Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics: Article Information
Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics: Article Information
Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics: Article Information
https://doi.org/10.1108/APJML-01-2016-0014
Downloaded on: 22 October 2018, At: 23:54 (PT)
References: this document contains references to 85 other documents.
To copy this document: [email protected]
The fulltext of this document has been downloaded 769 times since 2018*
Users who downloaded this article also downloaded:
(2018),"The chain of effects from brand personality and functional congruity to stages of brand
loyalty: The moderating role of gender", Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics, Vol. 30
Iss 1 pp. 84-105 <a href="https://doi.org/10.1108/APJML-01-2017-0016">https://doi.org/10.1108/
APJML-01-2017-0016</a>
(2017),"Are you able to recall the brand? The impact of brand prominence, game involvement and
persuasion knowledge in online – advergames", Journal of Product & Brand Management, Vol.
26 Iss 4 pp. 402-414 <a href="https://doi.org/10.1108/JPBM-02-2015-0811">https://doi.org/10.1108/
JPBM-02-2015-0811</a>
Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-
srm:601935 []
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald
for Authors service information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission
guidelines are available for all. Please visit www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company
manages a portfolio of more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as
well as providing an extensive range of online products and additional customer resources and
services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the
Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for
digital archive preservation.
Advergamers’
Effect of product involvement brand recall
and brand prominence on and brand
attitude
advergamers’ brand recall and
brand attitude in an emerging 43
Abstract
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to investigate the impact of product involvement and brand
prominence on advergamers’ brand recall and brand attitude in an emerging market context. Specifically, this
research illustrates the conditions under which brand placements in online games create attention,
elaboration and subsequent brand recall and brand attitude by drawing the insights from the limited capacity
model of attention and the elaboration likelihood model.
Design/methodology/approach – A 2 (product involvement: low or high) × 2 (brand prominence:
prominent or subtle) between-subject measures design is used. In total, 280 students participated in the study.
A 2 × 2 between-subjects MANOVA is used to test the hypotheses.
Findings – In the context of advergames, for a low-involvement product, a prominent brand placement
results in greater brand recall than a subtle brand placement. However, for a high-involvement product,
a subtle-placement results in greater brand recall than a prominent brand placement. Further, results reveal
that for a low-involvement product, a subtle brand placement results in more favorable brand attitude than a
prominent brand placement. For a high-involvement product, a prominent brand placement results in more
favorable brand attitude than a subtle brand placement.
Research limitations/implications – The findings and conclusions are very important for advertising
experts in terms of advergame designing, execution and for an operational use of brand placements
in advergames.
Originality/value – This investigation adds to the online advertising literature, specifically the advergames
context by examining and analyzing the real-time roles of advergame-specific factor, such as brand
prominence and the gamer-specific factor, such as product involvement in creating gamers’ brand recall
and brand attitude from attention and elaboration perspectives in an emerging market context like India.
Keywords Internet marketing, Advertising, Media effects, Persuasion, Branding strategies,
Integrated marketing communication (IMC)
Paper type Research paper
Introduction
In today’s world, smartphones play a major role in our lives. Along with making phone
calls and the ordinary usage, consumers install apps and even games on their mobile
devices (Cohen, 2016). For instance, on an average 16.152 billion games (24.85 percent
share of available apps: 65 billion in App Store) are downloaded from Apple App Store
(Statista, 2016a). A recent report shows that mobile phone gaming-penetration population
in the USA has increased from 25.9 percent in the year 2011 to 55.7 percent in the year
2016 and is projected to increase to 63.7 percent by 2020 (Statista, 2017). According to
eMarketer (2014), US adults spend 2 hours and 51 minutes per day on “non-voice
Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing
activities” on mobile devices. This suggests that, conceivably, there might be some and Logistics
symbols in those online games that remind gamers of the real life examples/issues. More Vol. 30 No. 1, 2018
pp. 43-61
precisely, those games might contain some brand logos or similar brand messages. In that © Emerald Publishing Limited
1355-5855
case, should such brand messages be transparent or presented at a subliminal level? DOI 10.1108/APJML-01-2016-0014
APJML Will the effectiveness of such brand messages be subjected to the product involvement?
30,1 These are the two questions addressed by this research. More specifically, the present
research uncovers two things. First, is there any effect of brand prominence and product
involvement on gamers’ brand attitude? Second, how deep is the effect of these two
variables on players’ attitudes toward brands advertised in the online games?
Existing advertising research indicates that this can be an opportunity to grab
44 customers’ attention (Cheyne et al., 2013; Keng and Lin, 2006; Vashisht, 2015) as they are
indirectly engaged in brand messages (Calvert, 2008; Vashisht, 2016a), which in turn may
add to the effectiveness of advertising (Redondo, 2012; Yoo and Eastin, 2016). Literature on
advergames show that advergames are more effective than traditional media channels
because of consumers’ high interactive engagement with the brands integrated in
advergames for a longer duration of time (e.g. Bellman et al., 2014; Rozendaal et al., 2013;
Vashisht, 2015; Waiguny et al., 2013). Also, the past studies reveal that advergames build
positive brand images (Rosado and Agante, 2011), excellently transfer subtle associations,
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
familiarity and experiences (rather than a solitary factual point) (Lee et al., 2009) and the
implanted interactivity of advergames persuades an experience of fun and relaxation
(Refiana et al., 2005; Lu and Wang, 2008). Furthermore, exhibiting ads in the background of
the games gives a sense of realism to the consumers (Marti-Parreno et al., 2013) which can
have an impact on brand recall and brand evaluations (d’Astous and Chartier, 2000;
Kretchmer, 2005; Schneider and Cornwell, 2005). The gaming literature also shows that
prominent brand placements (brands embedded in the foreground of the game) result in
higher brand memory than subtle placements (brands embedded in the background of the
game) (Van Reijmersdal, 2009; Van Reijmersdal et al., 2012; Vashisht, 2016b). Previous
research studies reveal that advertising effectiveness also gets influenced by the product
type (Nelson, 2002; Grigorovici and Constantin, 2004; Balasubramanian et al., 2006).
Greater brand recall rates were found for atypical and local brands than for national brands
embedded in games (Nelson, 2002) and also, the size of the object (big: car; small:
mobile phone) created differences in the players’ brand recall rates (Grigorovici and
Constantin, 2004). Literature on subliminal advertising indicates that subliminal advertising
could be less intrusive or annoying than paid media (Hudson and Hudson, 2006;
Nelson et al., 2004; Tsai et al., 2007). Also, the existing brand placement literature
acknowledges that product involvement could also be one of the factors that might affect
consumers’ brand memory and attitudes (Vashisht, 2017). Nonetheless, to our knowledge,
the existing literature in advergames has recognized the individual roles of brand
prominence and product involvement in determining advergaming effects
(Balasubramanian et al., 2006; Vashisht and Chauhan, 2014), but has not offered any
insights into how the transparency of advertisements in games (hereafter advergames)
may interact with product involvement and thereby impact the effectiveness of the
advergames. This is the gap that our research aims to fill.
Drawing from the limited capacity model (LCM) of attention (Kahneman, 1973) and
elaboration likelihood model (ELM: Petty and Cacioppo, 1986), we argue that for a
low-involvement product, a prominent brand placement results in greater brand recall than
a subtle brand placement. However, for a high-involvement product, a subtle brand
placement results in greater brand recall than a prominent brand placement. Also, it is
argued that for a low-involvement product, a subtle brand placement results in more
favorable brand attitude than a prominent brand placement, whereas, for a
high-involvement product, a prominent brand placement results in more favorable brand
attitude than a subtle brand placement. An experimental study confirms our prediction.
Furthermore, the emerging markets, such as India, China, Mexico, etc., have received
high attention in the global advertising arena (Paul and Mas, 2016). This is because
of the fact that emerging markets contribute almost 39 percent to the world GDP
(emerging markets GDP ¼ US$29.25 trillion and world GDP ¼ US$75.21 trillion) and account Advergamers’
for 70 percent of the world’s total population (Statista, 2016b). It has attracted a number of brand recall
international companies in the emerging markets (International Monetary Fund, 2016). and brand
Furthermore, emerging markets are pronounced by a diverse market and represented by
numerous cultural and linguistic groups, thus calling for a comprehensive research into these attitude
markets (International Monetary Fund, 2016). Over and above this, the consumer behavior of
emerging markets also tends to be different and challenges the prevailing advertising models 45
(International Monetary Fund, 2016). Thus, it can be said that what holds good for a
developed market may not find a fit in an emerging market. Therefore, there is a necessity to
perform a comprehensive research on emerging markets, to find out what remains
unobserved and to exhibit new consumer understandings. Therefore, by taking an emerging
market context into consideration, the present study investigates the impact of product
involvement and brand prominence on the gamers’ brand recall and brand attitude.
The remainder of this study is organized as follows. In the next section, a theoretical
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
ELM
This theory proposes that there are two roads to cognitive elaboration: the central route and
the peripheral route (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986). In the central route, cognitive elaboration
results from a person’s observant and attentive inspection of the correct features of the
message presented in favor of an involvement, whereas in case of peripheral route, cognitive
APJML elaboration is a result of a person’s association with the cues present in the stimulus.
30,1 One adopts the central route when he/she is highly motivated and interested, and has ability
to think about the persuasive brand message (in the present study, it is a brand logo or name
or any brand identifier). On the other hand, one adopts the peripheral route when he/she has
low motivation and lesser ability to process the brand message. Therefore, the elaboration
likelihood is regulated by one’s motivation and ability to evaluate the brand information
46 being presented. Hence, based on these insights, it can be argued that, in the context of
advergames, game playing is the central value and the in-game advertised brand is the
peripheral value.
brand-information processing and buying behavior (e.g. Celsi and Olson, 1988; Suh and
Yi, 2006). In advertising literature, product-category involvement has been defined as
“a person’s perceived relevance of the object based on inherent needs, values, and interest”
(Zaichkowsky, 1994, p. 61). It represents recognition that a specific product category may be
significant, to a more or lesser amount, to folks’ lives, their sense of identity and
individuality and their connection with others (Traylor, 1981). It is also described as
customers’ overall assessment of how vital the product is in his or her life, and is often
classified as situational and/or enduring involvement (Celsi and Olson, 1988; Houston and
Rothschild, 1978). Situational involvement can be induced by stimuli and cues from the
direct, external consumer environment, such as promotions and campaigns which may
trigger the personal goals and needs of the customers. Enduring involvement stays
apparent across conditions and situations, however, and can be accredited to the storing of
personal information within the knowledge structure of the product category, such as
personal experience. In this research, the product involvement is operationalized as an
enduring involvement with a product category, such as laptops and cars are high-involving
product categories; however, bathing soaps and shampoos are low-involving product
categories. In the literature, the enduring involvement is defined as “an individual difference
variable representing the arousal potential of a product or activity that causes personal
relevance” (Higie and Feick, 1989, p. 690). Particularly, with the enduring involvement,
individual relevance happens because the person relates the product to his/her self-image and
attaches some epicurean merits to the product. Thus, customers who are greatly involved with
a product category will attribute more meaning and significance to the product compared to a
product category with which they are less involved. This shows that one’s motivation for
information processing does get influenced by product-involvement factor.
In the context of advergames, gamers’ information-processing activity can best be
explained by the ELM theory (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986). As ELM suggests that sometimes
users get swayed by “peripheral cues” presented in a persuasive message and those
peripheral cues can be comprised of any variable that is capable of affecting memory
without analysis of the brand message presented. Product involvement is one such variable
that influences the brand memory (e.g. Celsi and Olson, 1988; Suh and Yi, 2006). Based on
the insights drawn from ELM, it can be argued that when a high-involvement product is
placed in the game, one’s motivation and the ability to process in-game advertising should
be higher than the low product-involvement product condition. Therefore, consumers give
more time, attention and efforts to process product-related information for high-involving
products than low-involving products.
Regarding brand prominence, in advertising literature, brand placements in different
media platforms are mainly categorized into two categories, i.e. prominent brand placements
and subtle brand placements. In the literature, prominent placements are described as the Advergamers’
placements of brands within media platforms (such as movies, programs, etc.) wherein brand recall
the products or/and other brand identifiers are highly visible and noticeable because of their and brand
big sizes or central positions on the screens or their supremacy to the action in the scenes
(Gupta and Lord, 1998; Vashisht and Royne, 2016). On the other hand, subtle brand attitude
placements are described as placements of brands in the background or peripherals of the
screens in small sizes or with low time of exposure (Gupta and Lord, 1998; Vashisht and 47
Royne, 2016).
Past research (e.g. Cauberghe and De Pelsmacker, 2010; Vashisht and Royne, 2016;
Yang and Roskos-Ewoldsen, 2007) has shown that in-game prominent placements result in
higher brand recall as brands are more perceptible and more likely to be retrieved from brand
memory than subtle brand placements. According to the ELM, in an advergame context,
game playing becomes the central value and the embedded brand is the peripheral value for
the gamers. Also, in accordance with LCM (Kahneman, 1973; Lynch and Srull, 1982),
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
when playing an advergame, the attention of game player is mainly focused on the action of
the game rather than the in-game advertising. Since in prominent placements, brands appear
in the central part of the game screen, thus, processing of in-game brand placements is more
integral to the game-playing task (i.e. primary task), and requires a less spare capacity to
successfully perform the secondary task (i.e. processing in-game brand placements). However,
in subtle brand placements, since brands appear in the peripheral regions of the game screens
which are not easily noticeable and hence, more spare capacity is required for in-game
brand-information processing. As more attentional capacity gets used up in the primary task
itself in case of in subtle brand placements, remaining mental capacity is not sufficient for
brand-information processing. Thus, prominent placements result in higher recall than subtle
placements (Vashisht and Royne, 2016).
However, it is not true in all the conditions. For instance, literature available on
product-involvement reconnoiters that if there are differences in meaning attached to a
low- and high-involvement product category (e.g. Suh and Yi, 2006) and its insinuation for
processing mechanism, the effect of brand prominence on brand outcomes may not be the
same in relation to the product type. As prominent brand placements (big-sized brand
names moving or centrally placed on computer screens) are more cohesive in nature,
i.e. integrated into the advergame (Chambers, 2005), thereby having a greater context
connection or relevance (Russell, 1998) compared to subtle placements (small-sized brand
names or logos appearing in the background), it is predicted that the meaning attributed to a
prominent placement is higher than to a subtle placement. Russell (1998) found a disparity
between two aspects of meaning (here product involvement and brand prominence)
can enhance brand recall as a result of the incongruence of the stimuli that initiates the
thinking process, which leads to high cognitive elaboration.
In this study, congruence between stimuli refers to a combination of subtle placement of a
low-involvement product in an advergame or a prominent placement of a high-involvement
product in an advergame. In other words, in the present research, a congruent situation
refers to either a high-high condition of product involvement and brand prominence
(high-involvement product placed in the central position, i.e. with high noticeability) or a
low-low condition of product involvement and brand prominence (low-involvement product
placed in the peripheral position, i.e. with low noticeability). Likewise, incongruence between
stimuli refers to a combination of subtle placement of a high-involvement product in an
advergame or a prominent placement of a low-involvement product in an advergame.
Vashisht (2016b) drawing the insights from LCM and ELM theories showed that
incongruence can increase the noticeability and the memory of the brands embedded in the
games as gamers cognitively elaborate upon the incongruity issue to find out the reasons
behind subtly placing a high-involvement product or prominently placing a low-involvement
APJML product because of their increased motivation, ability and opportunity (MAO). As a result,
30,1 in incongruence conditions (combination of subtle placement of a high-involvement
product in an advergame or a prominent placement of a low-involvement product in an
advergame), gamers exert more cognitive efforts to process advergame embedded brand
messages, which lead to higher brand recall than that in congruence conditions
(combination of subtle placement of a low-involvement product in an advergame
48 or a prominent placement of a high-involvement product in an advergame). With
respect to ELM, it can be said that playing activity becomes the central value and
information-processing activity becomes the peripheral value for the gamers. Also,
drawing the insights from LCM, it can be argued that while playing a game under
incongruent conditions (i.e. playing “a game prominently embedded with a low-involved
product” or “a game subtly placed with a high-involved product”), one’s spare capacity
will be high enough to process in-game brand placements which will result into an
increased cognitive elaboration. This in turn will lead to high brand memory unlike that in
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
Research methodology
Overview
The purpose of the present research is to investigate the cause and effect relationships of
product involvement and brand prominence on brand recall and brand attitude, therefore,
the appropriate research design used is an experimental design (Malhotra et al., 2012).
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
The following sections present the experiment in detail, including development of stimulus
materials, participants and design, manipulation of independent variables and measurement
of dependent variables.
appropriate for the present study. Also, past studies reported that 90 percent of teens are
gamers (Mediaedge:cia, 2005; Reisinger, 2011), which supports that the use of student
sample is apposite for this study. Student-gamers who participated in the study were
management undergraduates and between the ages of 16-19 years. First, a random selection
of 500 students was conducted from a list of all the university students. Then, we contacted
them to seek their permission to participate in an experimental study. Those who showed
the interest to participate in the study were given a food coupon of Rs150 each. A total of 280
students (males ¼ 150 and females ¼ 138) participated in the study. The purpose of selecting
a big sample size of 280 for the present study was that we had a low expectation of the
response rate, though it turned out to be different. These respondents were called to a
computer lab where they were asked to play the advergames on individual consoles for ten
minutes. Respondents were randomly assigned to the different experimental conditions
(70 student-gamers in each condition) and the advergames to be played on the given
computers. After their exposure to advergames, participants were asked to fill up the
questionnaire, with items of manipulation checks and eliciting their responses to brand
recall and brand attitude (Note: the respondents were unaware that they had to recall the
brand names after playing the games. During the game-playing task, they were not told that
they had to recall the brand names after finishing the game-playing task. When they
finished game-playing task, then the questionnaire was given to them to elicit their
responses to brand recall and brand attitude).
Measures
In the present study, two independent variables, i.e. product involvement and brand
prominence were used which were manipulated during the study. The manipulation of
product involvement was measured on a five-point ten-item scale of Zaichkowsky (1994).
Likewise, the manipulation of brand prominence was measured on a seven-point bipolar
scale (1 ¼ “not at all prominently placed” to 7 ¼ “very prominently placed”).
The dependent variable, brand recall, was measured by asking participants to list
the brand names that appeared in the advergames. Two coders who were blind to the
experiment counted the number of brand names recalled. If a subject listed an advertised
brand correctly, it was coded as a correct response. An answer was coded as an incorrect
response if the participant did not list the advertised brand or listed a non-advertised brand
name. The numbers of correct responses ranged from 0 to 3 as there were three different
brands embedded in the advergames. Inter-coder reliability was checked and it found to be
satisfactory (α ¼ 0.87). The second dependent variable, brand attitude, was measured by
using a semantic differential scale with the bipolar adjective items (good/bad, like/dislike,
favorable/unfavorable and positive/negative), adapted from Wise et al. (2008).
Data analyses and results Advergamers’
Sample characteristics brand recall
A Shapiro-Wilk’s test ( p W0.05) (Shapiro and Wilk, 1965; Razali and Wah, 2011) and a and brand
visual inspection of their histograms, normal Q-Q plots and box plots showed that brand
recall scores were approximately normally distributed for both high and low product
attitude
involvement with a skewness of −0.046 (SE ¼ 0.409) and a kurtosis of −1.184 (SE ¼ 0.798)
for low product involvement and a skewness of −0.061 (SE ¼ 0.491) and a kurtosis of −1.132 51
(SE ¼ 0.953) for high product involvement. Similarly, normality plots with tests showed that
brand recall scores were approximately normally distributed for both subtle and prominent
brand placements with a skewness of −0.227 (SE ¼ 0.501) and a kurtosis of −1.103
(SE ¼ 0.972) for subtle brand placement and a skewness of 0.233 (SE ¼ 0.403) and a kurtosis
of −0.811 (SE ¼ 0.788) for prominent brand placement (Cramer, 1998; Cramer and
Howitt, 2004; Doane and Seward, 2011).
Furthermore, a Shapiro-Wilk’s test ( pW0.05) (Shapiro and Wilk, 1965; Razali and Wah,
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
2011) and a visual inspection of their histograms, normal Q-Q plots and box plots showed that
brand attitude scores were approximately normally distributed for both high and low product
involvement with a skewness of 0.373 (SE ¼ 0.364) and a kurtosis of 0.583 (SE ¼ 0.716) for low
product involvement and a skewness of −0.011 (SE ¼ 0.387) and a kurtosis of −0.882
(SE ¼ 0.758) for high product involvement. Similarly, normality plots with tests showed that
brand attitude scores were approximately normally distributed for both subtle and prominent
brand placements with a skewness of 0.124 (SE ¼ 0.441) and a kurtosis of −1.001 (SE ¼ 0.858)
for subtle brand placement and a skewness of 0.208 (SE ¼ 0.448) and a kurtosis of 1.462
(SE ¼ 0.872) for prominent brand placement (Cramer, 1998; Cramer and Howitt, 2004;
Doane and Seward, 2011).
Manipulation checks
The manipulation check for product involvement through a one-way analysis of variance
(ANOVA) showed a significant mean difference (F (1, 120) ¼ 2.81, p o 0.05) between the
low-involvement product (M ¼ 2.243) and high-involvement product (M ¼ 4.655).
Likewise, manipulation check for brand prominence showed a significant mean
difference (F (1, 120) ¼ 2.95, p o 0.05) between the prominent-placement games
(M ¼ 5.251) and subtle-placement games (M ¼ 3.121). Also, full-model ANOVAs
(2 product involvement × 2 brand prominence) were done to check whether there were
confounding effects of brand prominence or product involvement manipulations on
product involvement (F (1, 120) ¼ 2.88, p ¼ 0.000) and brand prominence (F (1, 120) ¼ 2.92,
p ¼ 0.001) values. Manipulation check results showed that there were no confounding
effects of brand prominence or product involvement manipulations on product
involvement and brand prominence. Hence, overall, checks of the manipulations
revealed that all the manipulations were successful.
Hypotheses testing
An ANOVA on brand recall as the dependent variable revealed a significant main effect for
product involvement (F (1, 276) ¼ 44.02, p o0.05). Respondents reported higher brand recall
with low product involvement than high product involvement (Mlow involvement ¼ 2.11 vs
Mhigh involvement ¼ 0.58). The results revealed a significant main effect for brand prominence
(F (1, 276) ¼ 38.46, po0.05), indicating that prominent brand placement led to higher brand
recall than subtle brand placement (Mprominent placement ¼ 2.54 vs Msubtle placement ¼ 1.02).
Furthermore, a significant interaction effect was found between product involvement and
brand prominence on brand recall (F (1, 276) ¼ 165.83, po0.05). Follow-up contrasts indicated
that with low product involvement, respondents had higher brand recall with prominent
APJML placement (Mprominent placement ¼ 2.80 vs Msubtle placement ¼ 1.02; F (1, 138) ¼ 11.65, po0.01);
30,1 while with high product involvement, subjects reported higher brand recall with subtle
placement (Mprominent placement ¼ 1.11 vs Msubtle placement ¼ 2.03; F (1, 138) ¼ 8.32, po0.01).
Moreover, another ANOVA on brand attitude as the dependent variable revealed a
significant main effect for product involvement (F (1, 276) ¼ 113.51, p o0.05). Respondents
reported more favorable brand attitude with high product involvement than low product
52 involvement (Mhigh involvement ¼ 3.8 vs Mlow involvement ¼ 2.11). The results revealed a
significant main effect for brand prominence (F (1, 276) ¼ 119.62, p o0.05), indicating that
prominent brand placement led to more favorable brand attitude than subtle brand
placement (Mprominent placement ¼ 3.54 vs Msubtle placement ¼ 1.97). Furthermore, a significant
interaction effect was found between product involvement and brand prominence on brand
attitude (F (1, 276) ¼ 132.29, p o0.05). Follow-up contrasts indicated that with low product
involvement, respondents had more favorable brand attitude with subtle placement
(Mprominent placement ¼ 1.50 vs Msubtle placement ¼ 2.97; F (1, 138) ¼ 12.65, p o0.01); while with
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
high product involvement, subjects reported more favorable brand attitude with prominent
placement (Mprominent placement ¼ 2.54 vs Msubtle placement ¼ 1.27; F (1, 138) ¼ 6.41, p o0.01).
Thus, H1a-H2b are supported (see Figures 1 and 2). ANOVA results are tabulated in Table I.
Figure 1.
Brand prominence ×
product involvement
on brand recall
Low involvement High involvement
Figure 2.
Brand prominence ×
product involvement
on brand attitude
Low involvement High involvement
Effects Mean F-value Significance
Advergamers’
brand recall
Product involvement on brand recall (F (1, 276) ¼ 44.02, p o0.05) and brand
Low product involvement 2.11 44.02 0.000
High product involvement 0.58 attitude
Brand prominence on brand recall (F (1, 276) ¼ 38.46, p o0.05)
Prominent placement 2.54 38.46 0.000 53
Subtle placement 1.02
Product involvement on brand attitude (F (1, 276) ¼ 113.51, p o0.05)
Low product involvement 2.11 113.51 0.000
High product involvement 3.8
Brand prominence on brand attitude (F (1, 276) ¼ 119.62, po 0.05)
Prominent placement 3.54 119.62 0.000
Subtle placement 1.97
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
Product involvement (PI) × brand prominence on brand recall (F (1, 276) ¼ 165.83, po 0.05)
Low-PI × prominent placement 2.80 11.65 0.002
Low-PI × subtle placement 1.02
High-PI × prominent placement 1.11 8.32 0.000
High-PI × subtle placement 2.03
Product involvement (PI) × brand prominence on brand attitude (F (1, 276) ¼ 132.29, p o0.05)
Low-PI × prominent placement 1.50 12.65 0.000
Low-PI × subtle placement 2.97
High-PI × prominent placement 2.54 6.41 0.001 Table I.
High-PI × subtle placement 1.27 Results
this paper inspects the advergaming effects in an emerging market context like India.
Finally, the present study explores advergaming effects on 16-19 years age group.
This experimental study inspected the effect of advergames on gamers’ brand recall and
attitudes, specifically how the elaborative components, such as product involvement and brand
prominence shape the same. The study findings showed that for a low-involvement product, a
prominent brand placement resulted in higher brand recall than a subtle brand placement,
whereas, for a high-involvement product, a subtle brand placement resulted in superior brand
recall than a prominent brand placement. These findings support the theory that incongruence
conditions (referred to here as low involvement with prominent placement and high
involvement with subtle placement) can upturn the noticeability and brand recall as gamers
cognitively elaborate upon the incongruity issue because of their increased MAO to find out the
reasons behind subtly placing a high-involvement product or prominently placing a low-
involvement product. Consequently, incongruence conditions (prominent placement of a low-
involvement product and subtle placement of a high-involvement product) lead to greater recall
than congruence conditions (combination of subtle placement of a low-involvement product in
an advergame or a prominent placement of a high-involvement product in an advergame).
Furthermore, the study also revealed that for a low-involvement product, a subtle
brand placement resulted in more favorable brand attitude than a prominent brand
placement. However, for a high-involvement product, a prominent brand placement
resulted in more favorable brand attitude than a subtle brand placement. These findings
support that theory that not only incongruous conditions (prominent placement of a
low-involvement product and subtle placement of a high-involvement product) increase
recall, but also counter-argumentation or adverseness which may develop less favorable
attitudes toward advergame embedded brands. In contrast, the congruence conditions
(subtle placement of a low-involvement product or a prominent placement of a
high-involvement product in an advergame) make the brand placements appear more
regular and natural; therefore, in such congruent situations less cognitive efforts are
used to know the reasons of placing brands in advergames.
Also, the present research advances various recent and existing advergame studies, such
as, Lee and Cho (2017), Vanwesenbeeck et al. (2016), Vashisht (2017), etc. This study
advances the theory used in the existing advergame studies as the present study highlights
the importance of studying advergaming effects from an integrated angle, i.e. using
“attention” and “elaboration” theories together. In addition, the existing advergame studies
either highlight the impact of the game factors or the individual factors, whereas the present
research reveals the simultaneous impact of the game factor as well as the individual factor.
Hence, this paper adds to the advertising literature, specifically the non-traditional online
advertising literature, in an emerging context.
Implications Advergamers’
Theoretical implications brand recall
In spite of certain limitations, this study provides various serious theoretical and practical and brand
implications. First, from a theoretical viewpoint, much of the previous brand placement
research examined the individual impact of product involvement and prominence in
attitude
audio-visual content, such as television, film and video games. In this study,
the simultaneous effect of product involvement and brand prominence on consumers’ 55
brand memory and attitudes was investigated from attention and elaboration perspectives
in an emerging market context like India. Second, due to many variations in the
demographic composition of internet users (Internet Live Stats, 2016), this research signifies
a critical contribution as it adds to the limited body of research exploring consumers’ brand
responses in an emerging market context. Third, this research illustrates the conditions
under which brand placements in online games create attention, elaboration and subsequent
brand recall and brand attitude by drawing the insights from LCM and ELM theories.
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
Managerial implications
From a practical viewpoint, the brand recall results show that for a low-involvement
product, a prominent brand placement (2.93) results in higher brand recall than a subtle
brand placement (1.31), whereas, for a high-involvement product, a subtle brand placement
(2.51) results in superior brand recall than a prominent brand placement (1.73). Furthermore,
brand attitude results show that for a low-involvement product, a subtle brand placement
(2.88) resulted in more favorable brand attitude than a prominent brand placement (1.55),
whereas, for a high-involvement product, a prominent brand placement (2.94) results in
more favorable brand attitude than a subtle brand placement (1.21). The results suggest that
marketers must prudently consider the use of in-game brand placements when seeking to
attain attitudinal objectives. These findings suggest that if increasing gamers’ recall is the
advertiser’s goal, it would be best to prominently place a low-involvement product in an
advergame or subtly embed a high-involvement product in the game. However,
if developing a positive brand attitude is the advertiser’s goal, it may be best to subtly
place a low-involvement product or prominently embed a high-involvement product in an
advergame. In addition to these implications, this paper also highlights that to develop
highly effective advergames, instead of considering only attention factors or only cognitive
elaboration factors, both the factors together must be taken into consideration. Specifically,
this paper explores that other than game-specific factors, such as brand prominence, there
APJML are certain gamer-specific factors, such as product involvement, which can moderate
30,1 the effect of game factors on gamers’ brand memory and attitudes.
Largely, this paper contributes to the growing body of research investigating the impact
that different placement strategies of in-game advertising have on communication efficacy.
Marketers and advertisers unavoidably face a latent trade-off between high brand recall and
unfavorable brand attitudes. Results from this research indicate some stratagems that
56 practitioners can execute for in-game brand placements, particularly for advertising
practitioners who want to market a new brand or product by using a non-conventional
communication channel, i.e. through digital games.
culture among youth or game consumers especially the college going students. For instance,
these days students get exposed to numerous ads endorsing unhealthy food which give rise
to various health-related problems. Thus, for the betterment of the society and to educate
youth, some very informative and highly attractive advergames can be developed in which
this paper’s study findings can be exploited. In other words, advergames can be best
used as learning tools by considering the findings of this study, i.e. use of a right
combination of brand prominence and product-involvement factors while making
informative and educational advergames for the betterment of society. Furthermore, with
a right combination of game- (brand prominence) and gamer-specific (product-involvement)
factors, effective advergames can be developed and used as recruitment tools by the
companies. For example, an advergame “The Great Global Adventure” was developed by
AXA insurance company to raise awareness of AXA as an international graduate employer
(AXA Newsroom, 2015). Hence, effective advergames with such effects can be developed by
taking a right mix of brand prominence and product-involvement factors into consideration.
References 57
An, S. and Stern, S. (2011), “Mitigating the effects of advergames on children: do advertising breaks
work?”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 40 No. 1, pp. 43-56.
AXA Newsroom (2015), “AXA has won two awards at the CIPD recruitment marketing awards for the
great global adventure”, available at: www.axa.com/en/newsroom/news/two-awards-best-use
(accessed March 26, 2016).
Balasubramanian, S.K., Karrh, J.A. and Patwardhan, H. (2006), “Audience response to product
placements: an integrative framework and future research agenda”, Journal of Advertising,
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
perceptions, preferences, and requests”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 36 No. 2, pp. 87-100.
Mandler, G. (1982), “The structure of value: accounting for taste”, in Clark, M.S. and Fiske, S.T. (Eds),
Affect and Cognition: The 17th Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ,
pp. 3-36.
Marti-Parreno, J., Aldas-Manzano, J., Curras-Perez, R. and Sanchez-Garcia, I. (2013), “Factors
contributing brand attitude in advergames: entertainment and irritation”, Journal of Brand
Management, Vol. 20 No. 5, pp. 374-388.
Mediaedge:cia (2005), “Playing with brands”, available at: http://adage.com/images/random/
videogamestudy112805.pdf (accessed June 15, 2015).
Meyers-Levy, J. and Tybout, A.M. (1989), “Schema congruity as a basis for product evaluation”, Journal
of Consumer Research, Vol. 16 No. 6, pp. 39-54.
Nelson, M.R. (2002), “Recall of brand placements in computer/video games”, Journal of Advertising
Research, Vol. 42 No. 2, pp. 80-92.
Nelson, M.R., Keum, H. and Yaros, R.A. (2004), “Advertainment or adcreep game players’ attitudes
toward advertising and product placements in computer games”, Journal of Interactive
Advertising, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 3-21.
Olshavsky, R.W. (1994), “Attention as an epiphenomenon: some implications for advertising”, in Clark, E.M.
et al. (Hrsg.) (Eds), Attention, Attitude and Affect in Response to Advertising, Hillsdale, MI, pp. 97-106.
Paul, J. and Mas, E. (2016), “The emergence of China and India in the global market”, Journal of
East-West Business, Vol. 22 No. 1, pp. 28-50.
Pempek, T.A. and Calvert, S.L. (2009), “Tipping the balance: use of advergames to promote
consumption of nutritious foods and beverages by low-income African American children”,
Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, Vol. 163 No. 7, pp. 633-637.
Peters, S. and Leshner, G. (2013), “Get in the game: the effects of game-product congruity and product
placement proximity on game players’ processing of brands embedded in advergames”, Journal
of Advertising, Vol. 42 Nos 2/3, pp. 113-130.
Petty, R.E. and Cacioppo, J.T. (1986), “The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion”, Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 19, pp. 123-205.
Razali, N.M. and Wah, Y.B. (2011), “Power comparisons of Shapiro-Wilk, Kolmogorov-Smirnov,
Lilliefors and Anderson-Darling tests”, Journal of Statistical Modeling and Analytics, Vol. 2 No. 1,
pp. 21-33.
Redondo, I. (2012), “The effectiveness of casual advergames on adolescents’ brand attitudes”, European
Journal of Marketing, Vol. 46 Nos 11/12, pp. 1671-1688.
Refiana, L., Mizerski, D. and Murphy, J. (2005), “Measuring the state of flow in playing online games”,
Proceedings ANZMAC 2005 Conference Broadening the Boundaries, The University of Western
Australia, Fremantle, December 5-7, pp. 108-113.
APJML Reisinger, D. (2011), “91 percent of kids are gamers research says”, available at: www.meeglobal.com/
30,1 output/Page1463.asp; www.cnet.com/news/91-percent-of-kids-are-gamers-research-says/
(accessed October 29, 2015).
Rosado, R. and Agante, L. (2011), “The effectiveness of advergames in enhancing children’s brand recall
image, and preference/a eficácia dos jogos publicitários para potenciar a notoriedade, preferência e
imagem da marca nas crianças”, Revista Portuguesa de Marketing, Vol. 14 No. 27, p. 34.
60 Rozendaal, E., Slot, N., Van Reijmersdal, E.A. and Buijzen, M. (2013), “Children’s responses to
advertising in social games”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 42 Nos 2/3, pp. 142-154.
Russell, C.A. (1998), “Toward a framework of product placements: theoretical propositions”, Advances
in Consumer Research, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 357-362.
Schneider, L.P. and Cornwell, B.B. (2005), “Cashing in crashes via brand placement in computer
games”, International Journal of Advertising, Vol. 24 No. 3, pp. 321-343.
Shapiro, S.S. and Wilk, M.B. (1965), “An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples)”,
Biometrika, Vol. 52 Nos 3/4, pp. 591-611.
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
Statista (2016a), “Statistics and facts about app stores”, available at: www.statista.com/topics/1729/
app-stores (accessed December 10, 2016).
Statista (2016b), “Global economy – statistics & facts”, available at: www.statista.com/topics/1467/
global-economy/ (accessed December 10, 2016).
Statista (2017), “Mobile phone gaming penetration in the United States from 2011 to 2020”, available at:
www.statista.com/statistics/234649/percentage-of-us-population-that-play-mobile-games/
(accessed December 10, 2016).
Suh, J.C. and Yi, Y. (2006), “When brand attitudes affect the customer satisfaction-loyalty relation:
the moderating role of product involvement”, Journal of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 16 No. 2,
pp. 145-155.
Traylor, M.B. (1981), “Product-involvement and brand commitment”, Journal of Advertising Research,
Vol. 21 No. 6, pp. 51-56.
Tsai, M.T., Wen-Ko, L. and Liu, M.L. (2007), “The effects of subliminal advertising on consumer
attitudes and buying intentions”, International Journal of Management, Vol. 3 No. 1, pp. 3-14.
Van Reijmersdal, E. (2009), “Brand placement prominence: good for memory! Bad for attitudes?”,
Journal of Advertising Research, Vol. 49 No. 2, pp. 151-153.
Van Reijmersdal, E.A., Rozendaal, E. and Buijzen, M. (2012), “Effects of prominence, involvement and
persuasion knowledge on children’s cognitive and affective responses to advergames”, Journal
of Interactive Marketing, Vol. 26 No. 1, pp. 33-42.
Vanwesenbeeck, I., Walrave, M. and Ponnet, K. (2016), “Young adolescents and advertising on social
network games: a structural equation model of perceived parental media mediation, advertising
literacy, and behavioral intention”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 45 No. 2, pp. 183-197.
Vashisht, D. (2015), “Effect of advergames on customers’ brand memory, persuasive intent and brand
attitude: an empirical study in Indian context”, available at: http://ir.inflibnet.ac.in:8080/jspui/
handle/10603/73725 (accessed March 29, 2016).
Vashisht, D. (2016a), “Advergames: non-traditional and interactive form of advertising”, available at: www.
researchgate.net/publication/303498009_A_case_study_on_advergames (accessed April 4, 2017).
Vashisht, D. (2016b), “Impact of advergame-speed, game-product congruence and persuasion
knowledge on brand recall”, paper presented at IIM Kashipur International Marketing
Conference, New Delhi, April 18-19, available at: www.iimkashipur.ac.in/index.php/hi/seminars/
international-conference-in-marketing/conference-report (accessed March 31, 2017).
Vashisht, D. (2017), “How gamers with different need for cognition process in-game brand placements
under different game-involvement conditions”, Management Research Review, Vol. 40 No. 4,
pp. 471-490.
Vashisht, D. and Chauhan, A. (2014), “Impact of advergames on brand recall and brand attitude”,
Business Sciences International Research Journal, Vol. 1 No. 2, pp. 33-36.
Vashisht, D. and Royne, M.B. (2016), “Advergames influence and brand recall: the moderating effects of Advergamers’
brand placement strength and gamers’ persuasion knowledge”, Computers in Human Behavior, brand recall
Vol. 63, pp. 162-169.
Waiguny, M.K., Nelson, M.R. and Marko, B. (2013), “How advergame content influences explicit and
and brand
implicit brand attitudes: when violence spills over”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 42 Nos 2/3, attitude
pp. 155-169.
Wise, K., Bolls, P.D., Kim, H., Venkataraman, A. and Meyer, R. (2008), “Enjoyment of advergames and
brand attitudes: the impact of thematic relevance”, Journal of Interactive Advertising, Vol. 9
61
No. 1, pp. 27-36.
Yang, M. and Roskos-Ewoldsen, D.R. (2007), “The effectiveness of brand placements in the movies:
levels of placements, explicit and implicit memory, and brand choice behavior”, The Journal of
Communication, Vol. 57 No. 3, pp. 469-489.
Yoo, S.C. and Eastin, M.S. (2016), “Contextual advertising in games: impacts of game context on a
player’s memory and evaluation of brands in video games”, Journal of Marketing
Downloaded by North South University At 23:54 22 October 2018 (PT)
For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: [email protected]