Learning Disabilities in Older Adolescents and Adults
Learning Disabilities in Older Adolescents and Adults
Learning Disabilities in Older Adolescents and Adults
A Continuation Order Plan is available for this series. A continuation order will bring delivery of each
new volume immediately upon publication. Volumes are billed only upon actual shipment. For further
information please contact the publisher.
Learning Disabilities in Older
Adolescents and Adults
Clinical Utility of the Neuropsychological
Perspective
Lynda J. Katz
Landmark College
Putney, Vermont
Gerald Goldstein
VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System and
University of Pittsburgh
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
and
Sue R. Beers
University of Pittsburgh
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
No part of this eBook may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic,
mechanical, recording, or otherwise, without written consent from the Publisher
While numerous books have been written on diagnostic practices and interven-
tion strategies for school-age children, much less information is available for the
clinician and educator who works primarily with older adolescents and young
adults who meet criteria for a diagnosis of a specific learning disorder and/or
attention deficit disorder (ADHD). Many psychologists, including the authors of
this book, specialize in the area of neuropsychology. In particular, we have
worked with adolescents and young adults who seek evaluations to determine
the presence a learning disorder more typically diagnosed in childhood because
of the significant negative impact they have begun to experience in their later
academic or vocational career. Because they had not been previously diagnosed
despite, often times, ongoing concerns of parents and significant others, they
come to the attention of clinicians as diagnostic dilemmas in many instances.
Comorbidity issues emerge as these individuals enter the young adult years,
some of which have necessitated treatment at one point or another. Clinicians
who will be sought out to conduct assessments, document the presence of a dis-
ability, and offer recommendations regarding treatment/interventions and rea-
sonable accommodations will be required to have a comprehensive knowledge
of the various aspects of language based learning disorders, what constitutes
nonverbal learning disorders and high functioning autism, and how attention
deficit disorders are typically manifest in adults in order to competently address
the complex diagnostic and treatment issues raised.
This book is an effort to provide a comprehensive review of what we know
about certain of these disorders, specifically, disorders of reading and written
expression, mathematics, nonverbal learning disorders (NVLD), high function-
ing autism, and ADHD, as they manifest in the lives of young adults. It will
readily become apparent to the reader that hotly contested controversies sur-
round the diagnostic methodologies employed across the learning disorders (one
of which is not even categorized in the DSM-IV) and ADHD. In addition, how
to intervene and the effectiveness of any given intervention in the later years of
development and maturity are open for investigation. Much of what we know is
v
vi PREFACE
Lynda J. Katz
Gerald Goldstein
Sue R. Beers
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
vii
CONTENTS
Mathematics Disorder 43
ADHD 105
References 185
Index 231
ix
1
READING AND WRITING
DISORDERS
INTRODUCTION
1
2 CHAPTER 1
Educational Definitions
The term specific learning disability means a disorder in one or more of the
basic psychological processes involved in understanding or in using language,
spoken or written, which may manifest itself in an imperfect ability to listen,
speak, read, write, spell, or do mathematical calculations. The term includes
such conditions as perceptual handicaps, brain injury, minimal brain
dysfunction, dyslexia, and developmental aphasia. Such terms do not include
children who have learning difficulties which are primarily the result of
visual, hearing, or motor handicaps, of mental retardation, of emotional
disturbance, or of environmental, cultural, or economic disadvantage (U.S.
Office of Special Education, 1977, p. 6).
Torgeson (1991) wrote that the 1968 definition has at least four major
problems. First, the definition does not indicate that learning disabilities are a
heterogeneous group of disorders; second, the definition fails to recognize the
persistence of learning disabilities into adulthood; third, it does not clearly
specify that the final common path involves inherent alterations in the way
information is processed regardless of the cause. Finally, the definition does not
recognize that individuals with other disabilities or environmental limitations
may have a learning disability (LD) concurrently with these conditions.
In summary, Lyon (1996) stated that the federal definition is “virtually
useless with respect to providing clinicians and researchers with objective
guidelines and criteria for distinguishing individuals with LD from those with
other primary disabilities or generalized learning difficulties” (p. 6). In accord
with Lyon’s earlier statement, Stanovich (1999) has written that the umbrella
term learning disabilities does nothing but confuse. “The domain specific
disabilities should be treated separately and labeled separately....Comorbidity
becomes an issue ‘after’ the initial domain-specific classification has been
carried out” (p. 350). “The logic underlying the development of such a
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 3
On the other hand, it has been observed that there are no clear operational
definitions of a written language expression disorder that address all components
of the written language domain (Berninger, 1994). According to Hooper et al.
(1994) writing has been conceptualized as a complex problem-solving process
which involves (1) the writer’s declarative knowledge, (2) procedural
knowledge, and (3) conditional knowledge, all of which interact with the
instructional paradigm and the instructor’s knowledge of the writing process.
Declarative knowledge refers to specific writing and spelling subskills and
procedural knowledge refers to the individual’s competence in using such
knowledge in writing (Lyon, 1996).
Research studies conducted by Poteet (1978) and Houck and Billingsley
(1989) suggests that composing problems of students with learning disabilities
go beyond the mechanics such as spelling, punctuation, and grammar. Wong,
Wong, and Blenkinsop (1989), Newcomer and Barenbaum (1991), and Graham,
Schwartz, and MacArthur (1993) report higher-order cognitive and
metacognitive problems in this subgroup. The closest statement suggestive of a
definition proposes that members of this subgroup are characterized by problems
in basic text production skills, knowledge about writing, and lack of ability to
engage in writing as a process requiring planning and revision (Graham, Harris,
MacArthur, and Schwartz, 1991).
4 CHAPTER 1
Since the early 1970s there have been numerous studies conducted whose aim
has been the identification of subtypes among individuals diagnosed with
learning disorders ( Boder, 1973; Rourke and Finlayson, 1978; Lyon, Stewart,
and Freeman, 1982; Rourke and Strang, 1983; McCue, Goldstein, Shelly, and
Katz, 1986; Siegel and Heaven, 1986; Goldstein, Shelly, McCue, and Kane,
1987; Rourke, 1991; Newby and Lyon, 1991; Fletcher et al., 1993; Goldstein,
Katz, Slomka, and Kelly, 1993). Subtyping models have ranged from the simple
dichotomy proposed by Johnson and Myklebust (1967) between language and
nonverbal subtypes to one involving three subtypes as exemplified in the work
of Rourke and his associates (Rourke and Finlayson, 1978; Rourke and Strang,
1983; Rourke, 1985). Further, specific subtypes with respect to reading
disabilities in children have been identified by numerous researchers (Boder,
1970; 1973; Doehring and Hoshko, 1977; Lyon, Rietta, Watson, Porch, and
Rhodes, 1981; Lyon, 1983; Lovett, 1984; Lyon 1985; Lovett, 1987)). For a
comprehensive review of the history of the subtyping issue the reader is referred
to the work of Feagans and McKinney (1991).
One example of domain specific subtyping research carried out by
educational researchers is that of Siegel and her associates (Siegel and Heaven,
1986; Siegel and Ryan, 1988, 1989a, 1989b; Shafrir, Siegel, and Chee, 1990;
and Morrison and Siegel, 1991). While initially proposing a classification)
scheme based upon a reading disability, an arithmetic/written work disability,
and an attention deficit disorder, Siegel and colleagues revised their
classification system based on patterns of academic achievement such as that
used by Fletcher (1985); Brandys and Rourke (1991), and Russell and Rourke
(1991) with children.
In subsequent work with adults, (Shafrir and Siegel, 1994) similar subtypes
were identified based upon academic deficits (reading only, reading and
arithmetic, arithmetic only). The question of an attention deficit disorder was
not addressed because of the difficulties in establishing reliable diagnoses with
the particular age group in the study. The authors apparently limited themselves
to diagnosis solely based upon parent and teacher ratings as are common in the
assessment of children. We will discuss the Shafrir and Siegel (1994) study in
detail for a number of salient reasons. First, it is one of the few studies with
adults spanning such a wide age range while taking into account educational
levels (non-postsecondary and postsecondary). Previous outcome studies have
differentiated groups based upon educational level, particularly groups serviced
through the vocational rehabilitation system and those who are college students
and/or graduates. Second, the study is methodologically sound and grounded in
empirical research findings. Third, the relevance to clinical diagnostic work is
readily apparent.
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 5
The study population reported by Shafrir and Siegel (1994) consisted of 130
members in a normal achieving control group, 88 persons in the Arithmetic
Disorder (AD) group, 32 individuals in the Reading Disorder (RD) group, and
81 individuals in the Reading and Arithmetic Disorder (RAD) group. The age
range of the population was from 16 to 72 years, with all subjects meeting the
basic criterion of an estimated IQ score of 80 or more. Subjects in the AD group
performed at or below the percentile on the Arithmetic subtest of the Wide
Range Achievement Test-R (WRAT-R) and at or above the percentile on
the Reading subtest of the WRAT-R. Parallel percentiles were established for
the RD group with subjects in the RAD group performing at or below the level
of the percentile on both the Reading and Arithmetic subtests of the WRAT-
R. A discrepancy criterion was not used as Siegel among others has consistently
advocated its irrelevance to the definition of learning disabilities in general and
reading disabilities in particular (Siegel, 1988,1989; Stanovich, 1991).
With both the RD and RAD groups results indicated a deficit in phonological
processing, in reading and spelling, and in short-term memory. However, for
the RD group the phonological deficit was present independent of educational
level, while a visual deficit in reading appeared only in the non-postsecondary
group. The authors took this finding to support the view that a phonological
deficit lies at the core of reading disability, a view previously reported by
researchers such as Wagner and Torgesen (19887) and Stanovich (1988a,
1988b) among others. They go on to speculate that a “ higher level of print
exposure at the postsecondary level may result in better visual recognition skills
for words, and may be a partial explanation for this finding” (Shafrir and Siegel,
1994, p. 131).
In contrast, the AD group did not have a deficit in reading, spelling, or
phonological processing and their performance was similar to the NA group on
pseudoword reading and phonological processing. In addition, at the
postsecondary level a greater distinction could be drawn between the AD and
NA groups. Specifically, a deficit in visual-spatial functioning was found in the
AD group at the postsecondary level but not at the non-postsecondary level.
The RAD group (but not the RD group) showed a generalized deficit in visual-
spatial processing across both educational levels. The researchers took this
finding to mean that a reading disability alone is not associated with a spatial-
visual deficit. And finally, the addition of an education level criterion to the
classification scheme resulted in an increase in homogeneity of the subtype
groups on Block Design, visual and phonological reading tasks, and the Space
Relation Task. “It appears that print exposure may play an important part in
acquiring sensitivity to orthographic cues (visual aspects of reading) that are
related to visual-spatial tasks” (p. 132).
One of the earlier theory based subtyping studies conducted on reading
disabilities exclusively which appears to have current validity was that
conducted by Lovett (1984; 1987). Lyon (1996) wrote that one of the greatest
6 CHAPTER 1
strengths of the Lovett subtyping research program has been its extensive
external validation (Newby and Lyon, 1991). Lovett proposed two subtypes of
reading disability, those identified as accuracy disabled and those identified as
rate disabled. The accuracy disabled group were those children who failed to
achieve age-appropriate word recognition skills while the rate disabled were
those whose deficient performance was related to contextual reading and
spelling vs. phonemic analysis. In other words, Lovett’s rate-disabled group
demonstrated that naming-speed deficits can exist in poor readers without the
typical phonological deficits described in most dyslexia research (Wolf and
Bowers, 1999). Her work coincides with the view of researchers primarily in
the cognitive neurosciences (Rack, Snowling, and Olson, 1992; Wolf, 1997;
Meyer, Wood, Hart, and Felton, 1998a; 1998b) who tend to view phonological
processes as separate, specific sources of disability vs. the current practice
among most reading researchers “to subsume naming speed under phonological
processes” (Wolf and Bowers, 1999).
While restricted to the study of pre-adolescents, more recent studies have
documented the presence of numerous subtypes among the dyslexic (reading
disorders) subgroup. These include studies by Stanovich, Siegel, and Gottardo
(1997) and Morris, Fletcher, Shaywitz, et al. (1998) among others. In the case
of the work of Morris and his colleagues, seven reading-disabled subtypes were
identified. Two were globally deficient in language skills and 4 of the other 5
subtypes displayed a relative weakness in phonological awareness, variations in
rapid serial naming, and verbal short-term memory.
This study (Morris et al., 1998) did not yield a subtype in which spatial
cognitive deficits were the predominant characteristic. The authors go on to
suggest that the failure of previous studies to include measures of phonological
awareness accounts for this difference and pointed out that the phonology vs.
verbal short-term memory-spatial subtype would easily have resembled the
nonverbal/spatial subtypes observed by numerous studies previously. They then
suggest that this result “highlights the importance of approaching classification
issues in children with reading disabilities from well-developed hypotheses
guiding the selection of the classification attributes” (Morris et al., p. 370).
Their apparent blatant disregard for the validity of previous research in this
regard, including recent work by Wolf, Pfeil, Lotz, and Biddle (1994) and Wolf
(1996), and their adherence to the “primacy of the decoding problem, which,
with deficits in phonological awareness, appears to be the most salient
characteristic of people with reading disabilities” (Shaywitz, 1996, p. 370),
reminds one of the statement attributed to Gerald Coles. “If you trace the
history of dyslexia research, you always find the same pattern. First, there’s a
paper or two claiming a new explanation for the disorder. Then replication
research ultimately repudiates the initial claim” (Scientific American, 1995, p.
14).
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 7
A major system dealing with adults with disabling conditions once they leave
the aegis of the education system in this country, the Rehabilitation Services
Administration (RSA), only began to offer services to individuals with learning
disabilities in 1981. At that time a policy directive was issued to state agencies
making specific learning disabilities a medically recognized disability
(Rehabilitation Services Administration, 1981). The definition then officially
adopted by the RSA in 1985 stated:
In discussing the differences between the two definitions Dowdy, Smith, and
Nowell (1992) point out that the RSA definition reflects the position that a
learning disability is a lifelong (permanent) disability resulting from a central
nervous system dysfunction. In addition, the RSA definition exclusively lists a
series of deficits (attention, reasoning, processing memory, communication, etc.)
that are then used by vocational rehabilitation agencies as criteria for eligibility.
The RSA definition does not include an exclusionary clause for other primary
handicapping condition, and it does not reference specific discrepancy criteria as
found in the special education identification criteria issued in the Federal
Register (1977).
While the RSA provides a definition for a specific learning disability, diagnoses
are the province of a licensed physician or psychologist. As such diagnoses are
established using the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) of the American Psychiatric Association (1994).
However, as most clinicians are aware, the DSM-IV separates the category
Learning Disorders from Motor Skills Disorder and Communication Disorders
(oral language disorders) and uses instead specific domains of deficit that
include: Reading Disorder, Mathematics Disorder, Disorder of Written
Expression, and Learning Disorder Not Otherwise Specified. Similarly, the
ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders (ICD-10) utilizes
specific domains of deficit when defining, classifying, and coding learning
disorders and specific developmental disorders of scholastic skills. While
diagnostic labels and criteria for establishing a reading disorder or mathematics
8 CHAPTER 1
disorder are similar between ICD-10 and DSM-IV, disorders involving written
language skills are classified dissimilarly, as a disorder of written language in
DSM-IV and a specific spelling disorder in ICD-10.
According to the DSM-IV guidelines, learning disorders are diagnosed when
the individual’s achievement on individually administered, standardized tests in
reading, mathematics, or written expression is substantially below that expected
for age, schooling, and level of intelligence, and learning problems are judged to
significantly interfere with academic achievement in these areas. The
requirement for performance that is “substantially below” that expected is
“usually defined as a discrepancy of more than 2 standard deviations between
achievement and IQ” (p. 46). Exceptions to the 2 standard deviation rule of
thumb may occur when “an individual’s performance on an IQ test may have
been compromised by an associated disorder in cognitive processing, a
comorbid mental disorder or general medical condition, or the individual’s
ethnic or cultural background” (p. 46).
Thus, while the RSA definition of a specific learning disability does not
include specific discrepancy criteria, use of such discrepancy criteria is implicit
in establishing a medical diagnosis (required in order to establish eligibility for
services) if one is to adhere to DSM-IV diagnostic criteria. Further, since
vocational rehabilitation programs are not entitlement programs, “a diagnosis of
learning disabilities by a licensed professional does not automatically entitle one
to vocational rehabilitation services (Abbott, 1987)” (Dowdy, Smith, and
Nowell, 1992). Additional eligibility criteria require, among other things, that a
given disability constitutes or results in a substantial handicap to employment,
and there must be a reasonable expectation that vocational rehabilitation services
may benefit the individual in terms of employability. These criteria make it
clear that having an academic disability does not necessarily result in a
vocational handicap and highlight the importance attached to the special
manifestations included in the RSA definition.
Finally, under the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA) of 1990 and
Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act of 1973, individuals with learning
disabilities are guaranteed certain protections and rights to equal access to
programs and services. However, in order to access these rights, the individual
must present documentation indicating that his or her disability substantially
limits some major life activity, including learning. The term “substantially
limits” means that an individual is either (1) unable to perform a major life
activity that the average person in the general population can perform or (2)
significantly restricted as to the condition, manner or duration of the major life
activity in question, when measured against the abilities of the average person in
the general population (29 C.F.R. Section 1630).
The principal purpose of documentation is the establishment of an
individual’s entitlement to special education and reasonable accommodations in
education, testing, and employment. The documentation must include
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 9
the body of literature devoted to adolescents with learning disabilities was quite
limited, with even less written on adults.
Estimates of the numbers of individuals people affected by LD range from
5-20% of the population (Gerber and Reiff, 1994; Gadbow and DuBois, 1998),
meaning that anywhere from 5 million to 30 million adults have a learning
disability. Of the two and a half million adults enrolled in Adult Basic
Education (ABE) and General Educational Development (GED) programs,
estimates of adults with learning disabilities range from 20 to 89% (Bursuck et
al., 1989; Minskoff et al., 1989; Nightengale et al., 1991). In addition, a
research and evaluation report prepared by the Urban Institute of Washington,
DC (Nightengale et al., 1991) states that an estimated 15 to 30% of all Job
Training Partnership participants and 25 to 40 % of all adults on Aid to Families
with Dependent Children have a learning disability.
Reading Disorder
In our attempts to address the issues of incidence and prevalence rate among a
population of young adults, several recent survey reports were reviewed in
greater detail in order to determine whether they might provide some useful
information. Among the studies reviewed were the National Adult Literacy
Survey (NALS) (Kirsch, Jungeblut, Jenkins, and Kilstad, 1993), the National
Longitudinal Transition Study of Special Education Students (NLTS) (Wagner
and Blackorby, 1996; Blackorby and Wagner, 1997), a report by the National
Center for Education Statistics entitled “Students with Disabilities in
Postsecondary Education: A Profile of Preparation, Participation, and Outcomes,
(NPSAS) (1999), and the Connecticut Longitudinal Study (Shaywitz, Shaywitz,
Fletcher, and Escobar, 1990; Shaywitz, 1998).
The National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) was the result of efforts by the
Department of Education Office of Educational Research and Improvement,
National Center for Education Statistics to identify the basic educational skills
needed for literacy functioning (Vogel, 1996). This self-report survey was
administered to a national representative sample of 26,000 individuals ages 16
and older drawn from a population of 191 million adults in the nation. The
target sample represented 100.6 million individuals, 48% of whom were males
and 52% female. In response to the question, “Do you currently have a learning
disability?”, 392 individuals in the target sample responded positively,
representing 2.8 million adults (2.9% of the target population). The age range of
the self-report sample with learning disabilities (SRLD) was from 25-64 years,
and there was no significant difference in the incidence rate for SRLD between
African Americans and non-African Americans (3.6% and 2.7%, respectively).
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 13
Slightly more males (56%) than females (44%) were found in the SRLD
category (Vogel and Reder, 1998).
Prevalence estimates were then gleaned by comparing the percentage of
individuals in the SRLD category in each of the five Levels of Literacy
established by the National Education Goals Panel (1993). Whereas Level 3 is
regarded as meeting the national literacy goal, five times the percentage of
adults with SRLD as opposed to those not reporting a learning disability scored
within Level 1, the lowest literacy level. However, unlike previously reported
studies where school-aged girls were found to have significantly more severe
learning disabilities than boys (Levine and Fuller, 1972, Eno and Woehlke,
1980; Ryckman, 1981; Hassett and Gurian, 1984) differences by gender were
not significant. Vogel’s (1990) review of the literature on gender differences had
found that girls identified as learning disabled were generally older, of lower
intellectual ability, and functioned at a lower academic level than boys with LD.
However, Vogel concluded her review with the suggestion that the system
identified females with learning disabilities in the studies undertaken may not be
representative of the female population with LD in general. Further, 2% of the
adults in the SRLD category scored at Level 5, suggesting that they had either
overcome their SRLD or “perhaps had a math disability rather than a language-
based LD.... Yet, they still perceived themselves as having a learning disability”
(Vogel and Reder, 1998 p. 166). While this report among many other past
studies validates the persistence of learning disabilities into adulthood (Blalock,
1982; Hartzell and Compton, 1984; Bruck, 1987; Rogan and Hartman, 1990;
Johnson, 1994) actual prevalence rates for reading and writing disorders have
not been distinguished and depend in large part on the work of Blalock (1981,
1982), Johnson and Blalock (1987), and Zigmond and Thornton (1985 ) who
reported that 80% or more of individuals with a learning disability in their
studies had reading disabilities.
The second major survey, the National Longitudinal Transition Study of Special
Education Students (NLTS, 1996), was based on a nationally representative
sample of more than 8000 individuals with disabilities drawn from the rosters of
special education students from more than 300 school districts across the nation
(Wagner and Blackorby, 1996). Data were collected in 1987 and again in 1990
on special education students who were between the ages of 15 and 21 years in
the 1985-86 school year. Approximately 55% of the population under study had
been classified as learning disabled. Unlike their non-disabled peers, students
with disabilities and particularly learning disabilities were more likely to be
African American and male. Twenty-two percent of the students with learning
disabilities were African American even though African American students
constituted only 14% of the general population of students of similar age.
14 CHAPTER 1
The National Postsecondary Student Aid Study (NPSAS) undertaken during the
1995-96 academic year involved a nationally representative sample of 21,000
undergraduates who were queried regarding the presence of disabilities. Of the
6% of graduates reporting a disability, 29% said they had a learning disability.
These results are similar to those reported by Henderson (1999) in the triennial
statistical profile of college freshmen with disabilities where 32.2% of the
freshmen students surveyed reported a learning disability (double the figure
reported in 1988). Of these students, 78% were Caucasian, 8-10% were African
American, 4% were Asian American, and 2% were Mexican American.
Caucasian males were over-represented among the group in contrast to students
without disabilities (52 vs. 46%) and women were underrepresented (36 vs.
42%). Typically, students with disabilities had been out of high school longer
than their nondisabled peers.
Henderson’s (1999) survey results were similar to those reported through the
NPSAS. Overall, those with disabilities were more likely to have taken
remedial mathematics and English courses in high school, less likely to have
taken advanced placement courses, had lower high school GPAs, and had lower
than average SAT entrance exam scores. In summary, nearly one third of ill
college students with disabilities reported having learning disabilities. It is
inferred that 80% of these disabilities would be related to reading given the
incidence figures previously reported.
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 15
Finally, we turn to the longitudinal data emanating from the work of Shaywitz,
Shaywitz, Fletcher, and Escobar (1990) following 414 Connecticut children
identified as having a reading disability on the basis of school behavior and
objective research testing. While the prevalence of reading disorders was two to
four times more common in boys than girls when made on the basis of school
identification, there were no significant differences in the prevalence of a
reading disability between the genders when the diagnosis was made on the
basis of research identification. (Shaywitz, Holford, and Holahan, et al., 1995).
These findings persisted well into adolescence, with delayed reading levels
between disabled and nondisabled readers remaining relatively constant. There
was no evidence that children identified in the “persistently poor reader” group
caught up in their reading skills over time (Shaywitz, Fletcher, Holahan, et al.,
1999).
have demonstrated that reading and writing systems can be dissociated. “As
expected, many, but not all, individuals with writing disabilities manifest
deficits in reading” (Lyon, 1996).
In contrast to this work, in a most recent publication, Siegel (1999) has
concluded that while spelling difficulties can occur in the absence of severe
reading disabilities (Bruck and Waters, 1988; Lennox and Siegel, 1993), and
while there also may be problems with understanding or producing language,
these problems have not been documented as distinct learning disabilities and
are often components of dyslexia. The “existence of a separate written language
disability has not been clearly established nor is there a clear definition of it
especially in the adult population” (p. 306). However, it has been only since the
mid-1980s that the study of disorders of written expression has “risen from its
status as ‘a poor relative of aphasia’ (Shallice, 1988)” (Lyon, 1996, p. 35).
In summary, while there is a great deal of variability in the reporting of
incidence and prevalence rates among children and adults, it is clear that 80% of
individuals diagnosed with a learning disability do indeed meet criteria for a
reading disorder. The reasons for the high degree of variability would appear to
be related to factors such as methods of identification, assessment strategies, and
measures of literacy. In addition, while there appears to be no differentiation in
incidence rates by gender, the effects of race and socio economic status are
positively correlated with the diagnosis of a learning disability. Finally, there is
no reason to assume that the rates for adults should be any less than for children
as the chronic nature of the disorder has been well established in the
professional literature.
Early Work
It has been said that the investigator who put dyslexia “on the map” in the
United States was Samuel Torrey Orton, a neurologist and neuropathologist,
who recognized a distinctive syndrome that had symptoms in common with the
classical condition of acquired alexia with agraphia (Geschwind, 1985, p.1).
However, rather than being accepted by his colleagues in the medical profession
his work was taken up by educators who then began to develop methods of
remediation still in use today (Orton, 1966; Gillingham and Stillman, 1973). In
his report entitled “Word-Blindness in School Children” (1925), Orton
compared dyslexia to the aphasias, postulating that the problem was at the
symbolic level, and that dyslexia did not imply low intelligence. Orion’s
findings of crossed-eye/hand preference led him to postulate that the mechanism
of the behavior lay in a competition for the perception and memory of letters
between the two hemisphere, thus introducing the role of hemispheric
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 17
Genetic Research
For at least the past 30 years, genetic studies have attempted to define the nature
of the familial contribution to dyslexia (Zerbin-Rudin, 1967). Bakwin (1973)
had the largest single twin sample on which reading disabilities had been
reported up to that point. And while his study, and those reviewed by Zerbin-
Rudin previously, suffered from methodological limitations, a significantly
greater concordance rate for reading retardation in monozygotic twins than in
dizygotic twins was reported. Further, the observation that dyslexia aggregates
in families is nearly as old as the description of the disorder itself, dating back to
1905 (Pennington and Gilger, 1996).
More recently, two methodologically sound twin studies of dyslexia have
been conducted, one at the Institute for Behavior Genetics in Boulder, Colorado
(the Twin Family Reading Study) and the other at the Institute for Psychiatry in
London (Stevenson, Graham, Fredman, and McLoughlin, 1987). The Colorado
study began in 1979 and in a 1991 update the investigators reported that
“evidence has been obtained to indicate that the correlation between
phonological coding and word recognition is largely due to heritable influences,
whereas the relationship between orthographic coding and word recognition is
due primarily to environmental influences” (DeFries, Olson, Pennington, and
Smith, 1991, p. 83). Results obtained through linkage analysis suggested that
reading disability is etiologically heterogeneous. Linkage analysis is based on
the fact that genes that are close together on the same chromosome tend to be
inherited together as they are passed on from parent to child. Twenty percent of
the families with an apparent autosomal dominant transmission for reading
disability manifested linkage to chromosome 15 but not to chromosome 6.
Earlier case reports and family studies had led previous researchers to the
conclusion that reading disability was inherited as an autosomal dominant
condition (Hallgren, 1950). Subsequently, other studies demonstrated more than
one mode of transmission including autosomal dominant, autosomal recessive,
18 CHAPTER 1
With the seminal work of Galaburda in the early 1980s, the neuropathologic
basis of dyslexia began to be more fully elucidated. Four consecutive male
brains studied by Galaburda et al. (1985) showed developmental anomalies of
the cerebral cortex, consisting of neuronal ectopias (displaced neurons) and
architectonic dysplasias (distortions of the normal organized layering of the
cortex) (Rumsey and Eden, 1998). The anomalies, thought to be of prenatal
origin, were seen on both sides of the brain, but showed a predisposition for the
left perisylvian language cortex.
As ectopias are densely and aberrantly connected with other brain regions,
one result of ectopia formation is the alteration of brain organization. One such
alteration in dyslexia is the lack of asymmetry in a language-related cortical
region called the planum temporale, an auditory area that lies on the superior
surface of the temporal lobe. In contrast, in individuals without dyslexia the
planum temporale is usually larger in the left hemisphere (Sherman, 1995).
Also reported were developmental anomalies within the lateral geniculate
nucleus, a thalamic nucleus through which visual information is transmitted to
the cortex (Livingstone, Rosen, Drislane, and Galaburda, 1991). As described
by Rumsey and Eden, this structure consists of two ventral magnocellular layers
and four dorsal parvocellular layers. While the parvocellular (small-celled)
layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus appeared normal in the dyslexic brains,
the magnocellular layers were disorganized and the cell bodies within these
layers appeared smaller in dyslexic than in the control brains (Livingstone, et al,
1991). These findings were consistent with the hypothesis that deficits in rapid
temporal visual processing in dyslexia result from a dysfunctional magnocellular
visual system. Sherman (1995) goes on to write that the visual processing
disturbance could interfere with normal reading just as similar deficits in other
sensory pathways such as the auditory system (Tallal, 1976; Tallal and Piercy,
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 19
1979; Hagman, Wood, Buchsbaum, et al., 1992) could interfere with the
acquisition of phonological skills.
In conjunction with the work of Livingstone et al., (1991), more recently,
Stein and Walsh (1997) have postulated a general timing hypothesis. Namely,
that rather than singling out either phonological, visual or motor deficits,
temporal processing in all three systems may be impaired. They go on to
suggest that there is some evidence which seems to indicate that magnocellular
temporal processing deficits are not confined to vision and audition, but extend
to other systems such as the vestibular and motor systems. “Dyslexics are
notoriously clumsy and uncoordinated, their writing is appalling, their balance is
poor, and they show other soft cerebellar signs, such as reach and gaze overshot
and muscle hypotonia” (p. 151). The cerebellum is a major target of m-type
(magnocellular) efferents. They conclude by suggesting that selective damage
in utero to a particular magnocellular neuronal cell line that plays a major role in
temporal processing in all sensory, sensorimotor, and motor systems throughout
the brain may occur, resulting from genetic impairment of cellular development
or immunological attack.
Neuroimaging Work
1992), caudate (Wood, Flowers, Buchsbaum, and Tallal, 1991), and the occipital
lobe (Gross-Glenn et al., 1991). (The reader is referred to the paper by Riccio
and Hynd (1996) for a more detailed discussion of these findings). These results
led Riccio and Hynd to conclude that the conceptualization that reading involves
a widely distributed functional system, resulting in a heterogeneous group of
adults who may be diagnosed with dyslexia. This conceptualization was
supported and consistent with the interactive activation model of reading
development proposed by Adams (1990), Chase and Tallal (1991) among others.
During the 1990s fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) became
available and has been used in studies similar to that reported by Shaywitz et al.
(1997). Shaywitz et al. report that their neurobiologic investigations are
“informed by an empirically supported model of both reading and reading
disability....Identification of phonological processing as the core cognitive
deficit in dyslexia provides a conceptual template for planning and interpreting
studies seeking the neuronal underpinnings of dyslexia” (p. 24). Using fMRI
techniques, activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in male subjects engaged
in phonological processing tasks, was found whereas females doing the very
same task engaged both the left and right inferior frontal gyrus. Further, when
reading letters in a word (orthography) a region in the back of the brain was
activated. As the letters form sounds (phonology), the neuronal systems in the
inferior frontal gyrus (Broca’s area) were activated. As the sound structure of
the words was used to get to the meaning of the word (assembled phonology),
regions in the middle of the brain, the superior and middle temporal gyrus, were
activated.
Here, we again have another example of the rigorous adherence to one
particular theory by various researchers. As Stein and Walsh have written,
“Although the evidence in favour of the phonological weakness of dyslexics has
dominated the scene recently, it does not diminish the importance of the visual
perceptual problems that many dyslexics report” (p. 148). In the words of
Galaburda, the Harvard University neuroscientist in reference to Shaywitz:
“She’s wrong, and that’s the end of it”....”The distinctions we make about the
visual and auditory brain are somewhat arbitrary” (Scientific American, 1995,
p. 14).
A chapter by Rumsey and Eden (1998) describes the first PET and fMRI
studies of regional cerebral blood flow in dyslexia, investigating both language-
based and visually-based abnormalities in men with clear, persisting cases of
dyslexia. Their male subjects all had childhood histories of a developmental
reading disorder, continued to show at least mild deficits in decoding, and had
average intelligence. While their reading comprehension and verbal
comprehension skills were good, they continued to perform poorly on measures
of phonological processing, including tests of phonological awareness and
pseudoword reading.
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 21
Beginning in the mid to late 1980s a number of research studies emerged in the
literature relating to the neuropsychological profiles of adults with learning
disabilities and dyslexia in particular and the discriminant validity of
neuropsychological measures (Finucci, Whitehouse, Isaacs, and Childs, 1984;
McCue, 1984; Miles, 1986; Horn, O’Donnell, and Leicht, 1988; Badian,
McAnulty, Duffy, and Als, 1990; Felton, Naylor, and Wood, 1990; O’Donnell,
Romero, and Leicht, 1990; O’Donnell, 1991; Katz and Goldstein; 1993).
Research published in the prior decade (Coles, 1978; 1987) had challenged the
construct and discriminant validity of neuropsychological test batteries in so far
as they obtained to children with learning disabilities. We will review two of
these well-designed studies in some detail as they appear to exemplify what we
know about the neuropsychological profiles of adults with learning disabilities
22 CHAPTER 1
The second study conducted by Felton, Naylor, and Wood (1990) accessed 115
adults between the ages of 20 and 44 years who had initially been evaluated
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 23
between 1957 and 1972 by June Lyday Orton and for whom childhood
achievement and intelligence test scores were available. Finucci, Whitehouse,
Isaacs, and Childs’ (1984) method of classifying adults according to degree of
reading deficit was used. In addition to the administration of intelligence and
achievement tests, a battery of neuropsychological measures was utilized
including the: Boston Naming Test (BNT), Verbal Fluency Test (FAS), Rapid
Automatized Naming Test (RAN), Rapid Alternating Stimulus Test (RAS), the
Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test (LAC), the Word Attack subtest
from the Woodcock Psychoeducational Battery, Prose Recall, Rey Auditory
Verbal Learning Test (AVLT), Rey Complex Figure Drawing (CFT),
Trailmaking Test (Trails B), and Judgment of Line Orientation (JL).
Consistent with their original hypothesis regarding core deficits in the reading
disabled group, the tasks which most clearly differentiated the RD and the NDR
subjects were those requiring rapid, sequential retrieval of verbal labels,
phonetic decoding of nonwords, and analysis and manipulation of phonemes.
When controlling for IQ and socioeconomic status, no differences were found
between reading ability groups on measures of memory for visual or verbal
material, visual perception, visuomotor speed, mental flexibility, confrontation
naming, or verbal fluency. (Felton, Naylor, and Wood, 1990). In terms of
diagnosing dyslexia in individual adults, the accuracy of nonword reading is a
“potent indicator of a history of reading disability even in subjects with
relatively intact single word reading and comprehension skills” (p. 495). They
state further that within the average range of intellectual ability, cognitive
deficits involving rapid retrieval of verbal labels and the analysis and
manipulation of phonemes are frequent correlates of childhood reading
disability. They caution, however, that within these broad guidelines, there is
evidence of considerable heterogeneity among adult subjects citing the work of
Elbert and Seale (1988) among others.
In summary, research has confirmed that there are significant
neuropsychological differences between adults with and without learning
disabilities, with deficits tending to be somewhat nonspecific. However, when
one controls for intelligence, educational background, and some socioeconomic
status variables, deficits appear to be restricted to the language domain in
individuals with dyslexia (Bigler, 1992).
The literature dealing with the assessment of basic processes in adults with
reading disabilities and most particularly that driven by phonological processing
deficit theory appears more frequently in reading-related and
developmental/educational psychology journals vs. more traditional
24 CHAPTER 1
neuropsychology literature in the United States. This may speak to the crossing-
over and convergence of neurobiological and neuropsychological findings from
the field of reading disabilities itself into the educational literature. In any case,
the research literature reflects the specific definition of dyslexia used. As
described by Rack (1997), in practice one can make a distinction between
“broadly defined” and “narrowly defined” dyslexia (p.67). Narrowly defined
dyslexia is developmental phonological dyslexia; broadly defined dyslexia is a
difficulty in acquiring literacy skills which is related to any underlying specific
learning difficulty, not solely phonological processing. We suggest that this
rather generalized distinction captures the two current views of developmental
dyslexia, which have been applied to studies with adults.
The first theory clusters around the primacy of phonological awareness
deficits (phoneme awareness) among the phonological processing skills
positively correlated with early reading skills (the others include phonological
memory and rate of access for phonological information, i.e. rapid naming of
letters, digits, colors, objects. Underlying this theory is the work in the early
1970s by the psycholinguist Liberman (Liberman, Shankweiler, Liberman,
Fowler, and Fischer (1977), followed by systematic research in reading by
investigators such as Bradley and Bryant (1978, 1983); Wagner and Torgesen
(1987), Brady and Shankweiler (1991); Torgesen, Wagner, and Rashotte (1994),
and Foorman et al. (1997), among others.
Work by Pratt and Brady (1988), Bruck (1992), and Elbro, Nielsen, and
Petersen (1994) has confirmed the persistence of phoneme awareness deficits in
adults previously diagnosed with dyslexia as children. However, in the study by
Bruck (1992), her data suggested that the arrest in phoneme awareness skills
found in children and adults was associated with the failure to use orthographic
information when making phonological judgments. That is, in contrast to
nondyslexic children, for those with dyslexia, word recognition skill has very
little impact on the development of phoneme awareness and on the use of
orthographic information. Consistent with the underlying theory, her hypothesis
surrounding these results states: “...these data may be consistent with a more
general model of the relationship between phonological awareness and word
recognition by suggesting that initially dyslexic children encounter much
difficulty in learning to read because of pervasively deficient phonological
awareness skills. When they eventually acquire word recognition skills, there is
little interaction between orthographic and phonological codes” (p. 885).
Phoneme awareness skills develop as a function of word recognition skills in
normal children. But, the development of phoneme awareness is associated
with increases in the use of orthographic information when making phonological
judgments (Bruck, 1992). Thus, coming from a broader view of dyslexia one
may have reached another conclusion than that reached by this investigator
Mattis, 1978; Ehri, 1980; Corcos and Willion, 1993; Willows, Kruk, and
Corcos, 1993; Roberts and Mather, 1997). This view includes orthographic
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 25
In summary, one of the few salient pieces of recent research that we were able to
locate linking specific assessment strategies with adults to the most current
causal relationship theories addressed in the reading disability literature was that
of Gottardo, Siegel, and Stanovich (1997). While previous work by Bruck
(1990) and others has primarily been concerned with establishing the continued
presence of phonological awareness deficits in adults with reading disabilities,
the relationships between phonological deficits and more general cognitive
abilities investigated in children remain largely unexplored in the adult literature
according to Gottardo and colleagues. These investigators sought to establish
whether phonological awareness remains a unique predictor of reading ability
when controlling for syntactic processing.
26 CHAPTER 1
The battery used (Gottardo, Siegel, and Stanovich, 1997) required 2.5 hours
of administration time and consisted of the Reading subtest from the WRAT-R
(Jastak and Wilkinson, 1984), the Word Identification and Word Attack subtests
from the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test (Woodcock, 1987), the Nelson-
Denny Reading Test (Brown, Bennett, and Hanna, 1981), and the Block Design,
Vocabulary, and Digit Span subtests from the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981).
Listening comprehension was assessed via the listening comprehension subtest
from the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (Wechsler, 1992).
Experimental tasks were utilized to assess working memory, syntactic
processing, phonological processing skills, and pseudoword repetition.
Phonological processing skills were measured via the Auditory Analysis Test
(AAT) (Rosner and Simon, 1971). This instrument measures syllable and
phoneme deletion performance, and a modified Pig Latin Test (Pennington et
al., 1990).
Results of this study comparing poor readers (reading at or below the
percentile on the WRAT-R word recognition test) and average readers (reading
at or above the percentile on the same test), with a mean age of 33 years
and nonverbal IQ scores within the average range i.e. Scaled score > than 7 on
the Block Design subtest of the WAIS-R, demonstrated that adults with reading
disabilities performed significantly worse on all the standardized achievement
tests (reading and listening comprehension) and on the experimental measures of
syntactic processing, phonological processing, pseudoword repetition, and on
the recall component of the working memory task. Using hierarchical
regression analysis, both the AAT (phoneme deletion measure) and WAIS-R
Vocabulary scores were found to be “strong unique statistical predictors” of
raw scores on the word reading test (Gottardo, Siegel, and Stanovich, 1997, p.
50). However, the Pig Latin task appeared to be a stronger predictor of non-
word reading ability than was phoneme deletion. The investigators suggested
that performance on the Pig Latin task, which requires deleting a phoneme
followed by blending that phoneme with a new syllable, may have more
processes in common with novel word decoding than a simpler phoneme
deletion task. “Given that verbal working memory ability was statistically
controlled in these analyses, it is unlikely that performance differences on these
tasks are solely the result of differences in memory load” (p. 52).
In terms of an assessment battery for adults, Gottardo, Siegel, and Stanovich
(1997) recommended including a measure of phonological processing (phoneme
deletion task or the modified Pig Latin task), even though normative scores for
these tasks are not available for adults as well as a measure of explicit
knowledge of vocabulary such as the WAIS-R Vocabulary score. They also
conclude that an individual’s level of reading performance may best determine
the phonological processing measure to use in the assessment process.
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 27
Since the writing of the Gottardo et al. study (1997), Wagner, Torgesen and
Rashotte (1999) have published the Comprehensive Test of Phonological
Processing (CTOPP), the subtests of which “represent refined versions of
experimental tasks we devised over the past decade of research...” (CTOPP
Examiner’s Manual, 1999, p. vii). The authors go on to define phonological
processing as a kind of auditory processing that is most strongly related to
mastery of written language and clearly implicated as the “most common cause
of reading disabilities or dyslexia” (p.2). Three kinds of phonological
processing are deemed relevant to the mastery of written language: phonological
awareness, phonological memory, and rapid naming. The CTOPP was
developed for individuals between the ages of 5-0 and 24-11, thus its utility in
the evaluation of adolescents and adults. There are six core subtests and six
supplemental subtests with composite scores derived from them. The various
subtests include: Elision (“Say bold without saying /b/”); Blending Words
(“What word do these sounds make?”); Sound Matching (“Which word starts
with the same sound as pan? Pig, hat, or cone?”); Memory for Digits; Nonword
Repetition; Rapid Color Naming; Rapid Object Naming; Rapid Digit Naming;
Rapid Letter Naming; Blending Nonwords (“What made-up word do these
sounds make: nim-by?”); Segmenting Words; Segmenting Nonwords; and,
Phoneme Reversal (“Ood”, say “ood”; now say “ood” backwards”).
An audiotape accompanies the test manual for standard of administration of
a number of the subtests, practice items are presented for each subtest, and the
administration time is generally about 30 minutes. It is only necessary to
administer the core subtests in order to arrive at composite scores in
Phonological Awareness, Phonological Memory, and Rapid Naming. Grade
equivalent scores, percentiles, and standard scores conforming to those used
with the various Wechsler scales are provided. As would be expected, a
comprehensive discussion of norming procedures, demographics, and validity
and reliability studies are presented in the manual as well.
Finally, while no measures assessing writing skills in adults exist, the Test of
Written Language-3 (TOWL-3), which is normed on high school seniors, is still
useful with college age young adults if one takes into account a degree of score
inflation. The strength of the TOWL-3 is comprehensiveness as it covers both
contrived and spontaneous writing. For a more detailed discussion of both
formal and informal means of assessing writing skills in adolescents in
particular, the reader is referred to the chapter by Wong (1996).
We will not enter into a discussion of “the” appropriate assessment battery for
use by clinicians. This issue is one that a competent professional should be
28 CHAPTER 1
dealing with on a daily basis, one that is driven by a solid knowledge base,
consistent with standards of excellence and ecological validity, and grounded in
ethics of practice. Further, the chapter by Beers (1998) provides a
comprehensive discussion of a battery of neuropsychological and ancillary
measures that were used to diagnose learning disabilities in a college age
population and that were useful also in delineating the strengths and weaknesses
in these students for the purposes of educational accommodations (Beers,
Goldstein, and Katz, 1994). Similar approaches to neuropsychological
assessment with adults seeking services from state vocational agencies have
been previously documented by McCue, Katz, and Goldstein (1985), McCue
(1994), and Michaels, Lazar, and Risucci (1997).
Comorbidity
Remediation
The goal of remedial instruction for adults with reading disabilities is to help
them to acquire reading skills that parallel those of their nondisabled peers.
Regardless of the specific methodology employed, research in reading
instruction based upon work with children would suggest that instruction must
be more explicit and comprehensive, more intensive, and more supportive, both
emotionally and cognitively (Torgesen, 1998). It would appear that these same
guidelines should apply to adults as well. Even though adult basic education
professionals involved in literary programs might differ from professionals in
the reading disabilities field with respect to the definition of learning disabilities
(Ross and Smith, 1990), common to both groups are core principles that include
the importance of a trusting adult-to-adult, client-teacher relationship and the
use of a variety of techniques and materials, particularly materials relevant to the
life situations of adults (Ross-Gordon, 1996). In general, remediation efforts
focus on those deficits known to be associated with reading disability while
building on strengths.
Direct instruction in phonics and phonological awareness are two such
methods. Explicit phonics training programs do not include specific training in
30 CHAPTER 1
phonological awareness, per se. Clark and Urhy (1995) defined phonics as low
level rote knowledge of the association between letters and sounds and
phonological awareness as including a higher level metacognitive understanding
of word boundaries within spoken sentences, of syllable boundaries in spoken
words, and how to isolate phonemes and establish their location within syllables
and words.
Reading remediation programs based on explicit phonics include the Orton-
Gillingham approach (Gillingham and Stillman,1973), Alphabetic Phonics (Cox,
1985), Recipe for Reading (Traub and Bloom, 1975), and the Wilson Reading
System (Wilson, 1988). Among these, the Wilson Reading System is one of the
few remedial programs developed specifically for adolescents and adults with
dyslexia. It, like many of the other programs listed above, is based on Orton-
Gillingham principles, and as such is a multisensory, synthetic approach to
teaching reading and writing (Church, Fessler, and Bender, 1998).
In terms of specific instructional approaches based upon the phonological
awareness paradigm, Auditory Discrimination in Depth (ADD) was developed
by Charles and Pat Lindamood (1975). The aim of this program is to teach
auditory conceptualization skills basic to reading and spelling. It can be used
with adults who fail to read and spell successfully because of a failure to acquire
phonemic analysis skills. In a well-controlled remediation study comparing both
an Embedded Phonics approach and an adaptation of ADD (Phonological
Awareness Plus Synthetic Phonics) with 8 to 11 year old children, children in
both groups moved from substantially below average in performance on
measures of alphabetic reading into the average range. At the end of the
instruction period, a smaller of proportion of children in the ADD group
remained substantially impaired (> one standard deviation below average) in
alphabetic reading accuracy (9 vs. 26%). However, follow-up data were not yet
available at the time of this writing to answer questions regarding the impact of
stimulating alphabetic reading skills on subsequent growth in orthographic
reading ability (Torgesen, 1998). In the words of Torgesen: “Phonetic reading
skills are probably a necessary but not sufficient cause of growth in sight word
reading ability....We still do not have solid research evidence that explicitly
phonetic methods produce greater gains in comprehension than those
emphasizing whole word, or context oriented instruction” (p. 216).
Also in line with remediation interventions, in a recent study of college
students from a technical institute and a western Canadian university, Leong
(1999) was able to demonstrate quantitative and qualitative differences in
processing morphological words and letter strings between students identified
with reading disabilities and age matched contrast groups. These results are in
contrast to those reported by Bruck (1993) in her study with college students.
However, the finding of deficits in rapidity of morphological as well as
phonological processing, a previously well established finding, may have
relevance in terms of intervention strategies. Leong goes on to suggest that an
READING AND WRITING DISORDERS 31
approach to utilize with older learners is one that promotes systematic and
explicit teaching of word knowledge and spelling based on morphological
structure, word origin, and productive rules. “This approach emphasizes the
interrelation of symbol-sound correspondences, syllable and morpheme patterns,
and layers of language of Anglo-Saxon, Latin, and Greek origin (words of
Greco-Latin origin occurring much more frequently in science texts)” (p. 236).
Remedial strategies directed specifically at spelling disorders have been
addressed by Moats (1983; 1993a; 1993b; 1994). Her research has included
high school level students diagnosed with dyslexia and she has described the
nature of the persistent phonological and morphological spelling errors most
typical of the population under study. In her text Spelling Development,
Disability, and Instruction (1995) she details the elements essential to the
remediation and teaching of spelling. Moats comments that “Adolescents and
adults often view themselves as hopeless cases if they have spelling disabilities,
especially if prior instruction has been haphazard or linguistically uninformed.
Many of them, however, can make significant improvement if their disabilities
are addressed systematically, sequentially, and logically over a sustained period
of time” (p. 107).
Finally, work by Klein and Hecker (1994) documents the use of kinesthetic
and spatial strategies to teach essay writing to both college level visual thinkers
and students formally diagnosed with dyslexia. In both cases models of how
ideas relate were captured by the use of Tinkertoys, Legos, and colored pipe
cleaners as well as choreographing ones ideas through whole body movement.
A shift away from purely remedial models took place in the secondary schools
in the late 1980s, based upon the work of educational researchers such as
Palincsar and Brown (1984), Deshler and Schumaker (1986), McTighe and
Lyman (1988). Others (Jones, 1988) were applying strategy instruction to text
learning in the public schools and in the U.S. Army. These various strategy
training approaches (e.g. reciprocal teaching, specific strategy procedures for
teaching writing skills, word identification and paraphrasing, error monitoring
and comprehension, and cognitive mapping) rely heavily on the individual
student’s ability to learn how and when to apply a specific problem solving
strategy. Underlying the strategy training approach is an understanding among
educational researchers that there are commonalities among individuals with a
learning/reading disability. These include (1) difficulty organizing information
on their own (especially abstract information), (2) bringing limited background
knowledge to many academic activities, and (3) the requirement of feedback and
practice to retain abstract information (Gersten, 1998).
Built into most of the strategic learning models is the notion that teachers and
instructional materials need to be explicit about what is to be learned rather than
32 CHAPTER 1
Higgins, 1998). OCR (Optical Character Recognition) systems permit the direct
inputting of printed material into a computer by means of a scanner. Once
scanned, the material can be read back by the user by means of a speech
synthesis/screen-reading system. These systems can either be stand alone or are
PC-based and have been developed by companies such as Xerox (Bookwise),
Arkenstone (Open Book), and Kurzwweil (Omni) specifically for individuals
with learning disorders. These systems provide the advantage of allowing the
reader to highlight salient materials in the text and afterward produce a written
outline of the highlighted materials. At the same time the reader can margin
note while reading along with the speech synthesizer.
Elkind (1998) conducted a study with community college students in
California who were diagnosed as “learning disabled” using a California system-
wide discrepancy definition. Reading rate was a major problem for the majority
of the students in the study with a median percentile ranking at the
percentile on a standardardized reading test, the Nelson-Denny. Results
indicated that those who had poorer unaided scores obtained greater benefit
from the use of the Kurzweil 3000 than did those whose unaided comprehension
was better. Elkind wrote that: “...participants whose unaided comprehension is
poorer than that of graders are likely to experience gains in (timed)
comprehension from the use of the Kurzweil 3000” (p.5). Only three of the
participants (12%) indicated difficulty integrating the auditory and visual
information provided by the reading machine. Elkind summarized his findings
in terms of the characteristics of those individuals whose reading speed,
comprehension, or endurance improves when they use computer reader
technology as follow:
Hecker (1999) has reported on a pilot program for low decoders currently
under investigation at Landmark College in Putney, VT. The program
(Integrated Language Curriculum) was designed to allow adult students who
read poorly to work on reading skills at two levels simultaneously: learning the
fundamentals of decoding through remediation strategies and providing
exposure to print that is intellectually challenging. The program has a 2-year
baseline and comparative data that suggest remarkable progress in reading
comprehension scores for the group with access to both remediation and text-to-
speech software, specifically the Kurzweil Reader (Hecker, personal
correspondence).
For those individuals with written language deficits, and depending upon the
degree of severity of the deficit, various outlining programs, spell and grammar
check capacities, and speech recognition systems that allow for dictation have
36 CHAPTER 1
meaning of the target word (run for ran), and errors unrelated to meaning (rain
for ran).
In a study by Leyser, Vogel, Wyland, and Brulle (1999) (also discussed in
Chapter 3) the practices and attitudes of faculty in higher education with respect
to students with learning disabilities were examined. In response to a faculty
survey, various factors were elicited that appeared to affect the willingness of
those responding to provide accommodations either in the classroom or for
examinations. The use of technology in examinations was a widely accepted
accommodation, particularly use of a word processor with spell and grammar
check capacities. Surprisingly, these faculty were less willing to permit students
to answer test questions using a tape recorder, objecting to the time commitment
involved in listening to the students’ tape recorded responses.
reported good outcomes and two poor outcomes. Outcomes were defined in
terms of academic, occupational, social, and emotional parameters. In general,
findings from this review suggest that while childhood reading or learning
problems persist over time, the severity of the problems may decrease for some
individuals who then proceed to pursue further schooling and occupational
advancement. Whether these same individuals also experience fewer
psychological problems remains an unknown.
Results from an even more recent review (Levine and Nourse, 1998) shed
still less light on what we have learned from follow-up studies. Somewhat
uniquely, the authors present aggregate outcome data concerning the issue of
women with learning disabilities and suggest that the “uniformity of goals and
school curricular interventions for youth, and for young females in particular”
(p.230) are subject to challenge. In an earlier review (Levine and Edgar, 1995)
the gender issue in long-term postschool adjustment of youth with and without
disabilities was addressed. These authors concluded that the bulk of the
literature would say that males and females with disabilities differ in their
postschool outcomes in terms of employment, marriage, and parenting rates but
not in terms of postsecondary attendance or independent residence. Earlier
Bruck (1987) suggested that the poorer adjustments of females in the studies
she reviewed might reflect differences in societal attitudes towards men and
women who fail. “Females who fail may be more rejected by peers and less
accepted by adults than boys who fail. These interactions may be the precursors
of social and emotional problems of LD (sic) females” (p.260). In any case both
sets of conclusions raise critical issues for any long-term follow-up study.
Numerous other studies have appeared in the literature regarding the
educational, occupational, and social adjustment of young adults with learning
disabilities (Spekman, Goldberg, and Herman, 1992; Vogel and Hruby, 1993;
Lewandowski and Arcangelo, 1994; Greenbaum, Graham, and Scales, 1996;
Goldstein, Murray, and Edgar, 1998; Witte, Philips, and Kakela, 1998). The
topics range from job satisfaction of college graduates to employment earnings
and from social adjustment and self-concept to factors related to “success” in the
young adult years. As can be seen from the titles of the studies, there is a
tendency to demarcate a specific area for outcome research vs. the previous look
at numerous factors across the span of major life events and activities. It is too
early to say whether this approach will yield more useful data with respect to
outcome studies.
This concludes our review of outcome studies involving reading and writing
disorders in the young adult population. What we have learned is that the field
has far to go. The work of Shaywitz and her colleagues (1990) may well yield
significant data in the years to come because of the rigorous design of their
follow up study. On the other hand, the participant/ethnographic research
paradigm which marks the work of individuals such as Gerber (1991) may make
the content of outcome studies even richer and ultimately effect the nature of
42 CHAPTER 1
interventions and practices. Brack (1987) wrote that the most important
antecedents of positive outcome are early identification accompanied by early
intervention. It was the dimensions of intervention rather than dimension of IQ,
socioeconomic status, and severity of childhood disability that had the greatest
impact on positive outcome. We believe this message is still valid over a decade
later and will be valid until our practices are congruent with the scientific
knowledge base that we currently have at our disposal.
SUMMARY
The focus of this chapter was to present current thinking and research in the
areas of reading and written language disorders as they present in a young adult
population. Our goal also was to incorporate psychoeducational and
neuropsychological factors as they might impact the assessment process as well
as co-morbidity status. Clearly, however, controversy remains alive with
respect to diagnostic and assessment procedures, the documentation of disability
status, and justification for reasonable accommodations for young adults as well
as for children. Thus, a knowledge base by the clinician that is well grounded in
research, theory, and practice remains critical.
2
MATHEMATICS DISORDER
INTRODUCTION
43
44 CHAPTER 2
DEFINITIONS
(1) Adults with mathematical difficulties may have them on the basis of
deficiencies in cognitive development; adult acquired neurological disorders, or
a combination of the two. Even in young adults, the possibilities of head
trauma, substance abuse with implications for the central nervous system, and
neurological diseases that rarely but sometimes occur in young adults (e.g., brain
tumors; stroke) need to be taken into consideration. Impairment of calculation
ability is a very common symptom of a variety of brain disorders, and in that
way calculation is different from reading, which may remain relatively intact
despite the presence of substantial brain damage.
(2) The neurocognitive deficits that produced the mathematics difficulty during
childhood may have disappeared after later development because of
compensatory efforts, but the mathematics difficulty may remain. Thus, we may
have an individual with academic difficulty but without the typically associated
neuropsychological deficits, something that rarely appears in children. The
processes involved in learning a skill are not necessarily the same as those that
maintain it.
(3) Adults often go through a frustrating life long history of failure to achieve
goals because of difficulty with mathematics. They therefore may build up
substantial anxiety when doing mathematics, and even if they have acquired the
capability of doing well they may fail to do so because of the anxiety. Anxiety
may be a more significant consideration for mathematics than is the case for
reading and other academic disabilities.
(4) Adults may have had the advantages of many of the educational strategies
available for teaching mathematics to individuals with disability, and may have
achieved optimal outcome from this instruction. They therefore may not be
motivated, often appropriately so, to pursue further educational efforts.
Accommodation and counseling then become more helpful than instruction.
(5) In children, difficulties with mathematics are often associated with
difficulties in reading, producing a global subtype of learning disability. This
combination is less frequently found in adults, and particularly in adult college
students. That is likely to be the case because individuals with global learning
disability may frequently not meet entrance requirements for college. Therefore
the adult with an isolated mathematics disorder may less frequently have other
academic skill disorders.
In summary, unlike what may be more prototypical for children, mathematics
difficulty in adults is more likely to be an isolated deficit unrelated to other
academic skills and presently existent neuropsychological deficits, more likely
to be acquired as a result of structural brain damage or disease than is the case
for children, and highly exacerbated by anxiety. Remediation utilizing career
planning and accommodation is more likely to be pertinent than formal
education. While some adults with mathematics difficulties may meet specific
DSM-IV criteria for Mathematics disorder, in association with a specific pattern
of neuropsychological deficits, substantial numbers of others may not.
48 CHAPTER 2
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT
There are two major studies that deal directly with the matter of
neuropsychological aspects of a specific mathematical disorder in adults, one by
Morris and Walter (1991) based upon a sample of 104 college students and the
other, by McCue, Goldstein, Shelly, and Katz (1986), based upon a more diverse
sample of 100 mainly unemployed individuals seeking rehabilitation services.
The studies were remarkably similar with regard to tests used and method of
subtyping. Both used the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT), the Revised
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-R) (Wechsler, 1981), and portions of
the Halstead-Reitan Battery (HRNB) (Reitan and Wolfson, 1993). The system
of subtyping was originated by Rourke (1982) and was based upon WRAT
Results. One group is defined as being substantially better in Reading than
Arithmetic based on grade level or standard score discrepancies, the second does
better at Arithmetic than at Reading, and the third has a global disability,
without a significant discrepancy between reading and arithmetic. Our major
interest here would be in the first group.
The cognitive profile in the Morris and Walter study is characterized by the
absence of a discrepancy between average level Verbal and Performance IQ
scores, an uneven WAIS-R profile with relatively high scores on Digit Span and
Vocabulary, and low scores on Block Design and Object Assembly, below
MATHEMATICS DISORDER 49
average scores on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, and mildly reduced
tapping speeds. In the study by McCue et al. neuropsychological data were
obtained for the Rourke group characterized by poor arithmetic relative to
reading, based upon WRAT scores. Their sample had a slightly lower
Performance than Verbal IQ score, with general intellectual function falling into
the low average range. The WAIS subtest profile had a configuration
characterized by relatively high scores on Comprehension and Similarities and
low scores on Block Design and Digit Symbol. We would note that the
Arithmetic subtest score was 8.37 for Morris and Walter, and 6.75 for McCue et
al. both reflecting low performance relative to other verbal abilities.
On neuropsychological tests, while the specific tests used differed, both
studies found evidence of impaired spatial ability, in the case of Morris and
Walter using the Rey-Osterrieth Figure, and in the case of McCue et al. using
the Total Time and Location components of the Halstead Tactual Performance
Test (Reitan and Wolfson, 1993). McCue et al. also compared the reading better
than arithmetic with the other Rourke subtypes, finding that the reading better
than arithmetic subtype was significantly better on WAIS Information and
Vocabulary subtests and the Reitan Aphasia Screening Test, but significantly
worse on the Total Time component of the Tactual Performance Test. Both
studies described groups with low average to average general intelligence, good
verbal abilities, and impaired visual-spatial and spatial-constructional abilities.
This latter area is perhaps the most robust finding concerning individuals with
mathematics disorder. Over a very large number of studies, these individuals
often have been shown to have significant impairment of nonverbal skills
measured with drawings, constructional tasks, and complex visual perception
tasks such as identifying embedded figures. The McCue et al. study showed
that the neuropsychological profile of individuals with specific mathematics
difficulty had a different pattern of strengths and weaknesses from what was the
case for subtypes with specific reading or global disability. The combined
findings of Morris and Walter (1991) and McCue et al. (1991) confirm the
existence of a pattern of intact verbal skills and relatively deficient spatial
abilities in adults with specific mathematics difficulties. It is the same pattern as
seen in children, but generally not in as extreme a form.
Aside from the Wechsler scales and the HRNB, research in adult learning
disability (LD) has been accomplished with the Luria-Nebraska Battery (LNNB)
(Golden, Purisch, and Hammeke, 1985). The LNNB is a standard,
comprehensive neuropsychological test battery that contains individual scales
for the major neuropsychological functions including Motor Function, Attention,
Vision, Tactile Function, Receptive Language, Expressive Language, Memory,
and Intelligence. Of particular interest for LD is that there are separate scales
for Reading, Writing, and Arithmetic. It has been shown that these latter scales
constitute a "mini-WRAT" since they are highly correlated with the comparable
subtests of the WRAT (Shelly and Goldstein, 1982). Thus, the LNNB has the
50 CHAPTER 2
In like manner, Nolting (1988) has suggested that students who have learning
disabilities often experience more difficulty learning mathematics than other
subjects. He postulates that factors which may be relevant to diagnostics and
interventions are those of linear learning and memory. Linear learning occurs
when material learned one day is used the next day and the next and so forth.
With nonlinear subject matter (social sciences, for example), material learned
one day can be forgotten after the test and will not impact the learning of new
material. With mathematics, if the first concept taught during a class period is
misunderstood or not taught well, problems in understanding the remainder of
the class material will be paramount. He suggests that the basis for the learning
difficulty lies in problems either with short or long-term memory. In addition,
with those students who also have a reading disorder, securing the missed
information from textbooks will not be an easy task. Nolting (1997) has
published an excellent manual appropriate for students at the college level in
MATHEMATICS DISORDER 53
which he presents practical strategies for dealing with math anxiety, test-taking
skills, studying for examinations, use of a calculator in class, with special
references for students with LD, TBI, and ADHD.
In summary, description of the adult with mathematics difficulties is an
exceptionally complex area for a number of reasons. In adults more than
children, mathematics difficulties may be associated with acquired neurological
disorders in both a specific and more general way. That is, there are cases of
specific acalculia typically associated with left parietal lobe focal brain damage,
but impairment of mathematical abilities is very commonly a result of
generalized brain damage. Some, but clearly not all, adults have specific
Mathematics Disorder in association with the characteristic pattern of
neuropsychological deficits described in children. Most prominently, these
individuals show deficits in visual-spatial and motor abilities. Other adults have
the mathematics disability without the neuropsychological deficits. Some have
their mathematics disability in association with attentional difficulties, if not
frank ADHD. What often is a lifelong history of frustration and distress
centering on the area of mathematics may produce severe "math anxiety" in
adults. Various combinations of all of these considerations may exist in
individual cases.
INTERVENTIONS
General Considerations
For practical purposes, there appear to be two large subgroups of adults that seek
assistance. One of them consists of college students who are having difficulties
with their mathematics courses, and the other group consists of nonstudents who
are seeking employment or who are having on the job difficulties involving
mathematics. For example, individuals who are on a new job or who have been
reassigned and find themselves in work situations that require more
mathematical skill than was the case previously. Through contrasting the studies
of McCue et al. (1991) and Morris and Walters (1991), and, in a more direct
way, through a study of Beers (Beers, Goldstein, and Kate, 2000), it seems to be
the case that there are relatively high functioning and low functioning subgroups
of adults with learning disability. Beers has shown that their cognitive profiles
are similar, but that the high functioning group has an overall higher level of
cognitive ability than the low functioning group. Morris and Walter (1991) as
well as Beers et al., both of whom studied the high functioning group, remarked
on the extremely small number of high functioning individuals who were in the
Rourke reading worse than arithmetic and global disability subtypes. In our
own work (Beers, Goldstein and Katz, 2000) we found college students in all
MATHEMATICS DISORDER 55
three subtypes, but showed that there were substantial differences in the
proportion of Rourke subtypes between college student and vocational
rehabilitation client groups. The vocational rehabilitation clients were
predominantly in the global subtype, while the college students were
predominantly in the poor arithmetic subtype. Thus, college students who seek
remediation very frequently tend to have specific difficulties in mathematics,
with normal or better reading and spelling. On the other hand, McGue et al.
found adult subjects in all three subtypes.
These considerations would appear to have major implications for direct
remediation. For example, in the relatively low functioning group, remediation
could be based upon the underlying cognitive deficit area such as spatial
abilities, with the strategy being that improvement in these abilities generalize to
mathematics. However, high functioning individuals may not exhibit these
spatial deficits but still have mathematics problems, suggesting that a different
approach would be more productive. These individuals may benefit from a
more expanded assessment in which it is determined whether the mathematics
problem is attributable to anxiety, to some general difficulty such as poor
capacity to attend or bad study habits, or to some specific deficit in
conceptualization. Such an assessment should establish the best alternative or
combination of alternatives for treating the anxiety, providing more generic
remediation regarding attending, organizing material, and maintaining an
adequate study schedule, or providing highly specific training using an
educational technology that addresses the specific problem area. For example,
with respect to the latter deficit, some modification of the use of manipulatives
used with young children who have difficulties with forming abstract
representations of problems (Marsh and Cooke, 1996). Treatment planning of
this type is greatly benefited from an analysis of errors, which should provide
important information as to the spatial, mechanical arithmetic, or conceptual
source of the disability. For example, Wong (1996) has distinguished
calculation errors due to partial completion of the given problem, errors due to
incorrect placement and regrouping, errors due to incorrect procedures in
computation, and errors due to failure in mastering the concept of zero.
It is possible, but not thoroughly demonstrated, that the more low functioning
adults may benefit from neuropsychological rehabilitation. The question is,
Will training programs available for such abilities as memory, various language
skills, and spatial abilities help these individuals with their mathematics? There
is some support for the view that use of memory aids or training is useful in
mathematics instruction of individuals with mental handicaps (Judd and Bilsky,
1989). Often these individuals cannot read well and so mathematics problems
are presented orally, but such presentation requires memory, which may be
deficient. In a similar way, reading ability may provide a source of difficulty. If
normal reading is defined on the basis of the WRAT Reading score that could be
misleading because the WRAT only assesses mechanical reading (word
56 CHAPTER 2
continue well into the post-secondary setting. The work of Marolda and
Davidson is useful for the clinician attempting to delineate the nature of the
disorder as it manifests in a given individual and what interventions or
accommodations might then be appropriate.
According to Marolda and Davidson (1994), students tend to process
mathematical situations following two distinct patterns. One is linear; the other
is global or gestalt. Both patterns have been incorporated in learning profiles by
these researchers: Mathematics Learning Style I and Mathematics Learning
Style II. Both styles must be available for successful acquisition of
mathematical operations and concepts. However, often students with a
diagnosed mathematics disability are limited to one or the other learning style
and thus are unable to utilize strategies available to the other style. These styles,
their cognitive and behavioral correlates, and the implications for
teaching/intervention strategies are reprinted from an article by Marolda and
Davidson as described below. As will be seen, they are transferable to work with
college students who require remediation or developmental course work in the
area of mathematics.
B. Mathematical Behaviors
1. Approaches situations using recipes; “talks
through” tasks
Interprets geometric designs verbally
2. Approaches mathematics in a mechanical,
routine based fashion
Overwhelmed in situations in which there
are multiple considerations, such as in
multi-step tasks
Can generate correct solutions, but may
not recognize when solutions are
inappropriate
58 CHAPTER 2
B. Mathematical Behaviors
1. Benefits from manipulatives
Loves geometric topics
2. Prefers concepts to algorithms
Tolerates ambiguity and imprecision
Offers impulsive guesses as solutions
Uses estimation strategies spontaneously
Skims word problems first but must be encouraged
to re-read for salient details
Perceives overall shape of geometric configurations
at the expense of an appreciation of the
individual components
3. Requires a definition of overview before dealing with
exacting procedures
60 CHAPTER 2
Reprinted , not in its entirety, with permission from The International Dyslexia
Association quarterly newsletter, Perspectives, Summer 2000, Vol. 26, No. 3,
pp. 13-14, M. R. Marolda and P. S. Davidson.
In addition, Morris and Walter (1991) suspect that math anxiety may be a
major source of academic dysfunction in college students. The disruptive
effects of anxiety on cognitive function are well known, and can be quite
disabling. One can treat math anxiety in a number of ways including
psychotherapy, cognitive behavioral interventions (e.g., desensitization, anxiety
management), and pharmacological treatment. Perhaps a desensitization
procedure would be particularly effective, accomplished through specifically
reducing anxiety responses to situations related to mathematics. There does not
seem to be any specific treatment for math anxiety except to possibly consider it
as a form of phobia, perhaps a form of school phobia, that may be treated in a
targeted manner, such as with the use of desensitization, cognitive therapy, or
relaxation. The use of anti-anxiety medication might be considered in
appropriate cases.
ASSISTIVE DEVICES
settings. The basic point is that the individual with a mathematics difficulty may
not be able to make full utilization of a hand calculator to the extent that an
individual without a disability can, without special instructions. With regard to
design, it may be necessary to use specially modified hand calculators
engineered to accommodate the specifics of the deficit. For example, with new
developments in voice technology, it may be possible to utilize auditory as
opposed to visual calculators for people who do better with hearing than with
seeing information. For example, some individuals with mathematics
difficulties may do better by presenting problems through speaking into a device
and receiving spoken answers. Speech may also be employed by individuals
whose physical disabilities prevent use of a keyboard.
Vocational Choice
A CASE STUDY
Mathematics is in one respect a language that uses numbers and symbols rather
than words, a system of logical operations, such as dividing and subtracting, and
an analytic method that serves in problem solving. It has been described as a
“handmaiden to the sciences” since it supports such major scientific areas as
MATHEMATICS DISORDER 65
chemistry, physics, and some of the life and social sciences. Some individuals
have major difficulties in studying science because they can’t handle the
mathematics. They may have excellent language skills, reasoning ability, ability
to learn in areas other than mathematics, and excellent memory.
The case presented here is that of an individual who wanted to be a scientist
and possessed many of these positive cognitive characteristics. However, from
early in school, he did poorly at mathematics, with recollection of failure going
back to the first grade. Other schoolwork was performed at an average or better
level, but poor mathematical abilities compromised achievement in such areas as
physics and chemistry when he was in high school. It was noted early in school
that he had not developed a clear hand preference. He was primarily left
handed, but would write with either his left or right hand. Over time, he
remained predominantly left-handed, but increasingly wrote consistently with
his right hand. He was right eyed, making him crossed eye-hand dominant, and
possibly efforts to convert him to right-handedness were owing to the belief that
existed at the time of his education that not to do so would impede his ability to
learn to read. Ultimately, his reading turned out to be normal.
The quality of his mathematics education was high, and he could recall
having excellent, interesting teachers throughout school. He experienced
understanding everything during the lectures, but did exceptionally poorly in
solving problems at home and on tests. He minimally passed courses up to
elementary algebra, but failed intermediate algebra. Exponents, complex
equations, and related matters were not at all well-learned. Repetition of
intermediate algebra led to a minimal pass. Since his other grades were good, he
was admitted to an academically prestigious college, and since he had passed
intermediate algebra in high school, he was immediately placed into a required
two-semester calculus course. The first semester was failed, repeated and
minimally passed, and the second semester was minimally passed. However, he
had little real understanding of calculus. He was required to take a course in
statistics, and although the mathematics involved were less demanding than
calculus, he achieved a minimal pass.
Other grades still being good, he was admitted to graduate school where he
was required to take advanced statistics. Surprisingly, he got straight A’s in
both statistics courses, marking the beginning of a series of unexpected
developments. During graduate school and following completion of a Ph.D., he
became heavily involved in research. Most of the research involved quantitative
analysis of data, with a heavy emphasis on advanced multivariate statistics.
These methods involve use of matrix algebra, simultaneous equations, and
related advanced mathematical methods. However, when he read journal
articles, he had extensive difficulty in following mathematical arguments based
on sets of formulas and equations. Fundamental mathematical ability did not
appear to improve at all. Nevertheless, he authored a book in research design
66 CHAPTER 2
SUMMARY
Consistent with the other disorders and disabilities discussed in this volume,
mathematics disability exists in adulthood. We use the term “exists” rather than
“persists” because there is a greater prevalence of acquired mathematics
disability in adulthood than is the case for reading or writing. We also use the
term “disability” rather than “disorder” because many adults with mathematics
difficulties would not meet DSM-IV criteria for Mathematics Disorder or any of
the academic skill disorders. In brighter adults, notably college students, the
distribution of academic skill subtypes is different from what is the case for
children and lower to average ability adults. That is, in college students one
seldom sees individuals with global disability or the reading worse than
arithmetic subtype, but the reading better than arithmetic subtype is quite
common. Unlike children, there appears to be a high prevalence of adults with
substantial mathematics disability who either do not have the typically
associated spatial and motor neuropsychological deficits or who do not meet
DSM-IV criteria for Mathematics Disorder or other academic skill disorder.
Many adults who sustain diffuse brain damage or focal damage particularly to
the left posterior hemisphere may acquire mathematics disability. An unknown
but apparently substantial number of individuals have mathematics anxiety
either as the source of the disability or a reaction to it. Interventions may cover
a broad range including formal specific remediation, general remediation of
study habits and organizing skills, cognitive rehabilitation of attention, memory,
and problem solving ability, use of accommodation and assistive devices, anti-
anxiety treatment, and vocational counseling particularly with regard to
occupational choice.
3
NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY
INTRODUCTION
Myklebust (Johnson and Myklebust, 1971; Myklebust, 1975) coined the term
nonverbal learning disability to describe children with disturbed social relation-
ships, poor social skills, difficulty in interpreting the meanings of actions of oth-
ers, arithmetic deficits, and functional difficulties such as distinguishing left from
right, telling time, reading maps, and following directions. This chapter begins
with a description of nonverbal learning disability that considers diagnostic con-
siderations, epidemiological aspects, and proposed biological mechanisms for
the disorder. The neuropsychological aspects of nonverbal learning disability
are discussed, framing the subsequent consideration of psychosocial and voca-
tional function. Next, various treatments are reviewed, again within the frame-
work of the neuropsychological strengths and weaknesses associated with this
disorder. This chapter concludes with suggestions for future work.
69
70 CHAPTER 3
ger, 1979; Gillberg, 1985; Shea and Mesibov, 1985; Wing, 1985) and, albeit of
questionable validity, Developmental Gerstmann Syndrome (Kinsborne, 1968;
PeBenito, 1987; PeBenito, Fisch, and Fisch, 1988). Another classification
schema, and one of the few that has extended research into adulthood, is based
on a disorder of central processing that is descriptively labeled as a nonverbal
perceptual-organization-output disorder (Rourke and Fisk, 1981). While the
reader is referred to the Semrud-Clikeman and Hynd review and the other refer-
ences listed for a comprehensive discussion of classification methods, research
within each category has identified several unifying constructs. For example,
almost every study has identified delays in arithmetic, poor motor development,
and visual-spatial problems. In contrast, no matter how classified most subjects
showed relatively strong vocabulary skills but disorders in higher order language
skills (e.g., pragmatics). Subjects within the various classifications also showed
remarkable and striking similar difficulties in social understanding, interpretation
of gesture, and discrimination of the nuances of speech. As Semrud-Clikeman
and Hynd caution, “All of these classification schemes should be viewed as ex-
ploratory.... replication and refinement is needed...” (p. 205). Of the investiga-
tions discussed, the work of Rourke and his colleagues address these concerns in
both children and adults and will be discussed in some detail here.
During the 1980’s, the seminal work of Rourke and his colleagues provided
major support for the characterization of what has come to be termed NLD syn-
drome. This syndrome describes a constellation of deficits that co-occur at a rate
greater than chance and disrupt abilities generally felt to be subsumed by the
right hemisphere of the brain (Petrauskas and Rourke, 1979; Rourke, 1985).
Interestingly, these researchers were among the first to add comprehensive neu-
ropsychological assessment to the more standard psychoeducational evaluation
(i.e., intellectual ability and academic achievement tests) in order to describe the
cognitive deficits and strengths of children with learning disability. (For a com-
plete review of this literature, see Beers, 1998). Based on these early studies and
further work done across developmental levels, Rourke (1989a) developed a
dynamic picture of NLD. In Rourke’s model, children with NLD syndrome per-
formed better on neuropsychological measures associated with the left hemi-
sphere while the converse was true in children with reading disorders. In con-
trast children with NLD had more difficulty on tasks measuring tactile and visual
perception and attention, performed poorly on tests requiring complex psycho-
motor skills, and demonstrated problems in abstract concept formation and prob-
lem solving.
72 CHAPTER 3
As with other subtypes of learning disability, evidence suggests that NLD per-
sists into adulthood (Denckla, 1993; Rourke, Young, Strang, and Russell,
1986b); and, in fact, NLD may not be diagnosed until adulthood. Later in this
chapter we will review case material illustrating such an example. Two prob-
lems relating to the diagnosis of NLD are considered here. The first is that of the
lack of diagnostic specificity in the general classification method employed by
mental health professionals and sometimes educators. The second is that of dif-
ferential diagnosis.
Although the work of Rourke et al. has taken major steps in defining the
disorder, there is little consensus as to the definitive, operationalized definition
of NLD. As discussed in Chapter I, the specific characteristics of the descriptive
term “learning disability” usually reflect the perspective of the professionals
involved, be they educators, neuropsychologists, psychiatrists, behavioral neu-
rologists, or rehabilitation specialists. Although the literature is beginning to
report more concise and concrete definitions of various reading disorders, NLD
lacks what Stanovich (1999) terms a “domain specific” diagnosis that enables
researchers to address the issues of identification, diagnosis, intervention, and
prognosis. This may be because the problems with arithmetic so often noted in
children with NLD are frequently attributed to motivational or cultural factors
and thus tend to be overlooked rather than conceptualized as a single feature of
a more over riding cognitive disorder (Fletcher, 1989).
Within the mental health system, assigning a formal medical diagnosis to a
person having a “learning disability” is rendered even more problematic by the
inability of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edi-
tion (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) to adequately charac-
terize NLD. Indeed, when an actual diagnosis is required, that of Learning Dis-
ability Not Otherwise Specified (LD-NOS) with the addition of Cognitive Dis-
order NOS is probably the most appropriate. Certainly these catchall categories
fail to elaborate the principle identifying cognitive patterns and symptoms, dis-
cussed later in this chapter, that represent the defining characteristics of NLD.
Finally, because the diagnosis of LD-NOS also fails to reflect the emotional and
social ramifications so frequently associated with NLD, any appropriate Axis I
diagnosis (e.g., Major Depressive Disorder; Adjustment Disorder) should also
be assigned if the individual meets those particular criteria.
Differential Diagnosis
der. In contrast, Rourke (Rourke and Fisk, 1992) argues that the social, emo-
tional, and academic manifestations of NLD are a result of the interaction be-
tween neuropsychological deficits and preserved areas of cognitive function.
While Voeller discusses various DSM-based diagnoses that more or less suc-
cessfully capture the defining characteristics of SELD, the pervasiveness of so-
cial deficits across disorders, however, does not support the argument that
SELD is a primary disability. For example, in Gresham and Elliott’s view, spe-
cific social skill deficits are not unique to persons with learning disabilities
(Gresham and Elliott, 1989). Reiff, Gerber, and Ginsberg (1997) remind us that
the NJCLD definition for learning disabilities specifically states, “Problems in
self-regulatory behaviors, social perception and social interaction may exist...but
do not by themselves constitute a learning disability.” (p. 59) (Emphasis added.).
Thus, while the diagnosis of SELD might facilitate mental health services by
acknowledging the severity of social deficits, this label may actually restrict
other appropriate interventions. In order for these individuals to obtain appro-
priate treatment within the school system (for younger persons) or at vocational
rehabilitation agencies, psychoeducational and/or neuropsychological testing
must confirm deficits in cognitive abilities.
Later in this volume we will discuss the relationship between high func-
tioning autism and severe verbal learning disability. As noted by Rourke
(1989a) and others (Semrud-Clikeman and Hynd, 1990; Gillberg, 1983; Wein-
traub and Mesulam, 1983), another autistic spectrum disorder, Asperger Syn-
drome bears a rather striking similarity to NLD and may represent a more severe
expression of the same syndrome. In fact, Asperger Syndrome is distinguished
from high functioning autism by some of the defining characteristics of NLD.
That is, the normal development of language coupled with marked visual-spatial
deficits and motor clumsiness in the context of generally normal intellectual
function. While everyday language function is intact, as with NLD pragmatic
interactive language skills are usually deficient. In addition, individuals with
Asperger Syndrome display social impairment and as adults are frequently
overwhelmed by the complex demands of adult life.
A study by Klin, Volkmar, Sparrow, Cicchetti, and Rourke (1995) investi-
gated the validity of Asperger Syndrome in adolescents, comparing neuropsy-
chological performance to a group with high functioning autism. Noting the
similarities in presentation between Asperger Syndrome and NLD, the authors
hypothesized that the pattern of neuropsychological test scores in the AS group
would be similarly to that associated with NLD. Subjects who met rigorous di-
agnostic criteria for either high functioning autism or AS were selected from
consecutive admissions at a treatment center. Neuropsychological testing was
completed without knowledge of the psychiatric diagnosis. Using these results,
subjects were classified as meeting neuropsychological criteria for NLD by ex-
perienced clinical neuropsychologists after review of test results and behavioral
NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY 75
The processing deficits associated with NLD suggest that dysfunction in the
right, or nondominant, hemisphere of the brain may account for this disorder.
However, as with definition, the etiology of NLD remains controversial. Some
like Hiscock and Hiscock (1991) assert that NLD is a variant of “normal” learn-
ing while others speculate regarding genetic and/or encephalopathic origins of
the disorder (Denckla, 1983; Pennington, 1991; Rourke, 1988; Semrud-
Clikeman and Hynd, 1990; Voeller, 1997). These encephalopathic or environ-
mental etiologies of NLD are considered variable and include pre- and perinatal
complications, acquired insults such as early head trauma, untreated hydrocepha-
lus, cranial radiation, and agenesis of the corpus callosum. Rourke (1988; Tsat-
sanis and Rourke, 1995) has developed a “white matter model” to describe the
brain mechanisms responsible for NLD. This model is based on the assumption
of the divided responsibilities between the two brain hemispheres, with the right
hemisphere being more involved in the intermodal integration and sharing proc-
essed information with the left hemisphere. In contrast, the functions of the left
hemisphere are more intramodal and discrete. In more everyday terms, the right
side of the brain appears to be specialized to synthesize diverse information,
process novel stimuli, and develop novel concepts. As Rourke notes, all these
activities depend on the greater density of white matter or the myelinated fibers
that are responsible for communication within the brain. Thus, the mechanism of
NLD may be the changes in particular brain circuits that are disproportionately
represented in the right hemisphere and are in this altered state “...less evolved
and less specialized” (Tsatsanis and Rourke, 1995, p. 481). As noted above,
these changes in white matter occur for various reasons such as abnormal devel-
opment, degenerative processes, or injury. This, in fact, may account for the
NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY 77
tion of skills rather than a reflection of subtle neurological deficits. They charge
that the disease model justifies gathering extraneous information regarding per-
ception, attention, memory, motor skills, language, and reasoning that from an
educational perspective has little value. In fact, these authors feel that the appli-
cation of neuropsychological findings may actually distort the nature of the
learning disorder and contribute to unrealistic expectations regarding treatment
and outcome. As an alternative, Hiscock and Hiscock propose that investiga-
tions of LD should focus on the learning problem per se, with an emphasis on
academic remediation (For further discussion of the interface between special
education, neuropsychology, and neurobiology, also see Kershner, 1991 and
Duane, 1991). While we agree that early neuropsychological investigations fo-
cused almost exclusively on localization and lateralization (Morrow and Beers,
1995), the field has evolved from this narrow perspective. Based on both our
clinical experiences, the validation of neuropsychological methodology with
respect to LD (Selz and Reitan, 1979a, 1979b; O’Donnell, 1991), and recent
advances in neuroimaging, we feel that a comprehensive neuropsychological
assessment provides essential information for the accurate development of indi-
vidualized academic and psychological interventions. A neuropsychological
evaluation, based on the biopsychosocial model, actually seeks explanations
“other than” neurological impairment. When these other explanations are ruled
out, the evaluation synthesizes information regarding the integrity of the central
nervous system across the cognitive domains in order to develop effective re-
mediation and compensatory strategies. The next section of this chapter illus-
trates neuropsychology’s role in defining and treating NLD in particular.
Evidence from the few extant longitudinal studies suggests that there is a
developmental aspect to these deficits; that is, the cognitive and socioemotional
deficits become more debilitating as the child grows older (Casey, Rourke, and
Picard, 1991). This fact is particularly intriguing with respect to NLD as it
manifests in adults. However, a literature search conducted for the years 1997
though mid-2000 identified over 300 empirically based studies of adults with LD
but only three of these investigations differentiated between NLD and other sub-
types. Comprehensive adult studies of NLD are long overdue.
and those of others that investigated the construct and discriminate validity of the
Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery (HRNB) in young adults with
LD. Based on a sample of 233 young adults applying for college admission, his
work demonstrated that HRNB factor scores were correlated with academic
skills (i.e., reading, spelling, and arithmetic computation) and thus demonstrated
the test’s construct validity. In addition, the HRNB successfully differentiated
normal young adults from those with LD or head injury, supporting the discrimi-
nate validity of the instrument. In fact, results indicated that approximately 42%
of the young adults with LD experienced mild cerebral dysfunction, suggesting
that the neuropsychological evaluation serves as an appropriate tool for planning
interventions and accommodations in young adults. Finally, cluster analysis
techniques were applied to neuropsychological data, resulting in homogeneous
and valid LD subtypes. That is, the LD subtype with computational deficits in
mathematics was associated with visual-spatial and abstract reasoning deficits.
Other, smaller studies, have also explored the validity of LD subtypes within
the college population. For example, Morris and Walter (1991) applied Ro-
urke’s classification methods to study college students participating in a remedial
mathematics class. Results indicated that 21% of these students met criteria for
the arithmetic deficit subtype (21%), with the remainder exhibiting no learning
disability at all. In our own work with a group of college students with previ-
ously unclassified LD, a group with mild head injury, and controls, we regrouped
all students using a modification of the Rourke procedures (Rourke and Fin-
layson, 1978) into either a no LD or LD subtype (Beers, 1993; Beers et al.,
1994). The majority of students who met LD criteria (46%) were found to have
a global learning disability as defined by Rourke (Rourke and Finlayson, 1978).
That is, they showed similar deficiency in the three academic areas of reading,
mathematics, and written expression. Almost as many (43%) met criteria for an
arithmetic disability and exhibited the pattern of neuropsychological deficits
consistent with the more broadly defined NLD. A disability specific to reading
and writing was not as frequently identified, occurring at the rate of approxi-
mately 10%.
Although the pervasive and persistent aspects of LD were pointed out in a
position paper by the National Joint Committee of Learning Disabilities as early
as 1985 (Smith, 1996), by the year 2000 very few studies had focused on homo-
geneous groups of adults with LD outside the academic setting. In 1986, Sprean
and Haaf investigated the stability of three LD subtypes in an adult cohort (Mean
age = 24 years) that they had evaluated during childhood (Mean age = 10 years).
In both phases of this research the subjects were grouped using cluster analytic
techniques. While there was not complete concordance with cluster classifica-
tion at the two study points, visual-perceptual and graphomotor subtypes per-
sisted into adulthood. Important to the present topic is the finding that subjects
NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY 81
An Assessment Model
chological evaluation should also provide information relevant to the areas out-
lined in Table 1. As McCue (1993) notes, “Central to the task of providing
services to persons with learning disabilities is obtaining a clear understanding
of how the disability impairs the ability to function....in employment, in higher
education, and in independent living” (p. 56). Contrary to the conclusions of
Hiscock and Hiscock (1991), we assert that a comprehensive neuropsychologi-
cal evaluation can delineate the impairment or underlying mechanisms of the
disability, suggest appropriate remedial and or compensatory educational strate-
gies, and provide the foundation for career counseling or vocational rehabilita-
tion services (Beers, 1998). Various components of LD assessment including
the clinical interview, functional assessment, psychoeducational assessment, and
neuropsychological testing are demonstrated in the following case presentation.
Essentially, a carefully considered neuropsychological assessment begins the
actual intervention process and guides the further efforts of remediation, com-
pensation, accommodation, vocational rehabilitation, and individual psychother-
apy as well.
Case Study: “How do I get to the bank from the grocery store?”
mally diagnosed with LD, RS states that her parents told her she was “stupid.”
RS indicated that she has always read fluently, but with poor comprehension.
Math was consistently her most difficult subject. She described long-standing
difficulty working puzzles, poor coordination, difficulty learning new proce-
dures, and problems finding her way around shopping malls and the city in gen-
eral. When describing her navigation problems, RS indicated that she had to
return home between each errand, otherwise she lost her way. RS also described
feeling extremely uneasy when the furniture is rearranged in her home. She re-
ported the recurring dream of being blind and in a state of complete hopelessness
and panic. In spite of these problems, RS recounted her assets as successfully
completing a nursing program. She has been steadily employed and reported
that she is successful in her profession. However, RS noted that she has particu-
lar difficulty with unpredictable situations or when over learned sequences
change. At these times she indicated that she experiences heightened anxiety
that in turn further compromises her effectiveness. RS recounted her problems
working with unfamiliar medical equipment, stating that she has much difficulty
learning how to set up a piece of equipment by watching a demonstration.
The patient’s medical history is essentially unremarkable. She had a breast
biopsy (benign) approximately 10 years earlier and a tonsillectomy and adnoi-
dectomy in the remote past. She had hepatitis as a child. RS did not endorse the
current use of alcohol but felt that she has used alcohol as a means to alleviate
her anxiety in the past. She described attention and concentration problems that
have worsened over the past 5 years or so, reporting a somewhat cyclical pattern.
RS was treated by a psychologist for depressive symptoms approximately 5
years ago. According to the patient, she was diagnosed with PTSD secondary to
her ridicule by her parents and with a “visual agnosia.” RM is prescribed Well-
butrin, 75mg three times a day by her family physician. The patient described
learning problems in her mother and her siblings that are similar to her own. Her
father was described as dysthymic.
While tearful at times during testing, RS appeared motivated to do her best.
Test results, shown in Table 2, appear to present a valid measure of the patient’s
current level of cognitive function.
NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY 85
86 CHAPTER 3
Level of Performance
Language
RS had difficulty repeating digits, with a marked discrepancy noted between the
scores for the forward and backward conditions (10 vs. 4, respectively). Again,
a good performance was noted for an overlearned activity, but with relative
deficit indicated when more complex processing was required. Other tests of
attention, including the Rhythm Test and the Stroop Color/word Test, demon-
strated essentially the same phenomenon; normal performance on basic tasks but
mild impairment on more complex procedures. Memory function reflected a
similar pattern. RS preformed normally on immediate recall of stories, but there
was a substantial loss of information on delayed recall. On the California Verbal
Learning Test she did not appear to improve significantly over learning trials for
the first word list, and she demonstrated substantial susceptibility to interfer-
ence. Her delayed recall of nonverbal material was also poor. In general RS
appears to have a significant impairment of complex memory functions with
normal basic associative processes.
Basic sensory-perceptual tests were not accomplished for this patient, but there
was no apparent evidence of neglect or significant tactile, visual, or auditory
dysfunction. On motor tasks, RS had normal strength of grip, but mild bilateral
slowness of tapping speed. Dexterity was normal with her right hand, but mildly
impaired with her left.
These two domains showed a strong interaction and thus, will be discussed to-
gether. Based on her educational level, RS performed below expectation (i.e.,
11th percentile) on the Category Test. She performed even more poorly on
problem-solving tasks with perceptual-motor demands. As perceptual-motor
88 CHAPTER 3
PSYCHOSOCIAL CONSIDERATIONS
It is now appreciated that learning disabilities impact most, if not all, aspects of
everyday life. As Smith (1988) summarizes, “...adults with learning disability
are actually challenged and frustrated by their difficulties in achieving valued
cultural standards of literacy, job performance, social skills, and communication
skills” (p. 22). (Emphasis added). This statement is supported by the results of
a needs assessment of 381 adults with LD (mean age = 23.2 years) (Hoffmann et
al., 1987). Accomplished within a vocational rehabilitation setting, the survey
included the areas of daily living, social skills, and personal problems and was
completed by clients with LD, service providers, and advocates and/or parents.
Not surprisingly since 64% of the LD sample was either unemployed or working
only part time, all three groups concurred that adults with LD have major voca-
tional needs. Interestingly, both the service providers and advocates perceived
the LD sample as having significantly more social problems than did the adults
with LD themselves. All three groups agreed that personal problems were preva-
lent, with all indicating that low frustration tolerance, poor self-confidence, and
poor control of emotions and temper were particularly problematic. This survey
also demonstrated that few adults with LD had ever participated in any form of
therapy (i.e., 13%) and that none had ever participated in a social skills or self-
help group. In summary, these results not only strongly confirm the psychologi-
cal difficulties in adults with LD but also suggest that adults with LD either do
not avail themselves of such therapeutic services or do not know of their avail-
ability.
Although a comprehensive discussion of the psychosocial aspects of LD is
beyond the scope of this chapter, we refer the interested reader to a poignant
chapter that discusses in the most concrete of terms the social/emotional and
daily living issues faced by adults with LD (Reiff and Gerber, 1993). In spite of
a growing awareness of the pervasive implications of learning disabilities on
psychological function, it is our own clinical experience that adults with NLD
are frequently incorrectly diagnosed as having a primary psychiatric disorder.
There is often little or no appreciation of pre-existing learning deficits or the
continuing impact of these deficits on socio-emotional functioning.
Psychological Adjustment
study completed by Peter and Spreen (1979) provided not only evidence that
learning disability extends into adulthood but also that the significant behavior
problems and emotional maladjustment of this groups persists as well. This in-
vestigation followed 177 children with LD who initially completed neuropsy-
chological testing between the ages of 8 and 12. Subjects were followed for
varying periods, some for as long as 12 years. In this follow up study adoles-
cents and young adults were grouped according to CNS status (brain damage,
minimal brain damage, no neurological signs) and compared to a control group
with no history of learning problems or brain injury. After adjustment for differ-
ences in age, gender, and IQ, all three groups with learning problems demon-
strated significantly more behavior problems and psychological distress than the
normal control group. Although this early study strongly suggests a meaningful
relationship between learning deficits and later emotional problems, as noted
above only a few studies have investigated the course of learning disabilities
through adulthood and even fewer have explored the psychosocial functioning of
this group. One more recent study (Lapan, Koller, and Holliday, 1991) of clients
with LD referred for vocational rehabilitation services found that almost one-
third met DSM criteria for a psychiatric diagnosis, including adjustment disor-
der, mood disorders, and personality disorders. !
In spite of the growing recognition that LD affects psychological function-
ing in both work and family life, the psychosocial status of adults in academic
settings continues to be the most frequently studied area. Saracoglu, Minden,
and Wilchesky (1989) investigated the general adjustment to college of students
with LD. As might be expected, this investigation documented significantly
poorer academic and emotional adjustment in the LD sample in comparison to a
peer group with no LD. However, the positive correlation between measures of
self-efficacy and self-esteem in both groups suggests that students with LD as
well as those with other academic difficulties demonstrate similar emotional and
adjustment problems. While another study comparing LD and nondisabled adult
students showed only a trend toward between-group differences in the level of
reported depression, depressive symptoms were significantly correlated with
dysfunctional cognitions in the LD group (Mattek and Wierzbicki, 1998). Other
investigations have identified differences in the type of symptoms manifested by
adults with LD. Hoy and colleagues (Hoy, Gregg, Wisenbakeret al., 1997)
compared two groups of adults with LD not classified by particular subtype. In
this case, college students with LD showed more symptoms of anxiety than a
group of student controls. Adults with LD in a rehabilitation setting were the
only study group to demonstrate significant differences with respect to level of
depression. Although this study suggests that psychological function in adults
with LD may be closely related to the particular demands of the environment,
another study provides evidence to dispute this conclusion. A study of adults
with LD who had obtained at least a baccalaureate degree and maintained a
NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY 91
sion than the remaining study groups but the groups did not differ significantly
with respect to suicidality.
Our own group (Katz, Kelly, Goldstein, Bartolomucci, and Comstock,
1992) investigated incidence and prevalence various LD diagnoses, including
those defined by visual-spatial and perceptual/motor deficits, associated with
three distinct MMPI profiles. In this study, subjects were not necessarily defined
by a single diagnosis. A cluster analysis of the MMPI scores identified three
valid clusters of varying severity: 1) high levels of psychiatric distress, primarily
with features of neurotic depression and anxiety; 2) similar neurotic features but
with lower levels of psychological distress, and 3) a normal MMPI profile. The
various subtypes were not differentially represented in any of these MMPI clus-
ters. That is, visual-spatial disability did not predominate in one MMPI cluster
and reading disability in another. The number of LD diagnoses, however, was
significantly different among the subtypes, with Cluster 1 containing individuals
with the most diagnoses. This finding suggests a positive relationship between
number of cognitive deficits and level of psychological distress. These results
are highly consistent with Peter and Spreen’s (1979) early study that indicated
overall intellectual level moderated the degree of psychological distress in their
learning handicapped subjects.
Rourke’s hypothesis of increased psychological dysfunction associated with
NLD has recently come under scrutiny. White, Moffitt, and Silva (1992) studied
teens with various LD subtypes (i.e., specific reading disability, specific arithme-
tic disability, and global LD), comparing them to a group of nondisabled con-
trols. While they noted that the group with specific arithmetic disability had a
neuropsychological profile consistent with NLD, all three disabled groups
showed similarly poor social-emotional adjustment. O’Donnell (1991) reports
unpublished data by Leicht (1987) who performed a subtype analysis of person-
ality function of young adults with LD based on a brief personality inventory.
Some but not all adults with LD exhibited “maladjustment,” but this was not
related to either academic skills or neuropsychological status. While these re-
sults do not support the conclusion that some LD subtypes are more apt than
others to show particular patterns of emotional symptoms, they must be viewed
as tentative. The assessment of emotional status was apparently completed with
a self-report instrument and not supplemented with a comprehensive psychiatric
interview such as the SCID.
Two more recent studies, however, support the findings of Leicht. A study
by Waldo et al. (Waido, McIntosh, and Koller, 1999) compared the personality
profiles of 3 LD subtypes defined by Wechsler IQ differences. All participates
were in a vocational rehabilitation setting. Although no statistically significant
between group differences were identified between the LD subtypes, 20% of
their adult sample displayed significant clinical elevation on at least one MMPI
scale. As the authors point out, these finding argue for attention to the emotional
NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY 93
consequences across all aspects of LD. Likewise, a study that defined subtypes
by cluster analysis found no differences in the prevalence of psychiatric diagno-
sis by subtype (Dunham, Multon, and Koeller, 1999).
Vocational Function
pressed educational goals (e.g., 48% of the sample had expressed the goal of at
least 2 years of education past high school), the sample showed limited post-
secondary education. Only 21% completed more than 4 semesters of college.
Serious under employment was indicated by frequent job changes, limited earn-
ing capacity (e.g., 76% earned less than $6 per hour), and participation in un-
skilled jobs (52%). Thus, although these individual possessed definite assets,
their vocational functioning was highly limited, suggesting an inability to capital-
ize on strengths or perhaps, the tendency to overlook strengths in vocational
planning.
In spite of this increased interest in the vocational functioning of persons
with unclassified LD, information regarding the vocational functioning of indi-
viduals with NLD is sparse. Rourke et al. (Rourke, Del Dotto, Rourke, and Ca-
sey, 1990) provide anecdotal evidence of vocational difficulties in a young
woman, discussing her limitations in judgment and reasoning and their effect on
her vocational functioning. We located only one empirical study mat addressed
the occupational function of individuals grouped according to homogeneous
categories. Dunham et al. (1999) noted that the verbal vs. nonverbal LD distinc-
tion might be too simplistic to capture the cognitive, academic, and occupational
complexities of different jobs. They classified a group of 613 adults with LD,
using cluster analysis to group individuals according to their IQ (Verbal and
Performance IQs from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (Wech-
sler, 1981) and academic achievement (reading, mathematics, and writing sum-
mary scores from the Woodcock-Johnson Psychoeducational Test Battery
[Woodcock and Johnson, 1990]). They identified three meaningful and valid
clusters labeled as “Linguistically/globally academically impaired,” “Linguisti-
cally/writing impaired,” and “Eclectic.” The latter group did not display the
language problems of the other groups, but nevertheless showed the VIQ<PIQ
pattern and mild academic deficits in reading and writing. While none of these
groups seem to capture the essence of NLD, this study did identify important
differences in vocational outcome for the three verbal LD clusters, suggesting
that a similar analysis with an NLD sample might elaborate differences in voca-
tional function for this group as well. In contrast, there were no significant be-
tween group differences with respect to psychiatric diagnosis, including anxiety
and mood disorders, with 30% of all the subjects evidencing the presence of a
secondary psychiatric disability. These data replicate the more recent work in
this area with respect to subtypes and suggest that in adults, psychiatric involve-
ment is relatively common in any LD subtype.
In summary, adults with NLD often exhibit psychological, social, and occu-
pational problems. Typically, they exhibit low self-esteem, social isolation, de-
pression, and withdrawal. Their lack of problem-solving skills is attributed to
difficulties that range from under appreciation of the significance of a problem to
over reaction to minor inconveniences.
NONVERBAL LEARNING DISABILITY 95
INTERVENTIONS
Capitalization on Strengths
Remediation
to NLD based on this group’s difficulties with novel problem solving in general.
However, few of these programs are based on the results of empirical research.
Hooper and Willis (1989) compare and contrast three metacognitive ap-
proaches that emphasize training in academic strategies, but note that none are
refined enough to address the rather specific deficits associated with the various
LD subtypes. Bireley’s 1995 chapter on academic interventions, while generally
addressing childhood learning problems, outlines a metacognitive model that
may be particularly applicable to adults with NLD. This 6-step program depends
on mulitsensory input, group discussion to facilitate elaboration, guided practice
and feedback, and generalization activities. Unfortunately, data on the efficacy
of this intervention are apparently lacking. Other models use neuropsychologi-
cal data to direct remediation (Hooper and Willis, 1989), offering the advantage
of providing a means to conceptualize a realistic treatment program that com-
pensates for weaknesses and builds on strengths. One recent and promising
development are investigations that explore the efficacy of subtype-to-treatment
matching. However, upon review of these studies one notes that most involve
children who have subtypes of dyslexia.
Compensation
Avoidance
Support Groups
with LD, approximately 35% of whom had been diagnosed with nonverbal prob-
lems, expressed the desire to participate in groups composed of others with LD
who were experiencing similar problems. While most viewed this interaction as
beneficial, it was generally not characterized as a long-term intervention (Bla-
lock, 1987).
Vocational Rehabilitation
According to Dunham et al. (1999), persons with learning disability make up the
fastest growing population served by the vocational rehabilitation system. Voca-
tional rehabilitation services provide counseling, rehabilitation, and placement
services to persons with learning disability whose functional abilities such as
communication, self-direction, and social skills are severely limited (Dowdy et
al., 1996). In this case the focus moves from educational issues to that of em-
ployability. The need for such transitional programming and vocational counsel-
ing for young adults with LD is frequently noted (Holliday et al., 1999; McCue,
1993; Rojewski, 1999), with studies identifying traits that predict vocational
success. For example, attainment of higher level vocations is associated with
personal characteristics such as motivation, persistence, and the realistic ability
to develop compensatory strategies (Reiff et al., 1997; Stafford-Depass, 1998).
Interestingly, the possession of knowledge about LD in general and an apprecia-
tion of ones strengths are also traits associated with vocational success.
ACCOMMODATIONS
Academic
adjust the curriculum to meet the special needs of students with LD. Only 14%
of the schools provided curricular exemption or alternative courses.
The Rehabilitation Act of 1973 provides for a variety of accommodations or
“academic adjustments” in the post-secondary setting (Brinckerhoff, Shaw, and
McGuire, 1996). The major accommodations are adaptations in the manner of
teaching, modifications in academic requirements (e.g., changes in time limit to
complete degree; substitution of courses), and the use of auxiliary equipment or
technology (word processors; proofreading programs; outlining devices; per-
sonal data managers). As Raskind notes, this last category of accommodations is
particularly appropriate for adults with LD as it moves away from the remedia-
tion model that seeks to cure or instruct (Raskind, 1993). Although outcome
research is needed in this area, by building on strengths and enabling the indi-
vidual to attain at a level more commensurate with intellectual ability, assistive
technologies provide a way for adults to compensate for their deficits and thus
lead a more productive life.
In spite of these mandated procedures, accommodations remains controver-
sial (Stanovich, 1999) and problems remain in their application. Difficulties are
associated with both student and faculty perceptions. A small sample of college
students with LD was interviewed to discover the students’ perceptions of the
faculty attitudes toward accommodation for persons with hidden disabilities
(Beilke and Yssel, 1999). This paper identified instructional accommodations
such as extended time on exams and special seating arrangements that are often
made reluctantly and within a “less than positive classroom climate” (p. 364).
Problems were also noted from the faculty perspective. A paper by Bourke,
Strehorn, and Silver (2000) discusses the actual practices of instructional ac-
commodations for college students with LD. A large group of faculty members
were surveyed about their perceptions regarding accommodations that included
untimed, proctored, and alternate-form examinations, note taking assistance, and
additional time for assignments. As might be expected, the faculty who under-
stood the need for accommodation and who believed that accommodations
would be effective tended to provide them. In addition, support from academic
departments also influenced faculty involvement in the program. As Bourke et
al. note, their findings indicate that close communication among with faculty,
administration and service providers is essential in providing effective accom-
modations at the secondary level. Another large study also investigated faculty
attitudes regarding accommodations across multiple sites (Vogel, Leyser,
Wyland, and Brulle, 1999). Table 3 shows more and less favored teaching and
examination accommodation procedures that relate to various academic needs.
As noted in the previous study, certain faculty characteristics were associated
with more or less willingness to provide accommodations. At first look, these
studies indicate progress in attitudes toward academic accommodations at insti-
tutions of higher learning. However, the results are probably skewed by the low
102 CHAPTER 3
return rate by faculty. For example, in both studies the response rate was ap-
proximately 35%.
Workplace
chapter, we will highlight their findings that most directly relate to adults with
NLD in the employment setting.
Reiff and Gerber’s conclusion that, “social skills deficits, more than aca-
demic deficiencies or vocational incompetence, often lead to on-the-job difficul-
ties...” (p. 76) is particularly relevant to NLD. The authors’ extensive interviews
of successful adults with LD revealed several accommodations relevant to indi-
viduals whose deficits fall within the NLD spectrum. Accommodations to ad-
dress visual-perceptual problems included keeping work items displayed within
view to support weak visual memory skills, hiring a wardrobe planner to organ-
ize clothing so as to avoid discordant pattern or color combinations, and avoid-
ing settings with confusing visual arrays of products, such as the super sized gro-
cery store or pharmacy. Persons with NLD often exhibit problems driving or
even negotiating a large office building on foot. Suggestions to circumvent such
difficulties included relying on visual landmarks in place of maps, completing
multiple trial runs to learn the route to work or within the workplace, and provid-
ing a reminder of “left” or “right” by marking the appropriate side of the body
with a designated items such as a wristwatch or ring. Successful adults with LD
also provided suggestions to support organizational problems. These included
developing a rigid daily routine, obtaining assistance with shopping and schedul-
ing appointments, using visual supports such as a color-coded filing system,
computer programs, or extensive lists and notes. Finally, respondents suggested
that it is particularly important to allow extra time for most tasks.
Supported employment is another accommodation that has particular appli-
cation for adults with NLD. Supported employment is distinguished from other
vocational rehabilitation models in several ways (Inge and Tilson, 1993). First,
individuals are assisted in finding a job by a coach, or employment specialist,
who subsequently helps train the employee on work tasks and skills. Impor-
tantly, this coach also provides “off-the-job” training that includes negotiating
transportation, banking, or even the employee cafeteria. Second, supported em-
ployment differs from other models with respect to the amount and type of train-
ing provided. In this case the job coach provides an ongoing assessment of indi-
vidual training needs, providing only as much as is necessary to meet the particu-
lar job requirements and in the least intrusive manner. Instruction provided by
the job coach is tailored to the individual’s learning style and also relies on the
individual to help develop compensatory strategies and self-management proce-
dures to ensure successful employment. Finally, this system provides for follow-
up visits by the coach to determine if modifications are needed. With respect to
NLD, the coach acts as monitor and external “problem-solver,” supporting the
individual by teaching job related skills but also by helping him or her negotiate
the complex social system of the workplace.
104 CHAPTER 3
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
INTRODUCTION
105
106 CHAPTER 4
DEFINITIONS
Historical Foundations
First, we must consider the impact of the DSM-IV criteria on diagnosing adults
with ADHD. If the diagnosis has not been made during the childhood years,
consideration must be given to the initial manifestation of symptom behaviors,
the presence of co-morbid disorders as suggested by Triolo (1999), and the
presence of particular coping strengths, environmental supports, and strategies
which have played major roles throughout the developmental years. Problems
with the criterion “some symptoms causing impairment were present before 7
years of age” immediately comes to the fore. Unless a child is presenting
substantial management problems to his or her parents prior to the kindergarten
years, this child will not be referred to the pediatrician or child psychiatrist.
Thus, this criterion would seem best to apply to those children presenting with
hyperactivity almost exclusively, and this DSM-IV subtype (Predominantly
Hyperactive/Impulsive) was found primarily among preschool children in the
DSM-IV field trials (Applegate et al., 1995). According to Barkley (1997b),
symptoms of inattention associated with the Hyperactive-Impulsive Subtype
emerge later in the development of the disorder and primarily manifest as
problems with sustained attention (persistence) and distractibility. The types of
problems with inattention seen in the Predominantly Inattentive Subtype “appear
to have their onset even later...” (p.67). If age of symptom onset is a limitation
of the criterion with children, then it certainly becomes a limitation in the
diagnostic process when working with adolescents and adults.
With respect to meeting the criterion “some impairment is present in two or
more settings” when the initial diagnosis is made in adolescence or adulthood,
comorbidity may indeed be a salient issue. A number of years ago, researchers
such as Shaffer and Greenhill (1978) and Stewart, Cumming, Singer, and
Deblois (1981) reported that the diagnosis of ADD closely overlaps that of
Conduct Disorder. Thus, one could speculate that if, as a child, observed
behavior at the beginning of his or her school years did not meet criteria for a
Conduct Disorder or a juvenile presentation of Bipolar Disorder, then his or her
level of activity could well have seemed within the bounds of appropriate
classroom behavior particularly if the child were able to control energy levels
until after the classroom day, having then the opportunity to engage in fairly
rigorous physical activity such as that presented by competitive sports. By the
time evenings were spent in the home environment, hyperactive motor behaviors
may well have been substantially diminished due to physical exhaustion. On the
other hand, if these same adults were dysthymic as children, typically the
classroom teacher or the parent would not have found their level of low arousal
or shyness especially noteworthy. Also, they may well have been judged to be
less bright than their peers, and as such, their quiet nature and poor academic
performance inaccurately attributed to this (Triolo, 1999).
ADHD 109
Once entering the adolescent or adult years undiagnosed, the individual “has
most likely traveled a path to other pathologies” (Triolo, 1999, p. 66). The
presence of comorbid disorders in the adult ADHD population has been well
documented (Biederman, Newcorn, and Sprich, 1991; Biederman et al., 1993;
Hallowell and Ratey, 1994; Wender, 1995; Katz, Goldstein, and Geckle, 1998).
But, often adults will seek professional interventions because of secondary
issues such as marital conflicts, alcohol and other drug abuse, anxiety,
depression, etc., unaware of the possible co-existence of ADHD. As a result,
they will most likely be treated for these common adulthood problems (the
presenting problem) vs. a disorder which traditionally has been linked with
childhood even though evidence of “clinically significant impairment in social,
academic or occupational functioning” comes closest to defining the nature of
the disorder in the young adult and adult population. Again, criterion B (some
symptoms causing impairment present before 7 years of age) may not be part of
the developmental history in many cases.
Further, it has been demonstrated that ADHD symptoms tend to decline in
number during the growing years (Achenbach and Edelbrock, 1981). While
reduction of symptoms does not stop at adolescence, core impairments remain
prominent and stable (Barkley, Fischer, Edelbrock, and Smallish, 1991),
although more disruptive behaviors tend to be less overt in adulthood. Here, the
capacity of individuals to develop compensatory strategies is limited only by the
extent of the individual’s own creative potential. Thus, when reviewing the
symptoms of inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity with the individual, often
times, responses will include what he or she has done to cope with
distractibility, forgetfulness, lack of organization, fidgeting, interrupting, rapid
speech, etc. vs. the perceived impact of the symptom on functions in the home,
on the job, or in school. Years of employing strategies result in a tendency to
minimize possible negative consequences. Behaviors are viewed as problematic
only when the external environment does not reinforce the efficacy of such
strategies (e.g. poor performance appraisals on the job, near-failing grades,
marital discord).
For example, take the bright student who up until his freshman year in
college sailed through high school with minimal effort both in terms of input
and output, but whose parents provided the regulating, monitoring, and
structuring of his external environment on a daily basis. Failing out of a
prestigious school is a blow to the ego, can make one quite depressed, and the
process of self-doubt begins. When asked if there were problems with time
management, organization, distractibility, lack of follow through, or
forgetfulness during his earlier years in school or while growing up, the answer
may just as readily be “no” as “yes”. If his parents are available for collateral
information, they may well testify about their efforts. On the other hand, if one
or both of them also have undiagnosed ADHD, then their efforts may well be
par for the course rather than out of the ordinary.
110 CHAPTER 4
A final issue with respect to using DSM-IV criteria relates to the nature and
severity of current symptoms. Individuals may have significant difficulty
sustaining attention in a lecture hall or when reading required texts or when
filling out routine forms, while at other times they report no problems with
inattention or sustained focus when reading for pleasure, surfing the Internet, or
tackling a highly challenging and /or competitive endeavor. The DSM-IV docs
not take into account this vacillation between “hyperfocus” and lack of focus
often seen in adolescents and adults with ADHD. Parents will confirm the hours
spent and degree of intensity maintained by the young adult engaged in playing
with legos or computer games as a child playing hockey or watching sports
programs as an adolescent. At the same time, they will report that their child is
unable to sit quietly during a TV show in which other members of the family are
interested if the demands for attention are greater than 10 minutes. Adults often
incorporate the attributional labels given them on account of this waxing and
waning of attention and assign moral judgments to the perceived voluntary
nature of their behaviors. Thus, the symptom becomes a character trait, most
often a negative one, and again is not thought of as a characteristic symptom of a
disorder.
Subtyping
While the DSM-IV provides three subtypes and a “not otherwise specified”
category” (p. 85), a number of clinical researchers have begun to examine
subtypes based on brain imaging studies as well as neurochemical hypotheses.
Amen and Goldman (1998) have posited five clinical subtypes of ADHD for
both children and adults to which they have linked psychopharmacological
therapies based on SPECT studies. These include ADHD, combined Type,
ADHD, primarily inattentive type, Overfocused ADD, Temporal Lobe ADD,
and Limbic ADD. Both ADHD, Combined Type and ADHD, Predominantly
Inattentive Type show decreased activity in the basal ganglia and prefrontal
cortex during a concentration task. Overfocused ADD is identified with
increased activity in the anterior cingulate gyrus and decreased prefrontal cortex
activity and is associated with obsessive type behaviors. Temporal Lobe ADD
is associated with increased or decreased activity in the temporal lobes and
decreased prefrontal cortex activity and linked to aggressive behaviors. Limbic
ADD shows increased central limbic system activity and decreased prefrontal
cortex activity on SPECT and is associated with low arousal.
The theoretical work of Hunt (1997) attempts to link ADHD subtypes in
adults to four brain systems and disturbances in these multiple pathways and
their respective neurotransmitter networks (cognitive, arousal, behavioral
inhibition, and reward) resulting in four subtypes: Subtype I-Cognitive
Processing Deficit; Subtype II-Excessively Aroused ADD; Subtype III-Impaired
Behavioral Inhibition System; Subtype IV-Deficient Reward Systems.
ADHD 111
Given what we know about the history of the diagnostic process and the
problems inherent when attempting to apply current criteria to an adult
population, information about incidence and prevalence rates as well as
comorbidity can be somewhat problematic. What we do know from clinical
studies is that approximately 40% to 50% of children with ADHD have
symptoms persisting into adulthood (Gittelman, Mannuzza, Shenker, and
Bonagura, 1985; Weiss, Hechtman, and Perlman, 1985), which allows us to
project that impairment in some level of functioning exists in some 1.5% to 2%
of the adult American population (Hunt, 1997). It has been estimated also that
112 CHAPTER 4
Gender Issues
Comorbidity Factors
1) The comorbid disorders do not represent distinct entities but rather, are the
expression of phenotypic variability of the same disorder, 2) each of the
comorbid disorders represents distinct and separate clinical entities, 3) the
comorbid disorders share common vulnerabilities..., either genetic
(genotype), psychosocial (adversity), or both, 4) the comorbid disorders
represent a distinct subtype (genetic variation) within a heterogeneous
disorder..., 5) one syndrome is an early manifestation of a conduct or mood
disorder..., and 6) the development of one syndrome increases the risk for the
comorbid disorder...(p.565).
In their text, ADHD with Cormorbid Disorders, Pliszka, Carlson, and Swanson
(1999) write, “There is probably more confusion over the comorbidity of
learning disorders (or disabilities; LDs) and ADHD than any other topic we will
address” (p. 188). They suggest that the confusion may have originated with
the term “minimal brain dysfunction,” which included both children with
learning problems and those with hyperactivity. By far the vast majority of
studies support a higher prevalence rate for learning disabilities in reading,
spelling, and arithmetic in children with ADHD than in controls (Pliszka,
Carlson, and Swanson, 1999). However, prevalence rates vary depending on
whether one is talking about LD as the initial diagnosis or ADHD as the initial
diagnosis. In the Cantwell and Baker (1991) study, 37% of the sample of
children diagnosed with early speech and language impairments met criteria for
ADHD at a five year follow-up. Of those who met criteria for a comorbid
psychiatric disorder (70%), ADHD was the most common comorbid condition.
Similarly, in the Shaywitz and Shaywitz (1988) study, while 11% of ADHD
children met criteria for LD, 33% of the children with LD also had ADHD.
Dykman and Achkerman (1991) reported that over half of their clinic-referred
ADD sample met criteria for a specific reading disorder (RD).
In general, both RD and ADHD are more prevalent among boys than among
girls when the samples are based upon clinic participants. Taking this finding as
a given, Willcutt and Pennington (2000) examined the comorbidity of the two
disorders differentiated by gender and subtype in a large community sample of
twins between the ages of 8 and 18. Their findings indicated that (1) individuals
with RD were more likely than individuals without RD to meet criteria for
DSM-IV Inattentive Type and Combined Type; (2) the phenotypic relationship
is different in boys and girls, with the association between RD and ADHD
restricted to the ADHD, Inattentive type in the females; and (3) when the
sample was subdivided by FSIQ, both the high and low IQ groups of boys with
RD exhibited a significantly higher frequency of both Inattentive and Combined
Type ADHD than did boys without RD. However, boys with low IQ RD were
significantly more likely to meet criteria for ADHD, Inattentive Type. They
concluded, “Taken together, these results suggest that higher intelligence may
represent a protective factor against symptoms of inattention for individuals with
RD, but does not affect the association between RD and H/I”
(Hyperactivity/Impulsivity) (p. 188). “And while the phenotypic analyses
reported support the hypothesis that RD and ADHD co-occur “significantly
more frequently than would be expected based on chance” (p. 188), they do not
ADHD 115
Tourette’ s Syndrome
Finally, we refer the reader to the Pliszka, Carlson, and Swanson (1999) text for
an in-depth discussion of comorbid disorders and ADHD. In particular, there
are chapters devoted entirely to medical disorders, mental retardation and
pervasive developmental disorders, and tic and obsessive compulsive-disorders
as well as the more commonly discussed mood and anxiety disorders, although
the focus is on children rather than adults. In addition, the text edited by
Leckman and Cohen (1999) details the comorbidity and prevalence of Tourette’s
Syndrome (TS), tics, obsessions, and compulsions with ADHD and other
learning disorders. The chapter by Walkup, Khan, Scherholz, et al., (1999) is
devoted to Phenomenology and Natural History of Tic-Related ADHD and
Learning Disabilities, and in it the authors present data which estimate the
frequency of ADHD in Tourette’s Syndrome to be anywhere from 25% to 75%.
In a review of nine reports of 1500 cases of TS in individuals of all ages, ADHD
was present in approximately 62% of the cases (Comings and Comings, 1988).
Findings suggesting a relationship between the sequence of onset of Tourette’s
Syndrome and ADHD (earlier-onset ADHD being more etiologically
independent of Tourette’s Syndrome and later-onset ADHD being secondary to
the expression of Tourette’s) are regarded as hypothesis generating only at this
point in time (Pauls, Alsobrook, Gelernter, and Leckman, 1999). The text by
Fisher (1998) also devotes a chapter to comorbid disorders associated with ADD
including various neurobehavioral and neurological disorders including
Parkinson’s Disease, multiple sclerosis, epilepsy, neurofibromatosis, and
toxicity, among others.
116 CHAPTER 4
ETIOLOGY
Genetic Studies
in control families. These findings, however, are not conclusive with regard to
the direction of effects, meaning that just as strong a case could be made for the
idea that ADHD in the child results in family disruption as the reverse (Hallahan
and Cottone, 1997). Just as there has been little evidence to suggest that
cognitive training combined with medication yields an additive effect over
medication alone for most children with ADHD (lalongo, Horn, Pascoe, et al.
(1993; Jensen et al., 1999), studies evaluating the effectiveness of parent
training lack a systematic method of research, have many methodological
inconsistencies, and include extremely small sample sizes (Hallahan and
Cottone, 1997).
Taken as a whole, the genetic studies available, the equivocal nature of
prenatal and perinatal studies, the questionable etiological role of psychosocial
stressors, and the familial links associated with comorbid disorders tend to
suggest that environmental factors alone in the majority of instances cannot
account for this developmental neurobehavioral disorder. And while not all
cases of ADHD can be traced to a familial predisposition, there is a substantial
body of research demonstrating that family-genetic influences exist that are
independent of psychosocial adversity (Biederman, Faraone, Keenan, et al.,
1992) and that the genetic transmission of ADHD is independent of the genetic
transmission of learning disabilities (Faraone, Biederman, Lehman, et al., 1993).
However, as Thapar, Homes, Pulton, and Harrington (1999) have pointed out,
while ADHD should be considered a complex multifactorial disorder, relatively
little is known about the interplay between genes and environment. The
inclusion of environmental factors within genetically sensitive designs will be
important as even when susceptibility genes are identified, environmental
factors may play an important protective or mediating role (Taylor, 1994).
and 15 controls. Their findings supported specific predictions from the Posner
and Raichle (1996) neuroanatomical network theory of attention. Namely,
children with ADHD had smaller brain volumes in anterior superior regions
(posterior prefrontal, motor association, and midanterior cingulate) and anterior
inferior regions (anterior basal ganglia). These abnormal findings implicate the
neuroanatomical networks of executive control and alerting (Swanson,
Castellanos, Murias, et al., 1998). Thus, it has been proposed that the caudate
(Lou, 1996) and the braking mechanism provided by the indirect caudate-globus
pallidus route to thalamic output neurons (Castellanos, 1997) may be the site of
a “neuroanatomical deficit” (Swanson et al., 1998) that results in symptoms of
ADHD.
As mentioned earlier in this chapter, Barkley (1997) has presented a new
theoretical model for understanding the symptoms of ADHD based on the
theoretical work of Bronowski (1967). He had suggested that the unique ability
of man to delay response to a stimulus may be explained by four axes:
separation of affect, prolongation, internalization, and reconstitution. Barkley
proposes that an impairment in delayed responding (response inhibition and self-
regulating measures mediated by these four variables) represents the core
impairment in ADHD. He further postulates that impairment in delayed
responding is mediated by underfunctioning of the orbital frontal cortex and
subsequent connections to the limbic system. The result is a hyperresponsivity
to stimuli producing hyperactivity primarily and secondarily, inattentiveness
(Zametkin and Liotta, 1998).
Finally, the inferior posterior lobe of the cerebellar vermis has been found to
be smaller in children with ADHD, although the significance of the finding is
not yet explicated fully. However, the cerebellum does project to the prefrontal
cortex via the thalamus (Middleton and Strick, 1994) and has been found to be
involved in both motor (Kim, Ugurbil, and Strick, 1994) and non-motor
cognitive tasks (Jueptner, Rijntjes, Weiller, et al., 1995).
anterior executive system (prefrontal cortex [PFC] and anterior cingulate gyrus),
where the responsiveness of the PFC and anterior cingulate to incoming signals
is modulated primarily by dopaminergic (DA) input from the ventral tegmental
area of the midbrain. Ascending DA fibers stimulate postsynaptic Dl receptors
on pyramidal neurons in the PFC and anterior cingulate, which in turn facilitate
excitatory NMDA receptor inputs from the posterior attention system. DA
selectively gates excitatory inputs to both the PFC and the cingulate which then
effectively reduces irrelevant neuronal activity during the performance of
executive functions. “According to Pliszka et al (1996), the inability of NE to
prime the posterior attention system could account for the problems with
inattention seen in children with ADHD, and the loss of DA’s ability to gate
inputs to the anterior executive system may be linked to the deficit in executive
functions characteristic of ADHD” (Himelstein, Schulz, Newcorn, and Halperin,
2000, p. 463).
Hunt (1997) has proposed a neurobiological model of ADHD in which he
attempts to link the particular brain systems involved with information
processing, arousal, behavioral inhibition, and the limbic system, disturbances in
their neurotransmitter pathways (DA, NE, 5-HT, and neuropeptides and
endorphins), and the symptoms manifest in ADHD as a means to form the basis
for pharmacological interventions. “The neurotransmitters and neuropeptides
bridge information between neurons and are key to perception, cognitive
processing, arousal, inhibition, and reward....Medications mitigate the
neurobehavioral dysfunctions in ADHD by modulating the neurotransmitter
mechanisms at the synapse” (p. 579).
According to Hunt’s schema, a defect in the cognitive-processing system,
primarily a disorder of selective attention and information processing and
possibly related to hypoactivity of the dopamine system, is hypothesized to
involve the gating capacity of the nucleus accumbens, cortical sensory
integration centers, the ability of the hippocampus to identify change in the
environment, and response selection capacities of the prefrontal cortex.
Symptoms of excessive arousal (aggression, impulsivity, increased motor
activity and comorbid mania and/or conduct disorder) are thought to relate to
hyperactivity of noradrenergic circuitry involving the locus coerulus and the
reticular activating system. Impaired behavioral inhibition results from the
combined dysfunction of serotonergic and dopaminergic circuits in the
prefrontal cortex and in the caudate nucleus and thalamus. Deficits in the
reward system are thought to arise from affect regulation defects in limbic
circuitry, which then impact responsivity to reward and punishment and faulty
cognitive analysis and integration of information arising from defects in
prefrontal and associative cortex. Both dopamine and the neuropeptides and
endorphins are hypothesized to be dysregulated.
Taken as a whole, research devoted to the neurochemical aspects of ADHD
does appear to give evidence of dysfunction in multiple neurotransmitter
124 CHAPTER 4
Triolo (1999) has set forth six sequential components for the diagnostic process
with adults:
Rule out medical problems that may mimic ADHD symptoms
Conduct a clinical interview incorporating a retrospective history and
observations as well as DSM-IV criteria as appropriate
Conduct an independent collateral interview
Quantify subjective reports of ADHD symptoms with standardized
measures (Inventories)
Conduct a brief analysis of personality traits and emotional dispositions
When warranted, neuropsychological testing
In his text Triolo spends an entire chapter on the diagnostic process which is
complete and readily accessible by the clinician. Thus, we will not review in
detail all of the six components which he describes, but rather focus on the last
three, namely, the use of self-report inventories, personality assessment, and the
use of neuropsychological testing, as these are more typically included in
evaluations conducted by psychologists and neuropsychologists in addition to
the other three information sources.
Self-Report Inventories
While there are numerous checklists available for use with adults (Weiss, 1992;
Hallowell and Ratey, 1994; Murphy and LeVert, 1995, Amen, 1997)), they are
not standardized and at times the cutoff points are arbitrary at best. In addition,
many checklists included in the lay literature and on the Internet are so inclusive
that no one would escape identification as having ADHD. Triolo (1999) points
out also that some checklists and questionnaires can appear to be statistically
ADHD 125
developed with normative parameters but are not. Here, he cites the Owens and
Owens (1993) Adult Attention Deficit Disorder Behavior Rating Scales. That
having been said, there are at least four published instruments which are normed
through the entire age range for adults, the Adult Attention Deficit Evaluation
Scale, A-ADDES (McCarney and Anderson, 1996), the Brown Attention Deficit
Disorder Scales (Brown, 1996), the Attention Deficit Scales for Adults (ADSA)
(Triolo and Murphy, 1997), and newest, the Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating
Scales (CAARS) (Conners, Erhardt, and Sparrow, 1999). The ones we have
found most useful are the Brown ADD Scales, one for adolescents and one for
adults.
Impetus for the Brown ADD Scales, according to the developer (Brown,
1996), came from clinical conversations with students who suffered from
“chronic underachievement despite high IQ and apparently strong wishes to do
well in school” (p. 4). The Scales cover the age ranges of 12 through 18 years
and 18 years and older, and the 40 self report items are grouped into five clusters
of “conceptually related symptoms” (Brown, 1996) of attention deficit disorders
(ADDs). These are organizing and activating to work, sustaining attention and
concentration, sustaining energy and effort, managing affective interference, and
utilizing working memory and accessing recall. While the items assess
symptoms included in the DSM-IV criteria, they also incorporate symptoms
taken from clinical studies. Brown goes on to say that certain assumptions
underlie the nature of the ADDs and thus the construction of the test items.
These are listed below.
ADDs are dimensional disorders.
ADD symptoms may vary according to tasks and context.
Difficulty with activating and getting started is often an important element of
ADD impairment.
Chronic impairments in working memory and in recall of learned
information are often important in ADDs.
Hyperactivity/Impulsivity is not an essential element in ADDs.
ADDs often include aspects of other psychological disorders and frequently
overlap with other emotional or behavioral disorders in comorbid
combinations.
ADDs are not impairments of attention only; they are disorders affecting
many aspects of cognitive functioning.
Thus, Brown’s definition of ADHD with adolescents and adults takes into
account the frequency and intensity of symptoms, the stimulus bound nature of
sustained attention, problems with activation of behavior in addition to
insufficient inhibition, and impairments in active working memory (Baddeley,
1986; Goldman-Rakic, 1987, 1994) and its “file manager” (p. 8) function in
accessing recall. The central impairment of ADHD is not viewed as disruptive
behavior or failure of inhibition or of inattention only, but rather as impairments
in a broad range of cognitive functions, and that a significant overlap of
126 CHAPTER 4
symptoms between ADD and other disorders is a given. The Scales also include
a diagnostic form for the clinical history interview and the opportunity for multi-
rater capacity on the Scales themselves.
There is one caveat that Triolo (1999) discusses with respect to the IQ levels
of the groups on which the Brown Scales were normed. Of the 142 clinical
subjects, 52 had an IQ within the average range and 90 had an IQ of 110 or
greater. However, one must keep in mind that the nonclinical group used in the
discriminant validity study were matched for age and socioeconomic status but
not on IQ. That the IQ level of the majority of the clinical group is above
average is not surprising given the fact that Brown’s initial work with high
school and college level students led to the development of the Scales.
Should IQ level be a concern for the practitioner, perhaps the items elicited
by the ADSA (Triolo and Murphy, 1996) or those elicited by the CAARS
(Conners, Erhardt, and Sparrow, 1999) might be more useful. The ADSA was
initially normed on a community sample of 306 adults with no childhood history
of problems with attention or hyperactivity, estimated IQs of 80 or above, no
reported history of drug or alcohol abuse or felony convictions, and age 17 or
older. Data were later collected on 97 clinical subjects, age 17 years or older,
who were diagnosed with ADHD prior to involvement in the validity study of
the ADSA (Triolo, 1999). Conners et al. (1999) report age and gender based
norms from a sample of 2000 nonclinical community adults as well. The
CAARS has a both a self-report and an observer rating format as well as long,
short, and screening versions.
Triolo’s (1999) other concern was that the Brown Scales overemphasized the
core symptom of inattention. In this regard, the reader is referred to a study by
Millstein, Wilen, Biederman, and Spencer (1997) in which they evaluated the
clinical presentation of ADHD in adults. Their results indicated that inattentive
symptoms were most frequently endorsed in over 90% of their adult sample.
Fifty-six percent of the group met criteria for ADHD, Combined Type, 37%
Inattentive Type, and 2% Hyperactive/Impulsive Type. Also, psychiatric
comorbidity with ADHD was more prominent in adults with hyperactivity-
impulsivity as part of their clinical picture. They concluded, “Given that
ADHD adults are presenting from multiple domains, clinicians should carefully
query for the inattentive aspects of ADHD when evaluating these individuals”
(p. 159).
In summary, behavior rating scales have been the mainstay in the clinical
assessment of children with ADHD for more than 20 years (Halperin, Newcorn,
and Sharma, 1991). However, even with children, teacher ratings suffer from
halo effects (Schachar et al., 1986), and parent ratings have been found to have
poor reliability (Rapoport et al., 1986). With adults, in a study by Klee,
Garfinkel and Beauchesne (1986), while the index group (diagnosed with
ADHD) rated themselves as having more problems with concentration, lower
frustration tolerance, impulsivity, and restlessness as children than did the
ADHD 127
control group, there was greater similarity between the two groups on current
behavior rating as the index group rated themselves as much improved when
compared to their childhood ratings. These findings suggest real limitations in
relying on rating scales solely to identify symptoms in adults, when in fact, the
nature of symptomatology in adults may well be altered. In addition, with the
exception of the Brown Scales, behavior rating scales have generally not been
particularly helpful in the diagnosis of ADHD, Inattentive Type, which has been
documented to be more common in adults and in women (NIH Consensus
Statement, 1998). Given these concerns, the use of self-rating scales should be
considered as one among a number of measures used in the diagnosis of adults
Personality Measures
Having said that, there are certain cautions that must be taken into
consideration during the interpretation process, as with the exception of the
APS, none of the other measures have necessarily included individuals with
ADHD in their clinical norms. It is our experience, as has been reported
elsewhere in the literature, that the Obsessive-Compulsive Scale on the SCL-90-
R is generally elevated in adults and adolescents with ADHD because the items
included in this scale deal with worrying excessively, ruminating thoughts,
difficulty with decision making and concentration, and memory problems. With
the BDI-II, items involving past failure, self-disappointment, agitation, and
concentration difficulty are most often rated as problematic. These items on the
BDI-II illustrate to some degree the reactive nature of the depressed feelings
particularly when the individual has experienced failure in the school setting or
on the job. With the MCMI-III, the scales tend to take on a spurious nature with
individuals who have lived with their ADHD over the years particularly if they
have a childhood history of hyperactivity and/or impulsivity. Resultant profiles
often tend to reflect a pre-occupation with self and the lack of awareness or
reported concern for others, which may or may not be totally accurate. As a
result, the validity of personality measurement given the instruments available at
this time should be viewed with caution and always in light of information
derived from a comprehensive history and access to corroborating data
whenever possible.
Neuropsychological Assessment
performance tests (CPT), Barkley (1994b) concludes that while children having
ADHD often make more errors as a group than do groups of children without
such disorders, they have not been particularly effective in differentiating
between children with ADHD and “other clinical groups presumed not to have
sustained attention problems” (p. 80).
On the other hand studies utilizing neuropsychological measures have
demonstrated deficits in adults consistent with those found in children
(Matochik, Rumsey, and Zametkin, 1996; Downey, Stetson, Pomerleau, and
Giordani, 1997; Jenkins, Cohen, and Malloy, 1998; Seidman, Biederman, and
Weber, et al., 1998; Corbett and Stanczak, 1999; Lovejoy, Ball, and Keats,
1999). Further, work by Seidman et al. (1998), suggests that because the
diagnosis of ADHD in adults is controversial to the degree that it is dependent
on a retrospective diagnosis, external validation of cognitive-neuropsychological
functions, particularly attentional and executive processes, which have been
found to be frequently impaired in children with ADHD, “would help to clarify
the validity of the adult diagnosis and supplement the clinical picture” (p. 260).
In addition, cognitive performance measures are useful validating criteria
because they do not share method variance with other measures and by their
very nature allow direct assessment of performance. “Identification of core
neuropsychological deficits in adults with ADHD is also important both as an
empirical study of performance relevant to adaptive functioning and as a
window into hypothesized alterations in brain functioning in frontostriatal
systems (Grodzinsky and Diamond, 1992)” (Seidman et al., 1998, p. 260).
In the study involving unmedicated adults with ADHD and controls reported
by Seidman et al. (1998), the neuropsychological battery utilized included an
estimated Full Scale IQ from the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981), using the
Vocabulary and Block Design subtests. The Digit Symbol, Digit Span, and
Arithmetic subtests were used to compute a Freedom-from–Distractibility IQ as
well. In addition, the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised (WRAT-R)
(Jastak and Jastak, 1985), Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) (Rey, 1941),
the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) (Delis et al., 1987), the Wisconsin
Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Heaton et al., 1993), the Stroop Test (Golden,
1978), the scattered letters version of visual cancellations (Weintraub and
Mesulam, 1985), and an auditory CPT (Zametkin et al., 1990) were
administered. Overall, the adults with ADHD demonstrated milder
neuropsychological impairments than had been previously reported with
children and adolescents using an identical battery (Seidman, Biederman,
Faraone, et. al, 1997). The adults assessed were not impaired on the Stroop, the
WCST, or the ROCF as the children had been, nor did they show significant
impairment on the Freedom-from-Distractibility Index. However, they were
significantly more impaired than controls on the CVLT for words learned on
trials 1-5, on semantic clustering, and on the number of words recalled after long
130 CHAPTER 4
delay; the auditory CPT; and on the arithmetic subtest of the WRAT-R. These
results were similar to those reported by Holdnack et al. (1995).
In recognition of the high degree of comorbidity between ADHD and
depression, which has been documented from both epidemiologic (Anderson et
al, 1987; Bird et al., 1988) and clinical studies (Jensen et al., 1988; Woolston et
al, 1989; Biederman et al., 1990), we (Katz, Wood, Goldstein, et al., 1998)
investigated the potential of various neuropsychological measures to
discriminate between groups of adults with ADHD comorbid with depression
and those with a diagnosis of ADHD without depression. Variables derived from
the CVLT, the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) (Gronwall, 1977),
and the Stroop Test were found to discriminate between the groups at a level
significantly exceeding chance. But while the neuropsychological tests used
appeared to be quite sensitive to ADHD, they were also sensitive to depression
in some cases. Results suggested that the differential diagnosis of ADHD and
depression in adults may be complicated to some extent because of the shared
characteristics of the disorders in adults.
on math functions and operations should the need to rule out a specific
mathematics disorder seem appropriate. The TOMA-2 provides subtests
dealing not only with computation but also with story problems, math
vocabulary, general concepts, and attitude toward math. As it is normed
on individuals between the ages of 8 an 18-11, its use may be restricted
more appropriately to young adults unless it is used primarily for
providing qualitative information. Alternatively, the several math
measures (calculation and applied problems) from the Woodcock-
Johnson Tests of Achievement-Revised (Woodcock and Johnson, 1989)
provide a more in-depth view of math skills for the college population in
particular.
Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery-Revised (WLPB-R)
(Woodcock, 1991). The Woodcock provides several measures for
assessing written language skills including punctuation, spelling, and
grammar (usage). There is also a measure of writing fluency under
constraints of timed performance. As with the Woodcock-Johnson Tests
of Cognition and Achievement (WJ-R) (Woodcock and Johnson, 1989),
norms are provided for adult subjects.
Test of Written Language–3 (TOWL-3) (Hammill and Larssen, 1996).
Should a spontaneous writing sample be required to further assess
written language output, the TOWL-3 may be utilized, as long as one
takes into account a degree of score inflation as the referent group is
high school seniors. However, despite that issue, a qualitative analysis
of theme development, logical thought progression, and efficient
production of written language under timed and extended time
conditions can be made.
Nelson-Denny Reading Test (Brown, Fishco, and Hanna, 1993) and the
Gray Oral Reading Test (GORT-3) (Wiederholt and Bryant, 1992). We
routinely administer the Nelson-Denny, a silent reading comprehension
measure, which also allows for measurement of reading rate, in addition
to the GORT-3. Invariably, unless a reading disorder is present, the
individual with ADHD will improve his or her comprehension and
reading rate scores under conditions of oral reading. We suggest that
these measures help to demonstrate the impact of multi-modal sensory
input on working memory. Again, the norms are restricted to college
graduates on the Nelson-Denny and ages 18-11 on the GORT-3.
However, these measures much more closely resemble the kinds of
reading materials and multiple-choice exam questions an individual is
likely to experience in the later secondary and postsecondary years.
Also, it is our experience that while the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of
Achievement (Woodcock and Johnson, 1989) are normed on an adult
population as well as children, the subtest assessing passage
comprehension overestimates reading comprehension skills in
ADHD 133
Pharmacotherapy
Psychostimulants
Nonstimulant Medications
Despite the use of stimulant medication with adults diagnosed with ADHD,
some 30% to 50% do not respond positively to the medication, experience
unpleasant or untoward side effects, or have comorbid depressive or anxiety
136 CHAPTER 4
Alternative Medicines
In addition to the use of medication, the single most important intervention for
adults dealing with ADHD is education, whether in the academic arena, the
world of work, or in the context of personal and social relations. Hallowell
(1995) writes that the comprehensive treatment of ADHD begins with diagnosis
followed by education, structure, psychotherapy, and medication. “The first
two” (diagnosis and education) “are invariably essential; the third, almost
always; each of the final two may or may not be necessary” (p. 147).
Self-education and self-awareness are keys to well-informed decision making
and to understanding the expectations and roles that derive from being a student
in a post-secondary college or university setting. Without them, the student is
confronted with a myriad of pitfalls that mitigate the chance for success. Despite
a reliance on external structures that were in place in the earlier years or self-
devised compensatory strategies that might have proved more than adequate for
the demands posed by earlier educational experiences, once faced with the
complexity of demands on the person in this unstructured, self-directed, highly
diverse environment, the old approaches are no longer functional. Lack of well-
integrated and self-regulated time management, organization, and planning skills
coupled with the inability to cope with an inertia that feeds upon itself and
culminates in excessive procrastination, [that reacts only to intense pressure],
leaves the young adult student vulnerable to failure, low-esteem, and depression.
Self-evaluation becomes laden with self-criticism, negative projections, and self-
anger if the individual does not understand the role that ADHD plays in his or
her academic life.
Direct, explicit instruction in how to set priorities, how to manage the
demands on one’s time while at the same time planning for down-time, and how
to replace the unincorporated “parent monitor” with strategies and devices that
are one’s own, either internal or external, are critical. The instructor or
140 CHAPTER 4
counselor cannot assume that once the individual has a day-planner or palm
pilot, for example, that he or she will be able to follow a schedule, get to class
on time, hand in reports on time, or establish a sense of priorities. The key will
be in connecting relevance to behavior by establishing a direct link between
desired outcome and steps to achieve that outcome and positively reinforcing
behaviors that reflect cognitive and metacognitive understanding in their
enactment. Knowing “what works” and why and then being willing to give up
ineffective learning strategies that have been intermittently and successfully
reinforced in the past are examples of both cognitive and metacognitive
understanding.
additional expense to the student, unless such provision would pose an undue
hardship (Southwestern Community College v. Davis, 1979).
Life-Management Skills
When working with the young adult as therapist, counselor, or coach, the roles
outlined by Nadeau (1995) are highly appropriate in this regard. Nadeau writes
that the clinician working with adults with ADHD needs to take into
consideration all of the aspects of daily functioning that are affected by the
cognitive skills deficits associated with ADHD. Citing the work of Zametkin,
Nadeau states that as the frontal lobes are one of the major neurological
structures involved in attention deficit, the executive functions they control are
the “oversight or managerial functions so often affected in adults with ADD”
(p. 191). She lists various executive functions that include attention, memory,
organization, planning, initiation, self-inhibition (self-discipline), ability to
change set, strategic behavior, and self-monitoring in relation to time. Her
chapter is then devoted to the effect of executive functions on practical life
management skills.
Next, Nadeau (1995) sets forth roles for the therapist. She writes that an
essential role is to educate and thereby enable the individual to better understand
the neurological basis for the ADHD symptoms. The process of education is
supported through reading, participation in support groups, and ongoing therapy
in some cases. The therapist is also a supporter, moving the individual from
“victimization to empowerment in relation to the ADD symptoms” (p. 193).
The therapist is an interpreter, validating the disabling effect of ADHD on the
individual’s life whether to a spouse or employer. Finally, the therapist is a
structurer and a rehabilitation counselor, providing guidelines, homework
assignments, and information and strategies for both accommodations and
environmental restructuring. She goes on to detail strategies focused on
attention-enhancing techniques; on the avoidance of prolonged under or over
stimulation, everyday, semantic and prospective memory problems, and the use
of compensatory strategies; building problem-solving skills; time management
issues including chronic lateness; and stress management and the reduction of
distractions. Her model, which is based on an assessment of executive functions
in daily living, relies in part on the work of Sohlberg and Geyer (1986) and
Pollens, McBratnie, and Burton (1988).
Coaching
Hallowell (1995) writes that in psychotherapy with patients who have ADHD it
is useful to be overtly encouraging rather than emotionally neutral and to be
directive rather than withholding of advice. “In this posture the therapist
becomes like a coach: that individual standing on the sidelines with a whistle
142 CHAPTER 4
around his or her neck, barking out encouragement, directions, and reminders to
the player in the game” (p. 148). According to Nancy Ratey (1997) “coaching is
about a partnership ...it’s about helping the person learn how to manage their
brain” (p.8). Coaching is also an example of a point of performance
intervention (Barkley, 1997). Barkley sees the problems experienced by
individuals with ADHD not as skill deficits but rather problems with behavioral
execution. Because of the difficulties encountered in executive functions’
successful performance is impaired despite the individual’s existing knowledge
of effective coping strategies.
Consistent with Berkley’s theory, Turnock (1998) reported the results of a
survey conducted at Colorado State University involving students with ADHD.
Results suggested that students within the high-symptom group used
significantly fewer coping behaviors than their low-symptom peers. They
approached studying in a less organized, less methodical way, procrastinated
more, and employed fewer self-control/self-disciplinary behaviors. Academic
success in the high-symptom group was related primarily to their level of
intelligence, but significantly lower grades and high drop-out rates were greater
among this group than among their low-symptom peers. In the words of
Hallowell again, “This approach (coaching) takes into account the neurological
inability of the ADD mind to focus and organize as efficiently as other minds”
(p. 149).
In like manner, Ratey, Hallowell, and Miller (1997) define coaching as an
action oriented rather than insight oriented process. They suggest that while the
coach may be a partner or friend, retaining a professional coach may be the most
productive arrangement. The interaction between the coach and the adult with
ADHD may be face-to-face meetings, phone calls, faxes, or e-mails, and in this
relationship the coach acts as a kind of “neurocognitive prosthetic device” (p.
586). As such, the coach assists the individual to compensate for deficiencies in
his or her executive functions that impede the ability to plan, organize, and
monitor behavior.
Coleman and Sussman (Unpublished manuscript) suggest that a
comprehensive approach towards coaching individuals with ADHD can be
summarized in four words: structure, support, skills, and strategies. Structure is
operationalized as (1) clearly defined vision, goals and values that are clarified
in the early phases of coaching; (2) systems for managing daily life (e.g.,
shopping, bill paying, handling mail); (3) a time management system that
enables the individual to identify priorities, break them into manageable steps,
and schedule them into a calendar; (4) a single action taken every day to
reinforce a sense of accomplishment; and (6) daily habits that are small,
constructive actions done on a routine basis.
Support is provided through a variety of means. In this regard the coach
serves a variety of roles that include witness, empathizer, provider of feedback,
and a personal advocate, among others. Hallowell (1995) organizes these
ADHD 143
Developmental Issues
Although anecdotal and clinical reports are available that may be useful in
understanding the employment issues faced by individuals with ADHD (Weiss,
1992; 1994; Hallowell and Ratey, 1994; Latham and Latham, 1994; Kelly and
Ramundo, 1995; Levine, 1995; Nadeau, 1995), there is no empirical
documentation to support the use of any particular strategy (Carroll and
Ponterotto, 1998). Thus, we are left with relying on the clinical experiences of
these individuals for the most part. Further, Nadeau (1995) writes that career
counseling with adults who have ADHD is a complex process that requires
knowledge of career issues, neurodevelopmental issues as they affect workplace
performance, psychological disorders that may be related to ADHD, and
personality factors that interact with attentional difficulties. “There is no single
course of training that can provide this level of expertise in such a broad range
of disciplines” (p.326).
Moore (1997) writes that making the transition from student to worker is a
difficult process for most individuals, but the individual with ADHD may face
some significant obstacles from the start. In addition to possible issues
surrounding emotional maturity, the tendency toward impulsive responding may
be problematic from the perspectives of both interpersonal relationships (e.g.,
impulsive comments that interrupt coworkers or meetings) and
overcommitments, taking on projects that cannot be handled successfully by one
person. In addition, the “flush of enthusiasm” (p. 3) often gives way to boredom
when the task requires follow-through and attention to details that are perceived
to be tedious. Other obstacles involve frustrations by coworkers on account of
the problems with clutter, confusion, lack of time management, and
idiosyncratic “piling systems” that person with ADHD often evidence. Lastly,
according to Moore, the most difficult job relationship for many to handle may
be the crucial connection between the employee and the supervisor, because of
the tendency to display independent attitudes vs. a teamwork approach, chafing
at traditional procedures and rules, and a distaste for authority figures. She then
goes on to write about the work of Brainworks in Carrollton, Texas, where both
ADHD 145
individual and common problems for individuals with ADHD in the workplace
and in their personal relationships are the focus of treatment.
In an effort to assist school counselors with decisions regarding postsecondary
preparation or direct job placement, Levine (1995) investigated “ADHD-
friendly” occupations using three criteria: 1) does a given occupation facilitate
autonomy; 2) does it provide for active engagement and movement rather than
staying in one place all day; and 3) does it provide a variety of duties, that allow
creative contributions in some form. She investigated 30 job areas including
computer technology, consulting, engineering, education, nursing, technical,
legal, medical, managerial, laborer, construction, and scientific. As an example
in the technical area, some of the representative jobs that allowed for autonomy,
variety, and multiple settings were those involving biomedical equipment
technician, sound effects technician, field engineer, and electronics mechanic.
Interestingly, in the area of law, while the nature of the work of trial attorneys
and litigators was judged to be “ADHD-friendly,” insurance or corporate law,
requiring constant attention to detail, made it an “ADHD-unfriendly”
occupation. Her work has been cited by several authors working in the area of
employment counseling (Carroll and Ponterotto, 1998; Schwiebert, Sealander,
and Bradshaw, 1998).
Limitations
In reviewing nearly all follow-up and outcome studies involving adults with
ADHD, certain methodological concerns must be taken into account that suggest
that interpretations drawn from these studies must be narrowly defined. First
and foremost, nearly every major study (Hechtman, Weiss, Perlman, Hopkins,
and Wener, 1981; Weiss, Hechtman, Milroy, and Perlman, 1985; Mannuzza,
Klein, Bessler, 1993; 1998; Hansen, Weiss, and Last, 1999) has involved males
who were diagnosed as children and in DSM-IV terminology would meet
criteria for ADHD, Hyperactive/Impulsive Type. Data that address outcomes in
ADHD, Inattentive Type with adults are largely nonexistent (Schwean, 1999).
Second, the samples were all clinically drawn and thus, by their very nature,
reflect concern for behaviors that were judged to be highly indicative of
pathology in the first place, often times conduct disorder. And third, the loss of
one-third of the sample in the 15 year follow-up study reported by Weiss et al.
(1985) gives reason for pause in terms of generalization of results, although the
researchers speculated that those subjects lost might have represented a worst
outcome subgroup, coloring to some degree their outcome findings. They
concluded, however, that “There was evidence that the hyperactives had more
overall psychopathology and functioned generally less well than did normal
controls” (p.211).
Results reported by Biederman, Wilens, Mick, et al. (1995), Mannuzza et al
(1998), and Hansen, Weiss, and Last (1999) concur, suggesting that children
with ADHD are at significantly higher risk for a specific negative course marked
by antisocial and substance-related disorders, are more likely to have dropped
out of high school, and more likely than controls to report problems in
psychological functioning. Again, the samples were drawn primarily from
Caucasian (The Mannuzza study in particular, as the others do not specify race.)
ADHD 147
SUMMARY
Throughout this chapter we have made an effort to highlight the many problems
associated with the diagnosis of adults with ADHD as well as the presentation
and ramification of the disorder in their lives, with a particular focus on young
adults in postsecondary educational settings. In addition, we have reviewed a
number of significant studies in the fields of genetics, neurobiology,
neurochemistry, and neuropsychology in an attempt to capture data that are
relevant to our understanding of ADHD in an adult population. Strategies
including pharmacotherapy, psychosocial interventions, and current intervention
practices have been detailed, again with a focus on adults, recognizing that
much of the knowledge is based on clinical rather than empirical validation. [In
light of the existing limitations on research involving adults and the outcome
studies currently in the literature, in the future, we will be most anxious to see
research that includes both men and women, differentiates between Inattentive,
Hyperactive/Impulsive, and Combined Types in the subjects drawn for study,
begins to address the subtypes suggested by Amen and others for purposes of
treatment interventions. Investigations of treatment strategies such as coaching,
support groups, and directive psychotherapy as they may impact on the lives of
adults with ADHD, whether diagnosed as children or adults will be critical as
well].
5
HIGH FUNCTIONING AUTISM
INTRODUCTION
149
150 CHAPTER 5
disorders as well, and their influences would have to be determined in each case.
Higher functioning individuals often find their ways into regular schools and
special education classes, but that is rarely the case for lower-functioning
individuals. When these high functioning individuals enter school, they typically
demonstrate forms of learning disorder that appear to be relatively unique, and
are, in any event, quite different from what characterizes dyslexia and other
academic skill disorders.
DIAGNOSIS
The group of older children or adults with high functioning autism constitutes
about 20 - 25% of the cases. The remaining cases have varying degrees of
mental retardation or are mute. The conclusion reached by most experts is that
autism is the same disorder regardless of level of intelligence and
communicative ability. These latter factors are indicators of severity, and do not
define the disorder itself. Autism is thought to be present if DSM-IV criteria are
met, and if further clinical and laboratory examination does not rule it out.
The high functioning individuals, despite the early developmental delays, go
on to develop adequate language and often enter special education programs or
regular schools. As noted earlier, they often have academic difficulties, but they
are quite different from the learning problems typically experienced by children
with dyslexia or other forms of academic skill disorder. For this reason, special
attention needs to be paid to the educational needs of children with autism, since
the more traditional remedial methods do not appear to be applicable (Siegel,
Goldstein, and Minshew, 1996). We will elaborate on this point extensively as
we proceed. These differences appear not to be directly related to the social
skill impairment or clinical phenomenology of the autism, but to major
differences in cognitive function among autism, the specific academic skill
disorders, and normal function. Indeed, some authorities hold the view that the
clinical phenomenology of autism is founded in the way in which individuals
with autism think. There is a very large literature on cognitive function in
autism that we will briefly summarize before dealing more directly with the
matter of learning disability.
152 CHAPTER 5
The core cognitive deficit in autism has been sought for many years, and there is
no consensus in the field at present. Domains of function implicated have
included attention, memory, language, conceptual reasoning, and executive
function. More specifically, impairments of working memory and selective
attention have been viewed as areas of particular involvement. Autism has been
characterized in terms of comprehensive conceptualizations describing it as a
disorder of memory, executive function, or complex information processing.
While the neurobiology of autism is essentially unknown, these theoretical
formulations have suggested various localizations of the disorder including the
temporal-limbic system, the frontal lobes, the amygdala, and the cerebellum. A
wealth of formal experiments comparing individuals with autism with
appropriate control and comparison groups has been accomplished in efforts to
document these theories. Early work in this area is difficult to interpret because
of the lack of agreed upon diagnostic criteria, but since the appearance of DSM-
III, the diagnostic problem has been largely resolved.
We will briefly review the general theories of cognitive function in autism,
and attempt to make an evaluation and synthesis. Originally, autism was
thought by Kanner and his co-workers (Kanner, 1943; Kanner, 1977; Kanner,
Rodriguez, and Ashenden, 1972) to be an environmentally acquired disorder
produced by obsessive, distant parents; so-called "refrigerator parents." Since
these early observations, it has become increasingly apparent that autism is a
neurodevelopmental disorder, and that while Kanner's description of its
phenomenology was quite astute, his assumptions about its cause were
essentially incorrect. Most modern theories of autism are variants of the general
concept of a neurodevelopmental disorder, although the neuropathology is not
yet understood. There is also a, at least implicit, widely held view that autism is
fundamentally a cognitive disorder, despite the fact that the clinical
phenomenology most apparently involves social function and communicative
behavior. It could be said that autism is now thought to be a neuropsychological
disorder, and a great deal of the research done in recent years involves
experimental neuropsychology. There have been numerous studies of the major
domains of neuropsychological function, which include attention, memory,
language, spatial abilities, perceptual and motor skills, and conceptual abilities
or executive function.
Memory
Historically, the memory theory was the first of the neurocogitive theories
extensively considered. Some time ago, we had the so-called "amnesic theory"
of autism that asserted that memory is a cardinal deficit in autism, particularly
since post-mortem studies have identified abnormalities in the hippocampus
HIGH FUNCTIONING AUTISM 153
(Bauman and Kemper, 1985), and animal model studies have proposed a
resemblance between autistic behavior and that observed in monkeys with
medial temporal structure ablations (Bachevalier, 1991). Boucher and
Warrington (1976) found resemblances between the pattern of memory found in
individuals with autism and those with amnesic disorders. These resemblances
were impaired free recall, a reduced primacy effect with a normal recency effect
on list learning, and improvement with external cuing but not through use of
internally produced cues. That is, they shared the pattern of intact and impaired
memory function found among individuals with an amnesic syndrome.
Numerous studies confirmed the existence of a reduced primacy effect, and the
apparent lack of ability of individuals with autism to produce internal cues that
aided new learning. Subsequent studies supported the view that failure to
produce organizational strategies was the key to the memory difficulty in
autism, and further, when demands on such cuing was minimal, as in the cases
of simple associative learning or short-term memory, the memory of individuals
with autism was as good as that of appropriate normal controls.
To confirm this view a study of memory involving a detailed investigation of
the California Verbal Learning Test was done by Minshew and Goldstein
(1993). Comparing high functioning subjects with autism with matched controls.
They looked for differences on the many scores that can be derived from this
procedure. Very few significant differences were found. Of the 33 measures
compared, significant differences were obtained for number correct on Trial 5
for List A, List A Total Intrusions, List B - Number Correct, Semantic-Cluster
Ratio and Global Cluster Ratio, and List A Short Delay - Total Intrusions. The
major findings therefore were that the subjects with autism produced more
intrusions than controls, and they did less semantic clustering than controls.
They were also more susceptible than controls to proactive interference. We
should add the cautionary note that when a Bonferroni correction for multiple
comparisons was made, all significant differences disappeared. An additional
analysis determined for each measure whether the mean score for the autistic
sample was better, worse, or tied with the control sample. Of the 33 scores,
there were only 3 on which the subjects with autism did better than the controls.
Using a sign test, it was determined that this result far exceeded chance. It was
also found that the learning curves for the 5 A List trials had similar shapes in
both groups, but the score was lower for each of the trials in the case of the
autism group. It was concluded that memory in autism is less efficient than
normal as indicated by analysis of the direction of the differences between
autistics and controls.
In subsequent research in the area of memory, differences from controls were
found on delayed recall tasks but not on associative memory tasks (Minshew,
Goldstein, and Siegel, 1997). Increasing difficulty with memory as task
complexity increased was also found in autism. For example, on a stylus maze
task, the impairment of a sample of individuals with autism relative to controls
154 CHAPTER 5
Deficits of Attention
1991). This model was based upon a series of distinctions among types of
attention originally formulated by Zubin (197S). In this model, attention is
divided into the ability to 1) focus on a target object and perform a task in the
presence of distracting objects, 2) maintain vigilance over a sustained time
period, 3) adaptively shift focus of attention, and 4) efficiently receive and
interpret incoming information. The factors derived from this model were
termed focus-execute, sustain, shift, and encode. The model was applied in the
Mirsky et al., study in order to clarify what components of attention might be
intact and impaired in autism.
In many tests of attention, speed of performance is used as the response
measure. Such tests load in the Mirsky group factor analysis on the Focus-
Execute factor. Indeed Mirsky et al., (1991) characterized that factor as
reflecting perceptual-motor speed. It has been well established that individuals
with autism commonly have psychomotor deficits in the form of slowness,
awkwardness, or poor coordination (Bauman, 1992; Gillberg and Coleman,
1992; Hughes, 1996, Smith and Bryson, 1994; Rapin, 1997). Thus, the direct
interpretation of tests involving speed scores as measures of attention is
confounded in autism by psychomotor impairment, and it is necessary to
account for the influence of that impairment on performance outcome.
This comprehensive analysis of attentional functioning in individuals with
carefully diagnosed high functioning autism demonstrated that the major
dysfunctions relative to normal controls are on those measures of attention that
utilize psychomotor speed, as opposed to accuracy or span of apprehension, as a
dependent measure, or that require cognitive flexibility. That is, differences
were noted only on the Focus-Execute and Shift factors of the Mirsky group
model, and not the Vigilance and Encode factors. Correspondingly, the Mirsky
group was unable to confirm the view that individuals with autism have
difficulties in encoding information and sustaining attention over time. This
finding is well supported by other studies in which subjects with autism
performed normally at repeating digits and calculating (Minshew, Goldstein,
Muenz, and Payton, 1992; Minshew, Goldstein, and Siegel, 1997). The
sustained attention or vigilance data indicate that when a challenging attentional
task does not have a conceptual or psychomotor component, the autism group
does not differ from controls.
The data would suggest that in the case of autism, unequivocal evaluation of
attention cannot be accomplished using dependent measures based on speed of
movement. We would offer the proposal that significant differences between
individuals with autism and controls may be found on experimental measures of
attention that actually assess such processes as conceptual reasoning, executive
function, rapid decision making, problem solving, and working memory;
abilities that are widely believed to be impaired in autism (McEvoy, Rogers, and
Pennington, 1993; Minshew, Goldstein, and Siegel, 1997; Ozonoff, 1995b;
Ozonoff et al., 1994, Ozonoff, Pennington, and Rogers, 1991). With regard to
HIGH FUNCTIONING AUTISM 157
the Mirsky model shift factor, there is evidence from other research indicating
that individuals with autism do not have difficulty with elementary perceptual
shift tasks, but do have difficulty when shifting must be accomplished at a
conceptual level. In a study of saccadic eye movements, subjects with autism
performed normally on visually guided saccade tasks involving shifting of focus
of attention. However, they performed abnormally on a volitional saccade task
in which the eyes must accurately move to the point at which the target stimulus
was previously present, but had disappeared. This latter task has a substantial
working memory component (Minshew, Luna, and Sweeney, 1999).
Furthermore, numerous other studies involving the WCST have produced
equivocal results, with several reports of normal functioning by autism samples
on several of the measures derived from this test (Minshew, Goldstein, and
Siegel, 1997, Ozonoff, 1995a). In the Mirsky group study, the autism group did
not differ from controls on a relatively simple perceptual shifting task, but made
significantly more perseverative errors on the WCST than controls. The Mirsky
group's subjects averaged 9 years of age, and had a mean WISC-III Full-scale IQ
score of 78. The group studied by Minshew et al., (1992) with the WCST
averaged 21 years of age, and had a mean WAIS-R Full Scale IQ score of 96.
This group of individuals with autism made considerably fewer perseverative
errors on the WCST (M = 18.3) than the Mirsky group's autism sample (M =
60.7) Considering the literature as a whole, including these results, differences
in various scores from the WCST between individuals with autism and normal
controls may be a matter of the presence of autism in combination with
developmental considerations and general ability level. Thus, evidence for
impaired shifting of attention in autism appears to be associated with working
memory and other aspects of complex information processing, and not with
perceptual shifting of focus of the type that may be mediated by cerebellar
function. Furthermore, this deficit, at least when measured by such procedures
as the WCST, may not appear in higher functioning individuals with autism.
These findings make it appear likely that the well established cognitive deficits
and their associated abnormal behaviors associated with autism are not the result
of a failure to incorporate information, or to sustain concentration, or to resist
distraction. Such considerations as working memory, the ability to organize
information, and the capacity to monitor ongoing events and make rapid
adjustments are likely to be relevant considerations.
as the measures. On Word Attack, the subjects with autism were actually a little
better than the controls, and this relationship persisted into the 18 years and
older group. The picture for Passage Comprehension was quite different. The
younger subjects with autism did better than or as well as controls up until the
10-11 year old group, after which the controls did consistently better, and
dramatically so in the 18 and older group. It would therefore appear that
individuals with high functioning autism might keep up with their peers
indefinitely with regard to mechanical or procedural linguistic tasks, but lose
ground as the cognitive demands of tasks increase as appropriate for normal
language development.
Upon combining formal language and psychoeducational tests in studies of
autism there appears to be a consistent pattern of absence of significant
differences on the simple tests including measures of verbal fluency, phonetic
analysis, word knowledge, and calculation, as contrasted with significant
differences on measures of language comprehension, pragmatic language, and
comprehension of complex grammatical structures. The role of working
memory may be important in conceptualizing these results, but the deficits noted
on several of the complex tests suggested a more pervasive impairment of
language comprehension than could be explained solely on the basis of a deficit
in working memory.
Thus, if one divides abstraction, as the literature suggests, into rule learning,
attribute identification, and hypothesis testing or true concept formation rather
than identification, then subjects with autism in the non-retarded range appear to
have their major difficulty in the hypothesis-testing component. Another way of
putting it is that they can identify concepts, but have difficulty in forming them.
It appears that individuals with high functioning autism have cognitive deficits
in various domains at the complex, but not at the simple skill level. That
conclusion applies to memory, attention, language, and abstraction and problem-
solving domains. Recent investigations suggest there does not appear to be
modality specificity. That is, vision or hearing is not specifically involved in the
cognitive function of individuals with autism. However, visuospatial abilities
appear to remain intact, for unknown reasons, and individuals with autism may
do exceptionally well on spatial and constructional tasks that do not use social
stimuli.
In order to suggest that this cognitive profile has some biological significance,
we can briefly mention a portion of a study in which cognitive test results were
correlated with Phosphorous Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy (MRS) data
(Minshew, Rogers, and Pennington, 1993). We will only comment on the
correlational data for phosphomonoesters (PME) and phosphodiesters (PDE),
both of which reflect phospholipid metabolism. The conclusion reached was
that phospholipid findings in autism reflect under synthesis and enhanced
degradation of brain membranes. There were very robust correlations between
cognitive test scores and PME and PDE levels. PDE goes up and PME goes
down as scores get worse. These high correlations were not seen in the normal
controls, nor were they seen in the subjects with autism for such basic skill tasks
as the Reading Decoding and Spelling subtests of the Kaufman Test of
Educational Achievement. Correlations with low information processing load
tasks of this type were non-significant in both autism and control groups. While
this study was very preliminary, there is nevertheless the suggestion that there is
an association between cognitive function and metabolic energy state of
phospholipids involved in high functioning autism.
Based on these results and a review of the now extensive literature on high
functioning autism, we would like to propose the following conclusions.
1. Autism represents a deficit state in complex information processing with
sparing of basic, simple cognitive processes.
2. The underlying cognitive deficits for autism are not specific to any particular
domain of cognitive function or to any particular modality. Autism is not
primarily a deficit in attention, memory, language, or executive function, but
involves all of these cognitive domains.
HIGH FUNCTIONING AUTISM 161
3. The most likely neurobiological basis for autism is in the form of a network
of numerous subcortical and cortical structures. The actual pathology may be at
the cellular level, reflecting some variation in dendritic architecture. The
preliminary magnetic resonance spectroscopy findings would support this view.
4. Correspondingly, autism is not a localized or lateralized disorder. It is a
disorder at the system level. While frontal, temporal-limbic, and subcortical
localizations have been suggested, none of these views has shown a consistent
relationship between the hypothesized localizations and the clinical
phenomenology of the disorder.
5. The specifics of the network remain unknown, but would appear to be
ultimately discoverable with more research and emerging technologies.
In autism, the brain has invented a disorder that does not have a neat, discrete
neurocognitive space. It doesn't seem to live anywhere. It isn't like ideational
apraxia or the WCST or other entities that appear to have found homes in the
frontal lobes or elsewhere. What this appears to suggest is that the brain can
code complexity. If it couldn't, it couldn't produce a disorder in which
complexity appears to be selectively impaired. Furthermore, in autism,
complexity is selectively impaired regardless of domain or modality. The
impetus to localize autism in the frontal lobes, or cerebellum, or hippocampus
appears to be based upon traditional views arising from modularity or
localization concepts. However, it is a disorder that can be more economically
explained in what have been described in the past as mass action,
equipotentiality, or holistic concepts. These early conceptualizations have
evolved into system or network theories in which localized functions are
integrated in the manner of Luria's functional systems. Since the brain can
produce a disorder like autism, it would appear to be able to process information
at a system level just as if it can produce a disorder like alexia without agraphia,
it can process information in a highly discrete, circumscribed way. It is not
necessary to return to the days when there was some belief that the brain always
functions as a unified whole. On the other hand, autism helps teach us that it is
not always necessary to understand cognitive function as representing some
specific domain or modality, or even some combination of them. There appears
to be the implicit belief that complex activities are based on the interaction of
localized structures that work together in a seamless way, giving rise to the
illusion of unified behavior. When a link in the system is disrupted, the
behavior becomes impaired. The problem in autism is that we have not been
able to find the disrupted link. It is not in attention, or memory, or language, or
executive function, but is in all of them. It therefore appears more like the
mischief is in the system itself, and not in some particular component. Perhaps
we need to know more about the emergent properties of systems as they apply to
brain function.
162 CHAPTER 5
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT
Abstract Reasoning
Memory
Attention
Perhaps one of the most widely observed but puzzling aspects of cognition in
high functioning autism is the intact, and often better than intact, spatial abilities
found in individuals with autism. They are good at building things, analyzing
visual material, and solving pictorial puzzles, and seem to enjoy doing those
things. The exception seems to be the task that has a social content, such as the
Picture Arrangement subtest of the Wechsler intelligence scales, where
comprehension of the social situation contributes to the solution. As indicated
previously, Block Design is typically the high score on the Wechsler scales, and
other constructional and copying tasks are usually done in a comparably normal
manner. However, when a spatial task is combined with a delayed recall task
mild impairment may be found, particularly if the spatial task is complex. For
example, a study has shown that individuals with high functioning autism do
less well than controls on delayed recall of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure.
It has been suggested that the network dysfunction that underlies autism is not of
the type that is thought to mediate spatial abilities, such as the pattern of cerebral
organization found primarily in the right cerebral hemisphere. Largely because
of the apparent communicative problems seen in autism, it was suggested in the
past that it was primarily a disorder of the left cerebral hemisphere (Hoffman
and Prior, 1982), but that view is not widely supported at present.
With regard to perceptual and motor skills, autism does not involve any
apparent physical disability with regard to any of the senses or the motor system.
However, there is a commonly observed motor awkwardness or dyskinesia often
documented by neuropsychological tests of dexterity. The typical pattern is
normal simple motor skill, such as finger tapping, with impaired dexterity as
assessed with psychomotor speed or pegboard tasks. A similar pattern exists for
perception such that basic sensory-perceptual skills are usually intact, but there
may be impairment at a more complex level as is assessed by such procedures as
fingertip number writing from the Halstead-Reitan Battery.
It has been reported that relatively pure tests of psychomotor speed can
discriminate better between individuals with autism and normal controls than
can tests in which the psychomotor component of the task is combined with a
challenging cognitive component. Thus, individuals with autism may do
significantly worse than controls on Part A of the Trail Making Test, which has
minimal cognitive demands, but not on Part B, which requires substantial
cognitive resources (Minshew et al., 1994). The documentation of impairments
of skilled planned action in autism is provided by an extensive experimental
literature (Smith and Bryson, 1994).
HIGH FUNCTIONING AUTISM 169
INTERVENTIONS
Preliminary Remarks
Reading
Mathematics
time for receiving instructions, with the opportunity for repeating the
instructions in the case of oral presentation.
Teachers should be sensitive to the inability of individuals with autism to
misunderstand or not comprehend slang, jokes, pop culture jargon, and
nonverbal communication through gesture or expression. It is generally
desirable to explain social conventions, rules, and standards in concrete terms.
It is always necessary to consider that a message may not have been understood,
and may require repetition, rephrasing, or concretization. Sarcasm and non-
verbal communication to praise, kid, or reprimand through tone of voice, facial
expression, or physical posture may well not be understood by the high
functioning individual with autism. Active, declarative, and affirmative speech
without use of transformations or complex phrasing is preferred. Alternatively,
the student with autism should not be expected to deliver complex messages to
others.
Memory
Early clinical accounts of autism noted an excellent memory for remote events,
often phenomenal rote memory for poems and names, and the precise
recollection of complex patterns and sequences (Kanner, 1943). As part of the
observation of preoccupation with sameness, clinicians noted the remarkable
capacity of individuals with autism to detect the slightest change in a familiar
environment, such as a room. Memory ability in autism is quite different from
that of individuals with learning disabilities. For example, in a study comparing
members of the two groups, the participants with autism demonstrated intact
performance on an auditory rote memory task while demographically matched
dyslexics demonstrated deficient rote memory (Rumsey and Hamburger, 1990).
Considering intact rote memory processes involving visual and verbal
modalities, the capacity for high functioning individuals with autism to benefit
from the use of semantic information to encode information, and the difficulty
using strategies to organize new information, several implications emerge for
instruction that would make such instruction different from what would be
appropriate for dyslexia. A strength-matched approach could exploit the intact
ability to form verbal associations, as demonstrated by good performance on
paired-associate learning tasks. This method of developing recall can be
adapted to curriculum content. For example, this procedure might work in
geography by associating the names, locations, natural resources, and capitals of
states; in history for learning the names of explorers and their discoveries, and in
science when learning equations for velocity and acceleration. It is therefore
possible that intact associative learning can be utilized through providing
students with autism with organized lists of materials rather than expecting them
to generate meaningfully related content and categories from text.
the behavior of other students which may overload thresholds for stimulation.
Brief time-out periods may be beneficial in enabling the student to become
comfortable subsequent to excessive stimulation. These considerations extend to
adults not in school but who are with other people at work or in social situations.
A low-key, stable, small group environment seems to be preferable.
Educating high functioning individuals with autism poses unique challenges.
Knowledge of the neuropsychological profile is valuable in this effort because it
contributes to understanding aptitude and educational interactions specific to the
disorder. We have presented some suggestions for educational intervention
consistent with the neuropsychological findings. It was not intended to evaluate
the use of specific teaching methodologies developed for students with autism
such as auditory training or facilitated communication, special curriculum such
as TEACCH (Reichler and Schopler, 1976), or technology and materials such as
computers. Some of these interventions remain controversial and are often only
appropriate for severely impaired students, while some may be considered for
use with high functioning students with autism. It should also be noted that
while the strategies developed for instruction and the classroom environment
emerge from research on neuropsychological functioning in autism, they are
presented only as possible interventions.
SUMMARY
cannot be based only on group results, as the individual may depart from the
performance of the group by a considerable degree.
As a final comment on intervention, possibly the optimal treatment program
for high functioning autism involves a multidimensional approach coordinated
by a case manager who has particular expertise in the area. The case manager
can coordinate medical intervention activities, particularly if there is an
associated illness such as epilepsy, teachers, parents, and clinicians who provide
specialized procedures, notably behavior therapy. Very often, the case manager
is the individual who is most prepared to deal with the frequent life crises
commonly occurring with individuals with autism because of episodes of
inappropriate behavior or exposure to an unanticipated stressor. Behavioral
management techniques in combination with instructional strategies may
provide an ideal program for maximizing learning for the high functioning
student with autism.
6
CONCLUDING THOUGHTS
179
180 CHAPTER 6
When we then look at continued problems with efficient reading and/or com-
prehension in the young adult population, interventions based upon strategies
developed with young children tend to remain in practice in many literacy pro-
grams, although work on metacognitive strategies has produced curricular offer-
ings at the high school level (e.g., the work coming out of Deshler’s group at the
University of Kansas). Other investigators continue to focus on the non-utility
of intelligence measures in the assessment of reading disorders in children and
adults, and in their singular focus on this issue, fail to appreciate the cognitive
processing information that such instruments provide to the trained clinician that
consequently contribute to treatment plans involving remediation and/or appro-
priate accommodations for young adults in academic or vocational settings.
Even more discipline-specific is work in the areas of written language and
math disorders in adults. Neuropsychology has tended to focus on acquired dis-
orders affecting language or math calculations following some sort of brain
trauma, while special educators are still not of one mind with respect to the exis-
tence of these disorders as stand alone conditions. Other educational researchers
focus entirely on spelling as the single manifestation of written language
production again in terms of early acquisition of skills. While the use of
manipulatives has been demonstrated to enhance the acquisition of basic
mathematical operations, there is little if any empirical research on effective
methods for teaching higher-level math functions even though these skill
subjects remain especially difficult for many college level students. However,
more recent work by investigators such as Geary (2000) give us hope that
studies incorporating neuroimaging and work done on semantic and
phonological memory systems may shed light on the cognitive underpinnings of
mathematical disorders, which have been postulated in the literature (Nolting,
1988), and ultimately on intervention strategies.
In contrast to the predominant discipline-specific nature of research in the
academically based disorders, work done in the areas of autism and attention
deficit disorder illustrate what can be accomplished when research and practice
are multidisciplinary in nature. The work of all of the major researchers in both
fields utilizes data coming from the areas of educational psychology, neuropsy-
chology, psychopharmacology, and neurology. The work of Minshew’s group at
the University of Pittsburgh and the Biederman group at the University of Mas-
sachusetts illustrate the rich knowledge base that can be made available to the
clinician who works with adults, although work with high functioning autistic
adults in still in the infancy stage. Particularly rich is the more recent focus on
psychopharmacology with the ADHD population including adults as the disor-
der has come to be recognized as a life-long condition much in the same way
that “LD” was acknowledged to affect the lives of adults twenty years ago. That
research taps behaviors in a variety of settings and has included measures spe-
cific to academic performance as well as working memory.
CONCLUDING THOUGHTS 181
As with the reading and written language disorders literature, the issue of sub-
typing remains dominant in the ADHD diagnostic literature. Subtyping studies
with reading disorders in children have produced numerous subclassifications,
but their utility in the classroom setting has never been demonstrated. Studies
involving subtyping in adults with have been rather simplistic and thus have not
proved to be particularly useful to clinicians. On the other hand, subtyping clas-
sifications such as those postulated in studies with individuals, adults and chil-
dren alike, with ADHD would appear to be more useful to the clinician as well
as fruitful areas of investigation (see the neurochemical mechanisms section of
the ADHD chapter).
The new kid on the block, NLD, suffers from neglect to some degree. While
the Rourke group has done seminal work in the field in terms of identification,
there is little empirical research involving a cross-disciplinary approach perhaps
as a consequence of the controversy that surrounds the diagnosis of a specific
right-hemisphere disorder. Work such as that on the neuroanatomical underpin-
nings of dyslexia or on neuroimaging studies including fMRIs are surprisingly
absent in the literature. Studies involving the adjustment of adults with NLD,
academically, or vocationally, would seem to be lumped together with LD out-
come studies in general, and thus we can say little about outcome. Future stud-
ies including the spectrum of NLDs (e.g., Asperger’s) may shed light on appro-
priate interventions in much the same way that the early work on high-
functioning autism is now doing. The work of Klin et.al (1995) with respect to
the similarities in neuropsychological functions between NLD and Asperger
Syndrome give us strong hints that interventions for the two groups may well be
compatible.
Several other concerns need to be raised in our overall understanding of the
spectrum of learning disorders in young adults. The first of these involves how
these young adults or their parents can afford to access the extensive battery that
we have advocated with respect to the diagnosis of ADHD in particular. Insur-
ance companies routinely deny the need for an evaluation process that includes
psychological or educational testing even though we know that the comorbidity
for learning disabilities in the population of individuals with ADHD is 60% or
more and the comorbidity for other psychiatric disorders including major de-
pression, anxiety disorders, or Obsessive Compulsive Disorder has a prevalence
rate of between 15% and 20%. Organizations such as C.H.A.D.D. have been
working for some time with other mental health organizations to achieve parity,
but until researchers and clinicians across a variety of disciplines acknowledge
the need for such evaluations, the consumer will be forced to bear the expense of
the labor-intensive process. If the young adult is still within a secondary school
system, some of the psychoeducational components of the evaluation may be
accessed through the school district as covered by special education law or Sec-
tion 504 of the Rehabilitation Act. With adults facing major academic or voca-
tional failures and who have financial need, partial support for an evaluation to
182 CHAPTER 6
establish eligibility status might be available through the state vocational reha-
bilitation agency. And while paying for a comprehensive evaluation is costly,
that fact alone does not diminish its clinical validity or utility in much the same
way that an MRI is costly but critical to clinical care for certain conditions in
medical practice.
Second, there is a real necessity for work directed at the psychological se-
quelae of these various disorders of learning on adults. While follow-up studies
report a series of negative outcomes in terms of psychosocial functioning for
heterogeneous groups of individuals diagnosed with learning disabilities and/or
ADHD during childhood, the clinical utility of these studies is questionable, as
we have discussed previously throughout these chapters, because the target
population is never clearly defined and what constitutes positive or negative
outcomes is often narrowly defined. Gerber’s (1992) work on reframing and the
ethnographic research reported by him and his colleagues come closest to pro-
viding therapists and counselors with some understanding of the synergistic ef-
fect of the learning disabilities and their implications for adult functioning. The
effectiveness of interventions such as coaching and directive therapy have yet to
be addressed in any comparative studies, and yet they are in the mainline of ap-
proaches used with adults who have ADHD. This is not surprising, however, as
it has only been within the very recent past that behavior modification ap-
proaches have been compared with psychopharmacological therapies in any
systematic way in the case of children with ADHD, although their use was dis-
cussed extensively in the child ADHD literature as a substitute for medication.
Third, it is critical for persons with learning disorders that the incorporation
of assistive technology into the academic arena and work place be given the
highest priority by clinicians and researchers. It is our strong belief that assis-
tive technology has the potential to transform human cognitive potential in ways
that were previously unimaginable. We have seen just the tip of the iceberg as
voice recognition, for example, has many bugs still to be worked out. Thus, it is
imperative that practitioners and others with particular knowledge about neuro-
psychological functioning and learning have input into the development of assis-
tive technology devices to assure their efficacy for persons with learning dis-
abilities.
Finally, it is our contention that processes to foster cross-professional collabo-
ration are needed in terms of both research and clinical practice. This can cer-
tainly be accomplished by organizations sponsoring national meetings and
workshops and by organizations collaborating at the national level to influence
policies that affect their constituent members across diagnostic categories. The
recent efforts at collaboration between C.H.A.D.D. and the national organization
of Community Mental Health Centers is an example of efforts to reach out for
mutual benefit and with a stronger voice to federal agencies and policy makers,
as is C.H.A.D.D.’s effort to engage and utilize a multidisciplinary professional
advisory board. The various professional journals might also reach out in spe-
CONCLUDING THOUGHTS 183
Aaron, P. G., Malatesha, J., & Williams, K. (1999). Not all reading disabilities are alike.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(2), 120-137.
Abbott, J. (1987). Accessing vocational education and vocational rehabilitation training and
employment programs. Paper presented at the ACLD International Conference,
San Antonio, TX.
Abbott, R., & Beminger, V. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among
developmental skills and writing skills in primary and intermediate grade writers.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 478-508.
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile.
Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1981). Behavioral problems and competencies
reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged 4 through 16.
Monographs of the Society for Research and Child Development, 46, 1-82.
Ackerman, P. T., Anhalt, J. M., Dykman, R. A., & Holcomb, P. J. (1986). Effortful
processing deficits in children with reading and/or attention disorders. Brain and
Cognition, 5, 22-40.
Ackerman, P. T., Anhalt, J. M., & Dykman, R. A. (1986). Arithmetic automatization failure in
children with attention and reading disorders: Associations and sequilae. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 19, 222-232.
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Adelman, H. S., & Taylor, L. (1986). The problems of definition and differentiation and the
need for a classification schema. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 19(9), 514-520.
Akshoomoff, N. S., & Courchesne, E. (1992). A new role for the cerebellum in cognitive
operations. Behavioral Neuroscience, 43, 613-622.
Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., & Strick, P. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally
segrated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of
Neuroscience, 9, 357-381.
Alster, E. H. (1997). The effects of extended time on algebra test scores for college
students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
30, 222-227.
Amen, A. J., Johnson, S., & Amen, D. G. (1996). A teenager's guide to ADD. Fairfield, CA:
MindWorks Press.
Amen, D. G. (1997). Windows into the A.D.D. mind: Understanding and treating attention
deficit disorders in the everyday lives of children, adolescents and adults.
Fairfield, CA: MindWorks Press.
Amen, D. G., & Goldman, B. (1998). Attention-Deficit Disorder: A guide for primary care
physicians. Primary Psychiatry, July.
Amen, D. G., Paldi, J. H., & Thisted, R. A. (1993). Brain SPECT imaging. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1080-1081.
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (2nd Ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
185
186 REFERENCES
Biederman, J., Faraone, S., Milberger, S., Curtis, S., Chen, L., & et al. (1996). Predictors of
persistence and remission of ADHD into adolescence: Results from a four-year
prospective follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 343-351.
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Keenan, K., Benjamin, J., Krifcher, B., & et al. (1992).
Further evidence for family-genetic risk factors in attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 728-738.
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Keenan, K., Knee, D., & Tsuang, M. T. (1990). Family-
genetic and psychosocial risk factors in DSM-III attention deficit disorder. Journal
of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 526-533.
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Mick, E., & Lapey, K. S. (1994).
Gender differences in a sample of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Psychiatry Research, 53, 13-29.
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Norman, D., Lapey, K. S., Mick, E.,
Krifcher Lehman, B., & Doyle, A. (1993). Patterns of psychiatric comorbidity,
cognition, and psychosocial functioning in adults with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. American Journa/ of Psychiatry, 150(12), 1792-1798.
Biederman, J., Milberger, S., Faraone, S. V., Kiely, K., & et al. (1995). Impact of adversity
on functioning and comorbidity in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
34, 1495-1503.
Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Sprich, S. (1991). Comorbidity of attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive anxiety, and other disorders.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 564-577.
Biederman, J., Russell, R., Soriano, J., Wozniak, J., & Faraone, S. V. (1998). Clinical
features of children with both ADHD and mania: Does ascertainment source
make a difference? Journal of Affective Disorders, 51, 101-112.
Biederman, J., & Spencer, T. (1999). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as a
noradrenergic disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 124-1242.
Biederman, J., Wilens, T., Mick, E., Milberger, S., Spencer, T. J., & Faraone, S. V. (1995).
Psychoactive substance use disorders in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD): Effects of ADHD and psychiatric comorbidity. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 152, 1652-1658.
Bigler, E. D. (1989). On the neuropsychology of suicide. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
22, 180-185.
Bigler, E. D. (1992). The neurobiology and neuropsychology of adult learning disorders.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25(8), 488-506.
Bigler, E. D., Lajiness-O'Neill, R., & Howes, N. L (1988). Technology in the assessment of
learning disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(1), 67-82.
Bird, H. R., Canino, G., Rubio-Stipec, M., Gould, M. s., Ribera, J., Sesman, M., & et al.
(1988). Estimates of the prevalence of childhood maladjustment in a community
survey in Puerto Rico. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 1120-1126.
Bird, H. R., Gould, M. S., & Staghezza, B. M. (1993). Patterns of diagnostic comorbidity in a
community sample of children aged 9 through 16 years. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 361-368.
Bireley, M. (1995). Crossover children. Reston, VA: Council for Exceptional Children.
Blackorby, J., & Wagner, M. M. (1997). The employment of outcomes of youth with learning
disabilities: A review of findings from the national longitudinal transition study of
special education students. In P. J. Gerber & D. S. Brown (Eds.), Learning
Disabilities and Employment. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Blalock, J. (1981). Persistent problems and concerns of young adults with learning
disabilities. In W. Cruickshank & A. Silver (Eds.), Bridges to tomorrow: The best
of ACLD (Vol. 2, pp. 35-55). Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.
190 REFERENCES
Blalock, J. (1982). Persistent auditory language deficits in adults with learning disabilities.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 15(10), 604-609.
Blalock, J. W. (1987). Persistent problems and concerns of young adults with learning
disabilities. In D. Johnson & J. W. Blalock (Eds.), Adults with learning disability
(pp. 35-55). New York: Grune & Stratton.
Blouin, A., Bornstein, R., & Trites, R. (1978). Teenage alcohol use among hyperactive
children: A five-year follow-up study. Journal Pediatric Psychology, 3, 188-194.
Boder, E. (1970). Developmental dyslexia: A new diagnostic approach based on the
identification of three subtypes. The Journal of School Health, 40, 289-290.
Boder, E. (1973). Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on three atypical
reading-spelling patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15,
663-687.
Boder, E., & Jarrico, S. (1982). The Boder Test of Reading-Spelling Patterns: A Diagnostic
Screening Test for Subtypes of Reading Disability . New York: Grune and
Stratton.
Bolden-Watson, C., & Richelson, E. (1993). Blockage by newly developed antidepressants
of biogenic amine uptake into rat brain synaptosomes. Life Science, 52, 1023-
1029.
Boreland, B. L., & Heckman, H. K. (1976). Hyperactive boys and their brothers. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 33, 669-675.
Boucher, J. (1981). Memory for recent events in autistic children. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 11, 293-302.
Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1989). Memory impairments and communication in relatively
stable autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 99-122.
Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some
similarities to the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 73-87.
Bourke, A. B., Strehorn, K. C., & Silver, P. (2000). Faculty members' provision of
instructional accommodations to students with LD. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 33, 26-32.
Bradley, C. (1937). The behavior of children receiving Benzedrine. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 94, 577-588.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of
reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 746-747.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read - a casual
connection. Nature, 310, 419-421.
Brady, S., & Shankweiler, D. (Eds.). (1991). Phonological Processes in Literacy: A Tribute
to Isabelle Y. Liberman. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Brandys, C. F., & Rourke, B. P. (1991). Differential memory abilities in reading- and
arithmetic-disabled children. In B. P. Rourke (Ed.), Neuropsychological validation
of learning disability subtypes (pp. 73-96). New York: Guilford Press.
Breen, M. J. (1989). Cognitive and behavioral differences in ADHD in boys and girls.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 711-716.
Brinckerhoff, L, Shaw, S., & McGuire, J. M. (1993). Promoting postsecondary education for
students with learning disabilities. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Brinckerhoff, L. C., Dempsesy, K. M., Jordan, C., Keiser, S. R., McGuire, J. M., Pompian,
N. W., & Russell, L. H. (1998). Guidelines for documentation of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescents and adults : Consortium on ADHD
Documentation.
Brinckerhoff, S. C., Shaw, S. F., & McGuire, J. M. (1996). Promoting access,
accommodations, and independence for college students with learning
disabilities. In J. R. Patton & E. A. Pollock (Eds.), Learning disabilities: The
challenge of adulthood. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Bronowski, J. (1967). Human and animal languages. To Honor Roman Jakobson. (Vol. 1).
The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton & Cox.
REFERENCES 191
Brook, S. L., & Bowler, D. M. (1992). Autism by another name? Semantic and pragmatic
impairments in children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22, 61-
81.
Brown, A. L. (1980). Metacognitive development and reading, in R. J. Spiro, B.Bruce, & W.
E. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Earlbaum.
Brown, D. Counseling and accommodating the student with learning disabilities.
Washington, DC: President's Committee on Employment of the Handicapped.
Brown, D., & Gerber, P. J. (1993). Employing people with learning disabilities. In P. J.
Gerber & H. B. Reiff (Eds.), Learning disabilities in adulthood: Persisting
problems and evolving Issues (pp. 194-203). Boston, MA: Andover.
Brown, J. I., Fishco, V. V., & Hanna, G. (1993). Nelson Denny Reading Test. Chicago, IL:
Riverside Publishing Company.
Brown, J. J., Bennett, J. M., & Hanna, G. (1981). The Nelson-Denny Reading Test .
Chicago, IL: Riverside Publishing Company.
Brown, T. E. (1996). Brown attention-deficit disorder scales. San Antonio, TX: The
Psychological Corporation.
Brown, V. L., Cronin, M. E., & McEntire, E. (1994). Test of mathematical abilities. Austin,
TX: Pro-Ed.
Bruck, M. (1986). Social and emotional adjustments of learning disabled children. In S.
Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive social, neuropsychological aspects of learning
disabilities (Vol. 1, pp. 361-380). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
Bruck, M. (1987). The adult outcomes of children with learning disabilities. Annals of
Dyslexia, 37, 252-263.
Bruck, M. (1990). Word recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia.
Developmental Psychology, 26(439-454).
Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexia's phonological awareness deficits. Developmental
Psychology, 28(5), 874-886.
Bruck, M. (1993). Component spelling skills of college students with childhood diagnosis of
dyslexia. Learning Disability Quarterly, 26, 171-184.
Bruck, M., & Waters, G. (1988). An analysis of the spelling errors of children who differ in
their reading and spelling skills. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 77-92.
Bryson, S. E., Wainright-Sharp, J. A., & Smith, I. M. (1990). Autism: A developmental
spatial neglect syndrome? In J. Enns (Ed.), The development of attention:
Research and theory (pp. 405-427). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Buchanan, M., & Wolf, J. S. (1986). A comprehensive study of learning disabled adults.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 19, 34-38.
Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: Preliminary
evidence of an inefficient lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 535-543.
Burack, J. A., Enns, J. T., & Johannes, E. A. (1997). Attention and autism: Behavioral and
Electrophysiological Evidence. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook
of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 226-247). New
York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Burack, J. A., & larocci, G. (1995). Visual filtering and covert orienting in autism. Paper
presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.
Burenstein, B. (1995). Giving voice to student writing: Exploring the uses of speech
recognition and speech synthesis in a writing curriculum: Activities for word
processing (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED395110).
Bursuck, W. D., Rose, E., Cowens, & Yamaya, M. A. (1989). Nationwide survey of
postsecondary education services for students with learning disabilities.
Exceptional Children, 56, 236-245.
Buschsbaum, M. S., Haler, R. J., Sostek, A. J., Weingertner, H., & et al. (1985). Attention
dysfunction and psychopathology in college men. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 42, 354-360.
192 REFERENCES
Cohen, J. (1957). Factor analytically based rationale for Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 451-457.
Cohen, R. M., Sample, W. E., Gross, M., & et al. (1988). Functional localization of
sustained attention: Comparison to sensory stimulation in the absence of
instruction. Neuropsych Neuropsychol Behav Neurol, 1, 3-20.
Coldman-Rakic, P. A. (1987). Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of
behavior by representational memory. In F. Plum (Ed.), handbook of physiology,
the nervous system, higher functions of the brain (pp. 373-417). Bethesda, MD:
American Physiological Society.
Coleman, S., & Sussman, S. (1996). The four S's: A comprehensive program for coaching
people with ADD. .
Coles, G. (1978). The learning-disabilities test battery: Empirical and social issues. Harvard
Educational Review, 48, 313-340.
Coles, G. (1987). The learning mystique. New York: Pantheon Books.
Comings, D. E., & Comings, B. G. (1988). Tourette's syndrome and attention deficit
disorder. In D. J. Cohen, R. D. Bruun, & J. F. Leckman (Eds.), Tourette's
syndrome and tic disorders: Clinical understanding and treatment (pp. 120-135).
New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Comings, D. E., & Comings, B. G. (1990). A controlled family history of Tourette's
Syndrome, I: Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and teaming disorders.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 51(7), 275-280.
Comings, D. E., Wu, S., Chiu, C., Ring, R. H., Gade, R., & et al. (1996). Polygenic
inheritance of Tourette syndrome, stuttering, attention deficit hyperactivity,
conduct and oppositional defiant disorder the additive and subtractive effect of
the three dopaminergic genes -- DRD2, DbH, and DAT1. American Journal of
Medical Genetics, 67, 264-288.
Connors, K. C., Erhrdt, D., & Sparrow, E. (1999). Conners' adult ADHD rating scales
(CAARS). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Cook, E. H., Stein, M. A., Krasowski, M. D., Cox, N. J., Olkon, D. M., Kieffer, J. E., &
Leventhal, B. L. (1995). Association of Attention-Deficit disorder and the
dopamine transporter gene. American Journal of Human Genetics, 56, 993-998.
Copeland, E. D., & Copps, S. C. (1995). Medications for attention disorders and related
medical problems. Plantation, FL: Specialty Press, Inc.
Corbett, B., & Stanczak, D. E. (1999). Neuropsychological performance of adults
evidencing attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 14, 373-387.
Corcos, E., & Willows, D. M. (1993). The processing of orthographic information. In D. M.
Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading
disabilities (pp. 163-190). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum Associates, Inc.
Courchesne, E., Akshoomoff, N. A., Townsend, J., & Saitoh, O. (1995). A model system for
the study of attention and the cerebellum: Infantile autism.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology - Supplement, 44, 315-
352.
Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R.,
Lincoln, A. J., James, H. E., Haas, R. H., & Lau, L. (1994). Impairment in shifting
attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 848-
865.
Courchesne, E., Townsend, J. P., Akshoomoff, N. A., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Press, G. A.,
Murakmi, J. W., Lincoln, A. J., James, H. E., Saitoh, O., Egaas, B., Haas, R. H.,
& Schriebman, L. (1993). A new finding: Impairment in shifting of attention in
autistic and cerebellar patients. In S. H. Broman & J. Grafman (Eds.), Atypical
deficits in developmental disorders: Implications for brain function. New Jersey:
Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
194 REFERENCES
Dooling-Litfin, J. K., & Rosen, L. A. (1997). Self-esteem in college students with a childhood
history of attention deficit hyperactivtty disorder. Journal of College Student
Psychotherapy, 11, 69-83.
Douglas, V. I. (1972). Stop, look, and listen: The problem of sustained attention and
impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. Canadian Journal of
Behavioral Science, 4, 259-282.
Douglas, V. I. (1983). Attention and cognitive problems. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental
neuropsychiatry (pp. 282-329). New York: Guilford Press.
Dowdy, C. A., Smith, T. E. C., & Noewll, C. H. (1996). Learning disabilities and vocational
rehabilitation. In J. R. Patton & E. A. Pollock (Eds.), Learning disabilities: The
challenge of adulthood (pp. 61-70). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Dowdy, C. A., Smith, T. E. C., & Nowell, C. H. (1992). Learning disabilities and vocational
rehabilitation. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25(7), 442-447.
Downey, K. K., Stelson, F. W., Pomerleau, O. F., & Giordani, B. (1997). Adult attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder: Psychological test profiles in a clinical population.
Journal Nerv. Ment. Dis., 185, 32-38.
Drewe, E. A. (1975). Go-no learning after frontal lobe lesions in humans. Cortex, 11, 8-16.
Duane, D. D. (1983). Neurobiological correlates of reading disorders. Journal of
Educational Research, 77(1), 6-15.
Duane, D. D. (1991). Biological foundations of learning disabilities. In J. E. Obrzut & G. W.
Hynd (Eds.), Neuropsychological foundations of learning disabilities (pp. 7-27).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dunham, M. D., Multon, K. D., & Koller, J. R. (1999). A comparison of adult learning
disability subtypes in the vocational rehabilitation system. Rehabilitation
Psychology, 44, 249-265.
Dykman, R. A., & Ackerman, P. T. (1993). Behavioral subtypes of attention deficit disorder.
Exceptional Children, 60(2), 132-141.
Easton, J., & Sherman, D. (1976). Somatic anxiety attacks and propranolol. Arch Neurol,
33, 689-691.
Ebstein, R. P., Novick, O., Umansky, R., & et al. (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR)
exon III poly-morphism associated with the human personality trait of novelty
seeking. Nature Genet, 12, 78-80.
Eden, G. F., VanMeter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J. M., Woods, R. P., & Zeffiro, T. A.
(1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional
brain imaging. Nature, 382, 66-69.
Educational Training Service (ETS). (1998). Policy Statement for Documentation of
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Adolescents and Adults. Office of
Disability Policy Educational Testing Service. Princeton, NJ.
Ehri, L. C. (1980). The role of orthographic images in learning printed words. In J. F.
Kavanagh & R. L. Venezky (Eds.), Orthography, reading, and dyslexia (pp. 155-
170). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
Eicheler, A. J., & Antelman, S. M. (1979). Sensitization to amphetamine and stress may
involve nucleus accumbens and medial frontal cortex. Brain Research, 176, 412-
416.
Elbert, J. C., & Seale, T. W. (1988). Complexity of the cognitive phenotype of an inherited
form of learning disability. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 30,
181-189.
Elbro, C., Nielsen, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: Evidence for deficits in
non-word reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items. Annals
of Dyslexia, 44, 205-227.
Elkind, J. (1998). Computer reading machines for poor readers. Perspectives, 24(2), 2-7.
Eme, R. F. (1992). Selective female affliction in development of disorders of childhood: A
literature review. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 354-364.
196 REFERENCES
Emily Hall Tremaine Foudation. (1995). Learning disabilities and the American public: A
look at Americans’ awareness and knowledge. Washington, DC: Author.
Englert, C. S., & Mariage, T. V. (1991). Shared understandings: Structuring the writing
experience through dialogue. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24(6), 330-342.
Enns, J. T., & Akhtar, N. (1989). A developmental study of filtering in visual attention. Child
Development, 60, 319-337.
Eno, L., & Woehlke, P. (1980). Diagnostic differences between educationally handicapped
and learning disabled students. Psychology to the Schools, 17, 469-473.
Ernst, M., Liebenauer, L. L., Jons, P. H., King, A. C., Cohen, M. D., & Zametkin, A. J.
(1994, October). Sexual maturation and brain metabolism in ADHD and brain
metabolism in ADHD and normal girls. Paper presented at the 41st Annual
Meeting of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, New York.
Ernst, M., Liebenauer, L. L., King, C., Fitzgerald, G. A., Cohen, R. M., & Zametkin, A. J.
(1994). Reduced brain metabolism In hyperactive girls. Journal American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 858-868.
Faas, L. A., & D'Alonzo, B. J. (1990). WAIS-R scores as predictors of employment success
and failure among adults with leaming disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 23, 311-316.
Faigel, H. C. (1998). Changes in services for students with learning disabilities in the US.
and Canadian medical schools, 1991 to 1997. Academic Medicine, 73, 1290-
1293.
Faraone, S., & Beiderman, J. (1946b). Is attention deficit hyperactivity disorder familial?
Harvard Review Psychiatry, 1, 271-287.
Faraone, S., & Biederman, J. (1946a). Genetics of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Child Adoles Psychiatric Clin North America, 3, 285-302.
Faraone, S. V., Beiderman, J., Lehman, B. K., Keenan, K., & et al. (1993). Evidence of the
independent familial transmission of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and
learning disabilities: Results from a family genetic study. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 150, 891-895.
Faraone, S. V., & Biederman, J. (1997). Do attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and
major depression share familial risk factors? Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 185(9), 533-541.
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Menin, D., Wozniak, J., & Spencer, T. (1997). Attention-
Deficit Hyperactivity disorder with Bipolar disorder: A familial subtype? Journal
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(10), 1378-1387.
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Mennin, D., & Russell, R. (1997). Bipolar and antisocial
disorders among relatives of ADHD children: Parsing familial subtypes of illness.
American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 81, 108-116.
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Mennin, D., Russell, R., & Tsuang, M. T. (1998). Familial
subtypes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A 4-year follow-up study of
children from antisocial-ADHD families. Journal of Child Psychology Psychiatry,
39(7), 1045-1053.
Fargason, R. E., & Ford, C. V. (1994). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults:
diagnosis, treatment, and prognosis. Southern Medical Journal, 87(3), 302-309.
Feagans, L. V. (1991). Preface. In L. V. Feagans, E. J. Short, & L. J. Meltzer (Eds.),
Subtypes of learning disabilities: Theoretical perspectives and research (pp. iv-
ix). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Feagans, L. V., & McKinney, J. D. (1991). Subtypes of learning disabilities: A review. In L.
V. Feagans, E. J. Short, & L. Meltzer (Eds.), Subtypes of learning disabilities:
Theoretical perspectives and research (pp. 3-31). New Jersey: Earlbaum &
Associates.
Federal Register. (1977, Thursday, December 29). 65082-65085. Washington, DC.
Felton, R. H., Naylor, C. E., & Wood, F. B. (1990). Neuropsychological profile of adult
dyslexics. Brain and Language, 39, 485-497.
REFERENCES 197
Fibiger, H. C., & Phillips, A. G. (1988). Mesocorticolimic dopamine systems and reward.
Ann NY Academy of Science, 537, 206-215.
Filpek, P. A., Samrud-Clikeman, M., Steingard, R. J., Renshaw, P. F., & et al. (1997).
Volumetreic MRI analysis comparing subjects having attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder with normal controls. Neurology, 48, 589-601.
Finucci. J. M., Guthrie, J. T., Childs, A. L., Abbey, H., & Childs, B. (1976). The genetics of
specific reading disability. Annals of Human Genetics, 40, 1-23.
Finucci, J. M., Whitehouse, C. C., Isaacs, S. D., & Childs, B. (1984). Derivation and
validation of a quantitative definition of specific reading disability for adults.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 26, 143-153.
Fisher, B. C. (1998). Attention Deficit Disorder Misdiagnosis. Boston, MA: CRC Press.
Fleischner, J. E., Garnett, K., & Shepherd, M. (1982). Proficiency in arithmetic basic fact
computation by learning disabled and nondisabled children. Focus on Learning
Problems in Mathematics, 4, 47-55.
Fletcher, J. M. (1985). External validation of learning disability subtypes. In B. P. Rourke
(Ed.), Neuropsychology of learning disabilities: Essentials of subtypal analysis.
New York: Guilford Press.
Fletcher, J. M. (1989). Nonverbal learning disabilities and suicide: Classification leads to
prevention. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 176,179.
Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (in press). Conceptual
and methodological issues in dyslexia research: A lesson for developmental
disorders. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of developmental
disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Rourke, B. P., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1992). The
validity of discrepancy-based definitions of reading disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 25(9), 555-573.
Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Rourke, B. P., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1993).
Classification of learning disabilities: Relationships with other childhood
disorders. In G. R. Lyon, D. B. Gray, J. F. Kavanagh, & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.),
Better understanding learning disabilities: New views from research and their
implications for education and public policies (pp. 27-55). Baltimore, MD: Paul H.
Brookes Publishing Co.
Fletcher, J. M., ShaywItz, S. E., Shankwiler, D., Katz, L., Liberman, I., Stuebing, K.,
Francis, D. J., Fowler, A., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading
disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 86, 6-23.
Flynn, J. M., & Deering, W. M. (1989). Subtypes of dyslexia: Investigation of Boder's
system using quantitative neurophysiology. Developmental Medicine and Child
Neurology, 31, 215-223.
Foorman, B., Francis, D., Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., & Fletcher, J. (1997). The case for
early reading intervention. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading
acquisition (pp. 243-264). Hillssdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Foss, J. M. (1991). Nonverbal learning disabilities and remedial interventions. Annals of
Dyslexia, 41, 128-140.
Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and
dyslexic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 329-342.
Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Mathes, P. H., & Simmons, D. C. (1997). Peer-assisted strategies:
Making classrooms more responsive to diversity. American Educational
Research Journal, 34, 174-206.
Gadbow, N. F., & DuBois, D. A. (1998). Adult Learners with Special Needs. Malabar, FL:
Krieger Publishing.
Gainetdinov, R. R., Wetsel, W. C., Jones, S. R., & et al. (1999). Rote of serotonin in the
paradoxical calming effect of psychostimulants on hyperactivity. Science,
28(283), 397-401.
198 REFERENCES
Gajar, A. (1996). Current and future research priorities. In J. R. Patton & E. A. Pollock
(Eds.), Learning disabilities: The challenge of adulthood (pp. 185-203). Austin,
TX: Pro-Ed.
Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., Aboitz, F., & Geschwind, N. (1985).
Developmental dyslexia: Four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies.
Annals of Neurology, 18, 222-233.
Gander, M., & Shea, L. C. (1998). Rethinking the writing classroom: Meeting the needs of
diverse learners. In P. L. Dwinell & J. Higbee (Eds.), Developmental education:
Meeting diverse student needs. Morrow, GA: National Association of
Developmental Education.
Gardiner, S., & Schwean, V.L. (1998). The role of protective mechanisms in resilient
adults with AD/HD. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Garfinkel, B. D. (1986). Recent developments in attention deficit disorder. Psychiatric
Annals, 16, 11-15.
Gaub, M., & Carlson, C. (1997). Gender differences in ADHD: A meta-analysis and critical
review. Journal of American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(8),
1036-1044.
Geary, D. C. (1990). A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 363-383.
Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic
components. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 345-362.
Geary, D. C. (2000). Mathematical disorders: An overview for educators. Perspectives, The
International Dyslexia Association, 26(3), 6-9.
Geary, D. C., Hamson, C. O., & Hoard, M. K. (in press). Numerical and arithmetical
cognition: A longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with
learning disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.
Gelenberg, A. J., Bassuk, E. L., & Schoonover, S. C. (1991). The practitioner's guide to
psychoactive drugs. New York: Plenum Medical Book Company.
Gerber, P. J. (1997). Life after school: Challenges in the workplace. In P. J. Gerber & D. S.
Brown (Eds.), Learning Disabilities and Employment. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Gerber, P. J., Ginsberg, R., & Reiff, H. B. (1992). Identifying patterns in employment
success for highly successful adults with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 25, 475-487.
Gerber, P. J., & Reiff, H. (Eds.). (1993). Learning disabilities in adults. Stoneham, MA:
Butterworth-Heinemann.
Gerber, P. J., & Reiff, H. (Eds.). (1994). Learning disabilities in adulthood: Persisting
problems and evolving issues. Stoneham, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Gerber, P. J., & Reiff, H. B. (1991). Speaking for themselves: Ethnographic interviews with
adults with learning disabilities. Ann Arbor, Ml: The University of Michigan Press.
Gerber, P. J., Reiff, H. B., & Ginsberg, R. (1996). Refraining the learning disabilities
experience. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29(1), 97-101.
Gersten, R. (1998). Recent advances in instructional research for students with learning
disabilities: An overview. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 13(3), 162-
170.
Gerstman, J. (1940). Syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for right and left, agraphia
and acalculia. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 44, 398-408.
Geschwind, N. (1982). Why Orton was right. Annals of Dyslexia, 32, 13-30.
Geschwind, N. (1985). Dyslexia in neurological perspective. In F. H. Duffy & N. Geschwind
(Eds.), Dyslexia: A Neuroscientific Approach to Clinical Evaluation. Boston, MA:
Little, Brown & Co.
Geschwind, N., & Behan, P. O. (1982). Left-handedness: Association with immune disease,
migraine, and developmental learning disorder. Paper presented at the
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
REFERENCES 199
Goldstein, G., Katz, L., Slomka, G., & Kelley, M. A. (1993). Relationships among academic,
neuropsychological, and intellectual status in subtypes of adults with learning
disability. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, 41-53.
Goldstein, G., Katz, L., Slomka, G., & Kelly, M. A. (1993). Relationships among academic,
neuropsychological, and intellectual status in subtypes of adults with learning
disability. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, 41-53.
Goldstein, G., Minshew, N. J., & Siegel, D. J. (1994). Age differences in academic
achievement in high-functioning autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 671-680.
Goldstein, G., Shelly, C., McCue, M., & Kane, R. L. (1987). Classification with the Luria-
Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery: An application of luster and ipsative
profile analysis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 215-235.
Goldstein, K., & Scheerer, M. (1941). Abstract and concrete behavior: An experimental
study with special tests. Psychological Monographs, 53(2), 1-151.
Goldstein, S. (1997). Managing attention and learning disorders in late adolescence and
adulthood. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Goodman, R., & Stevenson, J. (1989). A twin study of hyperactivity: I. An examination of
hyperactivity scores and categories derived from Rutter teacher and parent
questionnaires. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 671-589.
Gottardo, A., Siegel, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). The assessment of adults win
reading disabilities: What can we learn from experimental tasks? Journal of
Research in Reading, 20, 42-54.
Gottesman, R. L. (1994). The adult with learning disabilities: An overview. Learning
Disabilities, 5(1), 1-14.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., MacArthur, C. A., & Schwartz, S. (1991). Writing and writing
instruction for students with learning disabilities: Review of a research program.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 14(2), 89-114.
Graham, S., Schwartz, S. S., & MacArthur, C. A. (1993). Knowledge of writing and the
composing process, attitude toward writing, and self-efficacy for students with
and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 237-249.
Green, M. F., Satz, P., Ganzell, S., & Vaclav, J. F. (1992). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
performance in schizophrenia: Remediation of a stubborn deficit. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 62-67.
Greenbaum, B., Graham, S., & Scales, W. (1996). Adults with learning disabilities:
Occupational and social status after college. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29,
167-173.
Greene, G. (1999). Mnemonic multiplication fact instruction for students with learning
disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 14, 141-148.
Greenhill, L. L., & Osman, B. B. (Eds.). (1991). Ritalin Theory and Patient Management.
Larchmont, NY: Mary Ann Liebert.
Gregg, N. (1995). Eligibility for learning disabilities rehabilitation services: Operationalizing
the definition. Journal of Vocational Rehabilitation, 4, 86-95.
Gregg, N., Scott, S., McPeek, D., & Ferry, B. (1999). Definitions and eligibility criteria
applied to the adolescent and adult population with learning disabilities across
agencies. Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 213-223.
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1989). Social skills deficits as a primary learning disability.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 120-124.
Grodzinsky, G., & Diamond, R. (1992). Frontal lobe functioning in boys with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 427-445.
Gronwall, D. M. A. (1977). Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task: A measure of recovery
from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 367-373.
Gross-Glenn, K., Duara, R., Barker, W. W., Loewenstein, D., Chang, J. Y., Yoshii, F.,
Apicella, A. M., Pascal, S., Boothe, T., Sevush, S., Jallad, B. J., Novoa, L., &
Lubs, H. A. (1991). Positron emission tomographic studies during serial word-
REFERENCES 201
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin
Card Sort Test Manual: Revised and Expanded. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
Hebert, B. M., & Murdock, J. Y. (1994). Comparing three computer-aided instruction output
modes to teach vocabulary words to students with learning disabilities. Learning
Disabilities Research & Practice, 9(3), 136-141.
Hecaen, H., Angelergues, R., & Houillier, S. (1961). Les varieties cliniques des acalculies
au cors des lesions retrorolandiques: Approche statistique du probleme [The
clinical varieties of the acalculies in retrorolandic lesions: A statistical approach to
the problem]. Revue Neurologique, 105, 85-103.
Hechtman, L. (1989). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adolescence and adulthood:
An updated follow-up. Psychiatric Annals, 19, 597-603.
Hechtman, L., Weiss, G., Perlman, T., & et al. (1981). Hyperactives as young adults:
Various clinical outcomes. Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, 295-306.
Hechtman, L., Weiss, G., Perlman, T., Hopkins, J., & Wener, A. (1981). Hyperactives as
young adults: Prospective ten-year follow-up. In K. D. Gadow & J. Loney (Eds.),
Psychosocial aspects of drug treatment for hyperactivity (pp. 417-442). Boulder,
CO: Westview Press, Inc.
Heilman, K. M., Voeller, K. K. S., & Nadeau, S. E. (1991). A possible pathophysiological
substrate of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology,
6(Supplement), S74-S79.
Helfgott, D. & Westhaver, M. (1997). Inspiration Software. , Portland, OR.
Henderson, C. (1999). College Freshmen with Disabilities. A triennial statistical profile:
American Council on Education, HEATH Resource Center.
Henningfield, J. E., Clayton, R., & Pollin, W. (1990). Involvement of tobacco in alcoholism
and illicit drug use. British Journal of Addictions, 85, 279-291.
Higgins, E. L, & Raskind, M. H. (1995). Compensatory effectiveness of speech recognition
on the written composition performance of postsecondary students with learning
disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 18, 159-174.
Himelstein, J., Schulz, K. P., Newcorn, J. H., & Halperin, J. M. (2000). The neurobiology of
Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Frontiers in Biosciences, 5, 461-478.
Hinchley, J., & Levy, B. A. (1988). Developmental and individual differences in reading
comprehension. Cognitive Instruction, 5, 3-47.
Hiscock, M., & Hiscock, C. K. (1991). On the relevance of neuropsychological data to
learning disabilities. In J. E. Obrzut & G. W. Hynd (Eds.), Neuropsychological
foundations of learning disabilities (pp. 743-774). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Hoffman, F. J., Sheldon, K. L, Minskoff, E. H., Sautter, S. W., Steidle, E. F., Baker, D. Pi.,
Bailey, M. B., & Echols, L. D. (1987). Needs of learning disabled students.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 43-52.
Hoffman, W. L., & Prior, M. R. (1982). Neuropsychological dimensions of autism in children:
A test of the hemispheric dysfunction hypothesis. Journal of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 4, 27-41.
Holdanack, J. A., Moberg, P. J., Arnold, S. E., & et al. (1995). Speed of processing and
verbal learning deficits in adults diagnosed with attention deficit disorder.
Neuorpsychiatry Neuropsychology Behavioral Neurology, 8, 282-292.
Holliday, G. A., Koller, J. R., & Thomas, C. D. (1999). Post-high school outcomes of high IQ
adults with learning disabilities. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 22, 266-
281.
Hooper, S. R., Montgomery, J., Schwartz, D., Reed, M., Sandler, A., Levine, M., & Watson,
T. (1994). Measurement of written language expression. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.),
Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views an
measurement issues (pp. 375-418). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
REFERENCES 203
Hooper, S. R., & Olley, J. G. (1996). Psychological comorbidity in adults with learning
disabilities. In N. Gregg, C. Hoy, & A. F. Gay (Eds.), Adults with learning
disabilities (pp. 162-183). New York: Guilford Press.
Hooper, S. R., & Willis, W. G. (1989). Learning disability subtyping: Neuropsychological
foundations, conceptual models, and issues in clinical differentiation. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Horn, J. L., O'Donnell, J. P., & Leicht, D. J. (1988). Phonetically inaccurate spelling among
learning-disabled, head-injured, and nondisabled young adults. Brain and
Language, 33, 55-69.
Horn, W. F., O'Donnell, J. P., & Vitulano, L. A. (1983). Long-term follow-up studies of
learning-disabled persons. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 16(9), 542-555.
Horn, W. F., Wagner, A. E., & lalongo, N. (1989). Sex differences in school-aged children
with pervasive attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 17,109-125.
Hornig, M. (1998). Addressing comorbidity in adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder. Clinical Psychiatry, 59,69-75.
Houck, C. K., & Billingsley, B. S. (1989). Written expression of students with and without
learning disabilities: Differences across the grades. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 22, 561, 567, 572.
Hoy, C., Gregg, N., Wisenbaker, J., Manglitz, E., King, M., & Moreland, C. (1997).
Depression and anxiety in two groups of adults with learning disabilities. Learning
Disability Quarterly, 20, 280-291.
Hughes, C. (1996). Brief report: Planning problems in autism at the level of motor control.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 99-107.
Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children's difficulty with mental disengagement
from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology,
29, 498-519.
Hunt, R. D. (1997). Nosology, neurobiology, and clinical patterns of ADHD in adults.
Psychiatric Annals, 27, 572-581.
Hunt, R. D., Arnsten, A. F. T., & Asbell, M. D. (1995). An open trial of Guanfacine in the
treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(1), 50-54.
Hunt, R. D., Capper, L, & O'Connell, P. (1990). Clonidine in child and adolescent
psychiatry. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 1(1), 87-102.
Hutt, C., Hutt, S. J., Lee, D., & Ounsted, C. (1964). Arousal and childhood autism. Nature,
908-909.
Hynd, G. W., Hern, K. L., Novey, E. S., Eliopolus, D., & et al. (1993). Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and asymmetry of the caudate nucleus. Journal of
Child Neurology, 8, 339-347.
Hynd, G. W., Lorys, A. R., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Nieves, N., Huettner, M. I. S., & Lahey, B.
B. (1991). Attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity: A distinct behavioral
and neurocognitive syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 6(Supplement), S36-
S43.
Hynd, G. W., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Lorys, A. R., Novey, E. S., & Eliopulos, D. (1990).
Brain morphology in developmental dyslexia and Attention Deficit
Disorder/Hyperactivity. Archives of Neurology, 47, 916-919.
Hynd, G. W., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Lorys, A. R., Novey, E. S., Eliopulos, D., & Lyytinen,
H. (1991). Corpus callosum morphology in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity
Disorder Morphometric analysis of MRI. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24,141-
155.
lalongo, N. S., Horn, W. F., Pascoe, J. M., Greenberg, G., & et al. (1993). The effects of a
multlimodal intervention with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder children: A 9-
month follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 32,182-189.
204 REFERENCES
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA). (1990). P.L. 101-476,20 U.S.C. 1400 at
seq.
Inge, K. J., & Tilson, G. P. (1993). Supported employment: Issues and applications for
individuals with learning disabilities. In P. J. Gerber & H. B. Reiff (Eds.), Learning
disabilities in adulthood: Persisting problems and evolving Issues (pp. 179-193).
Boston, MA: Andover.
Interagency Committee on Learning Disabilities. (1987). Learning disabilities: A report to
the U.S. Congress. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.
Jaffa, S. L. (1989). Pemoline and liver function. Journal of the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 457-458.
Jastak, J. F., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). The Wide Range Achievement Test - Revised.
Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.
Jenkins, M., Cohe, R., Malloy, P., & et al. (1998). Neuropsychological measures which
discriminate among adults with residual symptoms of attention deficit disorder
and other attentional complaints. Clinical Neuropsychology, 12, 74-83.
Jensen, J. B., Burke, N., & Garfinkel, B. D. (1988). Depression and symptoms of attention
deficit disorder with hyperactivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child arid
Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 742-747.
Jensen, P. S., Kettle, L., Roper, M. T., & et al. (1999). Are stimulants overprescribed?
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 797-
804.
Johnson, D. (1994). Clinical study of adults with severe learning disabilities. Learning
Disabilities: A Multidisplinary Journal, 5(1), 43-50.
Johnson, D. J., & Blalock, J. W. (1987). Adults With Learning Disabilities: Clinical Studies.
New York: Grune & Stratton.
Johnson, D. J., & Myklebust, H. R. (1967). Learning Disabilities: Educational Principles and
Practices. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Johnson, D. J., & Myklebust, H. R. (1971). Learning disabilities. New York: Grune and
Stratton.
Jones, B. F. (1988). Text learning strategy instruction: Guidelines from theory and practice.
In C. E. Weinstein, E. T. Goetz, & P. A. Alexander (Eds.), Learning and study
strategies, issues in assessment, instruction, and evaluation. New York:
Academic Press.
Jones, E. D., Wilson, R., & Bhojwani, S. (1997). Mathematics instruction for secondary
students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30,151-163.
Jones, E. D., Wilson, R., & Bhojwani, S. (1998). Mathematics instruction for secondary
students with learning disabilities. In D. P. Rivera (Ed.), Mathematics education
for students with learning disabilities (pp. 155-176). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Jordan, N. C., & Montani, T. O. (1997). Cognitive arithmetic and problem solving: A
comparison of children with specific and general mathematics difficulties. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 30, 624-634.
Judd, T. P., & Bilsky, L H. (1989). Comprehension and memory in the solution of verbal
arithmetic problems by mentally retarded and nonretarded individuals. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 81, 541-546.
Jueptner, M., Rijnties, M., Weiller, C., Faiss, J. H., & et al. (1995). Localization of a
cerebellar timing processing unit using PET. Neurology, 45, 1540-1545.
Kamps, D., Walter, D., Maher, J., & Rotholz, D. (1992). Academic and environmental
effects of small group arrangements in classrooms for students with autism and
other developmental disabilities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
22, 277-293.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact Nervous Child, 2, 217-250.
Kanner, L .(1971). Follow-up study of eleven autistic children originally reported in 1943.
Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1, 119-145.
REFERENCES 205
Kanner, L., Rodriguez, A., & Ashenden, B. (1972). How far can autistic children go in
matters of social adaptation? Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2,
9-33.
Katz, J. J., Wood, D. S., Goldstein, G., Auchenbach, R. C., & Geckle, M. (1998). The utility
of neuropsychological tests in evaluation of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder (ADHD) versus depression in adults. Assessment, 5(1), 45-51.
Katz, L., & Goldstein, G. (1993). The Luria-Nebraska neuropsychological battery and the
WAIS-R in assessment of adults with specific learning disabilities. Rehabilitation
Counseling Bulletin, 36(4), 190-197.
Katz, L., Goldstein, G., Rudisin, S., & Bailey, D. (1993). A neuropsychological approach to
the bannatyne recategorization of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales in adults with
learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(1), 65-72.
Katz, L. J., Goldstein, G., & Geckle, M. (1998). Neuropsychological and personality
differences between men and women with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders,
2(4), 239-248.
Katz, L. J., Kelly, M. A., Goldstein, G., Bartolomucci, E., & Comstock, E. J. (1992,
November). MMPI subtypes In a mixed LD sample. Paper presented at the 12th
Annual Meeting of the National Academy of Neuropsychology.
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L (1985). Kaufman Tests of Educational Achievement.
Circle Pines, MN:AGS.
Kavale, K. A., & Fomes, S. R. (1995). The nature of learning disabilities: Critical elements
of diagnosis and classification. Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum.
Kelly, J. (1998). Recording for the blind & dyslexic: Learning through listening for half a
century. Perspectives (Summer), 28-30.
Kelly, K., & Ramundo, P. (1995). You mean I'm not lazy, stupid, or crazy: A self-help book
for adults with attention deficit disorder. New York: Scribner.
Kelman, M., & Lester, G. (1997). Jumping the Queue. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Kerka, S. (1998). Adults with learning disabilities. ERIC Digest, 189.
Kershner, J. S. (1991). Neuropsychological perspectives in special education. In J. E.
Obrzut & G. W. Hynd (Eds.), Neuropsychological foundations of learning
disabilities (pp. 711-741). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kim, S. G., Ugurbil, K., & Strick, P. L (1994). Activation of a cerebellar output nucleus
during cognitive processing. Science, 265, 949-951.
Kinsborne, M. (1968). Developmental Gerstmann syndrome. Pediatric Clinics of North
America, 15, 771-778.
Kinsborne, M. (1997). Nonverbal learning disability. In T. E. Feinberg & M. J. Farah (Eds.),
Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology (pp. 789-794). New York: McGraw
Hill.
Kirsch, I. A., Jungeblut, A., Jenkins, L, & Kilstad, A. (1993). Adult literacy In America: A first
look at the results of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Washington, DC: U. S.
Department of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement,
National Center for Education Statistics.
Kite, J. L (1984). Rehabilitation planning with learning disabled persons. Journal of
Rehabilitation, 59-63.
Kltz, W. R., & Thorpe, H. W. (1995). A comparison of the effectineness of videodisc and
traditional algebra instruction for college-age students with leaming disabilities.
Remedial and Special Education, 16, 295-306.
Klee, S. H., Garfinkel, B. D., & Beauchesne, H. (1986). Attention deficits in adults.
Psychiatric Annals, 16(1), 52-56.
Klein, K., & Hecker, L (1994). The write moves. In A. Brand (Ed.), R. Graves. Portsmouth,
NH: Boyton/Cook Heinemann.
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Ciccheti, D. V., & Rourke, B. P. (1995). Validity
and neuropsychological characterization of Asperger syndrome: Convergence
206 REFERENCES
Levine, M., & Fuller, G. (1972). Sex differences on psychoeducational tests with three
classified deficit reading groups. Slow Learning Child, 19, 165-174.
Levine, M. D. (1987). Developmental variation and learning disorders. Cambridge, MA:
Educators Publishing Service.
Levine, M. D. (1994). Educational care: A system for understanding and managing learning
disorders. Cambridge, MA: Educators Publishing Service.
Levine, P., & Edgar, E. (1995). An analysis by gender of long-term postschool outcomes for
youth with and without disabilities. Exceptional Children, 61(3), 282-300.
Levine, P., & Nourse, S. W. (1998). What follow-up studies say about postschool life for
young men and women with learning disabilities: A critical look at the literature.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(3), 212-233.
Levine, S. (1995). Career counseling for people with attention deficit disorder. In T.
Hartman (Ed.), ADD success stories: A guide to fulfillment for families with
attention deficit disorder. Grass Valley, CA: Underwood Books.
Lewandowski, L., & Arcangelo, K. (1994). The social adjustment and self-concept of adults
with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 598-605.
Lewis, R. D., & Lorion, R. P. (1988). Discriminative effectiveness of the Luri-Nebraska
Battery for LD adolescents. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 11, 62-69.
Lewitter, F. I., DeFries, J. C., & Elston, R. C. (1980). Genetic models of reading disability.
Behavior Genetics, 10, 9-30.
Leyser, Y., Vogel, S., Wyland, B., & Brulle, A. (1998). Faculty attitudes and practices
regarding students with disabilities: Two decades after implementation of Section
504. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, 13(3), 5-19.
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A. M., Fowler, C., & Fischer, F. W. (1977).
Phonetic segmentation and recoding in the beginning reader. In A. S. Reber & D.
L. Scarborough (Eds.), Toward a psychology of reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Light, J. G., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (1998). Multivariate behavioral genetic analysis
of achievement and cognitive measures in reading-disabled and control twin
pairs. Human Biology, 70, 215-237.
Lincoln, A. J., Courchesne, E., Kilman, B. A., Elasian, R., & Allen, M. (1988). A study of
intellectual abilities in high-functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 18, 505-524.
Lindamood, C., & Lindamood, P. (1975). Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test.
Hingham, MA: Teaching Resources Corporation.
Livingstone, M. S., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W., & Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological
and anatomical evidence for a magna cellular defect in developmental dyslexia.
Paper presented at the Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA.
Loge, D. V., Staton, R. D., & Beatty, W. W. (1990). Performance of children with ADHD on
tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 540-545.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Goode, S. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A
standardized investigator-based instrument Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 19, 185-212.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. L (1994). Autism diagnostic interview revised: A
revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with
possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 24, 659-685.
Lou, H. C. (1996). Etiology and pathogenesis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD): Significance of prematurity and perinatal hypoxic-haemodynamic
encephalopathy. Acta Paediatr, 85, 1266-1271.
Lou, H. C., Henriksen, L., & Bruhn, P. (1984). Focal cerebral hypoperfusion in children with
dysphasia and/or attention deficit disorder. Arch Neurol, 41, 825-829.
Lou, H. C., Henriksen, L., Bruhn, P., Bomer, H., & Nielsen, J. B. (1989). Striatal dysfunction
in Attention Deficit and Hyperkinetic disorder. Archives of Neurology, 46, 48-52.
208 REFERENCES
Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., & Schriebman, L. (1979). Stimulus overselectivity In autism. A
review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1236-1254.
Lovejoy, D. W., Ball, J. D., Keats, M., & et al. (1999). Neuropsychological performance of
adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Diagnostic
classification estimates for measures of frontal lobe/executive functioning.
Journal Int Neuropsychology Soc, 5, 222-233.
Lovett, M. W. (1984). A developmental perspective on reading dysfunction: Accuracy and
rate criteria in the subtyping of dyslexic children. Brain and Language, 22, 67-91.
Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed
criteria for normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development, 58, 234-260.
Ludwikowski, K., & DeValk. M. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The
psychostimulants and beyond. Advance for Nurse Practitioners, 6, 55-60.
Lyon, G. R. (1983). Learning-disabled readers: Identification of subgroups. In H. R.
Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities (Vol. 5, pp. 103-134). New York:
Grune & Stratton.
Lyon, G. R. (1985). Educational validation studies of learning disability subtypes. In B. P.
Rourke (Ed.), Neuropsychology of learning disabilities. New York: Guilford Press.
Lyon, G. R. (1985b). Identification and remediation of learning disability sub-types:
Preliminary findings. Learning Disability Focus, 1, 21-35.
Lyon, G. R. (1995a). Research initiatives in learning disabilities: Contributions from
scientists supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development Journal of Child Neurology, 10, 120-126.
Lyon, G. R. (1995b). Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 3-26.
Lyon, G. R. (1996). The state of research. In S. C. Cramer & W. Ellis (Eds.), Learning
disabilities: Lifelong Issues. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brooks.
Lyon. G. R., Rietta, S., Watson, B., Porch, B., & Rhodes, J. (1981). Selected linguistic and
perceptual abilities of empirically derived subgroups of learning disabled readers.
Journal of School Psychology, 19, 152-166.
Lyon, R., Stewart, N., & Freeman, D. (1982). Neuropsychological characteristics of
empirically derived subgroups of learning disabled readers. Journal of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 4, 343-365.
Ma, X. (2000). A longitudinal assessment of antecedent course work in mathematics and
subsequent mathematical attainment. The Journal of Educational Research, 94,
16-28.
MacArthur, C., & Graham, S. (1987). Learning disabled students' composing under three
methods of text production: Handwriting, word processing, and dictation. Journal
of Special Education, 21, 22-42.
MacArthur, C. A. (1999). Word prediction for students with severe spelling problems.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 158-172.
MacArthur, C. A., Schwartz, S., Graham, S., Molloy, D., & Harris, D. (1996). Integration of
strategy instruction into whole language classroom: A case study. Learning
Disabilities Research and Practice, 11, 168-176.
Maccini, P., & Hughes, C. A. (2000). Effects of a problem-solving strategy on the
introductory algebra performance of secondary students with learning disabilities.
Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 15, 10-21.
Magoun, H. W. (1952). An ascending reticular activating system in the brain stem. Archives
of Neurology and Psychiatry, 67, 145-154.
Malhotra, A. K., Virkkunen, M., Rooney, W., Eggert, M., Linolla, M., & Goldman, D. (1996).
The association between the dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) 15 amino acid
repeat polymorphism and novelty seeking. Molecular Psychiatry, 1, 388-391.
Mann, V. A., & Brady, S. (1988). Reading disability: The role of language deficiencies.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 811-816.
REFERENCES 209
Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G.. Bessler, A., Malloy, P., & LaPadula, M. (1993). Adult outcome
of hyperactive boy: Educational achievement, occupational rank and psychiatric
status. Arc Gen Psychiatry, 50, 565-576.
Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G., Bessler, A., Malloy, P., & LaPadula, M. (1998). Adult
psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up. American Journal of Psychiatry,
155, 493-498.
Marsh, L. G., & Cook, N. L. (1996). The effects of using manipulatives in teaching math
problem solving to students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities
Research and Practice, 11, 58-65.
Marshall, R. M., Hynd, G. W., Handwerk, M. J., & Hall, J. (1997). Academic
underachievement In ADHD subtypes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 635-
642.
Matochik, J. A., Leibenauer, L. L., King, C., Szymanski, H. V., Cohen, R. M., & Zametkin, A.
J. (1994). Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder after chronic stimulant treatment American Journal of
Psychiatry, 151(5), 658-664.
Matochik, J. A., Rumsey. J. M., Zametkin, A. J., & et al. (1996). Neuropsychological
correlates of familial attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults.
Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology Behav. Neurol, 9, 186-191.
Mattek, P. W., & Wierzbicki, M. (1998). Cognitive and behavioral correlates of depression in
learning-disabled and nonlearning-disabled adult students. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 54, 831-837.
Mattes, J. A. (1980). The role of frontal lobe dysfunction in childhood hyperkinesis. Comp
Psychiatry, 21, 358-369.
Mattes, J. A., Boswell, L., & Oliver, H. (1984). Methylphenidate effects on symptoms of
Attention Deficit Disorder in adults. Archives General Psychiatry, 41, 1059-1063.
Mattis, S. (1978). Dyslexia syndromes: A working hypothesis that works. In A. L. Benton &
D. Pearl (Eds.), Dyslexia: An appraisal of current knowledge (pp. 43-58). London:
Oxford University Press.
McCarney, S. B., & Anderson, P. D. (1996). Adult attention deficit disorder evaluation scale
(AADDES). Columbia, MO: Hawthorne Educational Services.
McCracken, J. T. (1991). A two-part model of stimulant action on Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder in children. Journal Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci, 3, 201-
209.
McCue, M. (1984). Assessment and rehabilitation of leaming-disabled adults. Rehabilitation
Counseling Bulletin(May), 281-290.
McCue, M. (1993). Clinical and diagnostic functional assessment of adults with learning
disability. In P. J. Gerber & H. B. Reiff (Eds.), Learning disabilities In adulthood:
Persisting problems and evolving Issues (pp. 55-71). Boston, MA: Andover
Medical Publishers.
McCue, M. (1994). Neuropsychological Diagnostic and Functional Interview. Unpublished
manuscript.
McCue, M., & Goldstein, G. (1991). Neuropsychological aspects of learning disability in
adults. In B. Rourke (Ed.), Neuropsychological validation of learning disability
subtypes (pp. 311-329). New York: Guilford Publications, Inc.
McCue, M., Goldstein, G., Shelly, C., & Katz, L. (1986). Cognitive profiles of some subtypes
of learning disabled adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1, 13-23.
McCue, M., Katz, L., & Goldstein, G. (1985). A Training Program in Specific Learning
Disability (SLD) for Rehabilitation Psychologists. Unpublished manuscript.
McCue, M., Shelly, C., Goldstein, G., & Katz-Garris, L. (1984). Neuropsychological aspects
of learning disability in young adults. The International Journal of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 6, 229, 233.
210 REFERENCES
Millstein, R. B., Wilens, T. E., Biederman, J., & Spencer, T. J. (1997). Presenting ADHD
symptoms and subtypes In clinically referred adults with ADHD. Journal of
Attention Disorders, 2(3), 159-166.
Minshew, N. J. (1996). Autism. In B. O. Berg (Ed.), Principles of Child Neurology (pp. 1713-
1730). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1993). Is autism and amnesic disorder? Evidence from the
California Verbal Learning Test Neuropsychology, 7, 209-216.
Minshew, N. J., Goldsetin, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in
autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303-316.
Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1998). Autism as a disorder of complex information
processing. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research
Reviews, 4, 129-136.
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Dombrowski, S. M., Panchalingam, K., & Pettegrew, J. W.
(1993). A preliminary31 PMRS study of autism: Evidence for undersynthesis and
increased degradation of brain membranes. Biological Psychiatry, 33, 762-773.
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Muenz, L. R., & Payton, J. B. (1992). Neuropsychological
functioning in non-mentally retarded autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 740-761.
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Taylor, H. G., & Siegel, D. J. (1994). Academic achievement
in high functioning autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Nueropsychology, 16, 261-270.
Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (1999). Oculomotor evidence for neocortical
systems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurology, 23, 917-922.
Minshew, N. J., & Payton, J. B. (1988). The clinical spectrum of infantile autism: Part I.
Current Problems in Pediatrics, 18, 561 -610.
Minshew, N. J., Siegel, D. J., Goldstein, G., & Weldy, S. (1994). Verbal problem solving in
high functioning autistic individuals. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 31-
40.
Minshew, N. J., Sweeney, J. A., & Bauman, M. L. (1997). Neurological aspects of autism.
In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive
developmental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 344-369). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Minskoff, E. H., Hawks, R., Steidle, E. F., & Hoffmann, F. J. (1989). A homogeneous group
of persons with learning disabilities: Adults with severe learning disabilities in
vocational rehabilitation. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 521-528.
Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., Duncan, C. C., & et al. (1991). Analysis of the elements of
attention: A neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychotogical Review, 2, 109-
145.
Moats, L C. (1983). A comparison of the spelling errors of older dyslexics and second-
grade normal children. Annals of Dyslexia, 33, 121-139.
Moats, L C. (1993a). Assessment of spelling in learning disabilities research. In G. R.
Lyons (Ed.), Frames of Reference for the Assessment of Learning Disabilities:
New Views of Measurement Issues. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.
Moats, L. C. (1993b). Spelling error analysis: Beyond the phonetic/dysphonic dichotomy.
Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 174-185.
Moats, L. C. (1994). Principles of spelling instruction. In S. Brody (Ed.), Teaching Reading:
Language, Letters, and Thought. Milford, NH: LARC Publishing.
Moats, L C. (1995). Spelling, Development Disability and Instruction. Baltimore, MD: York
Press.
Montague, M. (1998). Cognitive strategy instruction in mathematics for students with
learning disabilities. In D. P. Rivera (Ed.), Mathematics education for students
with learning disabilities (pp. 177-199). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Moore, D. (1997,). Adult ADHD: Challenge of a lifetime. The ADHD Challenge,
September/October.
212 REFERENCES
Morris, R., Lyon, G. R., Alexander, D., Gray, D. B., Kavanagh, J., Rourke, B. P., &
Swanson, H. L. (1994). Editorial: Proposed guidelines and criteria for describing
samples of persons with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 17,
106-109.
Morris, R. D., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Lyon, R., Shankweiler, D. P.,
Katz, L., Francis, D. J., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability:
Variability around a phonological core. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(3),
347-373.
Morris, R. D., & Walter, L. W. (1991). Subtypes of arithmetic-disabled adults: Validating
childhood findings. In B. Rourke (Ed.), Neuropsychological validation of learning
disability subtypes (pp. 330-346). New York: Guilford Publications.
Morrison, J. R., & Stewart, M. A. (1973). The psychiatric status of the legal families of
adopted hyperactive children. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 28, 888-891.
Morrison, S. R., & Siegel, L. S. (1991). Arithmetic disability: Theoretical considerations and
empirical evidence for this subtype. In L. V. Feagans, E. J. Short, & L. J. Meltzer
(Eds.), Subtypes of learning disabilities (pp. 198-208). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Morrison, S. R., & Siegel, L. S. (1991b). Learning disabilities: A critical review of definitional
and assessment issues. In J. E. Obrzut & G. W. Hynd (Eds.), Neuropsychological
foundations of learning disabilities: A handbook of issues, methods, and practice
(pp.79-97).San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Morrow, L. A., & Beers, S. R. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment: When to ask for it,
what to expect. Journal of Psychological Practice, 1, 182-190.
Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the
EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neuropsychology, I, 455-473.
Mosberg, L., & Johns, D. (1994). Reading and listening comprehension in college students
with developmental dyslexia. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 9(3),
130-135.
Mosher, F. A., & Hornsby, J. R. (1996). On asking questions. In J. S. Bruner, R. R. Olver,
T. M. Greenfield, J. R. Hornsby, H. J. Kenney, M. Maccoby, N. Modiano, S. A.
Mosher, D. R. Olson, M. C. Potter, L. C. Reich, & A. M. Sonstroem (Eds.),
Studies in cognitive growth (pp. 86-102). New York: Wiley & Sons.
Murphy, K., & LeVert, S. (1995). Out of the fog: Treatment options and coping strategies for
adult attention deficit disorder. New York: Skylight Press.
Mykelbust, H. R. (1975). Nonverbal teaming disabilities: Assessment and intervention. In H.
R. Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities (3rd ed., pp. 85-121). New
York: Grune & Stratton.
Nadeau, K. (Ed.). (1995). A comprehensive guide to attention deficit disorders in adults:
Research, diagnosis, and treatment. New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc.
Naglieri, J. A., & Johnson, D. (2000). Effectiveness of a cognitive strategy intervention in
improving arithmetic computation based on the PASS theory. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 33, 591-597.
National Center for Education Statistics. (1995-96). National postsecondary student aid
study. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
National Center for Education Statistics. (1999). Students with disabilities in
postsecondary education: A profile of preparation, participation, and outcomes.
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
National Educational Goals Panel (1993). The 1993 national educational goals panel
report . Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office.
Newby, R. F., & Lyon, G. R. (1991). Neuropsychological subtypes of learning disabilities. In
J. E. Obrzut & G. W. Hynd (Eds.), Neuropsychological foundations of learning
disabilities: A handbook of issues, methods, and practice (pp. 355-385). New
York: Academic Press.
REFERENCES 213
Newcomer, P. L., & Barenbaum, E. M. (1991). The written composing ability of children
with learning disabilities: A review of the literature from 1980 to 1990. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 14, 578-593.
Nichelli, P., & Venneri, A. (1995). Right hemisphere developmental learning disability: A
case study. Neurocase, 1, 173-177.
Nichols, P. (1980). Early antecedents of hyperactivity. Neurology, 30, 439.
Nielson, K. H., Mrazik, M., Hiemenz, J. R., & Hynd, G. W. (1994). Rates of birth
complications and prenatal toxin exposure in children with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder: University of Georgia.
Nightengale, D., Yudd, R., Anderson, S., & Barnow, B. (1991). The learning disabled in
employment and training programs (Research and Evaluation Report Series 91-
E). Washington, DC: Urban Institute.
NIH Consensus Statement (1998). Diagnosis and Treatment of Attention Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder. Nov. 16-18,16, 2: 1-37.
Nolting, P. D. (1991). Math and the learning disabled student: A practical guide for
accommodations. Pompano Beach, FL: Academic Success Press.
Nolting, P. D. (1997). Winning at math: Your guide to learning mathematics through
successful study skills. Bradenton, FL: Academic Success Press, Inc.
Norman, C. A., & Zigmond, N. (1980). Characteristics of children labeled and served as
learning disabled in school systems affiliated with Child Service Demonstration
Center. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 13, 542-547.
Nussbaum. N. L., Grant, M. L., Roman, M. J., Poole, J. H., & Bigler, E. D. (1990). Attention
deficit disorder and the mediating effect of age on academic and behavioral
variables. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 11, 22-26.
O'Donnell, J. P. (1991). Neuropsychological assessment of learning-disabled adolescents
and young adults. In J. E. Obrzut & G. W. Hynd (Eds.), Neuropsychological
foundations of learning disabilities (pp. 331-353). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
O'Donnell, J. P., Romero, J. J., & Leicht, D. J. (1990). A comparison of language deficits in
learning-disabled, head-injured and nondisabled young adults: Results from an
abbreviated aphasia screening test. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 310-315.
Oestreicher, J. M., & O'Donnell, J. P. (1995). Validation of the General Neuropsychological
Deficit Scale with nondisabled, learning-disabled, and head-injured young adults.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 10, 185-191.
Olson, R., Forsberg, H., Wise, B., & Rack, J. (1994). Measurement of word recognition,
orthographic, and phonological skills. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference
for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues.
Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Ornitz, E. M. (1988). Autism: A disorder of directed attention. Brain Dysfunction, 1, 309-
322.
Ornitz, E. M., & Ritvo, E. R. (1968). Perceptual inconstancy in early infantile autism.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 18, 76-98.
Ornitz, E. M., Sugiyama, T., & deTraversay, J. (1993). Startle moducation studies in autism.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 619-637.
Orton, J. L. (1966). The Orton-GilIingham approach. In J. Money (Ed.), The disabled
reader: Education of the dyslexic child (pp. 243-277). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Orton, S. T. (1925). "Word-blindness" in school children. Archives of Neurology and
Psychiatry, 14, 581-615.
Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, writing and speech problems in children: A presentation of
certain types of disorders in the development of the language faculty. New York:
W. W. Norton.
Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Archives de Psychologie,
30, 206-356.
Owens, N., & Owens, B. W. (1993). Adult attention deficit disorder behavior rating scales.
214 REFERENCES
Ozonoff, S. (1995a). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of
autism. Neuropsychology, 9, 491-500.
Ozonoff, S. (1995b). Executive functions in autism. New York: Plenum Press.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-
functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081-1105.
Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function
abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome: An information processing approach.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1015-1032.
Palinscar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering
and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117-175.
Pardo, J. V., Fox, P. T., & Raichie, M. E. (1991). Localization of a human system for
sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature, 349, 61-64.
Pascualvaca, D. M., Fantie, B. D., Papageorgiou, M., & Mirsky, A. F. (1998). Attentional
capacities in children with autism: Is there a general deficit in shifting focus.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 28, 467-478.
Pasto, L., & Burack, J. A. (1995, June). A developmental study of visual filtering: Can
windows facilitate filtering efficiency? Paper presented at the Canadian
Psychological Association, Charlottetown, Canada.
Patton, J. R., & Polloway, E. A. (1992). Learning disabilities: The challenges of adulthood.
Journal of learning disabilities, 25(7), 410-415, 447.
Pauls, D. L., Alsobrook, J. P., Gelernter, L., & Leckman, J. F. (1999). Genetic vulnerability.
In J. F. Leckman & R. T. Schulz (Eds.), Tourette's syndrome - tics, obsessions,
compulsions (pp. 194-212). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
PeBenito, R. (1987). Developmental Gerstmann Syndrome: Case report and review of the
literature. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 8, 229-232.
PeBenito, R., Fisch, C. B., & Fisch, M. L. (1988). Developmental Gerstmann Syndrome.
Archives of Neurology, 45, 977-982.
Pelham, W., & Swanson, J. (1999, October). Do time-response effects vary among
stimulant medications? Symposium presented at the 46th Annual Meeting of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Chicago, Illinois.
Pennington, B. F. (1991). Diagnosing learning disorders: A neuropsychological framework.
New York: Guilford.
Pennington, B. F., & Gilger, J. W. (1996). How is dyslexia transmitted? In C. H. Chase, G.
D. Rosen, & G. F. Sherman (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia (pp. 41-61).
Baltimore, MD: York Press.
Pennington, B. F., Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., Griffith, E. M., Reed, D. T., & Shyu, V. (In
press). Validity tests of the executive dysfunction hypothesis of autism. In J.
Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Pennington, B. F., Van Orden, G. C., Smith, S. D., Green, P. A., & Haith, M. M. (1990).
Phonological processing skills and deficits in adult dyslexics. Child Development,
61, 1753-1758.
Peter, B. M., & Spreen, O. (1979). Behavior rating and personal adjustment scales of
neuropsychologically and learning handicapped children during adolescence and
early adulthood: Results of a follow-up study. Journal of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 1, 75-91.
Peterson, K. Coaching interventions and strategies for adults with attention deficit disorder.
Pierce, K., Glad, K. S., & Scheiebman, L. (1997). Social perception in children with autism:
An attentional deficit? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 265-
282.
Pliszka, S. R., Carlson, C. L., & Swanson, J. M. (1999). ADHD with comorbid disorders.
New York: Guilford Press.
Pliszka, S. R., McCracken, J. T., & Maas, J. W. (1996). Catecholamines in Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder Current perspectives. Journal of American Academy of
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(3), 264-272.
Policy statement for documentation of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescents
and adults. (1998). Office of Disability Policy, Educational Testing Service,
Princeton, NJ.
Pollens, R., McBratnie, B., & Burton, P. (1988). Beyond cognition: Executive functions in
closed head injury. Cognitive Rehabilitation (September/October).
Pontius, A. A. (1973). Dysfunction patterns analogous to frontal lobe system and caudatae
nucleus syndromes in some groups of minimal brain dysfunction. Journal of
American Medical Women Association, 28, 285-292.
Posner, M. L., & Raichle, M. (1996). Images of mind. (Revised ed.). Washington, DC:
Scientific American Books.
Poteet, J. A. (1978). Characteristics of written expression of learning disabled and non-
learning disabled elementary school students. Diagnostique, 4, 60-74.
Pratt, A. C., & Brady, S. (1988). Relation of phonological awareness to reading disability in
children and adults. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 319-323.
Price, L. A., Johnson, J. M., & Evelo, S. (1994). When academic assistance is not enough:
Addressing the mental health issues of adolescents and adults with learning
disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(2), 82-90.
Prior, M., & Hoffman, W. (1990). Neuropsychology testing of autistic children through an
exploration with frontal lobe tests. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 4, 581-590.
Public Law 94-142. (1975, November 29). Education for All Handicapped Children Act. U.
S. Congress.
Rack, J. (1997). Issues in the assessment of developmental dyslexia in adults: Theoretical
and applied perspectives. Journal of Research in Reading, 20(1), 66-76.
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in
developmental dyslexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 28-53.
Rapin, I. (1997). Current concepts: Autism. The New England Journal of Medicine, 337, 97-
104.
Rapoport, J. L., Donnelly, M., Zametkin, A., & Carrougher, J. (1986). Situational
hyperactivity in a U.S. clinical setting. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry,
27, 639-646.
Raskind, M. H. (1993). Assistive technology for adults with learning disabilities: A rationale
for use. In P. J. Gerber & H. B. Reiff (Eds.), Learning disabilities in adulthood:
Persisting problems and evolving issues (pp. 152-162). Boston, MA: Andover.
Raskind, M. H. (1995). Assistive technology for adults with learning disabilities. In P. J.
Gerber & H. B. Reiff (Eds.), Learning disabilities in adulthood. Austin, TX: Pro-
Ed.
Raskind, M. H., & Higgins, E. (1995). Effects of speech synthesis on the proofreading
efficiency of postsecondary students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 18, 141-158.
Raskind, M. H., & Higgins, E. L. (1998). Assistive technology for postsecondary students
with learning disabilities: An overview. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(1), 27-
40.
Ratey, J., Hallowell, E. M., & Leveroni, C. L. (1993). Pharmacotherapy for ADHD in adults.
Paper presented at the 5th Annual CHADD Conference, San Diego, CA.
216 REFERENCES
Ratey, J. J., Greenberg, M. S., & Lindem, K. J. (1991). Combination of treatments for
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Journal of Nerv Mont Dis, 179,
699-701.
Ratey, J. J., Hallowell, E., & Miller, A. (1997). Psychosocial issues and psychotherapy in
adults with attention deficit disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 27(8), 582-587.
Ratey, N. (1997). Supporters give ADD coaching credit for low attrition rates, improved
grades. Disability Compliance for Higher Education, 3, 5.
Rawson, M. (19968). Developmental and Language Disability: Adult Accomplishments of
Dyslexic Boys. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press.
Reback, M. B. (1996). Accommodating workers with learning disabilities. In S. C. Cramer &
W. Ellis (Eds.), Learning disabilities: Lifelong issues. Baltimore, MD: Paul H.
Brookes.
Rehabilitation Act of 1973. Public Law 93-112, 87 Stat. 355
Rehabilitation Services Administration. (1981, July 27). Acceptance of specific learning
disabilities as a medically recognized disability program instruction. Program
Information Memorandum RSA-PI-81-22, 1-7.
Rehabilitation Services Administration. (1985, January 24). Program policy directive.
Washington, DC: U.S. Office of Special Education and Rehabilitation Services.
Reichler, R. J., & Schopler, E. (Eds.). (1976). Developmental therapy: A program model for
providing individual services in the community: Psychopathology and child
development. New York: Plenum Press.
Reiff, H. B., & Gerber, P. J. (1993). Social/emotional and daily living issues for adults with
learning disabilities. In P. J. Gerber & H. B. Reiff (Eds.), Learning disabilities in
adulthood: Persisting problems and evolving issues (pp. 72-81). Boston, MA:
Andover.
Reiff, H. B., Gerber, P. J., & Ginsberg, R. (1997). Exceeding expectations: Successful
adults with learning disabilities. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery:
Theory and clinical interpretation. (2nd ed.). Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology
Press.
Rey, A. (1941). L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique.
Archives Psychology, 28, 286-340.
Reynolds, C. R. (1985). Measuring the aptitude-achievement discrepancy in learning
disability diagnosis. Remedial and Special Education, 6(5), 37-55.
Reynolds, W. M. (1987). Reynolds adolescent depression scale. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Reynolds, W. M. (1998). Adolescent psychopathology scale. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources, Inc.
Riccio, C. A., & Hynd, G. W. (1996). Neurobiological research specific to the adult
population with learning disabilities. In N. Gregg, C. Hoy, & A. F. Gay (Eds.),
Adults with Learning Disabilities: Theoretical & Practical Perspectives (pp. 127-
143). New York: Guilford Press.
Riccio, C. A., Hynd, G. W., & Cohen, M. J. (1997). Etiology and neurobiology of ADHD. In
W. N. Bender (Ed.), Understanding ADHD, a practical guide for teachers and
parents. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
Riccio, C. A., Hynd, G. W., Cohen, M. J., & Gonzalez, J. J. (1993). Neurological basis of
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Exceptional Children, 60(2), 118-124.
Richard, M. M. (1995). Pathways to success for the college student with ADD
accommodations and preferred practices. Journal of Postsecondary Education
and Disability, 11, 16-30.
Richard, M. M., Finkel, M. F., & Cohen, M. D. (1998). Preparing reports documenting
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder for students in post-secondary education:
What neurologists need to know. The Neurologist, 4, 277-283.
REFERENCES 217
Rincover, A., & Ducharme. J. M. (1987). Variables influencing stimulus overselectivity and
"tunnel-vision" in developmentally disabled children. American Journal of Mental
Deficiency, 91, 422-430.
Rivera, D. P. (Ed.). (1998). Mathematics education for students with learning disabilities.
Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Roberts, R., & Mather, N. (1997). Orthographic dyslexia: The neglected subtype. Learning
Disabilities Research & Practice, 12(4), 236-250.
Robinson, F. P. (1946). Effective Study. New York: Harper & Brothers.
Rogan, L. L., & Hartman, L.D. (1990). Adult outcomes of learning disabled students ten
years after initial follow-up. Learning Disabilities Focus, 5(2), 91-102.
Rogeness, G. A., Javors, M. A., & Pliska, S. R. (1992). Neurochemistry and child and
adolescent psychiatry. American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 3,
765-781.
Rogers, J. C., & Holm, M. B. (1998). Geriatric rehabilitation. In G. Goldstein & S. R. Beers
(Eds.), Rehabilitation (pp. 89-109). New York: Plenum.
Rojewski, J. W. (1999). Occupational and educational aspirations and attainment of young
adults with and without LD 2 years after high school completion. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 32, 533-552.
Rosner, J., & Simon, D. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial report. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 4, 40-48.
Ross, J. J., & Smith, J. O. (1990). Adult basic educator's perceptions of learning disabilities.
Journal of Reading, 33(5), 340-347.
Ross, J. M. (1987). Learning disabled adults: Who are they and what do we do with them?
Lifelong Learning, 11(3), 4-7.
Ross, J. M. (1988). Learning and coping strategies of leaming disabled ABE students.
Adult Literacy and Basic Education, 12(2), 78-90.
Ross-Gordon, J. M. (1989). Adults with Learning Disabilities: An Overview for the Adult
Educator. Columbus, OH: ERIC Clearinghouse on Adult, Career, & Vocational
Education.
Ross-Gordon, J. M. (1996). Sociocultural issues affecting the identification and service
delivery models for adults with learning disabilities. In N. Gregg, C. Hoy, & A F.
Gay (Eds.), Adults with Learning Disabilities: Theoretical & Practical
Perspectives. New York: Guilford Press.
Rothstein, L. (1998. April). Guidelines emerge for accommodating students who have
learning disabilities. The Chronicle of Higher Education.
Rourke, B. P. (1982). Central processing deficiencies in children: Toward a developmental
neuropsychological model. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology (4), 1 -18.
Rourke, B. P. (Ed.). (1985). Essentials of subtype analysis. New York: Guilford.
Rourke, B. P. (1985). Neuropsychology of Learning Disabilities: Essentials of Subtype
Analysis. New York: Guilford Press.
Rourke, B. P. (1988). The syndrome of nonverbal teaming disabilities: Developmental
manifestations in neurological disease, disorder, and dysfunction. The Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 2, 293-330.
Rourke, B. P. (Ed.). (1989). Nonverbal learning disabilities: The syndrome and the model.
New York: Guilford Press.
Rourke, B. P. (1989a). Nonverbal learning disabilities: The syndrome and the model. New
York: Guilford.
Rourke, B. P. (1989b). Nonverbal learning disabilities, socioemotional disturbance, and
suicide: A reply to Fletcher, Kowalchuk and King, and Bigler. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 22, 186-187.
Rourke, B. P. (1991). Validation of learning disabilities subtypes: An overview. In B. P.
Rouorke (Ed.), Neuropsychological validation of learning disability subtypes (pp.
3-11). New York: Guilford Press.
218 REFERENCES
Schumaker, J. B., Nolan. S. M., & Deshler, D. D. (1985). The error monitoring strategy:
Learning Strategies Curriculum. Lawrence, KS: The University of Kansas.
Schwean, V. L. (1999). Looking ahead: The adjustment of adults with disabilities. In
Schwean & Saklofske (Eds.), Handbook of psychosocial characteristics of
exceptional children (pp. 587-610). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum
Publishers.
Schwiebert, V. L., Sealander, K. A., & Bradshaw, M. L. (1998). Preparing students with
attention deficit disorders for entry into the workplace and postsecondary
education. ASCA Professional School Counseling, 2(1), 26-32.
Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act of 1973. 29, U.S.C. 794, 45 C.F.R. 81, 84.
Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Weber, W., & Oueltette, C. (1997). Towards
defining a neuropsychology of ADHD: Performance of children and adolescents
from a large clinically referred sample. Journal Consult Clin Psychol, 65, 150-
160.
Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Weber, W., & et al. (1998). Neuropsychological function in
adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry, 44, 260-268.
Selz, M., & Reitan, R. M. (1979a). Rules for neuropsychological diagnosis: Classification of
brain function in older children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
47, 258-264.
Selz, M., & Reitan, R. M. (1979b). Neuropsychological test performance of normal,
learning-disabled, and brain-damaged chlidren. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 167, 298-302.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., & Hynd, G. W. (1990). Right hemisphere dysfunction in nonverbal
learning disabilities: Social, academic, and adaptive functioning in adults and
children. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 196-209.
Shaffer, D., & Greenhill, L. (1978). A critical note on the predictive validity of the
"Hyperkinetic Syndrome". Journal Child Psychol Psychiatry, 20, 617-620.
Shafrir, U., Siegel, L. S., & Chee, M. (1990). Learning disability, inferential skills, and post-
failure reflectivity. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 506-517.
Shafrir, U., & Wiegel, L. S. (1994). Subtypes of leaming disabilities in adolescents and
adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27,123-134.
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: University Press.
Share, D. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes In early reading development:
Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in
Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 1,1-57.
Shaw, R. A. (1999). The case for course substitutions as a reasonable accommodation for
students with foreign language learning difficulties. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 32(4), 320-349.
Shaywitz, B. A., Cohen, D. J., & Bowers, M. B. (1977). CSF monomine metabolites in
children with minimal brain dysfunction: Evidence of alteration of brain dopamine.
Journal Pediatrics, 90, 67-71.
Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1992). Discrepancy
compared to low achievement definitions of reading disability: Results from the
Connecticut longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 639-648.
Shaywitz, B. A., Holford, T. R., Holahan, J. M., & et al. (1995). A Matthew effect for IQ but
not for reading: Results from a longitudinal study, Read Res Q, 30, 894-906.
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Byrne, T., Cohen, D. J., & Rothman, S. (1983). Attention
Deficit Disorder: Quantitative analysis of CT. Neurology, 33,1500-1503.
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Pugh, K. R., Gore, J. C., Constable, R. T.,
Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., Liberman, A. M., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L.,
Bronen, R. A., Marchione, K. E., Holahan, J. M., Francis, D. J., Klorman, R.,
Aram, D. M., Blachman, B. A., Stuebing, K. K., & Lacadie, C. (1997). The Yale
Center for the study of learning and attention: Longitudinal and neurobiological
studies. Learning Disabilities, 8, 21-29.
REFERENCES 221
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Liberman, I. Y., & et al. (1991). Neurolinguistic and
biologic mechanisms in dyslexia. In D. D. Duane & D. B. Gray (Eds.), The
Reading Brain (pp. 27-52). Parkton, MD: York Press.
Shaywitz, S. E. (1998). Current concepts: Dyslexia. New England Medical Journal, 338.
307-312.
Shaywitz, S. E., Escobar, M. D., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Makuch, R. (1992).
Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of
reading ability. New England Journal of Medicine, 326, 145-150.
Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., Schneider, A. E., Marchione, K. E.,
Stuebing, K. K., Francis, D. J., Pugh, K. R., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1999). Persistence
of dyslexia: The Connecticut longitudinal study at adolescence. Pediatrics,
104(6), 1351-1359.
Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1994). Issues in the definition and
classification of attention deficit disorder. Top. Lang. Disord., 14, 1-25.
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1988). Attention deficit disorder Current perspectives.
In T. Kavanagh & T. Truss (Eds.), Learning Disabilities. Parkton, MD: York Press.
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Escobar, P. L. (1990). Prevalence of
reading disability in boys and girls. Journal of the American Medical Association,
264, 998-1002.
Shaywitz, S. E. (1996,). Dyslexia. Scientific American, November, 98-104.
Shea, V., & Mesibov, G. B. (1985). Brief report: The relationship of learning disabilities and
high-level autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15, 425-435.
Shekim, W. O. (1989). Comprehensive evaluation of attention deficit disorder-residual type.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31, 416-425.
Shekim, W. O., Asarnow, R. F., Hess, E., Zaucha, K., & Wheeler, N. A. (1990). A clinical
and demographic profile of a sample of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder, residual state. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31, 416-425.
Shelly, C., & Goldstein, G. (1982). Intelligence, achievement and the Luria-Nebraska
battery in a neuropsychiatric population: A factor analytic study. Clinical
Neuropsychology, 4, 164-169.
Sherman, G. F. (1995). Dyslexia: Is it all in your mind? Perspectives, The Orton Society,
21(4), 1, 8.
Siebel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1988). Development of grammatical sensitivity, phonological,
and short-term memory skills in normally achieving and learning disabled
children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 28-37.
Siegel, D. J., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (1996). Designing instruction for the high-
functioning autistic individual. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
8, 1-19.
Siegel, D. J., Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1996). Wechsler IQ profiles in diagnosis of
high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26,
389-406.
Siegel, L. S. (1988). Evidence that IQ scores are irrelevant to the definition and analysis of
reading disability. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 42, 201-215.
Siegel, L. S. (1989). IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 22, 469-478, 486.
Siegel, L. S. (1993). Alice in Iqland or why IQ is still irrelevant to learning disabilities. In R.
M. Joshi & C. K. Leong (Eds.), Reading disabilities: Diagnosis and components
processes. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer.
Siegel, L. S. (1999). Issues in the definition and diagnosis of (learning disabilities: A
perspective on Guckenberger v. Boston University. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 32, 304-319.
Siegel, L. S., & Heaven, R. K. (1986). Categorization of learning disabilities. In S. J. Ceci
(Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social and neuropsychological aspects of learning
disabilities (Vol. 1, pp. 94-121). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
222 REFERENCES
Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989a). The development of working memory in normally
achieveing and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development, 60,
973-980.
Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. G. (1989b). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: The influence of
definitional variables. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 1, 257-
287.
Siegel, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). The assessment of adults with reading disabilities:
What can we learn from experimental tasks? Journal of Research in Reading,
20(1), 42-54.
Sitlington, P. L., & Frank, A. R. (1990). Are adolescents with learning disabilities
successfully crossing the bridge into adult life? Learning Disability Quarterly, 3,
97-111.
Smith, A. (1982). Symbol digit modalities test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
Services.
Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review.
Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259-273.
Smith, J. (1988). Social and vocational problems of adults with learning disabilities: A
review of the literature. Learning Disabilities Focus, 4, 36-58.
Smith, J. D. (1996). Adult development theories: An overview and reflection on their
relevance for learning disabilities. In J. R. Patton & E. A. Polloway (Eds.),
Learning disabilities: The challenges of adulthood (pp. 11-23). Austin, TX: Pro-
Ed.
Sohlberg, M. M., & Geyser, S. (1986). Executive function behavioral rating scale. Paper
presented at the Whittier College Conference Series, Whittier, CA.
Southwestern Community College v. Davis. (1979). 442 U.S. 397.
Sparks, R., Phillips, L., & Ganschow, L. (1996). Students classified with learning disabled
and the college foreign language requirement. In J. Liskin-Gasparro (Ed.), The
changing demographics of foreign language instruction (pp. 123-159). Boston:
Heinle & Heinle.
Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (1991). Retreat, regroup, or advance? An agenda for
empirical classification research in learning disabilities. In L. V. Feagans, E. J.
Short, & L. J. Meltzer (Eds.), Subtypes of learning disabilities: Theoretical
perspectives and research (pp. 33-52). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Spekman, N. J., Goldberg, R. J., & Herman, K. L. (1992). Learning disabled children grow
up: A search for factors related to success in the young adults years. Learning
Disabilities Research & Practice, 7, 161-170.
Spekman, N. J., Herman, K. L, & Vogel, S. A. (1993). Risk and resilience in individuals
with learning disabilities: A challenge to the field. Learning Disabilities Research
and Practice, 8(1), 59-65.
Spellacy, F., & Peter, B. (1978). Dyscalculia and elements of the developmental Gerstmann
syndrome in school children. Cortex, 14, 197-206.
Spencer, T., Biederman, J., Wilens, T., Harding, M., O'Donnell, D., & Griffin, S. (1996).
Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder across the life cycle.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 409-
432.
Spencer, T., Wilens, T. E.. Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Ablon, S., & Ras, J. F. (1995). A
double blind comparison of methylphenidate and placebo in adults with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1992). The structured clinical
interview for the DSM-III-R (SCID). Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 624-629.
Spreen, O. (1982). Adult outcomes of reading disorders. In R. N. Malatesha & P. G. Aaron
(Eds.), Reading disorders, varieties and treatments (pp. 473-498). New York:
Academic Press.
REFERENCES 223
Spreen, O., & Haaf, R. G. (1986). Empirically derived learning disability subtypes: A
replication attempt and longitudinal patterns over 15 years. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 19, 170-180.
Sprich-Buckminster, S., Biederman. J., Milberger, S., Faraone. S. V., & Lehman, B. K.
(1993). Are perinatal complications relevant to the manifestation of ADD? Issues
of comorbidity and familiality. Journal of the American Academy of Chid and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1032-1037.
Stafford-Depass, J. M. (1998). Psychological characteristics and life experiences of highly
successful learning disabled adults. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section
B: The Sciences & Engineering, 58 (9-B), 5142.
Stanovich. K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual
differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-
407.
Stanovich, K. E. (1988a). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-
variety poor reader The phonological-core variable-difference mode. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 21, 590-612.
Stanovich, K. E. (1988b). The right and wrong places to look for the cognitive locus of
reading disability. Annals of Dyslexia, 38,154-177.
Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Conceptual and empirical problems with discrepancy definitions of
reading disability. Learning Disability Quarterly, 14, 269-280.
Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Discrepancy definition of reading disability: Has intelligence led us
astray? Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 7-29.
Stanovich, K. E. (1999). The sociopsychometrics of learning disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 32, 350-361.
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). The phenotypic performance profile of reading-
disabled children: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-
difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24-53.
Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological
and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational psychology,
89, 114-127.
Steel, J. G., Gorman, R., & Flexman, J. E. (1984). Case report Neuropsychiatric testing of
an autistic mathematical idiot-savant. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Psychology, 23, 704-707.
Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To sea but not to read: The magnocellular theory of dyslexia.
Trends In Neurosciences, 20(4), 147-152.
Stevenson, J. (1992). Evidence for a genetic etiology in hyperactivity in children. Behavior
Genetics, 22, 337-344.
Stevenson, J., Graham, P., Fredman, G., & McLoughlin, V. (1987). A twin study of genetic
influences on reading and spelling ability and disability. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 28(2), 229-247.
Stevenson, J., Pennington, B. F., Gilger, J. W., DeFries, J. C., & Gillis, J. J. (1993).
Hyperactivity and spelling disability: Testing for shared genetic actiology. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 34, 1137-1152.
Stewart, M. A., Cumming, C., Singer, S., & Deblois, C. S. (1981). The overlap between
hyperactive and undersocialized aggressive children. Journal Child Psychol
Psychiatry, 22, 35-45.
Still, G. F. (1902). Some abnormal psychical conditions in children. The Lancet, I, 1008-
1012,1077-1082.
Strauss, A. A., & Lehtinen, L E. (1947). The psychopathy and education of the brain-
injured Child. (Vol. 1). New York: Grune & Stratton.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.
Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986). The Frontal Lobes. New York: Raven Press.
224 REFERENCES
Sullivan, C., & Grosser, G. S. (1996). Dyslexia Research and Resource Guide. Boston:
Allyn and Bacon.
Sunohara, G., Barr, C., Jain. U., Schachar, R., & et al. (1997). Association of the D4
receptor gene in individuals with ADHD. American Society of Human Genetics,
Baltimore.
Swanson, J., Castellanos, F. X., Miroas, M., Lahoste, G., & Kennedy, J. (1998). Cognitive
Neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder.
Current Opinion In Neurobiology, 8(2), 263-271.
Swanson, J. M., Sunohara, G. A., Kennedy, J. L, & et al. (1998). Association of the
dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene with a refined phenotype of attention deficit
hyperacttvity disorder (ADHD): A family-based approach. Mol Psychiatry, 3, 38-
41.
Szatmari, P. (1992). The epidemiology of attention-deficit hyperactive disorders. In G.
Weiss (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychiatry clinics of North America: Attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985). The conceptual basis for referential word meaning in children
with autism. Child Development, 56, 1167-1178.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children:
Further evidence of a cognitive deficit. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 9, 417-430.
Tallal, P. (1976). Rapid auditory processing in normal and disordered language
development. Journal of Speech and Hearing, 19, 561-571.
Tallal, P., Miller, S., & Fitch, R. H. (1996). Neurobiological basics of speech: A case for the
preeminence of temporal processing. In C. H. Chase, G. D. Rosen, & G. F.
Sherman (Eds.), Developmental Dyslexia (pp. 159-84). Baltimore, MD: York
Press.
Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1979). Defects of auditory perception in children with
developmental dysphasia. In M. A. Syke (Ed.), Developmental Dysphasla (pp.
63-84). Orlando, FL: Academic Press, Inc.
Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivlty disorder Advances in cognitive,
neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal Child Psychol Psychiat, 39, 65-98.
Tashman, B. (1995,). Misreading dyslexia. Scientific American, August, 14-16.
Taylor, E. (1994). Syndromes of attention deficit and overactivity. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor, &
L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: Modem approaches. Oxford,
England: Blackwell Scientific.
Taylor, E., Sandberg, S., Thorley, G., & Giles, S. (1991). The epidemiology of childhood
hyperactivity. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Technology-Related Assistance for Individuals with Disabilities Act (1988). P. L. 100-47,
29 U.S.C.2001.
Thapar, A, Holmes, J., Poulton, K., & Harrington, R. (1999). Genetic basis of attention
deficit and hyperactivity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 105-111.
Tomb, D. A. (1998). The psychiatric clinics of North America. Philadelphia. PA: W. B.
Saunders Company.
Torgesen, J. K. (1991). Subtypes as prototypes: Extended studies of rationally defined
extreme groups. In L. V. Feagans, E. J. Short, & L J. Meltzer (Eds.), Subtypes of
learning disabilities: Theoretical perspectives and research (pp. 229-240).
Hiltsato, NJ: Earlbaum.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of
phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 286-
286.
Torgeson, J. K. (1991). Learning disabilities: Historical and conceptual issues. In B. Y. L.
Wong (Ed.), Learning about learning disabilities. San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
REFERENCES 225
Weaver, S. (1993). The validity of the use of extended and untimed testing for
postsecondary students with learning disabilities. Unpublished Doctoral
dissertation, University of Toronto.
Wechsler, D. (1955). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale manual. San Antonio, TX:
Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised. San Antonio, TX: The
Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale. (Third ed). San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test. New York: The Psychological
Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX:
Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult Intelligence scale. (Third ed.). San Antonio, TX:
Psychological Corporation.
Weinberg, W. A., & McLean, A. (1986). A diagnostic approach to developmental specific
learning disorders. Journal of Child Neurology, 1, 158-172.
Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M. (1983). Developmental learning disabilities of the right
hemisphere: Emotional, interpersonal, and cognitive components. Archives of
Neurology, 40, 463-468.
Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M. M. (1985). Mental state assessment of young and elderly
adults in behavioral neurology. In M. M. Mesulam (Ed.), Principles of Behavioral
Neurology (pp. 71-123). Philadelphia, PA: Davis.
Weiss, G., Hechtman, L., Milroy, T., & Perlman, T. (1985). Psychiatric status of
hyperactives as adults: A controlled prospective 15-year follow-up of 63
hyperactive children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24,
211-220.
Weiss, L. (1992). Attention deficit disorder in adults: Practical help for sufferers and their
spouses. Dallas. TX: Taylor Publishing.
Weiss, L. (1994). The attention deficit disorder in adults workbook. Dallas, TX: Taylor
Publishing Company.
Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. (1987). Word recognition and
comprehension skills in hyperlexic children. Brain and Language, 32, 76-96.
Wender, E. H. (1995). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorders in adolescence. Journal of
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 16, 192-195.
Wender, P. H. (1971). Minimal brain dysfunction in children. New York: Wiley-lnterscience
Publishers, Inc.
Wender, P. H. (1998). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in adults. In D. A. Tomb
(Ed.), The Psychiatric clinics of North America (pp. 761-775). Philadelphia, PA:
W. B. Saunders Co.
Wender, P. H., & Reimherr, F. W. (1990). Bupropion treatment of attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder in adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147(8), 1018-
1020.
Wender, P. H., Reimherr, F. W., Wood, D., & Ward, M. (1985). A controlled study of
methylphenidate in the treatment of Attention Deficit Disorder, residual type, in
adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142(5), 547-552.
Wender, P. H., Reimherr, F. W., & Wood, D. R. (1981). Attention Deficit Disorder ('minimal
brain dysfunction') in adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 38, 449-456.
Wender, P. H., Wood. D. R., & Reimherr, F. W. (1985). Pharmacological treatment of
attention deficit disorder, residual type (ADD, RT, "minimal brain dysfunction,"
"hyperactivity") in adults. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21(2), 222-227.
228 REFERENCES
Wender, P. H., Wood, D. R., Reimherr, F. W., & Ward, M. (1983). An open trial of pargyline
in the treatment of attention deficit disorder residual type. Psychiatry Research,
9, 329-336.
Westberry, S. J. (1994). A review of learning strategies for adults with learning disabilities
preparing for the GED exam. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(4), 202-209.
Wetzel, K. (1996). Speech-recognizing computers: A written-communication tool for
students with learning disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 371-380.
White, J. L., Moffit, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1992). Neuropsychological and socio-emotional
correlates of specific-arithmetic disability. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
7, 1-16.
Whitehouse, D., & Harris, J. C. (1984). Hyperlexia in infantile autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 26, 423-438.
Wiederholt, J. L., & Bryant, B. R. (1992). Gray oral reading tests. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1989). Test of Language Competence - Expanded Edition. San
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wilens, T., Biederman, J., Mick, E., & et al. (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) is associated with early-onset substance use disorders. Journal Nerv
Ment Dis, 185, 475-482.
Wilens, T. E., Beiderman, J., & Mick, E. (1998). Does ADHD affect the course of substance
abuse? Findings from a sample of adults with and without ADHD. The American
Journal on Addictions, 7, 156-163.
Wilens, T. E., & Biederman, J. (1992). The stimulants. In D. Shafer (Ed.), The psychiatric
clinics of North America (pp. 191-222). Philadelphia, PA: W. B. Saunders.
Wilens, T. E., Biederman, J., & Spencer, T. (1995b). Venlafaxine for adult ADHD (letter).
American Journal of Psychiatry.
Wilens, T. E., Biederman, J., Spencer, T. J., & et al. (1994). Comorbidity of attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder and the psychoactive substance use disorders. Hosp
Community Psychiatry, 45, 421-435.
Wilens, T. E., Biederman, J., Spencer, T. J., & et al. (1999). A pilot controlled clinical trial of
ABT-418, a cholinergic agonist, in the treatment of adults with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(12), 1931-1937.
Wilens, T. E., Spencer, T. J., & Biederman, J. (1995a). Pharmacotherapy of adult ADHD. In
K. G. Nadeau (Ed.), A comprehensive guide to attention deficit disorder in adults
. New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc.
Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). The wide range achievement test. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range,
Inc.
Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder Differences by gender and subtype. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 33(2), 179-191.
Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Chhabildas, N. A., Frieman, M. C., & Alexander, J.
(1999). Psychiatric comorbidity associated with DSM-IV ADHD in a nonreferred
sample of twins. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 38(11), 1355-1362.
Williams, D. T., Meh., R., Yudofsky, S., Adams, D., & Roseman, B. (1982). The effect of
propranolol on uncontrolled rage outbursts in children and adolescents with
organic brain dysfunction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
21(2), 129-135.
Willows, D. M., Kruk, R. S., & Corcos, E. (1993). Visual processes in reading and reading
disabilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum Associates, Inc.
Wilson, B. A. (1987). Rehabilitation of memory. New York: Guilford Press.
Wilson, B. A. (1988). Wilson Reading System Program Overview. Millbury, MA: Wilson
Language Training.
REFERENCES 229
Wing, L. (1985). Early childhood autism and Asperger's syndrome. In P. Pichot, P. Berner,
R. Wolf, & K. Thau (Eds.), Psychiatry: The state of the art (pp. 47-52). New York:
Plenum.
Winokus, G. (1974). The division of depressive illness into depression spectrum disease
and pure depressive disease. International Pharmacopsychiatry, 9,5-13.
Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. (1971). Manual for the children's
embedded figures test Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Witte, R. H., Philips, L., & Kakela. M. (1998). Job satisfaction of college graduates with
learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(7), 259-263.
Wolf, M. (1982). The word-retrieval process and reading in children and aphasics. In K.
nelson (Ed.), Children's Language (pp. 437-493). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Wolf, M. (1991). Word-wraiths: The unique contribution of the naming speed to reading
prediction and intervention in developmental dyslexia. Paper presented at the
Annual meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA.
Wolf, M. (1991a). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive
neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26(2), 123-141.
Wolf, M. (1996, November). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias.
Paper presented at the Orton Dyslexia Society, Boston, MA.
Wolf, M. (1997). A provisional integrative account of phonological and naming-speed
deficits in dyslexia: Implications for diagnosis and intervention. In B. Blachman
(Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition (pp. 67-92). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental
dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 9(3), 415-438.
Wolf, M., Pfeil, C., Lotz, R., & Biddle, K. (1994). Towards a more universal understanding of
developmental dyslexias: The contribution of orthographic factors. In V.
Gerninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge (pp. 137-172). Boston:
Khmer Academic Publishers.
Wong, B. Y. L. (1996). The ABC's of learning disabilities. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Wong, B. Y. L., Butler, D. L, Ficzere, S. A., & Kuperis, S. (1996). Teaching adolescents
with learning disabilities and low achievers to plan, write, and revise opinion
essays. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29(2), 197-212.
Wong, B. Y. L, Butler, D. L, Ficzere, S. A., & Kuperis, S. (1997). Teaching adolescents
with learning disabilities and low achievers to plan, write, and revise compare-
and-contrast essays. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 12(1), 2-15.
Wong, B. Y. L., Butler, D. L., Ficzere, S. A., Kuperis, S., Corden, M., & Zelmer, J. (1994).
Teaching problem learners revision skills and sensitivity to audience through two
instructional modes: Student-teacher versus student-student interactive
dialogues. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 9(2), 78-90.
Wong, B. Y. L., Wong, R., & Blenkinsop, J. (1989). Cognitive and metacognitive aspects of
learning disabled adolescents' composing problems. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 12(4), 300-322.
Wood, D. R., Reimherr, F. W., & Wender, P. H. (1983). The use of l-Deprenyl in the
treatment of attention deficit disorder, residual type. Psychopharmacology
Bulletin, 19(4), 627-629.
Wood, D. R., Reimherr, F. W., Wender, P. H., & Johnson, G. E. (1976). Diagnosis and
treatment of minimal brain dysfunction in adults. Arc Gen Psychiatry, 33, 1453-
1459.
Wood, F., Felton, R., Flowers, L., & Naylor, C. (1991). Neurobehavioral definition of
dyslexia. In D. D. Duane & D. B. Gray (Eds.), The Reading Brain . Parkton, MD:
York Press.
Wood, F. B., Flowers, D. L., Buchsbaum, M., & Tallal, P. (1991). Investigation of abnormal
left temporal functioning in dyslexia through rCBF auditory evoked potentials, and
positron emission tomography. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,
4, 81-95.
230 REFERENCES
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests - Revised. Circle Pines, MN:
AGS.
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests - Revised. Circle Pines, MN:
American Guidance Service.
Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock language proficiency battery - revised. Chicago. IL:
Riverside Publishing Company.
Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson tests of achievement.
Chicago, IL: Riverside Publishing Company.
Woolston, J. L., Rosenthal, S. L., Riddle, M. A., & et al. (1989). Childhood comorbidity of
anxiety/affective disorders and behavior disorders. Journal of American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28,707-713.
Wozniak, J., Biederman, J., Kiely, K., & et al. (1995a). Mania-like symptoms suggestive of
childhood onset bipolar disorder in clinically referred children. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34,867-876.
Xin, M. (2000). A longitudinal assessment of antecedent course work in mathematics and
subsequent mathematical attainment. Journal of Education Research, 94,16-28.
Yakovlev, P. I., & Lecours, A. R. (1967). The myelogenic cycles of regional maturation of
the brain. In A. Minkowski (Ed.), Regional development of the brain in early life
(pp. 3-23). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Yellin, A. M., Hopwood, J. H., & Greenberg, L. M. (1982). Adults and adolescents with
Attention Deficit Disorder: Clinical and behavioral responses to psychostimulants.
Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2(2). 133-136.
Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1991). High functioning individuals with autism: Diagnosis,
empirical findings, and theoretical issues. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 669-
683.
Zametkin, A., & Rapoport, J. (1986). The pathophysiology of attention deficit disorder with
hyperactivity: A review. In B. B. Lahey & A. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical
child psychology (pp.177-216). New York: Plenum Press.
Zametkin, A. J., & Liotta, W. (1998). The neurobiology of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder. Clinical Psychiatry, 59,17-23.
Zametkin. A. J., Nordahl, T. E., Gross, M., King, C., & et al. (1990). Cerebral glucose
metabolism In adults with hyperactivity of childhood onset. New England Journal
of Medicine, 323, 1361-1366.
Zametkin, A. J., & Rapoport, J. L. (1987). Neurobiology of attention deficit disorder with
hyperactivity: Where have we come in 50 years? Journal of American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 676-686.
Zentall, S. S. (1993). Research on the educational implications of attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Exceptional Children, 60, 143-153.
Zentall, S. S., & Ferkis, M. A. (1993). Mathematical problem solving for youth with ADHD,
with and without learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 16, 6-18.
Zerbin-Rudin, E. (1987). Kongenitale wortblindheit oder spezifische dyslexie (congenial
word-blindness). Bulletin of the Orton Society, 17, 47-54.
Zigmond, N. (1990). Rethinking secondary school programs for students with learning
disabilities. Focus on Exceptional Children, 23(1), 1-21.
Zigmond, N., & Thornton, H. (1985). Follow-up of postsecondary age learning disabled
graduates and dropouts. Learning Disabilities Research,1, 50-55.
Zoccolillo, M. (1993). Gender and the development of conduct disorder. Development and
Psychopathology, 5, 65-78.
Zubin, J. (1975). Problem of attention in schizophrenia. In M. L. kietzman, S. Sutton, & J.
Zubin (Eds.), Experimental approaches to psychopathology (pp. 139-166). New
York: Academic Press.
INDEX
231
232 Index
Finger tapping test, 86t, 87, 168 High functioning autism (cont.)
Fluoxetine, 137 ecological considerations, 175–177
fMRI: see Functional magnetic resonance imag- interventions for, 170–177
ing neuropsychological assessment of, 162–170
Focus-execute factor, 156 Hippocampus, 123, 152–153
Follow-up H-O-P-E (Help, Obligations, Plans, Encourage-
of ADHD, 146–147 ment), 143
of reading disorders, 40–42 HRNB: see Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychologi-
Food additives, 118 cal Battery
Foreign language requirement, 37-38 5-HT: see Serotonin
Four factor neuropsychological model of Humor, lack of appreciation for, 79, 173
autism, 155–157 Hyperactive/aggressive ADD, 111
Fragile-X syndrome, 150 Hyperactive Children: A Handbook for Diagno-
Freedom from distractibility factor, 51, 129 sis and Treatment (Barkley), 107
Frontal lobes, 120, 141 Hyperlexia, 158, 171
Functional magnetic resonance imaging
(fMRI), 19, 20, 21, 181 36
ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behav-
Gender ioral Disorders (ICD-10), 7–8
ADHD and, 111, 112-113 IDEA: see Individuals with Disabilities Educa-
reading disorders and, 13, 15, 20–21, 41 tion Act of 1990
Genetic studies Immune disorders, 18
of ADHD, 116–118 Impaired Behavioral Inhibition System (Sub-
of dyslexia, 17–18 type III) ADHD, 110
Georgia-Pacific, 39 Incidence
Gerstman syndrome, 51, 71 of ADHD, 111–115
Global deficits, 54, 55, 80, 81, 92 of learning disabilities, 11–12
Globus pallidus, 120, 121 of nonverbal learning disability, 75–76
Goldstein-Scheerer Color Sorting Test, 165 of reading disorders, 12–15
Goldstein-Scheerer Object Sorting Test, 159, of writing disorders, 15–16
165 Individual Educational Plan (IEP), 99
GORT-3: see Gray Oral Reading Test Individuals with Disabilities Education Act of
Gray Oral Reading Test (GORT-3), 132–133 1990 (IDEA), 2, 10, 140
Grip strength, 86t, 87 Inferior frontal gyrus, 20
Guanfacine (Tenex), 137 Information processing, 169
Guckenberger v. Boston University, 37 InspirationR, 36, 143
Institute for Behavior Genetics, 17
Halstead Category Test, 159, 165 Institute for Psychiatry, London, 17
Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery Institute for Research in Learning Disabilities,
(HRNB) 32
ADHD and, 128 Insurance, 181
high functioning autism and, 162, 168 International Dyslexia Association, 3
learning disabilties and, 22 Interventions
mathematics disorders and, 48, 50, 51 for ADHD (pharmacotherapy), 134–139
nonverbal learning disability and, 80, 81 for ADHD (psychological), 139–144
Halstead Tactual Performance Test, 49 for high functioning autism, 170–177
Hand calculators, 61–64, 66 for mathematics disorders, 53–61
Hanfman-Kasanin Concept Formation Test, 165 for nonverbal learning disability, 95–100
Head trauma: see Brain damage/trauma for reading and writing disorders, 29–40
Hecaen’s anarithmetria, 46 IQ; see also specific assessment tools
High functioning autism, 73, 149–178 ADHD and, 114, 126
cognitive function in, 152–161 autism and, 149, 163
diagnosis of, 150–151 nonverbal learning disability and, 82,93,94
236 Index
Rapid Alternating Stimulus Test (RAS), 23 Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF), 49,
RAS: see Rapid Alternating Stimulus Test 129, 162, 168
Rate disabilities, 6 Rhythm Test, 85t, 87
Reading comprehension, 180 Right brain hemisphere, 45, 70, 77, 88, 122,
high functioning autism and, 167, 169, 181
171–172 Right-left confusion, 51, 103
nonverbal learning disability and, 83, 84, 88 Ritalin, 122, 134, 138–139
Reading disorders, 1–42, 71, 180; see also Dys- Ritalin SR, 134
lexia ROCF: see Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure
accommodations for, 29–40 Rote memory, 79, 175
ADHD and, 114–115 RSA: see Rehabilitation Services Administra-
defining, 2–11 tion
in DSM-IV, 7–9
educational definitions, 2–3 Saccadic eye movements, 157
etiology and neurobiological correlates, SAT: see Scholastic Aptitude Test
16–21 Savants: see Autistic savants
follow-up and outcome studies, 40–42 Scaffolding, 32, 36, 54
genetic research on, 17–18 Schizophrenia, 76
incidence of, 12–15 Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT), 14
interventions for, 29–40 SCID: see Structured Interview for the DSM-
mathematics disorders and, 52 III-R
neuroanatomical studies of, 18–19 SCL-90-R, 127, 128, 147
neuroimaging work, 19–21 Seizures, 169
neuropathological studies of, 18–19 Selective cholinergic channel activators, 137
neuropsychological assessment of, 22–23 Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors
neuropsychological assessment of comorbid- (SSRIs), 137
ity in, 28–29 Self-report inventories, 124–127
nonverbal learning disability and, 80, 81, 92 Self-report sample with learning disabilities
prevalence of, 12–15 (SRLD), 12–13
rehabilitation perspective definitions, 7–11 Serotonin (5-HT), 122, 123
subtypes, 4–6, 181 Sertraline, 137
Reading Memory Test, 166 Shift factor, 156, 157
Recipe for Reading, 30 Short-term memory, 6
Reciprocal teaching, 31, 32 SIM: see Strategies Intervention Model
Recordings for the Blind & Dyslexic Single photon emission tomography (SPECT),
(RFB&D), 34 19, 110, 120
Reframing, 39–40 Smith’s Symbol Digit Modalities Test
Refrigerator parents, 152 (SSDMT), 133
Regional blood flow studies, 120 Smoking, 118, 137
Rehabilitation Act of 1973, 8, 38, 100, 101, Social-emotional learning disability, 73–74
140, 181 Social skills, 70, 96, 103, 150, 173, 177
Rehabilitation perspective on learning disabili- Spatial abilities, 87–88, 168
ties, 7–11 Spatial acalculia, 45, 48
Rehabilitation Services Administration (RSA), Spatial cognitive deficits, 6
7, 8, 93 Spatial processing deficits, 22
Reitan Aphasia Screening Test, 49 Specific mathematics disorder, 46
Remediation SPECT: see Single photon emission tomography
for mathematics disorders, 47, 54–56 Speed of test performance, 156
for nonverbal learning disability, 96–97 Spelling, 3, 16, 30, 31, 114
for reading disorders, 29–31 Spelling Development, Disability, and Instruc-
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT), 23 tion (Moats), 31
Rey Complex Figure Drawing (CFT), 23, 86t SQ3R method, 33
Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale SSDMT: see Smith’s Symbol Digit Modalities
(RADS), 127 Test
240 Index
SSRIs: see Selective serotonin reuptake inhibi- Tricyclic antidepressants, 136, 137
tors Tuberous sclerosis, 150
Strategies Intervention Model (SIM), 32 Twenty Questions procedure, 159, 165, 167
Strategy training, 31–34 Twin Family Reading Study, 17
Strengths, capitalization on, 95 Twin studies, 17, 116
Stroke, 44, 61
Stroop Color-Word Interference Test, 85t, 87, University of Connecticut, 28
128, 129, 130, 133 University of Kansas, 32, 180
Structured Interview for the DSM-III-R University of Massachusetts, 180
(SCID), 91, 92 University of Pittsburgh, 180
Stuttering, 117 Urban Institute, 12
Substance abuse, 28, 113, 138; see also Alco-
hol abuse; Drug abuse Venlafaxine, 137
Subtypes Verbal Fluency Test (FAS), 23
of ADHD, 110–111 Verbalization, 143
of mathematics disorders, 54–56 Verbal learning disabilities, 158
of nonverbal learning disability, 70 Visualization, 143
of reading disorders, 4–6, 181 Visual-perceptual skills, 70, 71
Subtype-to-treatment matching, 97
Visual processing abnormalities, 18–19, 21
Sugar, 118 Visual-spatial functioning, 5, 79, 81, 83, 86t;
Suicide, 91 88, 160
Superior colliculus, 122
Vitamin megadose, 137
Superior parietal cortex, 122 Vocational choice, 64
Supported employment, 103 Vocational function, 93–94
Support groups, 99–100 Vocational rehabilitation, 81–82, 89, 90, 92,
Sustain factor, 156 93, 100, 182
Von Economo’s encephalitis, 121
Tactual Performance Test, 86t, 88
TEACCH, 177 WAIS: see Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale
Technology-Related Assitance Act of 1988 (PL WCST: see Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
100-407), 34
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), 22,
Temporal Lobe ADD, 110 162
Temporal lobes, 110 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-
Tenex (guanfacine), 137 III), 130–131, 162
Test anxiety, 98–99 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised
Test of Language Competence (TOLC), 167
(WAIS-R)
Test of Mathematical Abilities-2 (TOMA-2), ADHD and, 128, 129, 130
131–132 high functioning autism and, 157, 162, 163,
Test of Written Language-3 (TOWL-3), 27, 132 164f, 167, 169
Thalamus, 121, 123 language deficits and, 26
Tic disorders, 115 mathematics disorders and, 48–49, 50
Token Test, 167 nonverbal learning disability and, 84t, 86,
TOLC: see Test of Language Competence 87, 94
TOMA-2: see Test of Mathematical Abilities-2
Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children-III (W1SC-III), 157
Tourette’s Syndrome, 28, 115, 117
TOWL-3: see Test of Written Language-3 Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children-Re-
Trail Making Test vised (WISC-R), 128
ADHD and, 128, 133 Wechsler intelligence scales
high functioningautism and, 159, 165, 166, 168 high functioning autism and, 162, 163, 167,
nonverbal learning disability and, 86t
168
reading disorders and, 23
mathematics disorders and, 46, 51
Index 241