Slac-Literacy - Learning To Read
Slac-Literacy - Learning To Read
Slac-Literacy - Learning To Read
The Scho lastic Pho nic s Invento ry (SPI) is a research-based assessment that
measures the ability to read nonwords and sight words accurately and efficiently.
The SPI was created to identify 3rd-12th grade students who are poor decoders
and/or unable to recognize sight words with fluency, and to differentiate these
students from those who are adequate decoders and able to recognize sight words
with fluency. Concurrent validity was established for the SPI using the Test of
Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE) (Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999), and
the Woodcock-Johnson III Word Identification and Word Attack subtests
(Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). These results and others, as specified in
the SPI Technical Guide (Scholastic Research and Evaluation, 2008), reveal that
SPI is highly effective at identifying students with poor decoding skills and/or
limited sight word knowledge.
The SPI reflects the research described in this paper about efficient assessment of
phonological decoding and sight word reading. Thus, this computer-based, reliable
and valid assessment is an effective tool for identifying students who need
additional intervention directed at improving word-level reading skills before gains
in reading comprehension can be achieved.
Contents
Introduction 2
Learning to Read 3
References 11
1
INTRODUCTION
Reading refers to understanding the written message of an author (Wagner, Piasta, &
Torgesen, 2006; Snow, 2002). It involves a recursive process of extracting and constructing
meaning that requires a reader, written material to be comprehended, and typically a purpose for
reading. The complexity of this process is illustrated by Scarborough’s (2002) model of skilled
reading (Figure 1).
DECODING gly
sin
wo rds, whether by drawing on phonological correspondences)
a
rea ic
(alphabetic principle, spelling-sound
inc omat
u t
SIGHT RECOGNITION
decoding to sound out the word or by (of familiar words)
recognizing the word by sight. The more Figure 1: Strands of Early Literacy Development (Scarborough, 2002)
accurate and automatic readers become with these individual word recognition processes, the
more cognitive space can be freed up for comprehending strings of text. In fact, for elementary-
age students, word-level reading has been found to be the major determinant of reading
comprehension ( Jenkins et al., 2003; Stanovich, 1991). Consequently, assessment of word-level
reading in the form of both phonological decoding skills and sight word knowledge is important
even though the ultimate goal of reading is to comprehend the meaning of the text.
Difficulties with word-level reading become increasingly problematic as students get older.
Problems with phonological decoding and sight word fluency result in poor comprehension and
lower motivation (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998), and as texts become increasingly advanced
with each grade, poor readers fall farther behind. Although reports vary as to what percentage
of older struggling readers have poor phonological decoding skills, Hock et al.’s (in press) study
of struggling adolescent readers in urban schools found that a full 61% had problems with word-
level reading. These results underscore the importance of reliable and efficient methods for
assessing phonological decoding and sight word reading in the middle and upper grades,
especially in light of the fact that word-level reading skills can be improved substantially with
effective instruction and intervention for the vast majority of struggling readers.
2
LEARNING TO READ
Most scripts convey information about the pronunciation and the meaning of the
words used to convey the message (Rayner et al., 2001). To translate print to language, the
beginning reader needs to learn to connect the printed forms of words (orthography) to
their pronunciations (phonology) and meanings (semantics and morphology).
ORTHOGRAPHY
An orthography is a system of marks used to represent spoken language in writing. For
example, the English orthography consists of the 26 upper- and lower-case letters, the
numerals 0 through 9, punctuation marks, and some other symbols that have come to
represent meaning (e.g., # for pound, @ for at, $ for dollars, and & for and). Although no
writing system is completely pure and exceptions exist, writing systems in existence today
can be divided into three primary types: alphabets, syllabaries, and morpho-syllabaries
(Crowder & Wagner, 1992; DeFrancis, 1989; Gelb, 1952; Rayner et al., 2001; Rayner &
Pollatsek, 1989).
3
Syllabaries, the second kind of writing system, have orthographic units that correspond
to syllables. The Japanese Kana writing system is an example of a syllabary. The final kind
of writing system, morpho-syllabaries, have orthographic units that represent syllables that
also are morphemes. The Chinese character writing system and the Japanese Kanji writing
system are examples of morpho-syllabaries.
Although most of what is known about learning to read is about learning to read
alphabetic scripts in general, and English in particular, the amount of research on learning
to read other scripts is increasing at a rapid pace. The emerging picture is that the major
findings about learning to read English also apply to learning to read scripts associated
with all European languages (Zeigler & Goswami, 2005). When Asian languages are
included, differences are found in the relative importance of key underlying skills in
learning to read, but the fact that some individuals fail to learn to read appears to be
universal regardless of the nature of the written language to be mastered (McBride-Chang
et al., 2005).
PHONOLOGY
Speech information can be conveyed at different levels (Crowder & Wagner, 1992).
For the purpose of this paper, the two levels of most importance are the phonological and
morphophonological levels.
4
At the highest, morphophonemic level, speech is represented by strings of phonemes
that also represent morphemes or units of meaning. These strings are morphophonemes
because they convey both morphological and phonological information. Analogous to the
fact that allophones are phones associated with a single phoneme, allomorphs are
associated with a given morpheme or meaning unit. The ‘sign’ part of the words ‘sign’ and
‘signature’ are examples of allomorphs. Written English is morphophonemic in that its
spellings generally give priority to representing pronunciations but with compromises so as
to convey meaning. For example, SIGN and SIGNATURE share the spelling SIGN
despite the fact that the SIGN part of the two words is pronounced differently. HEAL
and HEALTHY provide a second example.
5
FAILING TO LEARN TO READ FLUENTLY
The vast majority of poor readers have difficulty in decoding individual words (Adams,
1990; Ehri, 1997; Jenkins et al., 2003; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998; Stanovich, 1982;
Vellutino, 1979). Poor readers also are impaired in reading comprehension, but for most
poor readers, their reading comprehension problems arise largely (directly and indirectly)
because of their inability to decode the words (Aaron, 1989; Bruck, 1990; Juel, 1988). Poor
word-level reading has its origins in the language rather than the visual system, and the
problem often is compounded by ineffective instruction (Spear-Swerling & Sternberg,
1996; Wagner, 2005; Wagner & Garon, 1999; Wagner & Torgesen, 1987). When poor
readers are compared to reading-level matched controls (i.e., younger normal readers
whose absolute level of reading is comparable to that of older poor readers), the older, poor
readers perform poorly on measures of phonological awareness and phonological decoding,
and they have fewer words that can be decoded by sight (Bradley & Bryant, 1983; Bruck,
1992; Ehri, 1998; Fox, 1994; Siegel & Faux, 1989; Wagner, 1988; Wagner & Torgesen,
1987).
Given the above, it should not be surprising that a hallmark feature of children who
have difficulty learning to read is poor performance decoding pronounceable nonwords or
pseudowords (see Rack, Snowling, & Olson, 1992 for review). Poor readers even continue
to struggle to read nonwords after they have demonstrated knowledge of similar
orthographic patterns in real words (Siegel & Faux, 1989). This limitation in decoding
nonwords persists into adulthood (Bruck, 1990, 1992, 1993).
6
The results just reviewed suggest that deficits in phonological decoding skills play a
causal role in failing to learn to read. The causal role of deficits in phonological skills is
further supported by the multitude of intervention studies that have utilized phonological
training and phonics instruction to produce gains in at-risk or dyslexic readers (e.g., Ball
& Blachman, 1991; Brady, Fowler, Stone, & Winbury, 1994; Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley,
1989, 1991, 1993, 1995; Byrne, Fielding-Barnsley, & Ashley, 2000; Ehri, Nunes, Stahl, &
Willows, 2001; Ehri et al., 2001; Foorman et al., 2003; Foorman, Francis, Fletcher,
Schatschneider, & Mehta, 1998; Foorman, Francis, Novy, & Liberman, 1991; Hatcher,
Hulme, & Ellis, 1994; Lovett, Steinbach, & Frijters, 2000; Rashotte, MacPhee, &
Torgesen, 2001; Schneider, Ennemoser, Roth, & Kuespert, 1999; Schneider, Roth, &
Ennemoser, 2000; Torgesen et al., 2001; Torgesen et al., 1999; Vellutino, Scanlon, &
Tanzman, 1998; Wise, Ring, & Olson, 1999; see also Adams, 1990; Bus & van Ijzendoorn,
1999; Chall, 1967/1983; National Reading Panel, 2000; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998).
Although academic careers have been made debating seemingly subtle aspects of
models that can account for a reader’s performance when presented with words and
nonwords, it is indisputable that phonological decoding is a basic building block upon
which fluent single-word reading and fluent reading of connected text for comprehension
are based. Efficient phonological decoding is essential for building good internal
representations of words and spelling patterns. A combination of phonological translation
and careful orthographic analysis eventually results in the development of a substantial
sight word vocabulary, which makes fluent reading of connected text and comprehension
possible. Thus, assessing phonological decoding at the word level represents an important
focus of reading assessment.
7
ASSESSING WORD-LEVEL READING
Nonword reading fluency has proven to be an effective measure for evaluating
phonological decoding (Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999; Vanderwood, Linklater, &
Healy, 2008). When presented with an unfamiliar word, readers must break it into parts,
retrieve sounds associated with the parts, and string them together to pronounce the
unfamiliar word. This process can be assessed by presenting examinees with
pronounceable nonwords. It has been shown that skilled readers analyze unfamiliar words
or nonwords more fully than do poor readers (McConkie & Zola, 1987). For example,
some poor readers tend to use initial consonant cues to guess at the rest of the word
(Vellutino & Scanlon, 1987). A full analysis of unfamiliar words contributes to their
becoming sight words over time. Thus, the nonword assessment can reveal whether a
student is decoding effectively by attending to all the letters and sounds that make up the
unknown word.
An advantage of assessing nonwords is that these assessments prevent the reader from
using context clues to identify the target word. Poor readers who have weak decoding
skills tend to over-rely on context clues to try to make meaning of text (Nicholson, 1991;
Stanovich, 1986). Although depending on context clues is an inefficient method of
discerning meaning—it has been estimated that only one out of every four words (25%)
can be predicted by using context (Gough, Alford, & Holley-Wilcox, 1981)—using
context may help poor readers compensate for weak decoding skills, potentially masking
this underlying problem.
8
Given that knowledge of actual words (i.e., lexical knowledge) can help readers decode
unfamiliar words, care should be taken when choosing nonwords for an assessment of
phonological decoding skill. For example, decoding PLONE might be facilitated by
knowledge of the pronunciations of the related words PLANE and CLONE. The extent
to which readers rely on their knowledge of real words when decoding the nonwords
depends on the nature of the nonwords (Treiman, Goswami, & Bruck, 1990). The
practice of creating nonwords by swapping a single phoneme in a real word (e.g., banana
becomes panana) should be avoided because it encourages use of knowledge of real words
rather than decoding.
Another element that contributes to fluency is sight word knowledge. Skilled readers
have a large vocabulary of sight words that can be recognized automatically. However, for
most people, efficient decoding plays an important role in the development of a large sight
word vocabulary. Repeated, accurate reading of the same word eventually leads to the
word being stored in memory as a sight word—one that is identified automatically and
without conscious thought. This store of automatically recognizable words is built
through frequent reading, and therefore struggling readers often have difficulty building a
large sight word vocabulary.
9
Assessing sight word reading and nonword reading provides important information
about the nature of the student’s reading difficulties. For example, an English language
learner may be fluent at decoding nonwords yet dysfluent at reading sight words because
he or she is not yet familiar with some English language vocabulary words. A student who
struggles with nonwords more than sight words may have an underlying problem in
phonological processing.
In the complex process of learning to read, problems with phonological decoding and
sight word knowledge can have serious consequences as students are required to read
increasingly challenging text with each new grade level. Many older struggling readers
who never learned to “crack the code” eventually become alienated from school and
demotivated by years of academic failure. It is therefore critical to effectively assess and
identify those older students who continue to struggle with foundational phonological
decoding skills. Nonword and sight word assessments efficiently isolate these skills,
ensuring that older, struggling readers with decoding problems receive the targeted,
intensive intervention they need to put them on the path toward successful
comprehension.
10
REFERENCES
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning abo ut print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Aaron, P. G. (1989). Qualitative and quantitative differences among dyslexic, normal, and nondyslexic poor
readers. Reading & W riting, 1, 291–308.
Arnbak, E., & Elbro, C. (2000). The effects of morphological awareness training on the reading and spelling
skills of young dyslexics. Sc andinavian Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Research, 44, 229–251.
Ball, E. W., & Blachman, B. A. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference
in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 49–66.
Brady, S., Fowler, A., Stone, B., & Winbury, N. (1994). Training phonological awareness: A study with
inner-city kindergarten children. A nnals o f Dyslexia, 44, 26–59.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature,
301, 419–421.
Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Develo pmental
Psycho lo gy, 26, 439–454.
Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits. Develo pmental Psycho lo gy,
28(5), 874–886.
Bruck, M. (1993). Word recognition and component phonological processing skills of adults with childhood
diagnosis of dyslexia. Develo pmental Review, 13, 258–268.
Bus, A. G., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1999, September). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-
analysis of experimental training studies. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 91(3), 403–414. URLJ:
http://www.apa.org/journals/edu.html
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1989). Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child's
acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 81, 313–321.
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1991). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young
children. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 83, 451–455.
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young
children: A 1-year follow-up. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 85, 104–111.
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1995). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young
children: A 2- and 3-year follow-up and a new preschool trial. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 87,
488–503.
11
Byrne, B., Fielding-Barnsley, R., & Ashley, L. (2000). Effects of preschool phoneme identity training after
six years: Outcome level distinguished from rate of response. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 92,
659–667.
Chall, J. S. (1967/1983). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-
distributed-processing approaches. Psycho lo gic al Review, 100, 589–608.
Crowder, R. G., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). The psycho lo gy o f reading: A n intro duc tio n (2nd ed.). London:
Oxford University Press.
DeFrancis, J. (1989). V isible speech: The diverse o neness o f writing systems. Honolulu, HI: University of
Hawaii Press.
Ehri, L. C. (1997). Sight word learning in normal readers and dyslexics. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Fo undatio ns
o f reading ac quisitio n and dyslexia: Implic atio ns fo r early interventio n (pp. 163–190). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., & Willows, D. M. (2001). Systematic phonics instruction helps
students learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel's meta-analysis. Review o f Educ atio nal
Research, 71, 393–447.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001).
Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel's
meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287.
Freedman, S. W. & Calfee, R. C. (1984). Understanding and comprehending. W ritten Co mmunic atio n,
1(4), 459–490.
Foorman, B. R., Chen, D. T., Carlson, C., Moats, L., Francis, D. J., & Fletcher, J. M. (2003). The necessity
of the alphabetic principle to phonemic awareness instruction. Reading and W riting: A n Interdisc iplinary
Jo urnal, 16, 289–324.
Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Schatschneider, C., & Mehta, P. (1998). The role of
instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in at-risk children. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal
Psycho lo gy, 90, 37–55.
Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Novy, D. M., & Liberman, D. (1991). How letter-sound instruction mediates
progress in first-grade reading and spelling. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 83, 456–469.
Fox, E. (1994). Grapheme-phoneme correspondence in dyslexic and matched control readers. British Jo urnal
o f Psycho lo gy, 85, 41–53.
Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: A
multilingual comparison. Jo urnal o f Experimental Psycho lo gy: Human Perc eptio n and Perfo rmanc e, 13,
104–115.
12
Gelb, I J. (1952). A study o f writing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Gough, P. B., Alford, J. A., Jr., & Holley-Wilcox, P. (1981). Words and contexts. In O. Tzeng & H. Singer
(Eds.), Perc eptio n o f Print: Reading Research in Experimental Psycho lo gy (pp. 85–102). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the
teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Develo pment, 65,
41–57.
Hock, M. F., Brasseur, I. F., Deschler, D. D., Mark, C. A., Stribling, J. W., Catts, H. W., Marquis, J. G. (in
press). What is the nature of struggling adolescent readers in urban schools? Learning Disability Quarterly.
Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual
differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 95, 719–729.
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth
grades. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 80(4), 437–447.
LaBerge, D. & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading.
Co gnitive Psycho lo gy, 6, 293–323.
Lovett, M. W., Steinbach, K. A., & Frijters, J. C. (2000). Remediating the core deficits of developmental
reading disability: A double-deficit perspective. Jo urnal o f Learning Disabilities, 33, 334–358.
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J. R., Lie, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, Al, Cheuk, C., Muse A. (2005).
Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure
awareness to vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and
America. Jo urnal o f Experimental Child Psycho lo gy, 92(2), 140–160.
McConkie, G. W. & Zola, D. (1987). Visual attention during eye fixations while reading. In M. Coltheart
(Ed.), A ttentio n and Perfo rmanc e XII: The psycho lo gy o f reading (pp. 385–401). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.
National Reading Panel. (2000). Repo rt o f the Natio nal Reading Panel: Teaching children to read
(No. 00–4769). Washington, DC.: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
National Research Council. (1998). Preventing reading diffic ulties in yo ung children. Washington, DC.:
National Academy Press.
Nicholson, T. (1991). Do children read words better in context or in lists? Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal
Psycho lo gy, 83, 444–450.
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia:
A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 28–53.
Rashotte, C. A., MacPhee, K., & Torgesen, J. K. (2001). The effectiveness of a group reading instruction
program with poor readers in multiple grades. Learning Disability Quarterly, 24, 119–134.
Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological
science informs the teaching of reading. Psycho lo gic al Sc ienc e in the Public Interest, 2, 31–74.
13
Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The psycho lo gy o f reading. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Scarborough, H. S. (2002). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence,
theory, and practice. In S. B. Neuman and D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbo ok o f Early Literac y Research
(pp. 97–110). New York: Guildford Press.
Schneider, W., Ennemoser, M., Roth, E., & Kuespert, P. (1999). Kindergarten prevention of dyslexia: Does
training in phonological awareness work for everybody? Jo urnal o f Learning Disabilities, 32, 429–436.
Schneider, W., Roth, E., & Ennemoser, M. (2000). Training phonological skills and letter knowledge in
children at risk for dyslexia: A comparison of three kindergarten intervention programs. Jo urnal o f
Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 92, 284–295.
Scholastic Research and Evaluation. (2008). Scholastic Phonics Inventory Technical Manual. New York,
NY: Scholastic, Inc.
Siegel, L. S., & Faux, D. (1989). Acquisition of certain grapheme-phoneme correspondences in normally
achieving and disabled readers. Reading & W riting, 1, 37–52.
Snow, C. E. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward a research and development program in reading
comprehension. Arlington, VA: RAND.
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.) (1998). Preventing reading diffic ulties in yo ung children.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Spear-Swerling, L., & Sternberg, R. (1996). O ff trac k: W hen po o r readers bec o me learning disabled.
Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Speece, D. L., Mills, C., Ritchey, K. D., & Hillman, E. (2003). Initial evidence that letter fluency tasks are
valid indicators of early reading skill. Jo urnal o f Spec ial Educ atio n, 36, 223–233.
Stanovich, K. E. (1982). Individual differences in the cognitive processes of reading: I. Word decoding.
Jo urnal o f Learning Disabilities, 15, 485–493.
Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the
acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.
Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming, phonological
awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlation evidence. Review o f Educ atio nal Research, 73(4),
407–440.
Torgesen, J. K., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Voeller, K. K. S., & Conway, T. (2001).
Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term
outcomes from two instructional approaches. Jo urnal o f Learning Disabilities, 34, 33–58.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin, TX:
PRO-Ed.
14
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Conway, T., et al. (1999).
Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual
responses to instruction. Jo urnal o f Educ atio nal Psycho lo gy, 91, 579–593.
Treiman, R., Goswami, U. & Bruck, M (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: Implications for reading
development and theory. Memo ry & Co gnitio n, 18(6), 559–567.
Vanderwood, M. L., Linklater, D., and Healy, K. (2008). Predictive accuracy of nonsense word fluency for
English language learners. Scho o l Psycho lo gy Review, 37(1), 5–17.
Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theo ry and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vellutino, F. R. & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological Coding, Phonological Awareness, and Reading
Ability: Evidence From a Longitudinal and Experimental Study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33(3), 321–63
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Tanzman, M. S. (1998). The case for early intervention in diagnosing
specific reading disability. Jo urnal o f Scho o l Psycho lo gy, 36, 367–397.
Wagner, R. K. (1988). Causal relations between the development of phonological processing abilities and the
acquisition of reading skills: A meta-analysis. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 34(3), 261–279.
Wagner, R. K. (2005). Reading impairment. In P. Strazny (Ed.), Enc yc lo pedia o f Psycho linguistic s.
New York: Routledge, Taylor, and Francis.
Wagner, R. K., & Garon, T. (1999). Learning disabilities in perspective. In Sternberg, R., &
Spear-Swerling, L. (Eds.), Pe rspec tives o n Learning Disabilities (pp. 83–105). Boulder, CO:
Westview, 1999.
Wagner, R. K., Piasta, S. B., & Torgesen, J. K. (2006). Learning to read. In M. A. Gernsbacher & M. J.
Traxler (Eds.), Handbo ok o f psycho linguistic s (pp. 1111–1142). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Wagner, R. K. & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the
acquisition of reading skills. Psycho lo gic al Bulletin, 101, 192–212.
Wise, B. W., Ring, J., & Olson, R. K. (1999). Training phonological awareness with and without explicit
attention to articulation. Jo urnal o f Experimental Child Psycho lo gy, 72, 271–304.
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K., & Mather, N. (2001). W o o dc o c k-Johnso n Tests o f Co gnitive A bility-Third
Editio n. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. C. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading
across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psycho lo gic al Bulletin, 131(1), 3–29.
15
ABOUT THE AUTHOR
Richard Wagner is a Distinguished Research Professor
and the Binet Professor of Psychology at Florida State
University, and Associate Director of the Florida Center for
Reading Research. He earned a Ph.D. in cognitive
psychology from Yale University in 1985. He previously
earned a Master’s Degree in School Psychology from the
University of Akron and worked as a school psychologist.
16
Scholastic Inc.
557 Broadway
New York, NY 10012
1-800-SCHOLASTIC