal Music: An
Interpretation of the Theory of Sets.” Music Theory Spectrum 1 (Spring 1979): 7-
22.

Burkhart, Charles. “The Symmetrical Source of Webern’s Opus 5, No. 4.” The Music
Forum 5 (1980): 317-334.

Forte, Allen. “The Magical Kaleidoscope: Schoenberg’s First Atonal Masterwork, Opus
11, Number 1.” Journal of the Arnold Schoenberg Institute 5, no. 2 (November
1981): 127-168.

Forte, Allen. The Structure of Atonal Music. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press,
1973.

Hanninen, Dora A. “Orientations, Criteria, Segments: A General Theory of


Segmentation for Music Analysis.” Journal of Music Theory 45, no. 2 (Autumn
2001): 345-433.

Hasty, Christopher. “Segmentation and Process in Post-Tonal Music.” Music Theory


Spectrum 3 (Spring 1981): 54-73.

The International Society for Gestalt Theory and its Applications. Gestalt Theory and its
Applications. http://www.gestalttheory.net.

Lefkowitz, David S. and Kristin Taavola. “Segmentation in Music: Generalizing a Piece-


Sensitive Approach.” Journal of Music Theory 44, no. 1 (Spring 2000): 171-229.

Lerdahl, Fred and Ray Jackendoff. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge,
MA: The MIT Press, 1983.

Schoenberg, Arnold. Drei Klavierstücke. Vienna: Universal Edition, 1924.

Straus, Joseph N. Introduction to Post-Tonal Theory. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall, 2005.

Tenney, James and Larry Polansky. “Temporal Gestalt Perception in Music.” Journal of
Music Theory 24, no. 2 (Autumn 1980): 205-241.

Webern, Anton. Fünf Sätze für Streichquartett. Vienna: Universal Edition, 1922.
60

Wittlich, Gary. “Interval Set Structure in Schoenberg’s Op. 11 No. 1.” Perspectives of
New Music 13, no. 1 (Autumn-Winter 1974): 41-55.
61

APPENDIX A: SCORES

SCHOENBERG: DREI KLAVIERSTÜCKE, Op. 11, No. 1


Excerpt, mm. 1-12

Source: Arnold Schoenberg, Drei Klavierstücke (Vienna: Universal Edition, 1924).

NOTE: Only measures 1-8 are studied in this thesis. Measures 9-12 are given for
contextual reference.
62

WEBERN: FÜNF SÄTZE, Op. 5, No. 4

Source: Anton Webern, Fünf Sätze für Streichquartett (Vienna: Universal Edition, 1922).

You might also like