Kats Laze Feld
Kats Laze Feld
Kats Laze Feld
The decade following World War II saw the rapid rise of research on the effects
mediated messages – first in print form, and later through radio and television broadcasts.
Many, in fact, feared the assumed power of the media to shape and control the ideas,
that the assumed ability of the media to control people’s attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors
was not being supported by media effect research. In his oft-cited book summarizing the
findings of mass communication studies to date, Klapper (1960) concluded that “mass
communication ordinarily does not serve as a necessary and sufficient cause of audience
effects, but rather functions among and through a nexus of mediating factors and
influences . . . [and] when mass communication does affect people, these effects tend to
assumed that the media were powerful social tools for shaping and altering the health-
related attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors of people. However, that assumption quickly
came into question as many mediated health campaigns produced either minimal or no
overall effects on various health outcomes (COMMIT, 1995; Farquhar et al., 1990;
Hornik, 2002; Lowery & DeFleur, 1995; Luepker et al.,1994; Rogers, 1998; Wallack,
1990; Winkleby et al., 1996). In short, health campaign scholars, like their mass
2
communication counterparts, discovered that “the mass media are not the magic bullet of
At the same time, health education researchers recognized that the mass media
that there is major change in health behavior, and also that this behavior
In short, contemporary health education scholars have concluded that the mass media can
influence health outcomes, but the nature of that role is poorly understood and certainly
far more complex than had once been suspected (Lowery & DeFleur, 1995; Wallack,
1990).
The complex nature of the influence of the media on health outcomes necessitates
a major change in thinking about the way researchers go about studying the effects of
mediated health campaigns on health outcomes. For one, research designs must focus
outcomes, and more attention on tracing more subtle and indirect patterns of influence
(Lowery & DeFleur, 1995). More importantly, though, health education scholars need to
rethink the theoretical underpinnings of their health campaign approaches. It is this latter
concern that is the focus of attention of this paper. Specifically, this paper presents, or
theory, and explicates how it can used to enhance the efficacy of mediated health
campaigns.
introduced a theoretical framework of concepts and ideas for understanding media effects
that departed radically from earlier thinking about the media (Lowery & DeFleur, 1995).
At the center of their framework was the notion of a “two-step flow of communication”
(Katz & Lazarsfeld, 1955, p. 32). Unlike earlier notions that assumed a direct flow of
information (and influence) from the media to mass audiences, the “two-step flow”
concept posited a movement of information and ideas from the media to “opinion
leaders,” and from them to other people in their social network (“primary group”). In
short, Katz and Lazarsfeld theorized that mass media messages affect people’s
communication about the messages’ content among friends and colleagues who comprise
knowledge/attitudes /behaviors).
Over the years, Katz and Lazarsfeld “two-step communication” theory acquired
the label of “intermedia process” (Gumpert & Cathcart, 1986), and appeared in the work
of a variety of researchers (e.g., Boekeloo et al., 1993; Kalichman & Hunter, 1992;
Meyer et al., 1980; Miller, 1987; Rogers et al., 1997; Singhal et al., 1996; Valente, Pope,
& Merritt, 1996; Wanta & Elliott, 1995). For the most part, however, the fundamental
ideas of Katz and Lazarsfeld’s “two-step flow” (or “intermedia”) theory has remained
Intermedia Theory
Intermedia theory begins with the proposition that the individual opinions, beliefs,
attitudes, values, and behaviors that media campaigns seek to modify are anchored in
“primary groups” (Katz & Lazarsfeld, 1955, p. 44). A “primary group” is defined by
family members, neighbors, and the like, that is characterized by regular interaction (or
collectively and continuously to generate and to maintain common ideas and behavior
patterns which they are reluctant to surrender or to modify unilaterally” (p. 44). They
point to the oft-cited “Hawthorne studies” (Rothlisberger & Dickson, 1939) as the first
people’s attitudes, values, and behaviors. This study found that despite the efforts of
management, workers at the Western Electric Company did not act in an individualistic
manner to maximize their own rewards, but instead self-regulated their output in
accordance with the norms and expectations of their fellow workers with whom they had
values, and behaviors are influenced by the primary group s/he beolongs to.
1. People benefit from sharing the opinions, attitudes, and behaviors of those with
whom they wish to be identified. Early group research established that positive outcomes
like acceptance (Newcomb, 1952; Rothlisberger & Dickson, 1939), social status
(Festinger, Schachter, & Back, 1950; Homans, 1950), upward mobility (Stouffer, 1949;
5
Warner & Lunt, 1941), and leadership (Merei, 1952) are linked to an individual’s
conformity to the norms, attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors of his/her primary group.
argue that because many aspects of everyday life are highly equivocal (i.e., subject to
multiple interpretations), we must rely on others to help us make sense of things (Weick,
1979). In short, what we believe to be true is, in large part, determined by what others
close to us likewise believe to be true (Berger & Luckman, 1966). As Festinger et al.
. . . the “social reality” upon which an opinion or an attitude rests for its
attitude in wrong and which is right in connection with social opinions and
attitudes as there are in the case of what might be called “facts” . . . . The
“reality” which settles the question in the case of social attitudes and
communication are believed to share these opinions and attitudes (p. 168).
evaluations. Mass communication scholars have long recognized that people do not
evaluate a media message solely on the merits of the message. Rather, they compare the
advocated position against those held by people in their social network, and then decide
whether the media message should accepted. Thus, when an individual is presented with
a media message, s/he is likely to discuss the message with his/her peers to gauge their
6
assessments of the message. Social network opinions are compared against the media
intermedia theory, a media message is likely to have its greatest influence when its
al. (1988), for example, found that when ninth-graders were presented with media
messages that warned them about the negative consequences of smoking, they often
turned to their friends to confirm that information. The researchers found that very few
ninth-graders were concerned about death from lung cancer as a consequence of smoking
because none of their friends expressed such concerns in social interactions. However,
many teens were very concerned about such negative consequences of smoking as bad
breath, loss of friends, and trouble with adults because those consequences were validated
in conversations with their friends. In short, intermedia theory posits that media
messages are most likely to be effective when they are reinforced or validated in social
network interactions.
Media messages tell people what they should do, but rarely provide instructions about
how to do it. For example, anti-smoking messages tell people to quit smoking, but do not
tell them how to go about doing so. Thus, people often do not perform what they are told
to do in a media message, not because they disagree with the message, but because they
lack the requisite knowledge or skills for doing so. In such cases, an individual will seek
the advice or guidance of those in his/her social network who the individual believes
possesses the requisite knowledge or skills to help him/her engage in the called-for
quit smoking, will ask a friend who has successfully quit smoking how s/he went about
feedback is a reason why studies have found that the most effective media messages are
those that are supplemented with interpersonal instruction. For example, Meyer et al.
diet, and exercise. The researchers chose three cities that were comparable in
characteristics and demographics. Two of the cities received a multimedia campaign, and
in one of those cities, high-risk individuals received both media programs and the
opportunity for interpersonal instruction. The third city served as the control city. The
study found that the multimedia campaign had a strong impact on knowledge and
behavior, with the group receiving both media and interpersonal instruction showing the
greatest improvements.
typically provide us with information designed to influence our beliefs, attitudes, and
ultimately our behaviors. However, media messages seldom provide us with information
about social norms – that is, what our friends, family, and associates think we ought to
do. These social norms must usually be obtained through direct interaction with our
peers, and hence we typically talk to our friends about what they think we ought to do in
light of the suggestions of media messages. For example, researchers have found that
men who have unprotected sex prior to being presented with media messages about the
dangers of unprotected sex, often talked to their peers about whether they should go for
an HIV test prior to actually going to the clinic to receive the test (Boekeloo et al., 1993;
Not only do primary groups influence the attitudes, beliefs, values, and behaviors
of individuals, but they also serve as channels for mass media transmission. According
to intermedia theory, within a primary group, some members are more available to media
pass on what they read, hear, or see in the media to others within their network who are
less exposed to the media. Gatekeepers are not passive relays of information in a primary
group. Rather, they often exert their own biasing effect on mediated messages by either
reinforcing or counteracting the messages they relay to others in their primary group
(Katz & Lazarsfeld, 1955, p. 45). In other words, gatekeepers have the ability to alter the
theory, gatekeepers can have two types of biasing effects: promotive and inhibitive
influence.
and Hunter (1992), data were gathered from men waiting for mass transportation in
downtown Chicago, before and after Earvin “Magic” Johnson’s televised press
because he was diagnosed as having the HIV virus. The researchers found a marked
increase in perceptions of AIDS, with an increased concern about AIDS, and greater
interest in AIDS-related information. They also found that all of the men interviewed in
the study had heard, or read, about Magic Johnson’s HIV infection; most (86%) had
engaged in frequent interpersonal discussion about AIDS with their friends during the
three days following his news conference; and most reported that their interpersonal
9
interactions with peers had reinforced their concerns about HIV and AIDS. In short,
Gatekeepers can also inhibit the effects of media messages. People do not simply
buy into what the media sources say, and often they openly express their disagreement or
message. In the study by Baumann et al. (1988), media messages purporting a causal
relationship between smoking and lung cancer deaths were rejected by ninth-graders
Rogers et al. (1997), it was found that people rejected family-planning information
family debunked the information presented in the messages. In both these cases, it is
group exert disproportinately greater influence on the attitudes, beliefs, values, and
behaviors of others in the group. Katz and Lazarsfeld (1955) refer to these individuals as
“opinion leaders” (p. 32) . In some primary groups, the same individual plays the roles of
gatekeeper and opinion leader. However, it is often the case that the gatekeeper and
opinion leader are two different people (p. 119). Opinion leaders, according to Katz and
Lazarsfeld, are “distributed in all occupational groups, and on every social and economic
level” (p. 32). They are “an integral part of the give-and-take of everyday personal
10
relationships” and can be thought of as individuals whose opinions and views are highly
regarded by members of the primary group. Because opinion leaders are highly respected
in the group, individuals often look to confirm and validate their own views and opinions
by comparing them to those held by the opinion leader(s). Opinion leaders thus play a
key role in shaping the attitudes, values, beliefs, and behavioral patterns of the entire
primary group (p. 33). More importantly, according to intermedia theory, they are the
individuals who essentially intervene between mass media messages and resultant
opinions, decisions, and actions by recipients of those messages. Opinion leaders, like
gatekeepers, then, can also promote or negate media messages circulating within the
group.
Intermedia theory posits that media messages have the best chance of making a
difference if gatekeepers and opinion leaders exert promotive influence within their
primary groups. If the gatekeepers and opinion leaders exert an inhibitive influence, the
media message has little chance of producing its desired effect. The key, then, is to
design media messages that lead to promotive communication within primary groups.
groups. The first of those is the extent to which the message content corresponds with
the prevailing opinions or views of the group. When media messages are inconsistent
gatekeepers are unlikely to relay the messages, or if they do, are likely to put a negative
spin on them. Similarly, opinion leaders are likely to counteract media messages that
assail prevailing attitudes, opinions, or habits (Katz & Lazarsfeld, 1955, p. 74).
11
communication within primary groups the extent to which the message is identified with
more likely to enlist the support of gatekeepers and opinion leaders. However, if the
message advocates a behavior that goes against established group norms, gatekeepers are
likely to prevent it from reaching the group, or opinion leaders are likely to advocate
quality of evidence contained in the message. Studies have shown that a media message
to people (Reinard, 1988). There are two aspects to evidence that make it believable to
people. The first is its credibility; in general, we are more likely to believe evidence from
a trustworthy source than one we are skeptical about. A second is its plausibility; that is,
evidence is more believable if it resonates and is consistent with our own experiences and
general, two-sided messages (those that present arguments for both sides of a
controversial issues) tend to be received and discussed in a more favorable light than one-
sided messages – especially when the one-sided message presents arguments in favor of
an unpopular position. But even when the one-sided message is consonant with the
views of the audience, people tend to see a two-sided message as less biased, and more
To summarize, intermedia theory calls into question the traditional media effects
model which assumes that media messages directly influence the opinions, attitudes,
The implications of intermedia theory for future health campaign research are
numerous:
relations, then health campaigns must focus on sociometric connections among targeted
audience members. That is, we need to discover whom audience members talk to on a
regular basis. More importantly, we need to discover whom the gatekeepers and opinion
2. Mass media health messages depend on the support of both gatekeepers and
opinion leaders in order to influence the rest of the members of a social network. As
such, health campaign designers need to produce messages whose characteristics appeal
to those individuals, or at least are likely to be supported by them. Since the preferences
of gatekeepers and opinion leaders are likely to vary across different social networks, it
stands to reason that health campaigns need to utilize multiple messages – each message
being targeted for particular types of gatekeepers and opinion leaders. Marketing
targeted for specific segments of the target population. Health campaign designers need
13
that in addition to identifying gatekeepers and opinion leaders, health campaign designers
need to gather as much information as they can about the gatekeepers and opinion leaders
messages.
3. Since the effectiveness of mass media health messages also depend on their fit
with valued group norms, or an endorsed new group norm, health campaign designers
need to also pay close attention to the normative components of their messages. This is
not an easy mandate since certain group norms are deeply embedded in culture norms
for health campaigns to be multi-staged – with initial stages aimed at shaping valued
group norms to make them more favorable to subsequent mass media messages. For
example, recent health campaigns like as “JEL” (Just Eliminate Lies) designed to
eliminate tobacco use gain their effectiveness from changes in social norms toward
tobacco use in the U.S. Conversely, health campaigns designed to eliminate alcohol
consumption are less effective because current social norms in the U.S. still make
conclusion is that until health campaigns can alter social norms so that any amount of
Conclusions
To conclude this paper, we again turn to the wisdom of Elihu Katz and Paul F.
reactions to the mass media. Thus, planning for future research of the
short-run influencing effects of the mass media must build, first, on the
everyday influences and the mass media. The aim of these chapters has
Katz and Lazarsfeld’s conclusions speak volumes for future health campaign research.
Unless and until we find ways of effectively merging mass media messages with
References
Baumann, K. E., Brown, J. D., Bryan, E. S., Fisher, L. A., Padgett, C. A., & Sweney, J.
23(1), 1-17.
Berger, P. L., & Luckman, T. (1966). The social construction of reality: A treatise in the
Boekeloo, B., Schiavo, L., Rabin, D., Jordan, C., & Mathews, J. R. (1993). Sexual risk
behaviors of STD clinic patients before and after Earvin “Magic” Johnson’s HIV-
results from a four year intervention. American Journal of Public Health, 85, 183-
192.
Farquar, J. W., Fortmann, S. P., Flora, J. A., Taylor, C. B., Haskell, W. L., Williams, P.
cardiovascular disease risk factors. The Stanford Five-City Project. Journal of the
Festinger, L., Schachter, S., & Back, K. (1950). Social pressures in informal groups.
Hornik, R. C. (Ed.) (2002). Public health communication: Evidence for behavior change.
Kalichman, S. C., & Hunter, T. L. (1992). The disclosure of celebrity HIV infection: Its
Katz, E., & Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1955). Personal influence: The part played by people in the
Klapper, J. T. (1960). The effects of the mass media. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Lazarsfeld, P. E., Berelson, B., & Gaudet, G. (1944). The people’s choice: How the voter
makes up his mind in a presidential campaign. New York: Duell, Sloan and Pearce.
Lowery, S. A., & DeFleur, M. L. (1995). Milestones in mass communication research (3rd
Luepker, R. V., Murray, D. M., Jacobs, D. R., Mittelmark, M. B., Bracht, N., Carlaw, R.,
Crow, R., Elmer, P., Finnegan, J., & Folsom, A. R. (1994). Community education
for cardiovascular disease prevention: Risk factor changes in the Minnesota Heart
Henry Holt.
Meyer, A. J., Nash, J. D., McAlister, A. L., Macoby, N., & Faruhar, J. W. (1980). Skills
Miller, J. D. (1987). The impact of the Challenger accident on public attitudes toward the
Associates.
Reinard, J. C. (1988). The empirical study of the persuasive effects of evidence: The
status after fifty years of research. Human Communication Research, 15, 3-59.
Rao, N., Svekerud, P. J., Sood, S., & Alford, K. L. (1996). Mass media effects through
Rogers, E. M. (1983). Diffusion of innovations (3rd ed.). New York: The Free Press.
Rogers, E. M. (1998). When the mass media have strong effects: Intermedia processes. In
J. S. Trent (Ed.), Communication: Views from the helm for the 21st century.
Rogers, E. M., & Dearing, J. W. (1988). Agenda-setting research: Where has it been,
Rogers, E. M., Vaughan, P. W., Swalehe, R. M. A., Rao, N., Svenrud, P., Sood, S., &
Rothlisberger, F. J,, & Dickson, W. J. (1939). Management and the worker. Cambridge,
Singhal, A., Sood, M., & Rogers, E. M. (1996). The Gods are drinking milk! Word-of-
mouth diffusion of a major news event in India. Unpublished paper. Athens: Ohio
Stouffer, S. A. (1949). The American soldier: Studies in social psychology in world war
education: Theory, research and practice (pp. 370-386). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass
Publishers.
Wanta, W., & Elliott, W. R. (1995). Did the “Magic” work? Knowledge of HIV/AIDS
Warner, W. L., & Lunt, P. S. (1941). The social life of a modern community (Vol. 1,
Winkleby, M. A., Taylor, C. B., Jatulis, D., & Fortmann, S. P. (1996). The long-term