Trevarthen 2003 Culture
Trevarthen 2003 Culture
Trevarthen 2003 Culture
Key Words
Companionship ` Group awareness ` Infant psychology, methods `
Intersubjectivity ` Language foundations
This beautifully crafted paper is both informative and provocative, and I be-
lieve it does excellent work for the science of human development in both respects.
It is presented modestly as a pioneering study of a handful of infants, but the situa-
tion in which these subjects were recorded is new, the method of interpretation is
rich and carefully thought out, and the analysis is detailed and revealing. I believe
some of the more provocative claims, while acting like a salutary ‘wake-up’ call,
reveal a political under-text that we have a right to question, but the main purpose
has been to show how restricted the established methods of research on infant so-
cial intelligence have become, and how much has been missed. The argument is so
erudite and tight and the evidence so enchantingly presented that I have difficulty
choosing my comments.
Why do we need this lesson in infant communitarianism? First we must accept
that the academic science, the teaching and the marketing of infant psychology
needs to be challenged where theories and methods fail to grasp essential qualities
of everyday human experience – how a person acts from birth, and how under-
standing and skill develop.
Contemporary research gives value to replicable facts about pre-defined be-
haviors that constitute measurable acts of intelligence or temperament. Psychologi-
cal subjects are usually tested for their perceptions, what they remember about cer-
tain recurring events, how they ‘think’ about limited propositions or operations, or
how they react emotionally to imposed stimuli. This fact-seeking is always selec-
tive in highly artificial ways, always tending to make ‘discoveries’ that depend
upon looking at subjects in a far from the ‘common sense’, sympathetic way we
look upon one another in ordinary life. It requires balancing by more sensitive and
comprehensive methods of enquiry that can appreciate what the unsophisticated
Motives are manifested in the ways infants’ move their whole bodies, not only
to perceive selectively and learn, but also to attain predicted outcomes when they
act on objects, or toward people. Above all, infants act as if they want to get into
habits of communication with other persons, and not just to perceive and recognize
them as either familiar or strange events. Nevertheless, since Robert Fantz [Fantz &
Nevis, 1967] proved, to philosophical sceptics, that newborn infants had surprising
and useful perceptual preferences for visual patterns that define 3-D objects remote
from their bodies, and that they distinguish human faces as particularly fascinating
objects, cognitive theory tends to draw researchers toward a physicalistic account
that restricts the field of enquiry about infant perception and recognition, as well as
limiting options open to experimental subjects captive in their infant seats. The
babies mainly just turn their heads. But even that simple orientation has a social
potential; and, though they may not see it that way, this sharable subjectivity is
what the experimenters are exploiting.
Perceiving is voluntary – cognition in the service of moving [von Hofsten,
2001]. The inquisitive activity of the infant consciousness was, of course, Piaget’s
discovery, but his philosophy and method led the way to an objective and rational-
istic epistemology – of the ‘little scientist’ in the crib. What Hanus Papousek
[1967] and Richard Held [Held et al., 1980], as well as Robert Fantz, observed in
their particular experiments with very young subjects would never have been re-
In the 1960s, as Selby and Sylvester Bradley report, microanalytic film studies
exposed intricately timed and emotionally qualified behaviors in infant-adult inter-
actions, contact-seeking behaviors that could not be explained as responses to sign
stimuli, or learned reactions. Here I have a small correction to make to their ac-
count. While one group was discovering this at Harvard [Bruner, 1968; Trevarthen,
1998], two other projects, in New York [Stern, 1971] and in Cambridge, Mass. at
MIT [Bateson, 1979], were making crucial and quite independent observations of
infants’ playful ‘interpersonal world’ and ‘protoconversational’ abilities, respec-
tively. This was not one group emerging under Jerome Bruner’s guidance in Har-
vard, but a kind of Zeitgeist or awakening, a response of an educational psycholo-
gist, biologists, a pediatrician, a psychiatrist and a linguist-anthropologist to a
growing awareness that infants have many talents that science may not know.
Infants are actors with expressive and receptive sociability, a sociability that
goes beyond what a young animal needs for protection and nurturance, one that
seeks friendship in the discovery of interesting experiences in a peculiar human
way [Trevarthen, 2003]. Though slow to develop independent mobility compared to
other mammals, and correspondingly more dependent on a mother for getting to
comfort, support, food and drink, a baby begins ready for more than care. The new-
born already shows signs of a unique playful conviviality or ‘chattiness’ by means
of facial, vocal and gestural expressions of inner motive states, including emotional
narratives of being alive and inquisitive in a ‘mind time’ that has expectant
rhythms, phrases and developing story-lines. In intimate contact, a newborn can
instantly respond to a wide range of expressions from the other person’s moving,
detecting their contingent responsiveness. Mothering has its special motivations,
and whether male or female, related or not, a sympathetic human, child or adult, is
inspired with tenderness and joy by proximity to a young infant, a state that is ex-
pressed in the special rhythms and accents of body movement and voicing. Infant
and other seek ‘intersubjective’ engagement, by which I mean engagement of the
intentions, attentions and feelings that define the consciousness of an animal crea-
ture [Trevarthen, 1979].
It is important to note that human expressive movements and sensibilities are
richer than those of other primates, and infants are born with mastery of their essen-
tial characteristics and uses. Thus, engagement even with a newborn is specifically
human in its intensity and narrative power. It is ‘protoconversational’, anticipating
Readiness for the Group, and for Sharing Habits and Knowledge
with Older Teachers, and with Peers
I believe, with Draghi-Lorenz et al. [2001], that the intricate moral aspects of
shared experience and the powerful motives for acquiring knowledge and skills
require ‘relational emotions’ that appraise immediately the standing one has vis-à-
vis the receiving other, and that track changes in that standing. These emotions
must be innate in human beings awareness of one another, and I see clear evidence
from infant behaviors as they learn and display skills that this is so [Trevarthen,
2002]. The so-called basic emotions which set the vocabulary for most psychologi-
cal research on emotional reactions in individuals, do not account for the com-
plexes and changes that actually govern our contacts and relationships. I remember
that Paul Ekman found that photographic composites or ‘blends’ between his emo-
tional Fixed Action Patterns actually made more poignant and morally significant
impressions. But even these were static pictures, and the power of moral encounters
depends on their vitality and its timing, which adds narrative significance to mix-
tures of expressive forms.
I am not quite sure about the elaboration of ‘context specific meanings’, be-
cause I do not see emotions of pride, shame, jealousy and the like as entirely ac-
quired by experience of social contexts, though social situations must condition
their strength and confidence, molding the impulses that come spontaneously with a
naive social awareness. In the descriptions of these infants’ messages these is abun-
dant evidence for the arising in the individuals of states of relating that shape the
discourse, as they weigh up each others’ contributions, not vice versa. So, I am not
easy with the statement that, ‘the physiological significance of particular behaviors
is defined by the shared meaning that they help constitute, not vice versa.’ I believe
that some of the values expressed in engagement are transparent, or instantly inter-
pretable, by a sympathetic/responsive participant/observer as states of the mind/
body. Even with infants, attitudes and moral responsibilities are effective as well as
emergent with meaning acquired in the recollected narrative.
In the delightful soap opera between Mona, Joe and Ann there are recogniz-
able elements in favor of the idea that the infants bring causes that contribute to
what is caused in the history of their encounters. Mona is younger, and she seems
less apt at the social maneuvers, and she gets help from Joe because he seems to
recognize that and acts affably to make her feel more accepted, which miffs Ann.
The elder ones transform Mona’s crying (because she misses her mother!) into the
cat’s chorus. Were the looking patterns influenced by the sex (not gender) of the
infants, since Fiamenghi found clear differences at this age in sociability between
boys and girls in pushchair dyads. There are many questions for further study.
On that ‘rude’ gesture, by the way, isn’t it difficult to know what was intended
and what was felt? I feel sure the posture, gesture and facial grimace is expressive
of a coordinated state of discomfort/avoidance and a clear social message, but did
she ‘intend’ an insult, or was she just so embarrassed that awkwardness overcame
This research seeks to capture and make sense of the spirit of narratives in
triad action, treating the infants as intentional and aware of intentions in each
other’s behavior. First a ‘thick description’ is written in terms of meanings and
feelings, and then a ‘fine grain analysis’ leads to a final ‘inevitably provisional’
interpretation in terms of the weight of ‘best available evidence’. The meaning is
taken to ‘come about’ in the social history of the group, as each infant finds out
what the others are interested in and wanting to show and share. They gained per-
suasive evidence that the infants are clever communicators, making complex emo-
tional evaluations of one another’s acts.
They conclude, ‘In being able to recognize infants as group members, we are
better able to think how to enhance the quality of their group living, to value their
pleasure and struggles in groups and to continue with social and cultural processes
which provide a theoretically sound basis for enabling women looking after the
very young to access companionship.’ (Emphasis added by me.) I hope the discov-
ery of the evolution of meaning and companionship among these infants does not
simply support a feminist position that mothers need liberating from care of infants,
that infants would benefit greatly from out-of-home care in groups, and that infants
need freeing from the oppression of attachment to their mothers. Our social obliga-
tions should not become the judges of our intimacies.
References
Bateson, M.C. (1979). ‘The epigenesis of conversational interaction’: A personal account of research
development. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication (pp.
63–77). London: Cambridge University Press.
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. International Journal of Psychoanalysis,
39, 1–23.
Bråten, S. (1992). The virtual other in infants’ minds and social feelings. In A.H. Wold (Ed.), Fest-
schrift for Ragnar Rommetveit [The dialogical alternative] (pp. 77–97). Oslo, Norway/Oxford,
England: Scandanavian University Press/Oxford University Press.
Bruner, J.S. (1968). Processes of cognitive growth: Infancy. (Heinz Werner Lectures, 1968) Worcester,
MA: Clark University Press with Barri Publishers.
Bruner, J.S. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Custodero, L., & Fenichel, E. (2002). ‘The Musical Lives of Babies and Families’ Zero to Three, Vol.
23, No. 1.
DeCasper, A.J., & Carstens, A.A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: effects on learning and emotion
in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 19–35.
DeCasper, A.J., & Fifer, W.P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother’s voices. Sci-
ence, 208, 1174–1176