Healing Environments
Healing Environments
Healing Environments
Author:
Agnes E. van den Berg, Ph.D.
Wageningen University and Research Center, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Advisors:
Frans C.A. Jaspers, M.D.
University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Cor Wagenaar, Ph.D.
University of Technology, Delft/University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen,The Netherlands
This publication is a product of the project The Architecture of Hospitals organized by the
Foundation 200 years University Hospital Groningen.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:34 Pagina 2
Author: A.E. van den Berg, Ph.D., Wageningen University and Research Center, Wageningen.
Advisors: Fr C.A. Jaspers, M.D., University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen;
C. Wagenaar, Ph.D., University of Technology, Delft/University Medical Center Groningen,
Groningen.
Reviewers: Prof. A.J. Knottnerus, M.D. Ph.D., Health Council of The Netherlands,
The Hague/University of Maastricht, Maastricht; T. Swinkels, M.Sc., BNA, UIA, Architecten
aan de Maas, Maastricht; L. Wessels, M.Sc., BNA, UIA, Netherlands Board for Hospital
Facilities, Utrecht.
Copy editing: DutchTrans, London.
Project management: R. Klijnsma, M.Sc., Center for International Cooperation and Projects,
University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen.
Graphic design: K. Hulsteijn & J.A.B. Wiltink, Alterra, Wageningen.
Printing: Van Eck & Oosterink, Dodewaard.
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced in any form or by any means
without the prior written permission of the publisher.
This review was prepared with the support of the University Medical Center Groningen, Alterra,
Wageningen, and Plants for People. However, any opinions, findings, conclusions or recom-
mendations expressed herein are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views
of the aforementioned parties.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:34 Pagina 3
Contents
Preface 5
1. Introduction 7
2. Healing Environments 9
Asclepieia of ancient Greece 10
Monastic infirmaries of the Middle Ages 10
The pavilion style hospital 11
Planetree hospitals 11
Conclusion: The importance of nature and natural elements 12
3. Evidence-based Design 13
An architectural parallel to evidence-based medicine 14
Evidence for health benefits of nature and natural elements 15
Summary 16
3
4. The Review 17
Types of studies included in this review 18
Search strategy 19
Assessing the quality and relevance of research 19
Summary 21
7. General Discussion 51
Conclusions 51
Research needs 53
Design guidelines 54
Towards a more comprehensive evidence base 55
References 57
Preface
Evidence-based design is a hot topic. Undeniably, architecture has a profound impact on the way
people feel, and architects have been all too eager to underline the healing influences of their work.
For a while, some of them even dressed like physicians to highlight the healing powers of their
architectural designs. When it comes to substantiating these claims, however, architects and town
planners usually had little to offer. Fortunately, times have changed since then. Leading experts in
evidence-based design now maintain that the effects of architecture on health and healing can be
scientifically demonstrated.
For obvious reasons, the hospital plays a dominant role in evidence-based design. If architecture
can promote healing, then hospitals are the places where such effects are most urgently needed.
As such, it is no coincidence that evidence-based medicine provided the inspiration for this new
area of research. As yet, however, evidence-based design does not play a dominant role in the
architecture of hospitals. One of the reasons for this lack of engagement is that healthcare
decision makers have questioned the quality of the available evidence for health impacts of healing
environments. Thus, if evidence-based design is going to revolutionize the architecture of hospitals,
it will first have to bridge the gap between rigorous medical standards for scientific research, and 5
what is accepted as evidence in the social sciences.
The present publication was written to stimulate and deepen the discussion on evidence-based
design during the international conference The Architecture of Hospitals, to be held in Groningen,
April 13-15, 2005. It provides a critical review of research on health benefits of four classical
elements of healing environments: nature, daylight, fresh air, and quiet. The review combines
previous surveys with new data, and offers a broad overview of relevant themes and topics. The
outcomes suggest that there is sound evidence for the health impacts of healing environments
which can no longer be ignored. How this will affect future research and in what way it will enrich
the architecture of hospitals will be among the fundamental issues that evidence-based design will
have to address in the near future.
6
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:34 Pagina 7
1. Introduction
Conventional healthcare settings are rarely therapeutic and frequently increase levels of stress
in patients, family and staff. Indeed, many people have had some of their worst experiences
with buildings in clinics and hospitals. But there are signs that things are changing. The field
of healthcare design is currently undergoing an exciting transformation that will significantly
change the appearance of our hospitals. More and more healthcare administrators and medical
professionals are becoming aware of the need to create healing environments that support the
needs of patients, family and staff. The key factor motivating this awareness has been growing
scientific evidence that the physical environment in which medical care is provided has an impact
on health and well-being.
7
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:34 Pagina 8
Nature and natural elements, such as daylight, fresh air, and quiet, have traditionally played an
important role in the design of healing environments. The use of nature in healthcare settings can
be traced as far back as the Asclepieia of ancient Greece. More recently, long-standing notions of
the healing powers of nature have received support from rigorous scientific research. Among other
things, this research has shown that contact with nature and natural elements can reduce anxiety,
lower blood pressure, and lessen pain (Ulrich, 1999).
As yet, there does not exist a comprehensive and systematic overview of the evidence for the
healing powers of nature and natural elements. Prior systematic reviews of evidence for health
benefits of healing environments have been restricted to clinical evidence (Rubin et al., 1998; Ulrich
et al., 2004). However, much of the evidence for the health benefits of nature and nature elements
has been collected outside clinical settings among healthy populations. Consequently, the narrow
focus of previous reviews may have led to an underestimation of the importance of nature and
natural elements as components of healing environments.
The aim of the present review is to provide a systematic overview of clinical and other evidence for
the health benefits of nature and natural elements as components of healing environments. Four
key features were selected that have been the topic of extensive research inside and outside
8 clinical settings: nature, daylight, fresh air and quiet (or absence of noise). Chapter 2 first provides
a brief historical overview to illustrate the importance of nature and natural elements in the design
of hospitals as healing environments. Chapter 3 discusses previous reviews of evidence for the
health benefits of healing environments. Chapter 4 provides the details of the current review.
Chapter 5 and 6 constitute the core of this review; these chapters discuss the evidence for
the health benefits of nature (views of nature, gardens, and indoor plants) and natural elements
(daylight, fresh air, quiet). The outcomes and implications of the review are summarized in
Chapter 7, along with a discussion of issues that need to be addressed in future research.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 9
2. Healing Environments
The concept of a healing environment is rooted in long-standing traditions of complementary
medicine and holistic healing. Unlike modern Western medicine, which focuses on the causes of
illness (pathogenesis) with curing as the ultimate goal, complementary medicine looks at the
causes of health (salutogenesis) with healing as its core mission. Within the latter tradition, a
healing environment has been defined as an environment that stimulates healing processes by
strengthening an individual's inner powers (Jonas et al., 2003).
Most people have at least some kind of intuitive notion of what a healing environment is (Huelat,
1998). However, there does not yet exist a precise and generally accepted definition of a healing
environment. This chapter describes four historical and contemporary examples of hospitals that
are generally perceived as healing environments. These examples will show that nature and natural
elements have always been key components of healing environments.
9
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 10
The architectural design of Asclepieia confirms the spiritual character of the health care provided in
these temples (Chatzicocoli-Syrakou, 1997). First, in line with the ancient Greek belief in the divine
powers of nature, Asclepieia were mostly erected in valleys at favorable wooded locations near hot
or cold springs. While water had important symbolic cleansing values in ancient Greece, it was also
favored because people thought that springs possessed oracular or prophetic powers (Risse,
1999). Thus, the presence of nature, particularly the presence of water, was an essential feature of
the Asclepieion. Another prominent feature of the Asclepieion was the abaton, the dormitory for the
10 patients. Often this feature was a long, narrow building with an open entry facing to the south so
that the patients could sleep and dream in a well-ventilated room that was warmed by sunlight.
The design of monastic infirmaries contained many elements to promote the spiritual healing
process. An important aspect of the layout of the monastery was the garden. Infirmaries in
monastic cloisters were deliberately located adjacent to a central courtyard or garden so that
patients could contemplate the scenery and make a connection with God. This courtyard was
usually designed to symbolize the Garden of Eden referred to in Genesis of the Bible. Patients,
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 11
in particular sick members of the community, were to lie in small cells with one or two beds that
were both placed at the window in order to enable them to benefit from the healing properties of
the garden. Gardens were also used to grow medicinal herbs, which were applied in the treatment
of patients. In the monastic infirmary, peace and quiet were highly valued, and patients were
encouraged to eat their meals in silence.
The pavilion style hospital was explicitly designed to make of use the natural environment as a
therapeutic instrument. The natural environment was assumed to exert its positive influence on 11
health through three characteristics: fresh air, sunlight, and peaceful, green surroundings.
In particular the principle of fresh air can be considered quite revolutionary. Until the development
of the pavilion style hospital, it had received little attention in the design of healing environments.
However, new ideas on the role of bad air and vapors in the transmission of malaria (literally from
the Italian, mal aria, or bad air) and other diseases led to the recognition of fresh air as one of the
most important healing instruments. Unfortunately, because general hospitals needed to be built
close to their patients in urban settings, a location among green surroundings was not always
feasible. Only more specialized long-term facilities, such as the sanatoriums for tuberculosis
patients, could afford to erect their buildings far from the cities in the vicinity of woods and open
farmland.
Planetree hospitals
In the late 1970s, more and more people, in particular patients and nurses, became dissatisfied
with the sterile and user-unfriendly design and organization of the hospitals of that period
(Frampton, Gilpin, & Charmel, 2003). One patient, named Angelica Thieriot, went through such
a traumatic hospitals experience that she took the initiative to found Planetree, a nonprofit
organization that strives for the transformation of healthcare settings into healing environments.
This new approach has been highly successful. Nowadays, more than 70 healthcare facilities have
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 12
been redesigned and reorganized according to the Planetree model, and numerous other facilities
from around the world have adopted similar models of patient-centered care.
The Planetree model provides guidelines for the design and organization of healthcare facilities.
With respect to facility design, the model dictates that buildings should include efficient layouts that
support the users needs and create home-like spaces. The design should also foster a connection
with nature. Healing gardens, fountains, fish tanks and waterfalls are provided to connect patients,
families and staff with the relaxing, invigorating, healing, and meditative aspects of nature. Auditory
needs are attended to, understanding that gentle sounds rather than blaring noises may help
people to remain calm and regain control.
3. Evidence-based Design
Traditionally, the design of healing environments has been based on practical experience and
philosophical considerations, rather than on scientific evidence. In recent years, however,
advocates of healing environments have begun to realize the importance of scientific evidence.
Scientific evidence is used to improve the effectiveness of design interventions and to gain the
support of healthcare providers, who are trained to rely solely on sound scientific data. This new
scientific approach to the design of healing environments is generally referred to as evidence-
based design.
This chapter begins with an overview of evidence-based design and how this movement has evolved
over the years. Next, the available evidence for the impact of nature and natural elements will be
discussed.
13
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 14
Evidence-based design originally kept a close link to healing environments, focusing on health
outcomes as evidence for the effectiveness of these environments (Ulrich, 1992). An important
benchmark in the history of evidence-based design was a pioneering study by Roger Ulrich
conducted in 1984. This study provided the first rigorous scientific evidence for health impacts of
healing environments. Surgery patients with a view of nature suffered fewer complications, used
less pain medication, and were discharged sooner than those with a view of a brick wall. In the
years since the publication of Ulrichs study, numerous additional studies by research groups from
different countries across the world have reported health benefits associated with views of nature
and other design components of medical facilities.
In 1998, Rubin, Owens, and Golden conducted a first systematic review of the rapidly growing
14 literature. The review was carried out under the auspices of the Center for Health Design, a non-
profit organization dedicated to improving the quality of healthcare environments. Rubin and her
team worked for over 10 years scrutinizing tens of thousands of articles. In the end, they found
only 84 studies that linked features of the physical environment to health outcomes, 39 of which
were classified as having a weak study design. Thus, this first review suggested that there was
only limited scientific evidence for the health impacts of healthcare environments. Nevertheless,
an encouraging result from this study was that the vast majority (88%) of the studies that met the
inclusion criteria found positive correlations between environmental features and health outcomes.
Recently, Ulrich, Zimring, and colleagues (2004) conducted another systematic review for the
Center for Health Design. Their review focused on several areas in which evidence-based design
can make a difference, including staff effectiveness, lower levels of medical errors, prevention of
injuries, better acoustics, and positive distraction. Using this broad approach, Ulrich and his team
identified more than 600 rigorous studies that point to a link between the physical environment and
patient and staff outcomes. According to the authors, this deep and wide evidence base
necessitates urgent action to improve the quality of healthcare environments.
The recent review by Ulrich at al. (2004) shows that evidence-based design has extended well
beyond its original scope. Nowadays, not only health outcomes, but any type of hard data on
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 15
positive impacts of the healthcare environment are incorporated into the evidence base. Moreover,
the kind of environmental features that are taken into consideration are no longer restricted to
those that are characteristic of healing environments. Indeed, in their recent review, Ulrich et al.
(2004) discuss several design features that seem more typical of the impersonal, sterile buildings
that have caused so many traumatic hospital experiences. Among these design features are
alcohol-based hand-rub dispensers and ultra-clean ventilation systems that require the sealing
of windows.
While evidence-based design has drifted away from healing environments, advocates of healing
environments have initiated their own scientific research programs. In particular, the Samueli
Institute, a nonprofit medical research organization dedicated to the scientific study of healing
processes, has recently launched a major initiative to define the components of healing
environments and to set a research agenda to investigate these components (Jonas et al., 2003).
The Samueli Institute has coined the term optimal healing environments to highlight the scientific
character of this approach. The first results of this project have recently been published in a
special issue of the Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine (Volume 10, October
2004).
Another limitation of existing reviews is that these have focused almost exclusively on clinical
evidence, that is, evidence collected in healthcare settings or among clinical populations. However,
the human impacts of nature, daylight, fresh air, and quiet (or noise) are the topics of extensive
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 16
research fields that extend well beyond the clinical setting. By disregarding the outcomes of this
research, reviews within the area of evidence-based design may well have underestimated the
available evidence for the health benefits of these elements.
Summary
Evidence-based design has provided scientific justification for deep-rooted notions on the
importance of the physical environment for health and healing. In recent years, a substantial
knowledge base has become available that can be used by healthcare providers and designers to
make optimal decisions on hospital design. This knowledge base contains some evidence for the
health impacts of classical elements of healing environments, such as nature, daylight, fresh air
and quiet. However, this evidence is difficult to retrieve, and probably incomplete because relevant
studies from non-clinical research have not been included. The aim of the present review is to
provide a comprehensive overview of evidence for the health benefits of nature, daylight, fresh air,
and quiet using outcomes from clinical as well as non-clinical research. The details of this review
process are described in the next chapter.
16
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 17
4. The Review
The current review represents a systematic attempt to collate evidence for the health benefits of
nature, daylight, fresh air, and quiet in healthcare settings. A systematic review differs from other
types of reviews in that it sets out explicit criteria for the scope and types of studies to be included
in the review, uses comprehensive search strategies to identify as much as possible from all the
relevant research, and specifies explicit rules for assessing the methodological quality and
relevance of research (cf. Chalmers & Altman, 1995). This chapter describes how each of these
aspects were handled.
17
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 18
Physical features
The review focuses on the following four physical features:
Nature (views of nature, visits to gardens, indoor plants);
Daylight (incl. artificial lamps that mimic daylight);
Fresh air (supply of outdoor air in buildings);
Quiet (noise-reducing environmental measures).
The evidence for the health impacts of nature is discussed in Chapter 5. The evidence for the other
three elements is discussed in Chapter 6.
An important criterion for the inclusion of studies was whether the way in which the feature was
manipulated was relevant to the architecture and design of buildings. Thus, for example, studies
on the health impacts of quiet (or noise) were only included if the noise was generated by
environmental sources, or modified by design interventions (e.g., noise reducing tiles). In a similar
18 vein, studies on the health impacts of light were only deemed relevant if the light was an integral
part of the indoor environment (e.g., windows, lighting systems), not if it was part of a therapeutic
intervention.
Health outcomes
There are two types of health outcomes distinguished in this review:
Clinical outcomes (e.g., length of stay, medicine intake, infection rate, physiological stress
measures, mortality);
Psychological outcomes (e.g., mood states, alertness, quality of sleep, subjective health and
well-being).
Only studies that employed quantitative measures of outcomes were included in this review.
Thus, studies that employed qualitative measures (e.g., content analysis of personal experiences
of patients or staff) were excluded.
Studies of peoples environmental preferences and perceptions were judged to lie outside the
scope of this review. Similarly, studies that addressed the costs of healthcare (including staff
turnover) were judged inappropriate for this review.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 19
Constraints
Only studies that were published in the English language in peer-reviewed journals after 1975 were
taken into consideration.
Search strategy
The literature search was carried out in two steps. First, prior reviews were scrutinized for relevant
evidence. These prior reviews varied from simple annotated bibliographies (e.g., Carmona et al.,
2001), conceptual reviews (e.g., Devlin & Arneill, 2003; Schweitzer, Gilpin & Frampton, 2004;
Ulrich, 1992; Winkel & Holahan, 1985/1986) and systematic reviews based on exhaustive literature
scans (Rubin et al., 1998; Ulrich et al., 2004). Specialized reviews of research in various sub-
domains including nature (Health Council of The Netherlands, 2004a; Maller et al., 2002; Ulrich,
1999), plants (Pearson-Mims & Lohr, 2000), light (Boyce, Hunter & Bowles, 2003; Veitch, 2001),
fresh air (Wargocki et al., 2002; Seppanen & Fisk, 2004) and noise (Health Council of The
Netherlands, 2004b) were also examined. 19
As a second step in the search process, databases such as PubMed and PsychLit and search
engines on the Internet were used to search for additional evidence. For all the studies described
in this review, at least the abstract was retrieved, and if necessary, the entire article. Annex A
provides a brief summary of each of the articles included in the core evidence base.
Methodological quality
The methodological quality of research is determined in large part by the degree of experimental
control. To rule out alternative explanations of relationships between environmental variables and
health outcomes, a researcher may apply two forms of control:
1. Control over environmental conditions to ensure that health outcomes can be related to the
influence of a specific environmental variable;
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 20
2. Control over the inclusion of respondents in the study, to ensure that differences in health
outcomes between respondent groups cannot be explained by pre-existing personal differences.
There are more and less rigorous forms of control. The most rigorous forms of control are
manipulation of environmental variables by the researcher and random assignment of individuals to
these manipulated environmental conditions. Unfortunately, in real-life settings such as hospitals,
such rigorous control is difficult to implement for reasons related to logistics and limited financial
resources.
Throughout this review, a clear distinction has been kept between studies that are well-controlled,
such as randomized controlled trials, and studies with weaker designs, such as correlational (or
cross-sectional) studies. Studies on multiple design interventions that lack any form of control over
environmental or personal variables, such as many post-occupancy evaluation studies, were not
included in this review.
Relevance
In general, a study is more relevant to the architecture of healthcare settings if it uses samples
from clinical populations (patients, visitors, staff), and/or if it is carried out in a clinical setting.
20 Accordingly, clinical evidence (e.g., evidence from studies conducted in healthcare settings or
among clinical populations) has been distinguished from other types of evidence. Non-clinical
evidence is not by definition weaker than clinical evidence. However, if a non-clinical study
investigated features with a low relevance to healthcare settings (e.g., a visit to a wilderness area,
noise from an airport), it was classified as weak evidence, even if the study was well-controlled.
Table 1 provides an overview of the different types of evidence distinguished in this review. Studies
of types a and b are classified as strong evidence. Studies of types c and d are classified as
weak evidence.
Summary
This chapter has set out the details of the review process. Basically, it was decided to:
Include studies that:
- Investigated impacts of nature, daylight, fresh air, and quiet;
- Employed clinical and/or psychological health outcomes.
Exclude studies that:
- Were not published in peer-reviewed journals;
- Were not published in the English language;
- Were published before 1975;
- Employed qualitative measures;
- Measured people's environmental preferences and perceptions;
- Addressed costs of healthcare (including staff turnover);
- Addressed outcomes only indirectly related to health and well-being;
- Lacked any form of control over environmental or personal variables;
- Are not relevant to the architecture and design of buildings.
Make use of the outcomes of previous reviews in the search process;
Make a distinction between;
Clinical and non-clinical evidence:
- Strong and weak evidence. 21
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 22
22
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 23
23
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 24
Views of nature
A great number of studies have shown that people, in general, experience views of nature as
beautiful and beneficial to their health (see Ulrich, 1993, for a review). In particular, this research
has found that viewing nature is an effective means of providing relief from stress and pain. Several
researchers have proposed that these so-called restorative effects of nature are the result of an
ancient evolutionary history (Ulrich, 1993; Kaplan, 1995). According to these authors, evolution has
made nature an innate source of fascination and restoration in order to stimulate people to seek
contact with non-threatening natural environments that contain resources and opportunities that are
necessary for survival.
Clinical evidence
The health benefits of real or simulated views of nature in clinical settings have been investigated in
1
at least ten studies . One of these studies is the well-known dossier study by Ulrich (1984) among
patients who underwent gall bladder surgery. A remarkable finding of this study was that patients
who were assigned to a room with a view of nature after their surgery required fewer strong
painkillers compared to those who were assigned to a room with a view of a brick wall. This
suggests that views of nature may act as a natural painkiller. However, other explanations cannot
be ruled out. In particular, a weakness of this study is that pairs of patients were matched within a
24 rather broad range of six years. In that period, numerous changes could have taken place in the
hospital that may have caused differences in medication needs between pairs of patients.
Six experiments on the benefits of so-called distraction therapy provide additional support for the
pain-reducing effects of views of nature (Miller, Hickman, & Lemasters, 1992; Tse et al., 2002a,
2002b; Diette et al., 2003; Schneider et al., 2003, 2004). In general, these studies have shown
that patients report less pain and display a higher pain tolerance when they are exposed to nature
scenes (with or without sounds) while undergoing painful procedures. For example, in a recent
study by Diette et al. (2003) 80 patients who were undergoing a painful bronchoscopy procedure
were randomly assigned to either a neutral condition or a condition in which nature scene murals
were placed at the bedside. Results show that patients in the natural intervention condition reported
less pain than patients in the control condition, after adjustment for age, gender, race, education,
health status, and dose of narcotic medication. These results are consistent with the notion that
views of nature may provide relief from pain. However because of the lack of equally distracting,
1
Another, well-designed and frequently cited study by Ulrich, Lundn & Eltinge (1993) on the effects of exposure to nature
paintings on patients recovering from heart surgery does not fulfill the inclusion criteria of this review because it has not been
non-natural control conditions, such as murals of urban settings, results of these studies do not
provide evidence for the superior pain-relieving quality of views of nature as compared to other
interventions.
A well-controlled study by Ulrich, Simons & Miles (2003) among 876 blood donors employed a
stronger design with appropriate control conditions. This study showed that blood donors in a
waiting room had lower blood pressure and pulse on days when a wall mounted television displayed
a videotape of natural settings (a park and a stream), compared to days when the television
displayed a videotape of urban settings (a commercial street and shopping mall). Blood donors also
had lower blood pressure and pulse on days when the television was off, as compared to days
when the television displayed continuous daytime programs. The restorative effects of watching
videotapes of nature were equally strong as the restorative effects of watching no television. Thus,
although this study confirms the superior restorative quality of views of nature as compared to
views of urban settings, it does not support the notion that views of nature are better than viewing
nothing at all.
Finally, two studies have investigated the beneficial effects of viewing aquariums on institutionalized
elderly. In a study by DeSchriver and Riddick (1990), 27 elderly, aged 62 and older, first received a
cognitive stressor and were then randomly placed in front of an aquarium, a video of an aquarium, 25
or a static television screen for eight minutes. Results did not reveal any significant differences in
physiological stress reduction between the treatment groups, everyone perceived whatever
treatment they had as relaxing. However, it should be noted that this study had only limited power
to detect differences between the treatments due to the small number of respondents. Another
study by Edwards and Beck (2002) investigated the influence of viewing an aquarium on food
intake of 62 individuals with Alzheimers disease. The results of this well-controlled study show that
Alzheimers patients who viewed an aquarium while eating showed higher increases in food intake
than patients in a control group who viewed a scenic ocean picture. Observational data suggested
that these effects may be explained by the fact that patients with a history of pacing and wandering
sat for longer periods at the dinner table. Moreover, patients who tended to be lethargic were more
attentive and awake in the presence of the aquariums, which may also have increased their
nutritional intake.
Other evidence
At least 13 non-clinical studies attest to the health benefits of viewing nature, most of which are
also described in a recent review by the Health Council of The Netherlands (2004a). Nine of these
studies consist of well-controlled experiments with strong designs (Ulrich, 1979; Ulrich et al., 1991;
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 26
2
Hartig et al., 1996, Study 1 ; Parsons et al., 1998; Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Lauman et al.,
2003; Van den Berg, Koole & Van der Wulp, 2003; Fredrickson & Branigan, in press, Study 1 &
Study 2).
3
26 In addition to these experimental studies, at least four quasi-experimental studies with weaker
designs have compared groups of respondents who stayed for prolonged periods in spaces that
4
offered window views on natural or built environments (Tennessen & Cimprich, 1995; Kuo, 2001 ;
Fayber-Taylor, Kuo & Sullivan, 2003; Kaplan, 2001). Although respondents in these studies were
not randomly assigned to conditions of natural or built window views, there was usually some
degree of control for systematic differences among groups. Results of these field studies suggest
that adults and children (in particular females) who live in houses with views of urban nature have a
greater ability to concentrate and are less aggressive and more self-disciplined than individuals who
live in houses with views of built environments. They also report greater well-being (Kaplan, 2001).
2
This article also contains a second study, which does not meet the inclusion criteria of this review due to lack of proper
control conditions.
3
A frequently cited study by Moore (1982) among prisoners is not included in this review because views of nature were
Discussion
More than 20 studies have demonstrated a relationship between views of nature and health
outcomes, in particular stress reduction. Thus, there is substantial evidence that viewing nature is
beneficial to peoples health. Nevertheless, for optimal application in healthcare settings, more data
on the effectiveness of different views of nature in different clinical populations is needed. For
example, it is conceivable that certain types of nature scenes, such as strong waves or dark
rainforests, are less suitable for certain groups of patients (e.g., psychotic patients, Alzheimer
patients). In a similar vein, it is conceivable that views of unspectacular nature, such as trees and
grass, are less effective for long-term patients who are bed-ridden and deprived of stimulation.
These patients may benefit more from viewing more spectacular nature scenes or more lively
(real or simulated) urban settings.
Gardens
An increasing number of healthcare facilities have started to create healing gardens near or even
inside their buildings to accommodate the needs of patients, family members, and staff. In doing
so, they make use of insights into the therapeutic benefits of gardens as they have been described
by landscape architects and other experts (e.g., Cooper Marcus & Barnes, 1995; Gerlach-Spriggs,
Kaufman & Warner, 1998; Sadler, 2001; Ulrich, 2002). According to these experts, a visit to
nature not only provides beautiful and restorative views, it may also improve peoples health and 27
well-being through other mechanisms, such as exercise and fostering access to social support.
But as we will see, the wide-spread belief in the benefits of healing gardens has thus far received
little back-up from rigorous scientific research.
Clinical evidence
The literature search did not reveal any studies on the beneficial effects of the use of gardens near
5
healthcare settings that met the criteria for inclusion in this review . One study by Cimprich & Ronis
(2003) investigated the effects of home-based nature activities among a clinical population. In this
intervention study, 157 women with newly diagnosed breast cancer were randomly assigned to a
nature-based intervention or a nonintervention group. The intervention comprised a home-based
program involving 120 minutes of nature-based activities (e.g., visiting a scenic spot, tending plants
or gardens, sitting by a window with natural views) per week for a period of about five weeks.
Attention capacity was measured at the start of the intervention (before surgery) and at the end of
the intervention (after surgery). The intervention group showed significantly greater improvement in
5
A carefully designed study by Whitehouse et al. (2001) does not meet the criteria for inclusion in this study because it lacked
performance on attention tasks from the start to the end of the treatment period than the
nonintervention group. The groups were controlled for differences in education, extent of
surgery and other variables. A weakness of this study is that the women in the nonintervention
group did not receive an alternative treatment that was unrelated to nature. Therefore, alternative
interpretations of the effects in terms of a greater sense of participation in self-care cannot be
ruled out. Moreover, the intervention comprised a wide range of activities, which makes it
impossible to distinguish effects of visits to gardens and other types of nature from effects of
views of nature.
Other evidence
Outside the clinical setting, at least four studies have investigated the health benefits of visiting
nature. In one well-controlled study (Hartig, Mang & Evans, 1991, Study 2), 34 healthy students
who were fatigued from performing mentally fatiguing tasks were randomly assigned to one of
three conditions: walking through an urban park, walking through a built-up area near the park, and
passive relaxation inside a building without window views. Results showed that students who walked
through the park displayed more positive changes in mood, and performed better on a post-test
concentration task. These findings were replicated in a quasi-experimental study among
backpackers who went on a wilderness trip (Hartig et al, 1991, Study 1) and in a more recent
28 experimental study among students who walked through a wilderness area (Hartig et al, 2003).
In this recent experiment, Hartig and colleagues also found evidence that walking through nature
provided positive changes in physiological stress. However, because these latter two studies
focused on remote wilderness areas instead of urban nature, the results are less relevant for
application to hospital settings.
A five-year cohort study among 3144 inhabitants of Tokyo aged 70 years or older found a
significant relationship between the presence of walkable green spaces in urban neighborhoods
and longevity. The probability of five-year survival of the senior citizens increased significantly in
proportion to the space for taking a stroll and the number of parks and treelined streets near the
residence. The relationship between walkable green space and longevity was statistically controlled
for confounding variables such as age, gender, socio-economic status and income. Nevertheless,
alternative explanations in terms of self-selection of the respondents (e.g., elderly in the green
neighborhoods were stronger and healthier to begin with, and thus had greater a-priori
life-expectancy) cannot be ruled out.
Discussion
Despite the widespread belief in the therapeutic effects of gardens near healthcare settings, such
effects have not yet been demonstrated in a rigorous, controlled manner. Positive outcomes of a
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 29
few studies in the context of urban nature and wilderness provide grounds for assuming that being
in nature is indeed beneficial to peoples health and well-being. An important challenge for future
research is to replicate these findings for gardens near healthcare settings. Once such evidence
has become available, research can begin to address more detailed questions on the effectiveness
of different types of gardens for different (clinical) populations and different health outcomes.
Indoor plants
People associate plants with personal well-being and perceive them as soothing and healing. Giving
plants or flowers to a friend or relative who is ill has become customary in many parts of Western
society. These intuitive notions and practices are supported by growing evidence that indoor plants
and flowers can indeed improve peoples health. Most importantly, plants may reduce stress levels
6
and mental fatigue, lift peoples mood, and absorb harmful substances in the air . The evidence for
these beneficial effects of indoor plants will be reviewed below.
Clinical evidence
In hospitals, plants and flowers are traditionally viewed as reservoirs for pathogenic bacteria that
may cause disease. Accordingly, most research on plants and flowers in clinical settings has
focused on health risks rather than health benefits (see LaCharity et al., 2003, for an overview).
As yet, this research has failed to demonstrate an association between bacterial organisms present 29
in the soil of plants and the water of flowers and hospital infections (e.g., Bartzokas, Holley, &
Sharp, 1975; Siegman-Igra et al., 1986). Nevertheless, investigators recommend prudence and
caution in the application of potted plants and flowers in hospitals, especially in acute care settings
with susceptible patients.
A study by Fjeld (2000, Study 2) investigated potential health benefits of plants in a clinical setting.
Foliage plants and full-spectrum lamps were placed in the radiology department of a Norwegian
hospital. On average, the 48 employees in this department reported a 25% decrease in typical sick
building symptoms, such as fatigue, feeling heavy headed, headache, dry throat and dry or itchy
hands. The design of this study suffers from several weaknesses. Because the effects of plants
and full-spectrum lamps were confounded, it is not possible to conclude whether these health
benefits were caused by the plants or the lamps. Moreover, since this study lacked control groups
6
Because the presence of harmful substances in the air is directly related to health, it was considered appropriate to include
this type of evidence in this review. However, evidence of more indirect health effects of plants, such as increasing humidity
levels, filtering dust from the air, or dampening sounds (e.g., Lohr & Pearson-Mims, 1996) were judged to lie outside the
that did not receive any treatment or different treatments, alternative explanations in terms of a
change in routine or special attention from researchers are also possible.
Other evidence
Fjeld (2000, Study 3) investigated the health benefits of foliage plants and full-spectrum light among
120 pupils in a junior high school. This study, which suffered from the same confounding between
plants and lights as the study among hospital workers (Fjeld 2000, Study 2), showed that pupils in
experimental classrooms displayed 21% less symptoms of discomfort. In another study among 51
office workers, Fjeld and colleagues (Fjeld et al., 1998; see also Fjeld, 2000, Study 1) manipulated
the presence of plants without varying the amount of light. They also employed proper control
conditions with nature posters on walls. Results of this well-controlled study once again showed a
23% decrease in symptoms of discomfort among workers in offices with plants.
One possible explanation for the health benefits of plants as demonstrated by Fjeld and colleagues
(1998) is an increase in (perceived) air quality. In line with this explanation, at least eight laboratory
studies have shown that plants can absorb harmful substances from the indoor air (Giese et al,
1994; Oyabu et al., 2001, 2004 (Study 1 & 2); Wolverton, McDonald & Watkins, 1984; Wolverton,
McDonald & Mesick, 1985; Wolverton & Wolverton, 1993; Wolverton & Wolverton, 1996). For
30 example, a group of German scientists (Giese et al., 1994) labeled formaldehyde with a radioactive
carbon 14 tag and followed its absorption and metabolic destruction inside a spider plant
(Chlorophytum comosum). The radioactive carbon 14 tag showed that the formaldehyde was
metabolized and converted into food products such as organic acids, sugars and amino acids.
Notably, the plants in this study became more effective at converting toxic chemicals into food the
longer they were exposed to the chemicals. Similar effects have been found for other potentially
harmful substances such as carbon dioxide and benzene.
A limitation of most research on air purification by plants is that it has been conducted under
artificial circumstances in sealed test chambers or energy-efficient buildings. A recent study by
Oyabu et al. (2004, Study 2) provides additional evidence for the effectiveness of potted plants to
purify the air in real-world room spaces. Three pots with plants were installed in an office room
where formaldehyde was emitted continuously. The concentration was decreased up to 60% by
7
installing the pots . These findings suggest that results of highly artificial experiments on air
purification by plants have some validity in the real world. However, it remains a weakness of this
7
A recent, not yet published, investigation by Burchett and colleagues in Australia (Burchett et al., 2004) found similar effects
type of research that absorption of harmful substances by plants has not yet been related to actual
health outcomes.
Plants may also affect health through psychological mechanisms, such as mood improvement or
reduction of stress and pain. At least six studies have investigated such psychological impacts of
plants in offices. A frequently cited study by Lohr et al. (1996) showed that participants who were
randomly assigned to perform a complex task in a computer room with plants showed quicker
responses and less stress than participants who performed the same task in a room without
plants. These findings were replicated by Russell (1999), who found that participants who
performed a complex task in a heavily planted room showed less stress as indicated by lower skin
conductance than participants who performed the same task in a room with no plants. A weakness
of both of these studies was that they lacked control conditions with equally distracting, non-natural
stimuli. A well-controlled study by Lohr & Pearsons-Mims (2000) provides more convincing evidence
for the pain-reducing effects of plants. They found that more subjects were willing to keep a hand
submerged in ice water for five minutes if they were in a room with plants present than if they were
in a room without plants, or in a room with equally distracting colorful objects.
Other well-controlled studies on psychological influences of plants in offices have reported more
mixed results. For example, Adachi, Rohde, & Kendle (2000) found that the presence of a floral 31
display in a room where participants were watching a movie increased positive feelings of
composition and confidence, but also negative feelings of annoyance. Similarly, the presence of a
foliage display increased feelings of clear-headedness, but also bad temper. Larsen et al. (1998)
investigated the impact of the density of plants in a workplace on concentration and mood. They
found that mood increased with increasing density of plants, but concentration decreased with
increasing density of plants. Shibata & Suzuki (2002) found a positive effect of the presence of
highly visible plants on performance of a creativity task in men but not in women.
Discussion
Plants undoubtedly have been, and continue to be, important decorative elements in healthcare
settings, particularly in waiting rooms and other public areas. Fear that plants and flowers may
transmit disease through their soil and water has not been confirmed by research. In contrast,
research is beginning to document the benefits of indoor plants for human comfort, air quality, and
well-being. However, this research area clearly needs more study, especially since several studies
have documented negative psychological impacts (e.g., lower concentration levels). Apparently,
more work is needed to identify the circumstances under which the presence of plants may
contribute positively and/or negatively to health and well-being.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 32
Summary
Table 2 provides a summary of the 45 studies discussed in this chapter. Only one third of these
studies employed a strong design, and only 12 studies were carried out in a clinical setting or
among clinical populations. Nearly all studies showed positive outcomes on at least one dependent
variable. Two studies found negative psychological impacts for contact with indoor plants.
The amount and strength of the evidence differs considerably depending on which type of
interaction with nature is studied. First, there is substantial and convincing evidence that viewing
nature on a screen or through a window can promote relief from stress and pain. Second, there is
only limited and weak evidence that an actual visit to a natural setting such as a garden or park is
beneficial to peoples health. Third, there is extensive but mostly weak evidence for the health
benefits of contact with indoor plants and flowers. Thus, counter-intuitively, the health benefits of
direct contact with real nature are less well established than the benefits of viewing nature on a
screen or through a window.
An important limitation of research on the health benefits of nature is that the majority of studies
have employed psychological outcome measures, in particular stress-related measures. Since
chronic stress is known to be a causative factor of cardiovascular and gastrointestinal disease, the
32 stress-reducing effects of nature certainly merit a large amount of attention in research. However,
it would improve the credibility of this research if more studies could demonstrate that contact with
nature can indeed help to fight disease and illness.
In sum, while there is mounting evidence for the stress-reducing effects of viewing nature, the
clinical implications of these effects are less well-established. Furthermore, there is a lack of
rigorous clinical evidence on the health benefits of contact with real nature, such as gardens or
plants. Fear that plants and flowers may transmit disease in hospitals has not been confirmed by
research. However, the finding that some studies have reported negative psychological impacts
for indoor plants warrants prudence and caution in the application of real nature inside hospitals.
The evidence
Views of nature
1. DeSchriver, M.M. & Riddick, C.C. (1990). Effects of watching aquariums on elders stress.
Anthrozoos, 4, 44-48.
2. Diette, G.B., Lechtzin, N., Haponik, E., Devrotes, A., & Rubin, H.R. (2003). Distraction therapy
with nature sights and sounds reduces pain during flexible bronchoscopy: A complementary
approach to routine analgesia. Chest, 123 (3), 941-948.
3. Edwards, N. & Beck, A. (2002). Animal-assisted therapy and nutrition in Alzheimers Disease.
Western Journal of Nursing Research, 24, 697-712.
4. Faber Taylor, A., Kuo, F.E., & Sullivan, W.C. (2002). Views of nature and self-discipline:
evidence from inner city children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22, 49-63.
5. Fredrickson, B.L., & Branigan, C. (in press, Study 1 & 2). Positive emotions broaden the scope
of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion.
6. Fredrickson, B.L. & Levenson, R.W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the
cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191-220.
7. Hartig, T., Bk, A., Garvill, J., Olsson, T., & Grling, T. (1996, Study 1). Environmental
influences on psychological restoration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 37, 378-393.
8. Kuo, F.E. (2001). Coping with poverty: Impacts of environment and attention in the inner city. 33
Environment & Behavior, 33 (1), 5-34.
9. Kaplan, R. (2001). The nature of the view from home: Psychological benefits. Environment and
Behavior, 33, 507-542.
10. Laumann, K., Grling, T., & Stormark, K.M. (2003). Selective attention and heart rate responses
to natural and urban environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 125-134.
11. Miller, A.C., Hickman, L.C., & Lemasters, G.K. (1992). A distraction technique for control of
burn pain. Journal of Burn Care and Rehabilitation, 13 (5), 576-580.
12. Parsons, R., Tassinary, L.G., Ulrich, R.S., Hebl, M.R., & Grossman-Alexander, M. (1998).
The view from the road: Implications for stress recovery and immunization. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 18, 113-140.
13. Schneider, S.M., Ellis, M., Coombs, W.T., Shonkwiler, E.L., & Folsom, L.C. (2003). Virtual
reality intervention for older women with breast cancer. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 6 (3),
301-307.
14. Schneider, S.M., Prince-Paul, M., Allen, M.J., Silverman, P., & Talaba, D. (2004). Virtual reality
as a distraction intervention for women receiving chemotherapy. Oncology Nursing Forum,
31 (1), 81-88.
15. Tennessen, T.M. & Cimprich, B. (1995). Views to nature: effects on attention. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 15, 77-85.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 34
16. Tse, M.M.Y., Ng, J.K.F., Chung, J.W.Y., & Wong, T.K.S. (2002a). The effect of visual stimuli on
pain threshold and tolerance. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 11 (4), 462-469.
17. Tse, M.M.Y., Ng, J.K.F., Chung, J.W.Y., & Wong, T.K.S. (2002b). The effect of visual
stimulation via the eyeglass display and the perception of pain. Cyberpsychology & Behavior,
5 (1), 65-75.
18. Ulrich, R. (1979). Visual landscapes and psychological well-being. Landscape research,
4, 17-23.
19. Ulrich, R.S. (1984). View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science,
224, 420-421.
20. Ulrich, R.S., Simons, R.F., Losito, B.D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M.A. & Zelson, M. (1991). Stress
recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 11, 201-230.
21. Ulrich, R.S., Simons, R.F., & Miles, M.A. (2003). Effects of environmental simulations and
television on blood donor stress. Journal of Architectural & Planning Research, 20 (1), 38-47.
22. Van den Berg, A.E., Koole, S.L., & Van der Wulp, N.Y. (2003). Environmental preference and
restoration: (How) are they related? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 135-146.
Gardens
34 1. Cimprich, B. & Ronis, D. (2003). An environmental intervention to restore attention in women
newly diagnosed with breast cancer. Cancer Nursing 26 (4), 284-292.
2. Hartig, T., Evans, G.W., Jamner, L.D., Davis, D.S., & Grling, T. (2003). Tracking restoration
in natural and urban field settings. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 109-123.
3. Hartig, T., Mang, M., & Evans, G.W. (1991, Study 1 and Study 2). Restorative effects of
natural environment experience. Environment and Behavior, 23, 3-26.
4. Takano, T., Nakamura, K., & Watanabe, M. (2003). Urban residential environments and senior
citizens longevity in megacity areas. The importance of walkable green spaces. Journal of
Epidemiological Community Health, 56, 913-918.
Plants
1. Adachi, M., Rohde, C.L.E., & Kendle, A.D. (2000). Effects of floral and foliage displays on
human emotions. HortTechnology, 10, 59-63.
2. Fjeld, T. (2000, Study 2 & Study 3) The effect of interior planting on health and discomfort
among workers and school children. HortTechnology, 10, 46-52.
3. Fjeld, T., Veiersted, B., Sandvik, L., Riise, G., & Levy, F. (1998). The effect of indoor foliage
plants on health and discomfort symptoms among office workers. Indoor and Built
Environments, 7, 204-209.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 35
4. Giese, M., Bauer-Doranth, U., Langebartels, C., & Sandermann, H. (1994) Detoxification of
formaldehyde by the spider plant (Chlorophytum comosum L.) and by soybean (Glycine max L.)
cell suspension cultures. Plant Physiology, 104 :1301-1309.
5. Larsen, L., Adams, J., Deal, B., Kweon, B.S., & Tyler, E. (1998). Plants in the workplace: The
effects of plant density on productivity, attitudes, and perceptions. Environment and Behavior,
30, 261-281.
6. Lohr, V. I., & Pearson-Mims, C.H. (2000). Physical discomfort may be reduced in the presence
of interior plants. HortTechnology, 10, 53-58.
7. Lohr, V.I, Pearsons-Mims, C.H., & Goodwin, G.K. (1996). Interior plants may improve worker
productivity and reduce stress in a windowless environment. Journal of Environment
Horticulture, 14, 97-100.
8. Oyabu, T., Onodera, T., Kimura, H., et al (2001). Purification ability of interior plants for
removing of indoor air polluting chemicals using a tin oxide gas sensor. Journal of Japan
Society for Atmospheric Environment, 34, 319-325.
9. Oyabu, T, Sawada, A. Hashimoto, T., & Yoshioka, T. (2004, Study 1 & 2). Removing
characteristic of indoor air pollutants according to activated carbon pot and plant using a tin
oxide gas sensor. Electrochemistry in Japan, 12, 813-817.
10. Russel, H. (1999). The psychological effects of plants on people in offices. Interiorscape, 19-21.
11. Shibata, S. & Suzuki, N. (2002). Effects of the foliage plant on task performance and mood. 35
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22, 265-272.
12. Wolverton, B. C., McDonald, R. C., & Watkins, Jr., E. A. (1984) Foliage plants for removing
indoor air pollutants from energy-efficient homes. Economic Botany 224-228
13. Wolverton, B.C., McDonald, R.C., & Mesick, H.H. (1985). Foliage plants for the indoor removal
of the primary combustion gases carbon monoxide and nitrogen oxides. Journal of the
Mississippi Academy of Sciences, 30, 1-8.
14. Wolverton, B.C. & Wolverton, J.D. (1993). Plants and soil microorganisms: Removal of
formaldehyde, xylene and ammonia from the indoor environment. Journal of the Mississippi
Academy of Sciences, 38, 11-15.
15. Wolverton, B.C., Wolverton, J.D. (1996). Interior plants: Their influence on airborne microbes
inside energy-efficient buildings. Journal of the Mississippi Academy of Sciences, 41, 99-105.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 36
36
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 37
Daylight
There is no doubt that people prefer daylight to electric lighting as their primary source of
illumination. Studies in the USA (Heerwagen & Heerwagen, 1986), Canada (Veitch, Hine, & Gifford,
1993) and the UK (Cuttle, 2002) have consistently shown that the majority of survey respondents
prefer to work and live in buildings illuminated by daylight. People do not only prefer daylight
because it is more pleasant than electric light, they also tend to believe that daylight is beneficial
for ones health. For example, in a survey by Heerwagen and Heerwagen (1986), 73% of the
respondents indicated that they thought that daylight is better for general and visual health than
electric lighting. 37
Daylight tends to be brighter and it has a more balanced spectrum of colors than most artificial
light sources. Theoretically, there are three routes by which these properties of daylight may
positively influence health and well-being (Boyce et al., 2003): through the visual system (e.g.,
increased visibility), the biological system (e.g., improvement of thr circadian cycle), or the
psychological system (e.g., alleviation of depression). This section reviews the empirical evidence
for health benefits of daylight and artificial lights that mimic properties of daylight.
Clinical evidence
Exposure to artificial high-intensity light (bright light therapy) has been successfully used in the
treatment of patients with seasonal affective disorder and Alzheimers disease (e.g., Ancoli-Israel
et al, 2003; Terman & Terman, 1999). Based on these findings, a number of researchers have
proposed that exposure to natural daylight may also be linked to favorable health outcomes. Up to
now, these ideas have been tested in five studies, all with serious flaws and weaknesses.
Three retrospective dossier studies have investigated the link between sunny hospital rooms
and length of stay and mortality. A study among 174 patients with severe depression found that
patients in sunny rooms had a 2.6-day shorter stay than patients in dull rooms (Beauchemin and
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 38
Hays, 1996). In this study, all the sunny rooms had an outdoor view, while all the dull rooms had
a courtyard view. Therefore, it is possible that the difference in length of stay was caused by a
difference in the quality of the view, rather than the amount of sunlight. A study among 415
unipolar and 187 bipolar depressed inpatients found that bipolar patients in rooms with direct
sunlight in the morning had a 3.67-day shorter stay on average than patients in rooms with sunlight
in the evening (Benedetti et al., 2001). A third study among 628 patients in an intensive care unit
who had had a first heart attack revealed that female patients who were treated in sunny rooms
had a 1.1-day shorter stay on average than female patients in dull rooms (Beauchemin and Hays,
1998). Moreover, mortality in both sexes was consistently higher in dull rooms (12%) than in sunny
rooms (7%). The latter two studies provide no information on the type of view from the windows,
which makes it impossible to conclude whether sunlight was manipulated independently from the
content of the view.
A prospective randomized study among 20 patients with seasonal affective disorder showed that
patients who were treated for one week with a daily one-hour morning walk outdoors (natural light)
showed reduced depressive symptomatology and improved patterns of melatonin and cortisol
secretion as compared to patients in a control group who were treated with low-dose artificial light
(Wirz-Justice et al., 1996). Since it is clear that a morning walk involves factors other than just light
38 exposure (e.g., increased motor behavior and decreased temperature), these findings do not
provide unequivocal evidence for the beneficial effects of daylight per se, as compared to other
factors.
Finally, a recent prospective study of pain medication use among 89 patients undergoing spinal
surgery showed that patients staying on the bright side of the hospital (with an average 46% higher-
intensity sunlight) experienced less perceived stress, marginally less pain, and took 22% less
analgesic medication per hour than patients on the dim side of the hospital (Walch et al., 2005).
Although this study was much better controlled than previous studies on health benefits of sunlight,
it is still likely that amount of sunlight was confounded with type of view. On the dim side of the
hospital, the sunlight was blocked by an adjacent building located 25 meters away. Thus, patients
on the dim side of the hospital probably had a less natural view than patients on the bright side of
the hospital.
Other evidence
Outside the clinical setting, there exists a long tradition of research on human impacts of indoor
lighting. This research has been dominated by investigations of relationships between luminance
and visual task performance, with the result that it is now well understood how light levels affect
visibility (see Veitch, 2001, for a review). Since visual task performance is not a health outcome,
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 39
this research lies outside the scope of this review. In recent years, an increasing number of studies
have investigated health effects of indoor lighting, in particular effects on mood, and alertness/
sleepiness. Only one of these studies investigated the benefits of daylight (Wilkins et al., 1989,
correlational study). In this study, it was found that employees in offices that overlooked a six
storey light well suffered more headaches the lower the office was within the well and thus the less
the amount of daylight. These findings cannot be explained by differences in the content of view,
since all floors looked out on the offices on the opposite side of the light well. However, employees
were not randomly assigned to floors, so alternative explanations in terms of differences in
organization culture or composition of staff across floors cannot be ruled out.
8
At least six studies have investigated health benefits of so-called full-spectrum lamps that mimic
daylight (Baron et al., 1992, Study 1; Boray et al., 1989; Knez, 2001; Kller & Wetterberg, 1993;
Veitch, Gifford, & Hine, 1991; Veitch, 1997). None of these studies provide support for the positive
effects of full-spectrum fluorescent lighting on mood or health. A frequently cited study by Baron et
al. (1992, Study 1) failed to demonstrate hypothesized positive effects of full-spectrum light on
mood. Another study by Boray et al. (1989) among 117 students found no differences in self-
reported arousal between warm white, cool white, and full-spectrum lighting at approximately equal
levels of illuminances. A study by Knez (2001) among 108 students also found no differences in
mood changes between conditions of warm, cool, and full-spectrum lighting. Kller and Wetterberg 39
(1993) studied the effects of full-spectrum and warm light at high and low illuminance levels in a
laboratory made to look like an office environment. They did not find evidence for an alerting effect
of full-spectrum light as measured by delta activity in EEG patterns. By contrast, they did find that a
condition involving full-spectrum lamps with a high illuminance evoked a decrease in social
evaluations of the office space, and an increase in visual discomfort.
Veitch and colleagues (1991; Veitch, 1997) tested the hypothesis that positive beliefs about full-
spectrum lighting, rather than the lamp itself, underlie the persistent anecdotal reports of the
benefits of full-spectrum light. Their research failed to find consistent support for this hypothesis.
However, results of both studies confirmed that type of lamp (cool white fluorescent vs. full-
spectrum) in itself did not influence self-reported mood or task performance.
A number of studies have systematically varied levels of illuminance and lamp color. These studies
have reported somewhat inconsistent results. For example, Kller and Wetterberg (1993) found
8
A study by Wohlfarth & Gates (1985) on the effects of full spectrum light in schools on blood pressure and mood was
that participants EEGs showed less delta activity at higher lighting levels (1700 lux) than at low
lighting levels (450 lux), indicating that bright light has an alerting effect on the central nervous
system. However, Baron et al. (1992) found that lower illuminances were generally associated with
positive affect and task performance, while Knez (1995) reported no significant changes in mood
or task performance after exposure to bright or dim light. Similarly inconsistent findings have been
reported for lamp color.
Knez and colleagues (Knez, 1995, Study 1 and Study 2; Knez & Engmarker, 1998; Knez & Kers,
2000, Knez, 2001) have argued that the inconsistent findings from research on lamp color and
illuminance may be partly explained by the fact that age and gender differences have not been
taken into account. Consistent with this explanation, they found that impacts of lamp color on mood
and performance differed between men and women, and older and younger people. However,
different effects of gender were found in different studies. For example, Knez (1995, Study 1)
found that exposure to cool light increased negative affect in women, but not in men, while Knez
and Engmarker (1998) found that exposure to warm light increased negative affect in women, but
not in men. Accordingly, it seems that the role of personal characteristics in effects of lamp color
and illuminance is not yet completely understood and needs more research.
40 Finally, two experimental studies have shown that flicker from fluorescent light can cause stress
and headaches, even when it is not visible (Wilkins et al., 1989, experimental study; Kller & Laike,
1998). These findings attest to the benefits of daylight, since it is certainly free of flicker. However,
flicker can also be prevented by using high-frequency circuit controls instead of conventional
controls.
Discussion
In modern buildings, daylight is mostly integrated as an architectural statement and for energy
savings. The results of this brief review suggest that the benefits of daylight may extend beyond
architecture and energy. Consistent with classical ideas about healing environments, exposure to
daylight in healthcare settings has been associated with positive health outcomes such as reduced
length of stay, medication intake, and depression. However, because most studies have con-
founded daylight with the content of the view from the window, more research on this issue is
needed. The notion that artificial lights that mimic the properties of daylight is beneficial for
people's health has thus far not received support from empirical studies. However, there is some
indication that certain properties of indoor lighting, such as luminance level, lamp color, and flicker
can affect peoples mood and performance, even though the precise conditions under which
positive or negative effects occur are not well understood.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 41
Taken together, it can be concluded that the research on the health benefits of daylight and indoor
lighting conditions that mimic daylight is as yet far from conclusive and suffers from serious
weaknesses. There is more disagreement than agreement about many dimensions within the
scientific literature, and a considerable research effort will be necessary in order to identify the
best luminous conditions for health outcomes.
Fresh air
The process in which stale indoor air is exchanged for fresh outdoor air is generally referred to
as ventilation. The rate at which the indoor air is renewed per unit of time is called the ventilation
rate, which is usually measured in liters per second (l/s). Ventilation can be achieved by natural or
mechanical systems, the main difference being that mechanical ventilation systems use (electric)
fans to drive the air. Air-conditioning systems are advanced mechanical ventilation systems that
modify the temperature and humidity of the fresh air before it is released into the indoor
environment. These systems often have a low ventilation rate because they recycle air from the
indoor environment.
Ventilation may improve peoples health in several ways: by providing air for breathing, by removing
and diluting indoor pollutants, by adding or removing moisture, and by heating or cooling the
indoor environment. Ventilation may also have harmful effects on indoor air quality and climate if 41
contaminated air is sucked in (Seppnen & Fisk, 2004). This section reviews the scientific evidence
for the health benefits and health risks of ventilation in clinical and other settings.
Clinical evidence
Research on health impacts of ventilation in clinical settings has focused primarily on the health
risks of ventilation systems and the benefits of installing High Efficiency Particulate Air (HEPA) filters
in those systems. At least three well-controlled studies have shown that ventilation in hospitals can
cause an outbreak of infections during construction activities, when the outdoor air that is brought
in is filled with dust and other harmful particulates (Loo et al., 1996; Opal et al., 1986; Oren et al.,
2001). In each of these studies, the outbreak could be controlled by the installation of HEPA-filter
air purifiers and other measures that purify or block the outdoor air, such as sealing windows.
Two studies have investigated the health benefits of increased ventilation in clinical settings. In a
well-controlled study by Everett & Kipp (1991), ventilation rates by air-conditioning systems in three
operating rooms were systematically varied to assess the impact on infection rates. Results
showed that increasing the supply of outdoor air in summer led to a decrease in infection rate
among 1998 patients. A retrospective study among 690 elderly residents of a nursing home found
that attack rates of influenza were much higher in buildings with 30% or 70% recirculation of indoor
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 42
air, than in buildings with no recirculation of air (Drinka et al., 1996). These findings speak to the
benefits of fresh air in buildings. However, participants were not randomly assigned to the buil-
dings, and the ventilation system type was confounded with other architectural factors, such as the
amount of public space and office space. Therefore, it is possible that the lower influenza rates in
one of the buildings were caused by factors other than the ventilation systems.
Non-clinical evidence
The non-clinical literature on the health effects of ventilation in non-industrial indoor environments
(offices, schools, homes, etc.) has recently been reviewed by a multidisciplinary group of European
9
scientists, called EUROVEN (Wargocki et al., 2002). The group found 30 studies that provided
conclusive data on the health effects of ventilation, 22 of which meet the inclusion criteria of this
review. An additional search revealed two additional, more recently published studies (Myatt et al.,
2002; Wargocki, Wyon & Fanger, 2004).
Nine well-controlled studies investigated the relationship between ventilation rates in schools and
offices and self-reported Sick Building Syndrome (SBS) symptoms. Six studies found that higher
ventilation rates were generally associated with decreases in self-reported SBS symptoms (Apte et
al, 2000; Jaakkola et al., 1991, experimental study; Jaakkola & Miettinen, 1995a; Smedje et al.,
42 2000; Sundell et al., 1994; Wargocki et al., 2000). A recent experimental study by Wargocki et al.
(2004) showed that the positive effects of increased outdoor air supply were only obtained with
new filters. When the new filters were replaced with old filters, SBS symptoms increased again,
despite the higher ventilation rate. In two studies, no effects were found on self-reported SBS
symptoms for an altered ventilation rate (Jaakkola et al, 1991, cross-sectional analysis; Menzies
et al., 1993).
Six studies investigated the relationship between ventilation rates in homes, schools, a jail and an
army training camp and medically diagnosed health outcomes. In five well-controlled studies, it was
found that higher ventilation rates were related to a decrease in health outcomes such as asthma
and pneumonia (Brundage et al., 1988; Hoge et al., 1994; Norbck et al, 1995; ie et al, 1999;
Wliner et al., 1998). One study (Warner et al., 2000) found that the installation of mechanical
ventilation systems (which presumably increased ventilation) in homes of 40 asthmatic patients
improved their histamine levels. However, ventilation rates were not measured, so alternative
9
The EUROVEN group also detected 43 studies with serious weaknesses and flaws, most of which detected positive
relationships between ventilation rate and health. These studies are not discussed in the present review, although some of
explanations for these findings in terms of other characteristics of the ventilation system are also
viable.
Two well-controlled studies investigated the relationship between ventilation rates in offices and sick
leave. In one study, it was found that lower levels of outdoor air supply led to an increase in sick-
leave among 3,720 hourly employees of a large manufacturer in 40 buildings with 115
independently ventilated work areas (Milton et al., 2000). Another study among 294 hourly workers
reported no effects on sick leave for an altered ventilation rate (Myatt et al., 2002).
Seven well-controlled studies investigated relationships between ventilation system types and self-
reported SBS symptoms. Six studies found that self-reported SBS symptoms were higher in air-
conditioned buildings than in naturally or mechanically ventilated buildings (Burge et al, 1987; Fisk
et al., 1993; Jaakkola & Miettinen, 1995b; Mendell et al., 1996; Skov et al., 1990; Zweers et al.,
1992). One study found no increased risk of SBS symptoms in buildings in which air was
recirculated by the ventilation system (Sundell et al., 1994).
Discussion
This review of the health impacts of fresh air underlines the importance of taking into account both
clinical and non-clinical evidence when assessing impacts of the physical environment on health. 43
Until now, clinical research has focused almost exclusively on health risks of ventilation. This
research has shown that under certain circumstances, such as construction activities, the supply of
outdoor air may cause an outbreak of infections, especially among susceptible patients. Based on
these findings, it has been recommended to add ultra-clean HEPA filters to ventilation systems
which require the sealing of patient windows (Schulster & Chinn, 2003). However, one may wonder
whether the benefits of such drastic measures outweigh the negative side effects of depriving
patients and staff of fresh air.
There is now strong evidence from research in non-clinical settings, such as homes, schools, and
offices, that the supply of fresh air via ventilation is associated with positive health outcomes, such
as decreased SBS symptoms, decreased asthma, and decreased sick leave. Furthermore, the non-
clinical literature indicates that in buildings with air-conditioning systems there may be an increased
risk of SBS symptoms compared with naturally or mechanically ventilated buildings, and that
improper maintenance, design, and functioning of air-conditioning systems contributes to increased
prevalence of SBS symptoms. Taken together, these findings strongly suggest that, under normal
circumstances, natural and mechanical ventilation systems that allow generous amounts of fresh air
into healthcare buildings are preferable to air-conditioned systems. Only in unusual circumstances
(e.g., during construction activities), or in spaces that require the highest standards of air quality
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 44
(e.g., operating theaters and Intensive Care Units), are ultra-clean air-conditioned systems
preferable to natural ventilation.
Quiet
There is little doubt that hospital environments are less than peaceful. Excessive noise in hospitals
may be generated by three types of sources: (1) personnel and other people; (2) equipment; and
(3) environmental design and acoustics (Milette & Carnevale, 2003). This section focuses on the
evidence for the influence of the latter source of noise on health outcomes.
Clinical evidence
There is substantial evidence that noise can have negative effects on patients and staff (see Biley,
1994; Millette & Carnevale, 2003; Schuster & Weber, 2003; Stansfeld et al, 2000; Topf, 2000, for
reviews). In particular, noise has been found to increase the need for oxygen in neonates, disturb
sleep patterns in children and adults, and increase stress levels. Unfortunately, the role of
environmental design in these negative effects of noise is less well-established. A few studies have
demonstrated a relationship between noise-reducing environmental interventions such as the
application of sound-absorbing tiles on noise levels (e.g., Berens & Weigle, 1996; Philbin & Gray,
10
2002; Schuster & Weber, 2003 ). However, most of these studies did not measure actual health
44 impacts of noise-reducing measures.
Only two studies have measured actual health impacts of noise-reducing environmental
interventions in clinical settings. A well-controlled study by Berg (2001) showed that EEG-arousals in
12 sleeping subjects were significantly reduced when reverberation time was reduced with sound-
absorbing ceiling-tiles. Hagerman et al. (2005) changed the acoustics in a coronary heart unit by
replacing the ceiling tiles with sound-absorbing tiles. They compared the cardiovascular condition of
31 patients who were in the unit before the change (during bad acoustics) to the condition of 63
patients who were in the unit after the change (during good acoustics). Patients with acute
myocardial infarction and unstable angina showed lower pulse amplitude during the night in the
good acoustics period. Moreover, the overall incidence of rehospitalization was higher for the bad
acoustics group. A weakness of this study is that the good and bad acoustics groups consisted
of different individuals and were not randomly assigned. However, the outcomes were controlled
for differences in age, gender, and severity of illness.
10
A frequently cited study by Moore et al. (1998) is not mentioned here because the environmental intervention (closing
Other evidence
The relationship between noise and health is the topic of an extensive field of research, with its own
professional associations and journals. A great deal of this research has focused on the health
impacts of exposure to traffic noise (e.g., noise from airplanes, trains, cars) in schools, houses and
offices. In a recent review of this literature, the Health Council of The Netherlands (2004b) identified
more than 100 empirical studies that demonstrated the adverse effects of traffic noise, at least 50
of which were published in peer-reviewed journals. The main conclusion of this review is that there
is sufficient evidence that traffic noise can negatively affect sleep quality (measured by physio-
logical and self-reported measures) and well-being (measured by the use of sleeping pills and self-
reported mood). These findings have some relevance for the architecture of hospitals, since
hospitals are often located in urban settings near roads, railways and other sources of traffic noise.
However, compared to indoor noise, traffic noise is a relatively unimportant source of noise in
hospitals. Moreover, architects and designers usually have only limited freedom in the selection of
building sites, which is subject to numerous constraints. Therefore, studies on the negative health
impacts of exposure to traffic will not be included in the body of evidence for health impacts of
hospital environments.
Ohrstrom and Bjrkman (1983) investigated traffic noise related sleep disturbance in an apartment
building before and after the installation of noise insulating windows. A weakness of this study is 45
that only three tenants were investigated. However, each of these three tenants that participated in
this study demonstrated a decrease in the number of body movements after the windows had been
insulated, and two reported improved sleep quality. Two other studies by Fidell & Silvati (1991) and
Utley et al. (1986) used much larger samples to investigate the effectiveness of residential
insulation to reduce annoyance by traffic noise. However, none of these studies measured actual
health outcomes.
Discussion
There is substantial and consistent evidence from clinical and non-clinical studies that noise
exposure can harm peoples health and well-being. This evidence attests to the importance of quiet
in healthcare settings. However, the role of the physical environment in the health impacts of noise
(or quiet) is less well-established. Research on the adverse effects of traffic noise in non-clinical
populations provides some indirect evidence for the beneficial effects of locating healthcare
settings in quiet and peaceful surroundings. However, it has not yet been established that exposure
to traffic noise constitutes a health risk for people in healthcare settings. Studies on the
effectiveness of noise-reducing environmental interventions to reduce decibel levels provide some
indirect evidence for the importance of design measures to reduce the negative health impacts of
noise. However, as yet, direct evidence for a relationship between noise-reducing environmental
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 46
measures and health is very scarce. Taken together, the interrelations between the physical
environment, noise, and health are clearly an area of great importance and future research
potential.
Summary
Table 3 provides a summary of the 52 studies on the health impacts of daylight, fresh air, and
quiet that were discussed in this chapter. Most of these studies (82%) employed a strong design,
but less than one-quarter of the studies were carried out in clinical settings or among clinical
populations. The outcomes do not attest unequivocally to the benefits of natural elements. Just
over 60% of the studies showed positive outcomes on at least one dependent variable. Seven
studies found negative impacts on health for daylight and fresh air.
The amount and strength of the evidence varies considerably across the three elements. For
daylight and fresh air it is now well-established that these elements can positively influence health,
although more research in clinical settings and among clinical populations is needed. In particular,
the finding that quite a few studies have demonstrated negative health impacts for daylight and
fresh air warrants the need for more clinical research. The evidence for the health benefits of quiet
is more indirect. On the one hand, there is ample evidence for negative health impacts of noise
46 (and thus, health benefits of quiet) from clinical and non-clinical research. On the other hand, the
role of the physical environment in producing or reducing negative health impacts of noise has only
been demonstrated in a handful of studies.
In sum, while there exists a wealth of evidence for health benefits of daylight, fresh air, and quiet,
more research is needed to identify the optimal environmental conditions for the application of
these benefits in clinical settings. This is particularly true for daylight and fresh air, which have
also been associated with health risks.
The evidence
Daylight
1. Baron, R.A., Rea, M.S., & Daniels, S.G. (1992, Study 1,2,3). Effects of indoor lighting
(illuminance and spectral distribution) on the performance of cognitive tasks and interpersonal
behaviors: The potential mediating role of positive affect. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 1-33.
2. Beauchemin, K.M., & Hays, P. (1996). Sunny hospital rooms expedite recovery from severe
and refractory depressions. Journal of Affective Disorders, 40 (1), 49-51.
3. Beauchemin, K.M., & Hays, P. (1998). Dying in the dark: Sunshine, gender and outcomes in
myocardial infarction. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 91 (7), 352-354.
4. Benedetti, F., Colombo, C., Barbini, B., Campori, E., & Smeraldi, E. (2001). Morning sunlight
reduces length of hospitalization in bipolar depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 62 (3),
221-223.
5. Boray, P.F., Gifford, R., & Rosenblood, L. (1989). Effects of warm white, cool white, and full-
spectrum fluorescent lighting on simple cognitive performance, mood and ratings of others.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 9, 297-308.
6. Knez, I. (1995, Study 1 & Study 2). Effects of indoor lighting on mood and cognition. Journal
of Environmental Psychology, 15, 39-51.
7. Knez, I. (2001). Effects of colour of light on nonvisual psychological processes. Journal of 47
Environmental Psychology, 21, 201-208.
8. Knez, I. & Engmarker, I. (1998). Effects of office lighting on mood and cognitive performance
and a gender effect in work-related judgment. Environment and Behavior, 4, 580-594.
9. Knez, I., & Kers, C. (2000). Effects of indoor lighting, gender, and age on mood and cognitive
performance. Environment and Behavior, 6, 817-831.
10. Kller, T., & Laike, R. (1998). The impact of flicker from fluorescent lighting on well-being,
performance and physiological arousal. Ergonomics, 41, 433-447.
11. Kller, R, Wetterberg, L. (1993). Melatonin, cortisol, EEG, ECG and subjective comfort in
healthy humans: Impact of two fluorescent lamp types at two light intensities. Lighting
Research and Technology, 25 (2), 71-81
12. Veitch, J.A. (1997). Revisiting the performance and mood effects of information about lighting
and fluorescent lamp type. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 17, 253-262.
13. Veitch, J.A., Gifford, R., & Hine, D.W. (1991). Demand characteristics and full spectrum lighting
effects on performance and mood. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11, 87-95.
14. Walch, J.M., Rabin, B.S., Day, R., Williams, J.N., Choi, K., & Kang, J.D. (2005). The effect of
sunlight on post-operative analgesic medication usage: A prospective study of spinal surgery
patients. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67 (1), 156-163.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 48
15. Wilkins, A.J., Nimmo-Smith, I., Sater, A.J., & Bedocs, L., (1989, Study 1 & 2). Fluorescent
lighting, headaches and eyestrain. Lighting Research and Technology, 21, 11-18.
16. Wirz-Justice, A., Graw, P., Krauchi, K., Sarrafzadeh, A., English, J., Arendt, J., & Sand, L.
(1996). Natural light treatment of seasonal affective disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders,
37 (2-3), 109-20.
Fresh air
1. Apte, M.G., Fisk, W.J. and Daisey, J.M. (2000). Associations between indoor CO2
concentrations and sick building syndrome symptoms in US office buildings: an analysis of the
1994-1996 BASE study data, Indoor Air, 10, 246-257.
2. Brundage, J.F., Scott, R.M., Lednar, W.M., Smith, D.W. & Miller, R.N. (1988). Building-
associated risk of febrile acute respiratory diseases in Army trainees. Journal of the American
Medical Association, 259, 2108- 2112.
3. Burge, S., Hedge, A., Wilson, E., Bass, J.H. & Robertson, A. (1987). Sick Building syndrome:
a study of 4,373 office workers. Annals of Occupational Hygiene, 31, 493-504
4. Drinka, P.J., Krause, P., Schilling, M., Miller, B.A., Shult, P. & Gravenstein, S. (1996). Report of
an outbreak: nursing home architecture and influenza-A attack rates, J. Am. Geriatr. Soc., 44,
910- 913.
48 5. Everett W.D., & Kipp, H. (1991). Epidemiologic observations of operating room infections
resulting from variations in ventilation and temperature. American Journal of Infection Control,
19 (6), 277-82.
6. Fisk, W.J., Mendell, M.J., Daisey, J.M., Faulkner, D., Hodgson, A.T., Nematollahi, M., & Macher,
J.M. (1993). Phase 1 of the California healthy building study. Indoor Air, 4, 246-254
7. Hoge, C.W., Reichler, M.R., Dominguez, E.A., Bremer, J.C., Mastro, T.D., Hendricks, K.A.,
Musher, D.M., Elliott, J.A., Facklam, R.R. & Breiman, R.F. (1994). An epidemic of
pneumococcal disease in an overcrowded, inadequately ventilated jail. New England Journal of
Medicine, 331, 643-648
8. Jaakkola, J.J., & Miettinen, P. (1995a). Ventilation rate in office buildings and sick building
syndrome. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 52, 709-714.
9. Jaakkola, J.J. & Miettinen, P. (1995b). Type of ventilation system in office buildings and sick
building syndrome, American Journal of Epidemiology, 141, 755-765.
10. Jaakkola, J.J.K., Heinoneon, O.P, & Seppanen, O. (1991). Mechanical Ventilation in Office
Buildings and the Sick Building Syndrome. An Experimental and Epidemiological Study. Indoor
Air, 1, 111-121
11. Loo, V.G., Bertrand, C., Dixon, C., Vitye, D., DeSalis, B., McLean, A.P., et al. (1996). Control of
construction-associated nosocomial aspergillosis in an antiquated hematology unit. Infection
Control and Hospital Epidemiology, 17 (6), 360-364.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 49
12. Mendell, M.J., Fisk, W.J., Deddens, J.A., Seavey, W.G., Smith, A.H., Smith, D.F., Hodgson,
A.T., Daisey, J.M., & Goldman, L.R. (1996). Elevated symptom prevalence associated with
ventilation type in office buildings. Epidemiology, 7, 583- 589.
13. Menzies, R., Tamblyn, R.M., Farant, J-P, Hanley, J., Nunes, F., Tamblyn R.T. (1993). The effect
of varying levels of outdoor-air supply on the symptoms of sick building syndrome. New
England Journal of Medicine, 328, 821-827
14. Milton, D., Glencross, P., & Walters, M. (2000). Risk of sick-leave associated with outdoor air
supply rate, humidification and occupants complaints. Indoor Air, 10, 212-221.
15. Myatt, T.A., Staudenmayer, J., Adams, K., Walters, M., Rudnick, S.N., & Milton, D.K. (2002).
A study of indoor carbon dioxide levels and sick leave among office workers. Environmental
Health,1 (1):3.
16. Norbck, D., Bjrnsson, E., Janson, C., Widstrm M.J., & Boman, G. (1995). Asthmatic
symptoms and volatile organic compounds, formaldehyde, and carbon dioxide in dwellings.
Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 52, 388-395.
17. ie, L., Nafstad, P., Botten, G., Magnus, P., & Jaakkola, J.K. (1999). Ventilation in homes and
bronchial obstruction in young children. Epidemiology, 10, 294- 299.
18. Opal, S.M., Asp, A.A., Cannady, P.B., Jr., Morse, P.L., Burton, L.J., & Hammer, P.G. (1986).
Efficacy of infection control measures during a nosocomial outbreak of disseminated
aspergillosis associated with hospital construction. Journal of Infectious Diseases, 153 (3), 49
634-637.
19. Oren, I., Haddad, N., Finkelstein, R., & Rowe, J.M. (2001). Invasive pulmonary aspergillosis in
neutropenic patients during hospital construction: Before and after chemoprophylaxis and
institution of HEPA filters. American Journal of Hematology, 66 (4), 257-262.
20. Skov, P. & Valbjrn, O. (1987). The sick building syndrome in the office environment: The
Danish Town Hall study. Environment International, 13, 339-349.
21. Smedje, G. & Norbck, D. (2000). New ventilation systems at select schools in Sweden -
effects on asthma and exposure. Archives of Environmental Health, 55, 18-25.
22. Sundell, J., Lindvall, T., & Stenberg, B. (1994). Associations between type of ventilation and air
flow rates in office buildings and the risk of SBS-symptoms among occupants. Environment
International, 20, 239- 251.
23. Wlinder, R., Norbck, D., Wieslander, G., Smedje, G., Erwall, C. & Venge, P. (1998). Nasal
patency and biomarkers in nasal lavage - the significance of air exchange rate and type of
ventilation in schools. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 71,
479-486.
24. Wargocki, P., Wyon, D.P. & Fanger, P.O. (2004). The performance and subjective responses
of call-center operators with new and used supply air filters at two outdoor air supply rates.
Indoor Air, 14 Suppl 8, 7-16.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 50
25. Wargocki, P., Wyon, D.P., Sundell, J.,Clausen, G., & Fanger, P.O. (2000).The effects of
outdoor air supply rate in an office on perceived air quality, Sick Building Syndrome (SBS)
symptoms and productivity. Indoor Air, 10, 222-236.
26. Warner, J.A., Frederick, J.M., Bryant, T.N., Wiech, C., Raw, G.J., Hunter, C., Stephen,
F.R., McIntyre, D.A. & Warner, J.O. (2000). Mechanical ventilation and high-efficiency vacuum
cleaning: a combined strategy of mite and mite allergen reduction in the control of mite-
sensitive asthma. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunolology, Part 1, 105, 75-82.
27. Zweers, T., Preller, L., Brunekreef, B., & Boleij, J.S.M. (1992). Health and Indoor Climate
Complaints of 7043 office Workers in 61 Buildings in the Netherlands. Indoor Air, 2, 127-136.
Quiet
1. Berg, S. (2001). Impact of reduced reverberation time on sound-induced arousals during sleep.
Sleep, 24 (3), 289-292.
2. Ohrstrom, E. & Bjorkman, M. (1983). Sleep disturbance before and after traffic noise
attenuation in an apartment building. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73 (3),
877-9.
3. Hagerman, I., Rasmanis, G., Blomkvist, V., Ulrich, R., Anne Eriksen, C., & Theorell, T. (2005).
Influence of intensive coronary care acoustics on the quality of care and physiological state of
50 patients. International Journal of Cardiology, 98 (2), 267-70.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 51
7. General Discussion
The preceding two chapters have reviewed 97 studies, representing thirty years of research on
the health impacts of nature, daylight, fresh air, and quiet. The fact that more than 60% of these
studies were published in the last ten years reflects the growing interest in the scientific under-
pinnings of the health benefits of healing environments.
This chapter begins with a summary of the main conclusions. Next, needs for future research
will be discussed, followed by a summary of design guidelines and recommendations for further
reviews.
Conclusions
How strong is the evidence?
Nature, daylight, fresh air, and quiet are often mentioned together as a classical recipe for
creating healing environments. However, the present literature review indicates that the empirical
evidence for the effectiveness of this recipe varies considerably depending on which ingredient is
being considered. This makes it impossible to draw any general conclusions on the strength of the
evidence for these four elements. 51
For each element separately, the available evidence can be summarized as follows:
There is solid evidence that ventilation of fresh air is associated with improved self-reported and
medically diagnosed health;
There is sufficient evidence that viewing nature can reduce stress and pain;
There is weak evidence that the presence of indoor plants can lift people's mood and reduce
self-reported symptoms of physical discomfort;
There is weak and inconclusive evidence for health benefits of daylight in buildings;
There is some evidence for health benefits of environmental measures that promote quiet in
buildings;
There is a lack of direct evidence for benefits of visits to healing gardens.
Health risks
Not all the studies included in this review found positive outcomes. In particular, for fresh air,
daylight, and indoor plants, there were some indications that the presence of these features can
also have negative impacts on psychological and/or clinical health outcomes. In most of the studies
which revealed negative outcomes, qualifying conditions which had probably influenced the results
could be identified. For example, negative health impacts of fresh air were only found when the
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 52
outdoor air was contaminated by construction activities. Whether bright lights and lamp colors
produce a deterioration in mood seemed to depend on what the individuals preferences and
expectations are. And for plants and flowers, it appears that abundance and excessive colorfulness
can induce annoyance and complaints. In sum, it can be concluded that the health risks of nature,
daylight, fresh air and quiet in healthcare settings are small, and can be controlled by taking the
right precautions.
Assuming that the creation of buildings that are both healthy and healing is the ultimate goal of
healthcare design, evidence for the health benefits of features that are perceived as healing as well
as evidence for health risks of features that are perceived as not healing should be given the
highest priority in healthcare design. Evidence for health benefits of elements that are perceived as
unattractive and not healing is less readily applicable, as is evidence on health risks of elements
that are perceived as attractive and healing. This ambiguous evidence requires a careful balancing
of physical health benefits against the psychological well-being of patients and staff.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 53
Taken together, it can be concluded that the architecture of hospitals has much to gain from
considering the combined impacts of environmental features on health and healing.
Research needs
A number of actions can be undertaken to improve the quality of the research base on health
benefits of healing environments. In general, there should be more coordination of efforts across
clinical and non-clinical research disciplines. Furthermore, studies should be designed that meet the
standards of medical research and that allow for accumulation of results by examining common
outcome variables.
More specifically, the following recommendations for further research on health benefits of healing
environments can be made:
1. Design research with adequate control conditions. Controlled research requires a basis for
comparison such that alternative explanations for observed results can be eliminated. Typically,
medical research accomplishes this through the formation of randomly formed groups, one of
which receives a treatment/intervention and one that does not. However, because environmental
interventions are often confounded with other variables, additional control groups that receive
different interventions are typically required to rule out effects of these confounding factors. 53
Furthermore, in order to establish a good comparative base, pre-measures need to be collected
and analyzed so that groups can be matched or, alternatively, statistical control can be applied.
2. Target health outcomes as the primary outcome variable. Although it is useful and interesting to
know about users preferences and perceptions of environmental interventions, it is more
important to obtain data on peoples health as an outcome variable. Preferably, research studies
should include multiple measures of health that combine both psychological and clinical
measures.
3. Conduct more epidemiological studies. In healthcare research, epidemiological (or correlational)
studies have traditionally been used mostly for prevention purposes, to identify the demographic,
behavioral, and biological risk factors for certain diseases. However, the research on ventilation
discussed in this review shows that this type of research is also very useful for identifying the
environmental correlates of health and well-being. The inclusion of large numbers of (healthcare)
settings and subjects allows for statistical control of possible confounding variables, and at the
same time ensures a high external validity. In designing environmental epidemiological studies,
evidence-based design has much to learn from experiences with this type of research in
toxicology and other fields that study environmental risks (Weed, 2002).
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 54
Design guidelines
The present review focused on (quasi-)experimental studies that were designed to establish causal
relationships between design features and health outcomes. The applied value of this type of
research lies mainly in the fact that it provides a rationale for redesigning hospitals. However,
because the findings cannot readily be generalized to complex, real-life settings, experimental
research is not the best source for detailed guidelines on how to implement certain design
measures. Despite these inherent limitations, the studies in this review provide enough information
to formulate some general design guidelines. These guidelines are:
1. Install ventilation systems that allow a generous supply of fresh air. Under normal
circumstances, natural and mechanical ventilation systems that allow fresh air into healthcare
buildings should be preferred over air-conditioned systems. Only in unusual circumstances, e.g.,
during construction activities, in very hot climates, or in spaces that require the highest
standards of air quality, are air-conditioned systems preferable to natural ventilation. Mechanical
ventilation systems should always be implemented with a maintenance program that ensures
efficient operation.
2. Provide visual access to nature. Offer patients, visitors and staff the opportunity to enjoy views
of gardens, trees and other natural elements inside and outside the building. Ensure that
54 reception desks, beds, seats and other furniture are placed in such a way that users have
access to views of nature. Put up video screens or wall-mounted pictures displaying nature
scenes in areas where people are under stress or have to undergo painful procedures. Avoid
obstacles, such as parking lots, that obstruct views of nature from windows or doors.
3. Use sound-absorbing ceiling tiles. There is no doubt that excessive noise is bad for peoples
health. Sound-absorbing ceiling tiles are an effective means to create a quiet environment that
improves health.
4. Be cautious with the application of daylight and lamps that mimic daylight in buildings. Although
daylight is generally preferred, it is not always beneficial to peoples health. The effectiveness
of daylight will depend on how it is delivered. Therefore, design measures aimed at improving
lighting conditions should be carefully evaluated and tested prior to implementation.
5. There is no need to avoid indoor plants in hospitals. Plants and flowers are often avoided in
hospitals out of fear that they can spread disease. At present, there is no empirical evidence
to support this notion. As a precaution, plants and flowers should be avoided in units with
susceptible patients. However, in all other areas, plants are recommended because they can
lift peoples mood and reduce stress and physical discomfort.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 55
To enhance the applicability of the evidence base to architecture and design, a taxonomy of
health-influencing physical dimensions of healthcare settings should preferably use the architects
language. As every architect knows, it is not possible to design even the simplest room without 55
making decisions on materials, textures, colors, shape, and size. Thus far, these basic
architectural dimensions have received little attention in research on health impacts of built
environments. The pattern language approach, in which successful design examples are compared
to unsuccessful examples, may provide a useful tool for developing a common vocabulary for
architects and researchers (e.g., Alexander et al., 1977).
These recommendations represent only a few of the many challenges that are currently facing
evidence-based design of healthcare settings. For the benefit of patients, visitors, and staff, it is
desirable that these challenges soon be overcome and turned into solid guidelines for the
architecture of healthy and healing hospitals.
56
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 57
References
Note: The following is a list of articles, background papers, literature reviews, and other resources
that are not included in the main body of evidence. The references to the research papers that
fulfilled the criteria of this review are included at the end of chapters 5 & 6.
Ancoli-Israel, S., Martin J.L., Gehrman, P., Shochat, T., Corey-Bloom, J., Marler, M., Nolan S., &
Levi, L. (2003). Effect of light on agitation in institutionalized patients with severe Alzheimer
disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11 (2),194-203.
Bartzokas, C., Holley, M., Sharp, C. (1975). Bacteria in flower vase water: incidence and
significance in general ward practice. British Journal of Surgery, 62, 295-297.
Berens, R.J. & Weigle, C.G. (1996). Cost analysis of ceiling tile replacement for noise abatement.
Journal of Perinatology, 16(3 Pt 1) :199-201.
Biley, F.C. (1994) Effects of noise in hospitals. British Journal of Nursing, 3 (3), 110-113. Boyce, P.,
Hunter, C., & Howlett, O. (2003). The benefits of daylight through windows. New York:
Lighting Research Center (available at www.lrc.rpi.edu/programs/daylightdividends ).
Burchett, M., Wood, R., Orwell, R., Tarran, J., Torpy, F., & Alquezar, R. (2004). Plants to improve
office air quality: How many make a difference? First installment, effects of pot-plants on 57
TVOC levels. Draft report to Flower Council of Holland.
Carmona, M., Carmona, S., Clarke, W. (2001). A bibliography of design value. London: The Bartlett
School of Planning (available at www.cabe.org.uk ).
Chalmers, I., & Altman, D. (Eds.). (1995). Systematic reviews. London: BMJ Publishing Group.
Chatzicocoli-Syrakou, S. (1997). The Asklipieions healing environment: Learning from the past.
World Hospitals and Health Services, 33 (2), 22-7.
Cook, G.C. (2002). Henry Currey Friba (1820-1900): Leading Victorian hospital architect, and early
exponent of the pavilion principle Postgraduate Medical Journal, 78, 352-359.
Cooper-Marcus, C. & Barnes, M. (1995). Gardens in Healthcare Facilities: Uses, Therapeutic
Benefits, and Design Recommendations. Martinez, CA: The Center for Health Design.
Cuttle, C. (2002). Identifying the human values associated with window. International Daylighting,
5, 3-6.
Devlin, A.S. & Arneil, A.B. (2003). Health care environments and patient outcomes: A review of the
literature. Environment & Behavior, 35, 665-694.
Fidell S. & Silvati L. (1991). An assessment of the effect of residential acoustic insulation on
prevalence of annoyance in an airport community. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America,
89 (1), 244-247.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 58
Frampton, S.B., Gilpin, L., & Charmel, P (2003). Putting patients first: Designing and practicing
patient-centered care. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Gerlach-Spriggs, N., Kaufman, R.E., & Warner, S.B., Jr. (1998). Restorative gardens: The healing
landscape. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Hamilton, D.K. (2004). Certification for evidence-based projects. Healthcare Design, 4 [Mar]:43-6.
Health Council of the Netherlands (2004b). Effects of noise on sleep and health. Publication no.
2004/14. The Hague: Health Council of the Netherlands.
Health Council of the Netherlands (2004a). Nature and Health. The influence of nature on social,
psychological and physical well-being. Publication no. 2004/09. The Hague: Health Council of
the Netherlands and Dutch Advisory Council for Research on Spatial Planning.
Heerwagen, J.H. & Heerwagen, D.R. (1986). Lighting and psychological comfort. Lighting Design +
Application, 16 (4), 47-51.
Huelat, B.J. (1998). 10 myths of healing environments. A healthy space doesn't have to be a huge
headache. Health Facilities Management, 11 (2):24-8, 30.
Jonas, B.J., Chez, R.A., Duffy, B., & Strand, D. (2003). Investigating the impact of optimal healing
environments. Alternative Therapies, 9 (6), 36-40.
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Towards an integrative framework. Journal
of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169-182.
58 K.L. Poll Foundation for Education, Art and Science (2003). Het gebouw als denkbeeld: Het zieken-
huis [Buildings as ideas: The hospital]. Amsterdam: Verstegen & Stigter.
Kuo, F.E. & Sullivan, W.C. (2001). Aggression and violence in the inner city: Effects of environment
via mental fatigue. Environment and Behavior, 33, 543-571.
LaCharity, L.A. & McClure, R.l. (2003). Are plants vectors for transmission of infection in acute
care? Critical Care Nursing Clinics of North America, 15, 119-124.
Lohr, V.I. & Pearson-Mims, C. (1996). Particulate matter accumulation on horizontal surfaces in
interiors: Influence of foliage plants. Atmospheric Environment, 30, 2565-8.
Maller, C, Townsend, M., Brown, P, St. Leger, L. (2002). Healthy parks healthy people: The health
benefits of contact with nature in a park context. A review of the current literature. Report to
Parks Victoria and the International Park Strategic Partners Group. Melbourne: Deakin
University.
Mens, N. (2003). De architectuur van het psychiatrisch ziekenhuis. [The architecture of the
psychiatric hospital]. Wormer: Inmerc.
Mens, N. & Tijhuis, A. (1999). De architectuur van het ziekenhuis [The architecture of the hospital].
Rotterdam: NAI.
Milette, I.H. & Carnevale, F.A. (2003). Im trying to heal...noise levels in a pediatric intensive care
unit. Dynamics, 14 (4), 14-21.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 59
Moore, E.O. (1982). A prison environments effect on health care service demands. Journal of
Environmental systems 11 (1), 17-34.
Moore, M.M., Nguyen, D., Nolan, S.P., Robinson, S.P., Ryals, B., Imbrie, J.Z. & Spotnitz, W. (1998).
Interventions to reduce decibel levels on patient care units. American Surgeon, 64 (9), 894-9
Pearson-Mims, C.H. & Lohr, V.I. (2000). Reported impacts of interior plantscaping in office environ-
ments in the United States. Hortechnology, 10 (1), 82-86.
Philbin, M.K. & Gray, L. (2002). Changing levels of quiet in an intensive care nursery. Journal of
Perinatology, 22, 455-60.
Risse, G.B. (1999). Mending bodies, saving souls: A history of hospitals. New York, Oxford
University Press.
Rubin, H.R., Owens, A.J., Golden, G. (1998). Status report (1998): An investigation to determine
whether the built environment affects patients medical outcomes. Martinez, CA: The Center
for Health Design.
Sadler, B. (2001). Design to compete in managed healthcare. Facilities Design & Management
(March).
Schuster, R.J. & Weber, M.L. (2003). Noise in the ambulatory health care setting: How loud is too
loud? Journal of Ambulatory Care Management, 26 (3), 243 - 249.
Schweitzer, M., Gilpin, L., & Frampton, S. (2004). Healing spaces: Elements of environmental
design that make an impact on health. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary 59
Medicine, 10, supplement 1, 71-83.
Silverman, B.C. (2002). Monastic medicine: a unique dualism between natural science and spiritual
healing. John Hopkins Undergraduate Research Journal, 1, 10-17.
Siegman-Igra, Y., Shalem, S.A., Berger, S.A., Livio, S., & Michaeli, D. (1986). Should potted plants
be removed from hospital wards? Journal of Hospital Infections, 7, 82-5.
Seppanen, O.A., & Fisk, W.J. (2004). Summary of human responses to ventilation.
Indoor Air. 2004; 14 Suppl 7 :102-18.
Stansfeld, S.A., Haines, M.M., Burr, M., Berry, B., & Lercher, P. (2000). A review of environmental
noise and mental health. Noise and Health, 2 (8), 1-8.
Topf, M. (2000). Hospital noise pollution: an environmental stress model to guide research and
clinical interventions. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 31 (3), 520-528.
Terman M. & Terman, J.S. (1999). Bright light therapy: side effects and benefits across the
symptom spectrum. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60 (11), 799-808.
Utley, W.A., Buller, I.B., Keighley, E.C. & Sargent, J.W. (1986). The effectiveness and acceptability
of measures for insulating dwellings against traffic noise. Journal of Sound and Vibration, 109
(1), 1-18.
Ulrich, R.S. (1984) View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science, 224,
420-421.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 60
Ulrich, R.S. (1992). How design impacts wellness. Healthcare Forum Journal, September/October,
20-25.
Ulrich, R.S. (1993). Biophilia, biophobia and natural landscapes. In: Kellert, S.R. & Wilson, E.O.
(Eds.). The biophilia hypothesis. Washington, DC: Island press.
Ulrich, R.S. (1999). Effects of gardens on health outcomes: Theory and research.
In C.Cooper-Marcus & M. Barnes (Eds.), Healing Gardens: Therapeutic Benefits and Design
Recommendations. New York: John Wiley, pp. 27-86.
Ulrich, R.S. (2002). Health benefits of gardens in hospitals. Paper for conference, Plants for
People, International Exhibition Floriade.
Ulrich, R.S., Lundn, O., and J.L. Eltinge (1993). Effects of exposure to nature and abstract
pictures on patients recovering from heart surgery. Paper presented at the Thirty-Third
Meeting of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, Rottach-Egern, Germany. Abstract
in Psychophysiology, 30 (Supplement 1, 1993) :7.
Ulrich, R., Zimring, C., Quan, X., Joseph, A., Choudhary, R. (2004). The role of the physical
environment in the hospital of the 21st century: a once-in-a-lifetime opportunity. Martinez,
CA: The Center for Health Design.
Van den Noort, J. (1999). Blokken op de bres, geschiedenis van planning en bouw van het nieuwe
Academisch Ziekenhuis Groningen (1967-1997) [History and planning of the new University
60 Hospital Groningen]. Groningen: AZG.
Veitch, J.A. (2001). Lighting quality contributions from biopsychological processes. Journal of the
Illuminating Engineering Society, 30 (1), 3-16.
Veitch, J.A., Hine, D.W., & Gifford, R. (1993). End-users knowledge, beliefs, and preferences for
lighting. Journal of Interior Design, 19 (2), 15-26.
Wargocki, P., Sundell, J., Bischof, W., Brundrett, G., Fanger, P.O., Gyntelberg, F., et al. (2002).
Ventilation and health in non-industrial indoor environments: Report from a European
multidisciplinary scientific consensus meeting (EUROVEN). Indoor Air 12 :113-128.
Weed, D.L. (2002). Environmental epidemiology: basics and proof of cause-effect. Toxicology.
181-182, 399-403.
Whitehouse, S., Varni, J.W., Seid, M., Cooper-Marcus, C., Ensberg, M.J., Jacobs, J.R., et al. (2001).
Evaluating a children's hospital garden environment: Utilization and consumer satisfaction.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21 (3), 301-314.
Wohlfarth, H. & Gates, K.S. (1985). The effects of color-psychodynamic environmental color and
lighting modification of elementary schools on blood pressure and mood: a controlled study.
International Journal of Biosocial Research, 7, 9 -16.
Winkel, G.H., & Holahan, C.J. (1985/1986). The environmental psychology of the hospital: Is the
cure worse than the illness? Prevention in Human Services, Fall/Winter (4 1-2), 11-33.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 61
Viewing Nature
DeSchriver, M.M. & Riddick, C.C. (1990). Effects of watching aquariums on elders stress. Anthrozoos, 4, 44-48.
AIM: To test the relaxation theory by having elderly persons view a live or videotaped aquarium. METHOD: 27 elderly
were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: experimental group members watched a fish aquarium or a fish
videotape, while control group members viewed a placebo videotape. Three eight-minute treatment sessions were
held one week apart. RESULTS: Members of all three groups perceived their treatments as relaxing. Aquarium
observers tended to experience a decrease in pulse rate and muscle tension and an increase in skin temperature;
however these findings were not significant.
Diette, G. B., Lechtzin, N., Haponik, E., Devrotes, A., & Rubin, H.R. (2003). Distraction therapy with nature sights
and sounds reduces pain during flexible bronchoscopy: A complementary approach to routine analgesia. Chest,
123(3), 941-948.
AIM: To determine whether distraction therapy with nature sights and sounds during flexible bronchoscopy (FB)
reduces pain and anxiety. METHOD: 80 patients undergoing FB with conscious sedation were randomly assigned to 61
one of two conditions In the experimental condition, nature scene murals were placed at the bedside, and patients
were provided a tape of nature sounds to listen to before, during, and after the procedure. Patients assigned to the
control group were not offered either the nature scene or the sounds. RESULTS: In a multivariate ordinal logistic
regression model, the odds of better pain control were greater in the intervention patients than in the control
patients, after adjustment for age, gender, race, education, health status, and dose of narcotic medication. Older
patients and patients with better health status reported significantly less pain. There was no difference in self-
reported anxiety between the two groups. CONCLUSION: Distraction therapy with nature sights and sounds
significantly reduces pain in patients undergoing FB.
Edwards, N., & Beck, A. (2002). Animal-assisted therapy and nutrition in Alzheimers Disease. Western Journal of
Nursing Research, 24, 697-712.
AIM: This study examined the influence of fish aquariums on nutritional intake in individuals with Alzheimers disease
(AD). METHOD: Sixty-two individuals with AD who lived in specialized units were studied. Baseline nutritional data
were obtained followed by a 2-week treatment period when the aquariums were introduced. The treatment data
were collected daily for 2 weeks then weekly for 6 weeks. RESULTS Nutritional intake increased with 21% when
the aquariums were introduced and continued to increase during the 6-week weekly follow-up. Weight increased
significantly over the 16-week period. In addition, participants required less nutritional supplementation, resulting in
health care cost savings.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 62
Faber Taylor, A., Kuo, F.E., & Sullivan, W.C. (2002). Views of nature and self-discipline: evidence from inner city
childeren. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22, 49-63.
AIM: To examine the relationship between near-home nature and self-discipline. METHOD: 169 inner city girls and
boys in 12 architecturally identical high-rise buildings with varying levels of nearby nature were compared with
respect to their self-discipline. Parent ratings of the naturalness of the view from home were related to childrens
performance on tests of concentration, impulse inhibition, and delay of gratification. RESULTS: Regressions
indicated that, on average, the more natural a girls view from home, the better her performance at each of these
forms of self-discipline. For boys, who typically spend less time playing in and around their homes, view from home
showed no relationship to performance on any measure. CONCLUSION: These findings suggest that, for girls, green
space immediately outside the home can help them lead more effective, self-disciplined lives. For boys, perhaps
more distant green spaces are equally important.
Fredrickson, B.L., & Branigan, C. (in press, Study 1 & 2). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and
thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion.
AIM: To test the broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001) which hypothesizes that positive emotions
broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. METHOD: Two experiments with 104 college
students were conducted. In each, participants viewed a film that elicited (a) amusement, (b) contentment,
(c) neutrality, (d) anger, or (e) anxiety. Scope of attention was assessed using a global-local visual processing task
62 (Experiment 1) and thought-action repertoires were assessed using a Twenty Statements Test (Experiment 2).
RESULTS: Compared to a neutral state, positive emotions elicited by short films of penguins and nature broadened
the scope of attention in Experiment 1 and thought-action repertoires in Experiment 2. In Experiment 2, negative
emotions, relative to a neutral state, narrowed thought-action repertoires.
Fredrickson, B.L., & Levenson, R.W. (1998, Study 1). Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular
sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191-220.
AIM: To test the hypothesis that certain positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of
negative emotions. METHOD: In Study 1, 60 subjects viewed an initial fear-eliciting film, and were randomly assigned
to view a secondary film that elicited: (a) contentment; (b) amusement; (c) neutrality; or (d) sadness. RESULTS:
Compared to subjects who viewed the neutral and sad secondary films, those who viewed the positive films showing
a puppy and waves exhibited more rapid returns to pre-film levels of cardiovascular activation. COMMENT: This
paper also includes a second study that is irrelevant to health benefits of nature.
Hartig, T., Bk, A., Garvill, J., Olsson, T. & Grling, T. (1996, Study 1). Environmental influences on psychological
restoration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 37, 378-393.
AIM: To test environmental influences on psychological restoration. METHOD: 102 students were randomly assigned
to one of three conditions: viewing slides of nature, slides of built environments, or no slides. In each condition, half
of the subject performed mentally fatiguing task before watching the film RESULTS: Compared to the control
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 63
groups, groups who viewed the natural environment simulation engendered generally more positive emotional self-
reports. Consistent performance effects were not found which suggests that attentional restoration as reflected in
performance is a more time-intensive process. COMMENT: This paper also includes a second study that does not
meet the criteria for inclusion in this review.
Kuo, F.E. (2001). Coping with poverty: Impacts of environment and attention in the inner city. Environment &
Behavior, 33(1), 5-34.
AIM: To investigate whether exposure to green environments can enhance human effectiveness and make lifes
demands in poor inner cities seem manageable. METHOD: 145 female residents of buildings with and without
nearby nature were compared with respect to attentional functioning and effectiveness in managing major life
issues. RESULTS: Residents living in buildings with nearby trees and grass reported better functioning in life, and
performed better on a concentration task than did their counterparts living in barren surroundings. Mediation tests
showed that better life functioning in green surroundings could be explained by better ability to concentrate.
CONCLUSION: These findings suggest that urban public housing environments can be configured to enhance
residents' psychological resources for coping with poverty.
Kaplan, R. (2001). The nature of the view from home: Psychological benefits. Environment and Behavior,
33, 507-542.
AIM: To test the notion that looking out the window may provide numerous opportunities for restoration. METHOD: 63
A survey with both verbal and visual information among 188 residents of six low-rise apartment communities.
RESULTS: Having natural elements or settings in the view from the window contributes substantially to residents
satisfaction with their neighborhood and with diverse aspects of their sense of well-being. Views of built elements, by
contrast, affected satisfaction but not well-being. Views of the sky and weather did not have a substantial effect on
either outcome. CONCLUSION: The potential of nature content in the view from home to contribute so significantly
to satisfaction and well-being suggests clear action mandates.
Laumann, K., Grling, T., & Stormark, K.M. (2003). Selective attention and heart rate responses to natural and
urban environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 125-134.
AIM: To test the hypothesis that exposure to nature stimuli restores depletes voluntary attention capacity and
affects selective attention METHOD: 28 subjects first completed a proofreading task to induce mental load and
then performed Posners attention-orienting task before and after viewing a video of either a natural or an urban
environment. Cardiac inter-beat interval (IBI) was measured continuously to index autonomic arousal. RESULTS:
Before the video both groups reacted faster to validly versus invalidly cued targets in the attention-orienting task.
After the video, the urban group was still faster on validly versus invalidly cued trials, but in the nature group this
difference disappeared. During the video the nature group had a lower heart rate measured as the difference from
baseline than the urban group. CONCLUSION: The results suggest that reduced autonomic arousal during the video
engendered less spatially selective attention in the nature group compared to the urban group.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 64
Miller, A.C., Hickman, L.C., & Lemasters, G.K. (1992). A distraction technique for control of burn pain. Journal of
Burn Care and Rehabilitation, 13(5), 576-580.
AIM: The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of a distraction therapy, in which videos were used in
combination with administration of analgesics, on intensity and quality of pain and on levels of anxiety. METHOD: 17
patients undergoing burn dressing changes were randomly assigned to the treatment or the control group. The
treatment group viewed video programs that were composed of nature scenes accompanied by music. Each was
asked to score his or her present pain intensity and pain rating index with the McGill questionnaire and anxiety with
the Spielberger questionnaire before and after the dressing change. RESULTS: A nested general linear model,
adjusted for age, percent partial-thickness burn, and choice of topical agent, demonstrated that the use of videos
during the dressing changes significantly reduced pain and anxiety
Parsons, R., Tassinary, L.G., Ulrich, R.S., Hebl, M.R., Grossman-Alexander, M. (1998). The view from the road:
Implications for stress recovery and immunization. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 18, 113-140.
AIM: To examine whether stress recovery and/or immunization varies as a function of the roadside environment.
METHOD: 160 students viewed one of four different video-taped simulated drives through outdoor environments
immediately following and preceding mildly stressful events. RESULTS: Participants who viewed nature dominated
drives (in particular drives through a golf course), as compared to participants who viewed artifact-dominated drives,
showed less autonomic activity indicative of stress (e.g., elevated blood pressure and electrodermal activity), as well
64 as altered somatic activity indicative of a decrease in negative affect (e.g., elevated electromyographic (EMG)
activity over the brow region and decreased activity over the cheek region). In addition, participants who viewed
nature-dominated drives experienced quicker recovery from stress and greater immunization to subsequent stress
than participants who viewed artifact-dominated drives. CONCLUSION: These findings support a sympathetic-specific
mechanism that underlies the effect of nature on stress recovery and immunization.
Schneider, S.M., Ellis, M., Coombs, W.T., Shonkwiler, E.L., & Folsom, L.C. (2003). Virtual reality intervention for
older women with breast cancer. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 6(3), 301-307.
AIM: This study examined the effects of a virtual reality distraction intervention on chemotherapy-related symptom
distress levels. METHOD: 16 women aged 50 and older were randomly assigned to receive the VR distraction
intervention (using a head-mounted display) during one chemotherapy treatment and received no distraction
intervention (control condition) during an alternate chemotherapy treatment. The Symptom Distress Scale (SDS),
Revised Piper Fatigue Scale (PFS), and the State Anxiety Inventory (SAI) were used to measure symptom distress.
For two matched chemotherapy treatments, one pre-test and two post-test measures were employed. RESULTS:
Analysis using paired t-tests demonstrated a marginally significant decrease in the SAI (p = 0.10) scores
immediately following chemotherapy treatments when participants used VR. No significant changes were found in
SDS or PFS values. There was a consistent trend toward improved symptoms on all measures 48 h following
completion of chemotherapy.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 65
Schneider, S.M., Prince-Paul, M., Allen, M.J., Silverman, P., & Talaba, D. (2004). Virtual reality as a distraction
intervention for women receiving chemotherapy. Oncology Nursing Forum, 31(1), 81-88.
AIM: To explore the use of virtual reality as a distraction intervention to relieve symptom distress in women receiving
chemotherapy for breast cancer. METHOD: Using a crossover design, 20 adult subjects served as their own
controls. For two matched chemotherapy treatments, one pretest and two post-test measures were employed.
Participants were assigned randomly to receive the a virtual reality distraction intervention consisting of images of
nature during one chemotherapy treatment and received no distraction intervention (control condition) during an
alternate chemotherapy treatment. An open-ended questionnaire elicited each subjects evaluation of the
intervention. RESULTS: Significant decreases in symptom distress and fatigue occurred immediately following
chemotherapy treatments when women used the virtual reality intervention. CONCLUSIONS: The distraction
intervention decreased symptom distress, was well received, and was easy to implement in the clinical setting.
Tennessen, T.M. & Cimprich, B. (1995). Views to nature: effects on attention. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
15, 77-85.
AIM: The purpose of this study was to explore whether university dormitory residents with more natural views from
their windows would score better than those with less natural views on tests of directed attention. METHOD: Views
from dormitory windows of 72 undergraduate students were categorized into four groups ranging from all natural
to all built. The capacity to direct attention was measured using a battery of objective and subjective measures.
RESULTS: Natural views were associated with better performance on attentional measures. 65
Tse, M.M.Y., Ng, J.K.F., Chung, J.W.Y., & Wong, T.K.S. (2002a). The effect of visual stimuli on pain threshold and
tolerance. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 11(4), 462-469.
AIM: To investigate the therapeutic potential of visual stimulation as a nursing intervention. METHOD: In a
randomized, controlled, cross-over study, pain was produced by a modified tourniquet technique in 46 healthy
volunteers. Subjects were randomly allocated to two groups with subsequent cross-over. Those in the experimental
group watched a soundless video display of natural scenery during tourniquet inflation, whereas control group
subjects watched a static blank screen. RESULTS: With the use of visual stimuli, there was a significant increase
in pain threshold and pain tolerance. Gender and the sequence of visual stimuli did not have any significant effect
on pain threshold and pain tolerance.
Tse, M.M.Y., Ng, J.K.F., Chung, J.W.Y., & Wong, T.K.S. (2002b). The effect of visual stimulation via the eyeglass
display and the perception of pain. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 5(1), 65-75.
AIM: To study the effectiveness of an eyeglass display to block off the anxiety-inducing sights and sounds of the
hospital surroundings and create a pleasant environment. METHOD: In this randomized, controlled, crossover study,
72 healthy university student volunteers were asked to wear a light-weight eyeglass that projected a feeling of
watching a 52-inch television screen at 6 1/2 feet in distance while pain was produced by a modified tourniquet
technique. Subjects were randomly assigned to participate in an experimental or control session first, with
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 66
subsequent cross-over. In an experimental session, subjects were instructed to wear the eyeglass and watch the
soundless display of natural scenery during the inflation. In a control session, the eyeglass that subjects wore would
project a static blank screen. RESULTS: During experimental sessions, there was a significant increase in pain
threshold and pain tolerance. The degree of fascination was positively correlated with improvement in pain
threshold, whereas the anxiety level was negatively correlated with improvement in pain threshold.
Ulrich, R. (1979). Visual landscapes and psychological well-being. Landscape research, 4, 17-23.
AIM: To study effects of viewing nature on mood. METHOD: The basic design of the experiment involved showing
colored slides of outdoor environments to a sample of 46 mildly stressed subjects. One group viewed 50 slides of
unspectacular nature scenes dominated by green vegetation. The other group viewed 50 urben scenes lacking
natural elements. The affect states or feelings of the subjects, defined primarily in terms of anxiety emotions, were
measured both immediately before and after the slide exposures. RESULTS: The findings suggest that stressed
individuals feel significantly better after exposure to nature scenes rather than to American urban scenes lacking
nature elements. Compared to the influences of the urban scenes, the salient effect of the nature exposures was
to increase positive affect - including feelings of affection, friendliness, playfulness, and elation.
Urich, R.S. (1984) View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science, 224, 420-421.
AIM: To study relationships between views of nature and recovery from surgery. METHOD: Records on recovery
66 after cholecystectomy of 46 patients in a suburban Pennsylvania hospital between 1972 and 1981 were examined
to determine whether assignment to a room with a window view of a natural setting might have restorative
influences. RESULTS: Patients assigned to rooms with windows looking out on a natural scene had shorter
postoperative hospital stays, received fewer negative evaluative comments in nurses notes, and took fewer potent
analgesics than matched patients in similar rooms with windows facing a brick building wall.
Ulrich, R.S., Simons, R.F., Losito, B.D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M.A. & Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure
to natural and urban environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11, 201-230.
AIM: To test the hypothesis that if individuals are stressed, an encounter with most unthreatening natural
environments will have a stress reducing or restorative influence, whereas many urban environments will hamper
recuperation. METHOD: 120 subjects first viewed a stressful movie, and then were exposed to color/sound
videotapes of one of six different natural and urban settings. Data concerning stress recovery during the
environmental presentations were obtained from self-ratings of affective states and a battery of physiological
measures: heart period, muscle tension, skin conductance and pulse transit time, a non-invasive measure that
correlates with systolic blood pressure. RESULTS: Findings from the physiological and verbal measures converged
to indicate that recovery was faster and more complete when subjects were exposed to natural rather than urban
environments. There was no evidence of pronounced parasympathetic involvement in responses to the urban
settings. Both the stressor film and the nature settings elicited high levels of involuntary or automatic attention,
which contradicts the notion that restorative influences of nature stem from involuntary attention or fascination.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 67
CONCLUSION: Findings were consistent with the predictions of the psycho-evolutionary theory that restorative
influences of nature involve a shift towards a more positively-toned emotional state, positive changes in physiological
activity levels, and that these changes are accompanied by sustained attention/intake.
Ulrich, R.S., Simons, R.F., & Miles, M.A. (2003). Effects of environmental simulations and television on blood donor
stress. Journal of Architectural & Planning Research, 20(1), 38-47.
AIM: To study whether stress in patients undergoing a procedure known to be stressful - blood donation - is affected
by modest changes in a clinic environment. METHOD: Four different environmental conditions were presented to
872 blood donors using wall-mounted television monitors: a videotape of nature settings (Nature); a tape of urban
environments (Urban); daytime television (Television); or a blank monitor (No Television). RESULTS: Blood-pressure
and pulse-rate findings converged to indicate that stress was lower during No Television than Television, and during
Low Stimulation (No Television + Nature) than High Stimulation (Television + Urban). Pulse rates were markedly
lower during Nature than Urban. CONCLUSION: An important clinical implication of the findings is that the common
practice of playing uncontrollable daytime television in healthcare waiting areas where stress is a problem may
actually have stressful, not stress-reducing influences on many patients/consumers.
Van den Berg, A.E., Koole, S.L., & Van der Wulp, N.Y. (2003). Environmental preference and restoration: (How) are
they related? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 135-146
AIM: To test whether the widely documented tendency to prefer natural over built environments is mediated by the 67
greater restorative potential of natural environments. METHOD: 114 participants viewed a frightening movie, and
then were shown a video of either a natural or a built environment. Participants mood ratings were assessed before
and after they viewed the frightening movie, and again after viewing the environmental video. Participants also rated
the beauty of the environment shown (to indicate preference) and performed a test of concentration after viewing
the environmental video. RESULTS: The results indicate that participants perceived the natural environments as
more beautiful than the built environments. In addition, viewing natural environments elicited greater improvement
in mood and marginally better concentration than viewing built environments. Mediational analyses revealed that
affective restoration accounted for a substantial proportion of the preference for the natural over the built
environments. CONCLUSION: These results substantiate the adaptive function of peoples environmental
preferences.
Gardens/Being in nature
Cimprich, B. & Ronis, D. (2003). An environmental intervention to restore attention in women newly diagnosed with
breast cancer. Cancer Nursing 26(4), 284-292.
AIM: This study tested the efficacy of a natural environment intervention aimed at restoring attention. METHOD: 157
women with newly diagnosed breast cancer were assessed for their capacity to direct attention with a brief battery
of objective measures at two time points: approximately 17 days before surgery (time 1) and 19 days after surgery
(time 2). A randomly assigned intervention protocol was initiated after the first assessment and before any treat-
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 68
ment. The intervention comprised a home-based program involving 120 minutes of exposure to the natural
environment per week. RESULTS: The intervention group (n = 83) showed greater recovery of capacity to direct
attention from the pretreatment (time 1) to the preadjuvant therapy period (time 2), as compared with the
nonintervention group (n = 74). A significant effect of the natural environment intervention was observed even after
control was used for the effects of age, education, attention scores at time 1, other health problems, symptom
distress, and extent of surgery. CONCLUSION: The findings suggest therapeutic benefits for capacity to direct
attention from early intervention aimed at restoring attention in women with newly diagnosed breast cancer.
Hartig, T., Evans, G.W., Jamner, L.D., Davis, D.S., & Grling, T. (2003). Tracking restoration in natural and urban
field settings. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 109-123.
AIM: To compare psychophysiological stress recovery and directed attention restoration in natural and urban field
settings. METHOD: Repeated measures of ambulatory blood pressure, emotion, and attention were collected from
112 randomly assigned young adults. To vary restoration needs, half of the subjects began the environmental
treatment directly after driving to the field site. The other half completed attentionally demanding tasks just before
the treatment. RESULTS: After the drive or the tasks, sitting in a room with tree views promoted more rapid decline
in diastolic blood pressure than sitting in a viewless room. Subsequently walking in a nature reserve initially fostered
blood pressure change that indicated greater stress reduction than afforded by walking in the urban surroundings.
Performance on an attentional test improved slightly from the pretest to the midpoint of the walk in the nature
68 reserve, while it declined in the urban setting. Positive affect increased and anger decreased in the nature reserve
by the end of the walk; the opposite pattern emerged in the urban environment. The task manipulation also affected
emotional self-reports.
Hartig, T., Mang, M., & Evans, G.W. (1991, Study 1 & 2). Restorative effects of natural environment experience.
Environment and Behavior, 23, 3-26.
AIM: To investigate the utility of different theoretical models of restorative experience. METHOD: Restorative effects
of nature were investigated in a quasi-experimental field study and a true experiment. The former included
wilderness backpacking and nonwilderness vacation conditions, as well as a control condition in which participants
continued with their daily routines. The latter had urban environment, natural environment, and passive relaxation
conditions. Assessments of restoration consisted of self-reports of affective states, cognitive performance, and, in
the latter study, physiological measures. RESULTS: Convergent self-report and performance results obtained in both
studies offer evidence of greater restorative effect arising from experiences in nature.
Takano, T., Nakamura, K., & Watanabe, M. (2003). Urban residential environments and senior citizens longevity
in megacity areas. The importance of walkable green spaces. Journal of Epidemiological Community Health, 56,
913-918.
AIM: To study the association between public greenery nearby a residence and the longevity of senior citizens in the
densely populated city of Tokyo. METHOD: A cohort study analyzed the five year survival of 3144 people born in
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 69
1903, 1908, 1913, or 1918 who consented to a follow up survey from the records of registered Tokyo citizens in
relation to baseline residential environment characteristics in 1992. RESULTS: The survival of 2211 and the death of
897 (98.9% follow up) were confirmed. The probability of five year survival of the senior citizens studied increased
in accordance with the space for taking a stroll near the residence, parks and tree lined streets near the residence,
and their preference to continue to live in their current community. CONCLUSION: Living in areas with walkable
green spaces positively influenced the longevity of urban senior citizens independent of their age, sex, marital
status, baseline functional status, and socioeconomic status.
Indoor plants
Adachi, M., Rohde, C.L.E., & Kendle, A.D. (2000). Effects of floral and foliage displays on human emotions.
HortTechnology. 10, 59-63.
AIM: To investigate changes in human emotions during exposure to floral and foliage displays. METHOD: 20 subjects
(10 males and 10 females) were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: floral display present, foliage display
present, and no display present. The subjects were shown a video that introduced the University of Reading and
included scenes of landscapes. It was shown that a floral display had positive effects on human emotions, such as
composition and confidence, however, some evidence of a significant increase in annoyance was also found for this
treatment. The foliage display had a somewhat negative effect by slightly increasing bad temper, and the foliage
display tended to have a positive effect on clearheadedness.
69
Fjeld, T. (2000, Study 2 & 3) The effect of interior planting on health and discomfort among workers and school
children. HortTechnology. 10, 46-52.
AIM: To assess the effect of foliage plants or a combination of foliage plants and full-spectrum fluorescent lamps
on self-reported health and discomfort complaints. METHOD: Self-reported health and discomfort complaints of 48
workers in an X-ray department in a Norwegian hospital, and 120 pupils of a junior high school. were assessed
periods with and without plants and full-spectrum lights RESULTS: Health and discomfort symptoms were found to
be 21% to 25% lower during the period when subjects had plants or plants and full-spectrum lighting present
compared to a period without plants. Neuropsychological symptoms, such as fatigue and headache, and mucous
membrane symptoms, such as dry and hoarse throat, seemed to be more affected by the treatments than skin
symptoms, such as itching skin. COMMENT: This paper also describes a study among office workers (Study 1)
which is reported in more detail in Fjeld et al. (1998).
Fjeld, T., Veiersted, B., Sandvik, L., Riise, G., & Levy, F. (1998). The effect of indoor foliage plants on health and
discomfort symptoms among office workers. Indoor and Built Environments, 7, 204-09.
AIM: To assess the effect of foliage plants in the office on health and symptoms of discomfort among office
personnel. METHOD: A cross-over study with randomized period order was conducted; one period with plants in the
office and one period without. A questionnaire consisting of 12 questions related to neuropsychological symptoms,
mucous membrane symptoms and skin symptoms was distributed among the 51 healthy subjects who participated
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 70
in the study. RESULTS: The score sum of symptoms was 23% lower during the period when subjects had plants in
their offices compared to the control period. Complaints regarding cough and fatigue were reduced by 37 and 30%,
respectively, if the offices contained plants. The self-reported level of dry/hoarse throat and dry/itching facial skin
each decreased approximately 23% when plants were present. Overall, a significant reduction was obtained in
neuropsychological symptoms and mucous membrane symptoms, while skin symptoms seemed to be unaffected by
the presence of plants. CONCLUSION: The results from this study suggest that an improvement in health and a
reduction in symptoms of discomfort may be obtained after introduction of foliage plants into the office
environment. COMMENT: This Study is also described in Fjeld (2000).
Giese, M., U., Bauer-Doranth, Langebartels, C., & Sandermann, H. (1994) Detoxification of formaldehyde by the
spider plant (Chlorophytum comosum L.) and by soybean (Glycine max L.) cell suspension cultures. Plant Physiology,
104:1301-1309.
AIM: To assess detoxification of formaldehyde by plants. METHOD: Shoots of spider plants soybean were exposed
to [14C]-formaldehyde over 24 h. RESULTS: Approximately 88% of the recovered radioactivity was plant associated
and was found to be incorporated into organic acids, amino acids, free sugars, and lipids as well as cell-wall
components. Serine and phosphatidylcholine were identified as major metabolic products. CONCLUSION: The results
obtained are consistent with the concept of indoor air decontamination with common room plants such as the spider
plant Formaldehyde appears to be efficiently detoxified by oxidation and subsequent C1 metabolism.
70
Larsen, L. , Adams, J., Deal, B., Kweon, B.S., & Tyler, E. (1998). Plants in the workplace: The effects of plant
density on productivity, attitudes, and perceptions. Environment and Behavior, 30, 261-281.
AIM: To study the effects of indoor plants on participants productivity, attitude toward the workplace, and overall
mood in the office environment. METHOD: In an office randomly altered to include no plants, a moderate number
of plants, and a high number of plants, 81 paid participants performed timed productivity tasks and completed a
survey questionnaire. RESULTS: The results of the productivity task show an inverse linear relationship to the
number of plants in the office, but self-reported perceptions of performance increased relative to the number of
plants in the office. Subjects reported higher levels of mood, perceived office attractiveness, and (in some cases)
perceived comfort when plants were present than when they were not present. Decreased productivity scores were
linked to the influence of positive and negative affect on decision making and cognitive processing.
Lohr, V.I., & Pearson-Mims, C.H. (2000). Physical discomfort may be reduced in the presence of interior plants.
HortTechnology, 10, 53-58.
AIM: To examine the role of plants in pain tolerance. METHOD: 198 subjects were randomly assigned to one of
three conditions: a room with plants, a room with equally distracting colorful objects, and a room without plants.
They were asked to hold their hand in ice water RESULTS: More subjects were willing to keep a hand submerged in
ice water for 5 min if they were in a room with plants present than if they were in a room without plants or colorful
objects. Results from a room assessment survey confirmed that the room with colorful, nonplant objects was as
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 71
interesting and colorful as the room with plants present, but the presence of plants was perceived as making the air
in the room fresher.
Lohr, V.I., Pearsons-Mims, C.H., & Goodwin, G.K. (1996). Interior plants may improve worker productivity and
reduce stress in a windowless environment. Journal of Environmental Horticulture, 14, 97-100.
AIM: To study the effect of plants on productivity and stress in a windowless office. METHOD Subjects were
randomly assigned to be tested in a computer lab at Washington State University when plants were absent or
when they were present. Interior plants, when present, were placed around the sides of the room. Subjects blood
pressure and emotions were monitored while they performed a slightly stressful computer task that measured
reaction times in response to seeing and decoding the shape of a simple object on the screen. RESULTS: When
plants were added to the lab, the subjects were more productive (12% quicker reactions on the computer task)
and less stressed (systolic blood pressure lower). They also reported feeling more attentive when the plants were
present.
Oyabu, T., Sawada, A. Hashimoto, T., & Yoshioka, T. (2004, Study 1 & 2). Removing characteristic of indoor air
pollutants according to activated carbon pot and plant using a tin oxide gas sensor. Electrochemistry in Japan,
12, 813-817.
AIM: To examine the capability of plants to absorb chemical substances in the air. METHOD: In Study 1, an interior
plant pot was made of the activated carbon and installed in an experimental chamber. In Study 2, three pots were 71
installed in a real office room and examined for their removing capability. In the room, formaldehyde was emitted
continuously and the saturated concentration was almost constant. RESULTS: In Study 1, the pot could entirely
absorb the formaldehyde of about 6.5 ppm in five hours. It also had a removing capability to larger molecular
weight chemicals, for example toluene and xylene. In Study 2, the concentration of formaldehyde was decreased
to 60% by installing the pots. CONCLUSION: Plants have a high removing capability to absorb formaldehyde in
experimental chambers and real office room.
Oyabu, T., Onodera, T. Kimura, H. et al (2001). Purification ability of interior plants for removing of indoor air
polluting chemicals using a tin oxide gas sensor. Journal of Japan Society for Atmospheric Environment,
34, 319-325.
AIM: To study the purifying ability of house-plants METHOD: The removal rate of indoor air pollutants from an
experimental sealed chamber by house plants (Golden pothos) was examined. Pa was derived from tin oxide
gas-sensor characteristics for a chemical. Pa is the value of which the peak of the characteristic is divided by the
full-width at half maximum. The smaller the molecular weight of a chemical is, the larger Pa becomes. The following
chemicals were examined; formaldehyde, toluen, xylene, cigarette smoke and car exhaust. RESULTS: The Pa of
Golden pothos was 25 for formaldehyde, and 80 for ammonia gas.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 72
Russel, H. (1999). The psychological effects of plants on people in offices. Interiorscape, 19-21.
AIM: This study tested whether the presence of plants in a room affected the stress levels of people undertaking a
complex test. METHOD: Participants in the study were invited to an office that had no plants or was heavily planted.
Sensors were attached to the skin of the participants to record skin conductivity, heart rate and blood pressure. A
base line recording of stress levels was recorded during a period of 10 minutes, in which participants could get
used to their surrounding. After this 10 minute period, the participants were asked to add up a list of 78 numbers
without using their fingers or counting aloud. During the test, additional distractors, such as the sound of a ringing
telephone or traffic noise, were played to them at random intervals. After the test, a further 10-minute period of rest
was allowed. RESULTS: Of the three types of measurements, skin conductivity showed the greatest difference
between the group exposed to plants and the group in the unplanted office. The results showed that, for this
measurement, stress was reduced in the planted office. The other tests showed very little difference between the
two situations. The results also showed that those people in the planted office recovered from their stress more
quickly than those in the unplanted office during the 10-minute post-test rest period.
Shibata, S. & Suzuki, N. (2002). Effects of the foliage plant on task performance and mood. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 22, 265-272.
AIM: To investigate the effect of leafy plants on subjects task performance and mood. METHOD: 146
undergraduate students performed an association task or sorting task under one of three room arrangements
72 (plants in front of subjects, to the side of subjects, or no plants). The association task was to create no more than
30 words for 20 different items. The sorting task was to sort 180 index cards into Japanese syllabary order.
RESULTS: Task performances of the male subjects using the front arrangement were higher than that of the male
subjects working without plants. CONCLUSION: It was concluded that the presence of the plants affected the
association task more than the sorting task, and male subjects more than female subjects. It was also suggested
that the presence of the leafy plants might affects creative work positively.
Wolverton, B.C., McDonald, R.C., & Watkins, Jr., E.A. (1984) Foliage plants for removing indoor air pollutants from
energy-efficient homes. Economic Botany 224-228.
AIM: To evaluate the ability of foliage plants to remove formaldehyde from contaminated air. METHOD: Golden
pothos (Scindapsus aureus), nephthytis (Syngonium podophyllum) and spider plant (Chlorophytum elatum var.
vittatum) were placed in a sealed, Plexiglas chamber with temperature and humidity control and illuminated
externally with wide spectrum grow lights. RESULTS: The spider plant proved most efficient by sorbing and/or
effecting the removal of up 2.27 mug formaldehyde/cm2 leaf surface area in 6 h of exposure. CONCLUSION:
The immediate application of this new botanical air-purification system should be in energy-efficient homes that
have a high risk of this organic concentrating in the air, due to outgassing of urea-formaldehyde foam insulation,
particleboard, fabrics and various other synthetic materials.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 73
Wolverton, B.C., McDonald, R.C. & Mesick, H.H. (1985). Foliage plants for the indoor removal of the primary
combustion gases carbon monoxide and nitrogen oxides. Journal of the Mississippi Academy of Sciences, 30, 1-8.
AIM: To evaluate the ability of foliage plants to sorb carbon monoxide and nitrogen dioxide, the two primary gases
produced during the combustion of fossil fuels and tobacco. METHOD: Plants were placed in sealed chambers.
RESULTS: The spider plant (Chlorophytum elatum var. vittatum ) could sorb 2.8 g CO/cm2 leaf surface in a 6 h
photoperiod. The golden photos (Scindapsus aureus ) sorbed 0.9g CO/cm2 leaf surface in the same time period.
In a system with the spider plant, 99% of an initial concentration of 47 ppm NO2 could be removed in 6 h from a
void volume of approximately 0.35 m3. One spider plant posted in a 3.8 liter container can sorb 3300 g CO and
effect the removal of 8500 g NO2 per hour, recognizing the fact that a significant fraction of NO2 at high
concentrations will be lost by surface sorption, dissolving in moisture, etc.
Wolverton, B.C. & Wolverton, J.D. (1993). Plants and soil microorganisms: Removal of formaldehyde, xylene and
ammonia from the indoor environment. Journal of the Mississippi Academy of Sciences, 38, 11-15.
AIM: To study the ability of interior plants in potting soil and potting soil without plants to remove formaldehyde,
xylene, and ammonia. METHOD: Plants and soil were placed in sealed chambers. RESULTS: Over thirty interior
plants were tested and Nephrolepis exaltata Bostoniensis (Boston fern), Chrysanthemum morifolium (pot mum),
and Phoenix roebelenii (dwarf date palm) were the most effective in removing formaldehyde. The dwarf palm date
was the most effective plant in removing xylene. Lady palm was one of the most effective plants in removing
ammonia. The Boston fern was also the most effective plant in continuously removing out-gassing formaldehyde 73
from particle board in a sealed chamber. CONCLUSION: This data indicates that house plants such as ferns, pot
mums, and palms may be a cost effective means of improving the indoor air quality in tightly constructed facilities
ranging from mobile homes to high-rise office facilities.
Wolverton, B.C. & Wolverton, J.D. (1996). Interior plants: Their influence on airborne microbes inside energy-efficient
buildings. Journal of the Mississippi Academy of Sciences, 41, 99-105.
AIM: This study addresses the influence of large numbers of houseplants on airborne microbial levels inside energy-
efficient buildings. METHOD: Parts of a tightly sealed, energy-efficient home located in South Mississippi served as
real world test chambers. A plant-filled sunroom and adjacent living room were tested in two studies, each three
months in duration and at different times of the year. A plant-free bedroom, located in another section of the home,
served as a control. RESULTS: Although humidity levels in the plant filled sunroom were higher than the plant-free
bedroom, airborne microbial levels were found to be more than fifty percent higher in the plant-free bedroom.
CONCLUSION: These findings indicate that houseplants are influencing the level of microbes in air where large
numbers of plants are grown.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 74
Natural elements
Daylight
Baron, R.A., Rea, M.S., & Daniels, S.G. (1992, experiment 1). Effects of indoor lighting (illuminance and spectral
distribution) on the performance of cognitive tasks and interpersonal behaviors: The potential mediating role of
positive affect. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 1-33.
AIM: To examine the effects of key aspects of indoor lighting on the performance of tasks that did not primarily
involve visual processing. METHOD: 3 studies with 243 undergraduates were conducted. Experiment 1 included
full-spectrum light to other types of lamps. RESULTS: In experiment 1, full-spectrum light was not associated with
positive affect, nor did it produce the best performance or social behavioral results. Across the 3 investigations,
variations in 2 aspects of indoor lighting (illuminance and lamp color) were found to exert significant effects on (1)
evaluations of a fictitious employee, (2) word categorization, (3) preference for resolving interpersonal conflicts
through collaboration vs avoidance, (4) reported expected ability to perform a clerical task, and (5) willingness to
offer help to others. In addition, subjects reported contrasting subjective reactions to the experimental rooms and
to the lighting conditions in them under varying combinations of illuminance and color.
Beauchemin, K.M., & Hays, P. (1996). Sunny hospital rooms expedite recovery from severe and refractory
depressions. Journal of Affective Disorders, 40(1), 49-51.
74 AIM: To study the effectiveness of sunlight in hospital rooms as a treatment for seasonal affective disorder.
METHOD: The lengths of stay of depressed patients in sunny rooms of a psychiatric inpatient unit were compared
with those of patients in dull rooms. RESULTS: Those in sunny rooms had an average stay of 16.9 days compared
to 19.5 days for those in dull rooms, a difference of 2.6 days (15%).
Beauchemin, K.M., & Hays, P. (1998). Dying in the dark: Sunshine, gender and outcomes in myocardial infarction.
Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 91(7), 352-354.
AIM: To examine the possibility that sunny rooms are conducive to better outcomes. METHOD: The sample
consisted of 628 patients admitted directly to the CICU with a first attack of myocardial infarction (MI). Outcomes
of those treated in sunny rooms and those treated in dull rooms were retrospectively compared for fatal outcomes
and for length of stay in the CICU. RESULTS: Patients stayed a shorter time in the sunny rooms, but the significant
difference was confined to women (2.3 days in sunny rooms, 3.3 days in dull rooms). Mortality in both sexes was
consistently higher in dull rooms (39/335 dull, 21/293 sunny).
Benedetti, F., Colombo, C., Barbini, B., Campori, E., & Smeraldi, E. (2001). Morning sunlight reduces length of
hospitalization in bipolar depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 62(3), 221-223.
AIM: To study the impact of sunlight on non-seasonal depression. METHOD: Length of hospitalization was recorded
for a sample of 415 unipolar and 187 bipolar depressed inpatients, assigned to rooms with eastern or western
windows. RESULTS: Bipolar inpatients in eastern rooms (exposed to direct sunlight in the morning) had a mean
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 75
3.67-day shorter hospital stay than patients in western rooms. No effect was found in unipolar inpatients.
CONCLUSIONS: Natural sunlight can be an underestimated and uncontrolled light therapy for bipolar depression.
Boray, P.F., Gifford, R., & Rosenblood, L. (1989). Effects of warm white, cool white, and full-spectrum
fluorescent lighting on simple cognitive performance, mood and ratings of others. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 9, 297-308.
AIM: To investigate the effects of lighting conditions on cognitive performance, mood and ratings of others.
METHOD: 117 university students were randomly assigned to conditions of warm white, cool white, and full-
spectrum fluorescent spectra lighting at approximately equal illuminances . Dependent measures included simple
verbal and quantitative tasks, salary recommendations, rated attractiveness and friendliness of others, judged room
attractiveness, estimated room size, and self-reported pleasure and arousal. RESULTS: No significant differences
were found among the 3 lighting types on any of the dependent measures. A subsequent power analysis indicated
that if differences do exist, they are quite small. CONCLUSION: Cool white or warm white lamps are recommended
as less expensive alternatives to full-spectrum lamps.
Knez, I. (1995, Study 1 & Study 2). Effects of indoor lighting on mood and cognition. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 15, 39-51.
AIM: To study the effect of indoor lighting on cognitive performance via mood. METHOD: Experiment 1 varied two
lighting parameters in a factorial, between-subject design: two illuminance levels (dim; 300 lx vs bright; 1500 lx) by 75
two colour temperatures (warm white; 3000K vs cool white; 4000K) at high CRI (Colour Rendering Index; 95). In
experiment 2 the parameters of lighting were identical to the first experiment, except for the low CRI (CRI; 55).
RESULTS: In experiment 1 it was found that a color temperature which induced the least negative mood enhanced
the performance in the long-term memory and problem-solving tasks, in both genders. In experiment 2, the
combination of color temperature and illuminance that best preserved the positive mood in one gender enhanced
this genders performance in the problem-solving and free recall tasks. CONCLUSION: Subjects mood valences and
their cognitive performances varied significantly with the genders emotionally different reactions to the indoor
lighting.
Knez, I. (2001). Effects of colour of light on nonvisual psychological processes. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 21, 201-208.
AIM: This study investigated the influence of color of light on subjects self-reported mood, cognitive performance
and room light estimation. METHOD: 108 subjects were randomly assigned to one of three lighting conditions
(warm, cool and artificial daylight white lighting). RESULTS: Although no direct effect on positive and negative
mood was indicated, a main effect of gender on several room light estimation dimensions showed that females
perceived the room light, across all light settings, as more expressive than did males. Main effects of color of light
on short-term memory and problem solving showed that subjects performed better in the warm than in the cool
and artificial daylight white lighting. Interaction effects between colour of light and gender on long-term memory
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 76
showed that males performed best in the warm and cool white lighting, and that women performed better than
men in the artificial daylight white lighting.
Knez, I. & Engmarker, I. (1998). Effects of office lighting on mood and cognitive performance and a gender effect in
work-related judgment. Environment and Behavior, 4, 580-594.
AIM: To investigate the effects of the recommended office lighting on mood and cognitive performance METHOD:
Subjects performed a cognitively demanding appraisal task under conditions of reddish and bluish lighting. A gender
effect in task performance was examined, both as a between- and within-subject factor. RESULTS: The results
showed no significant effect of the lighting on the performance of cognitive tasks. However, an interaction between
gender and color temperature on mood showed that 3000K (more reddish) and 4000K (more bluish) office lighting
may communicate different affective loadings or meanings to each gender. The cognitive workload induced by
almost 2 hrs of intellectual work diminished positive mood and augmented a negative mood. Moreover,
independently of their gender, the raters evaluated the neutral female significantly different from the neutral male
ratee.
Knez, I., & Kers, C. (2000). Effects of indoor lighting, gender, and age on mood and cognitive performance.
Environment and Behavior, 6, 817-831.
AIM: To examine the impact of indoor lighting, gender, and age on mood and cognitive performance. METHOD: A
76 between subjects experiment. RESULTS: A 2-way interaction between type of lamp and age on negative mood
showed that younger adults (about 23 yrs old) best preserved a negative mood in the warm (more reddish) white
lighting while working with a battery of cognitive tasks for 90 min; for the older adults (about 65 yrs old), cool
(more bluish) white lighting accounted for the identical effect. The younger females were shown to preserve the
positive mood as well as the negative mood better than the younger males, and a main effect of age in all cognitive
tasks revealed the superiority of younger to older adults in cognitive performance.
Kller, T., & Laike, R. (1998). The impact of flicker from fluorescent lighting on well-being, performance and
physiological arousal. Ergonomics, 41, 433-447.
AIM: To study the impact of the non-visible flicker from fluorescent tubes on well-being, performance and
physiological arousal. METHOD: 37 healthy males and females were subjected to fluorescent light powered by
conventional and high-frequency ballasts condition in a laboratory office on two separate occasions with 1 week in
between. RESULTS: When the light was powered by the conventional ballasts, individuals with high critical flicker
fusion frequency (CFF) responded with a pronounced attenuation of EEG alpha waves, and an increase in speed
and decrease in accuracy of performance. These results may be understood in terms of heightened arousal in the
central nervous system in response to the pronounced light modulation caused by the conventional ballasts.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 77
Kller, R. & Wetterberg, L. (1993). Melatonin, cortisol, EEG, ECG and subjective comfort in healthy humans: Impact
of two fluorescent lamp types at two light intensities. Lighting Research and Technology, 25 (2) 71-81.
AIM: To investigate the impact of fluorescent light on endocrine, neurophysiological, and subjective indices of well
being and stress. METHOD: Impacts of two types of fluorescent lamps, daylight and warm-white, were compared,
each at two different levels of illuminance. Exposure lasted one day for each of the four combinations. RESULTS:
The condition involving daylight lamps with a high illuminance evoked a negative response pattern. The social
evaluation of the office space went down, and at the same time the visual discomfort increased. The EEG contained
less delta rhythm under the high illuminance conditions. During the day of light exposure the alpha rhythm became
attenuated under the 1700 lux daylight lamps. CONCLUSION: The results warrant the conclusion that fluorescent
light of high illuminance may arouse the central nervous system and that this arousal will become accentuated if the
lamps are of the daylight type.
Veitch, J.A. (1997). Revisiting the performance and mood effects of information about lighting and fluorescent lamp
type. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 17, 253-262.
AIM: To study the effects of information sets about full-spectrum lighting on performance and mood.METHOD: In a 2
(lamp type) 4 (information sets) 2 (gender) factorial experiment, 104 male and 104 female participants were led
to expect good, poor or indifferent outcomes of working under full-spectrum or cool-white fluorescent lighting, or
they were not given any information. RESULTS: There were no effects of lamp type or information set on
performance or mood. 77
Veitch, J. A., Gifford, R., & Hine, D.W. (1991). Demand characteristics and full spectrum lighting effects on
performance and mood. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11, 87-95.
AIM: To test the hypothesis that demand characteristics, not lamp type per se, underlie the anecdotal reports of
beneficial effects of full spectrum lighting on performance and mood. METHOD: 86 subjects worked under either full
spectrum or cool white fluorescent lamps. Under full spectrum lamps they were given one of three instruction sets:
Full spectrum lamps improve performance and decrease fatigue (positive set); There is no evidence to support
such claims (negative set); or neutral information about light. The participants in the cool white condition received
only the neutral information set. RESULTS: When existing beliefs about lighting are controlled, both positive and
negative information about full spectrum lighting effects lead to improved reading performance and increased
self-reported arousal. No effect of lamp type was found.
Walch, J.M., Rabin, B.S., Day, R., Williams, J.N., Choi, K., & Kang, J.D. (2005). The effect of sunlight on post-
operative analgesic medication usage: A prospective study of spinal surgery patients. Psychosomatic Medicine,
67(1), 156-163
AIM: To evaluate whether the amount of sunlight in a hospital room affects a patients psychosocial health, the
quantity of analgesic medication used, and the pain medication cost. METHOD: A prospective study of pain
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 78
medication use was conducted in 89 patients undergoing elective cervical and lumbar spinal surgery where they
were housed on either the bright or dim side of the same hospital unit. Analgesic medication was converted to
standard morphine equivalents for interpatient comparison. The intensity of sunlight in each hospital room was
measured daily and psychologic questionnaires were administered on the day after surgery and at discharge.
RESULTS: Patients staying on the bright side of the hospital unit were exposed to 46% higher-intensity sunlight on
average. Patients exposed to an increased intensity of sunlight experienced less perceived stress, marginally less
pain, took 22% less analgesic medication per hour, and had 21% less pain medication costs. Age also affected
medication use, but patients housed on the bright side of the hospital consistently used less analgesic medications
in all age groups. COMMENT: Amount of sunlight was confounded with the content of the view.
Wilkins, A.J., Nimmo-Smith, I., Sater, A.J., & Bedocs, L., (1989, Study 1 & 2). Fluorescent lighting, headaches and
eyestrain. Lighting Research and Technology, 21, 11-18.
AIM: To study the effect of daylight and different types of fluorescent lighting on headaches and eyestrain. METHOD:
In a double-blind cross-over design, the weekly incidence of headaches among office workers was compared when
the offices were lit by fluorescent lighting. Incidence of headaches was also related to amount of daylight coming
from a light well. RESULTS: The average incidence of headaches and eyestrain was more than halved under high-
frequency lighting. The incidence was unaffected by the speed with which the tubes ignited. Headaches tended to
decrease with the height of the office above the ground and thus with increasing natural light from a light well.
78 Office occupants chose to switch on the high-frequency lighting for 30% longer on average.
Wirz-Justice A, Graw P, Krauchi K, Sarrafzadeh A, English J, Arendt J, Sand L. (1996). 'Natural' light treatment of
seasonal affective disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 37(2-3), 109-20.
AIM: To investigate the effects of exposure to natural light on seasonal affective disorder. METHOD: Patients with
seasonal affective disorder were treated for 1 week either with a daily 1-h morning walk outdoors (natural light) or
low-dose artificial light. The latter treatment (given under double-blind conditions) can be considered mainly placebo.
RESULTS: Artifical (placebo) light did not improve any of the depression self-ratings, whereas natural light exposure
improved all self-ratings. According to the Hamilton depression score, 25% remitted after low-dose artificial light and
50% after the walk. Sleep duration or timing were not crucial for the therapeutic response. The morning walk phase-
advanced the onset and/or offset of salivary melatonin secretion, but individual clinical improvement could not be
correlated with specific phase-shifts. Morning cortisol was decreased. Low-dose artificial light did not modify
melatonin or cortisol patterns.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 79
Fresh air
Apte, M.G., Fisk, W.J. and Daisey, J.M. (2000) Associations between indoor CO2 concentrations and sick building
syn-drome symptoms in US office buildings: an analysis of the 1994-1996 BASE study data, Indoor Air, 10,
246-257.
AIM: To assess the relationship between indoor CO2 concentrations and SBS symptoms in office buildings.
METHOD: The relationship between indoor CO2 concentrations and self-reported SBS symptoms in 1579 occupants
of 41 U.S. office buildings was estimated using multivariate logistic regression (MLR) analyses. Two CO2 indexes
were constructed: average workday indoor minus average outdoor CO2 (dCO2) and maximum indoor 1-h moving
average CO2 minus outdoor CO2 concentrations (dCO2MAX). RESULTS: A dose-response relationship between
ventilation and sore throat, nose/sinus, tight chest, wheezing was observed for both CO2 measures. COMMENT:
Ventilation rate was not actually measured in this study.
Brundage, J.F., Scott, R.M., Lednar, W.M., Smith, D.W. & Miller, R.N. (1988) Building-associated risk of febrile acute
respiratory diseases in Army trainees. Journal of the American Medical Association, 259, 2108- 2112.
AIM: To study whether energy conservation measures that tighten buildings are associated with increased risks of
respiratory infection among building occupants. METHOD: At four army training centers during a 47-month period,
incidence rates of febrile acute respiratory disease were compared between basic trainees in modern (energy-
efficient design and construction) and old barracks. RESULTS: Rates of febrile acute respiratory disease were
significantly higher among trainees in modern, and relative risks were consistent at the four centers. CONCLUSION: 79
These results support the hypothesis that tight buildings with closed ventilation systems significantly increase risks
of respiratory-transmitted infection among susceptible occupants.
Burge, S., Hedge, A., Wilson, E., Bass, J.H. & Robertson, A. (1987). Sick Building syndrome: a study of 4373 office
workers. Annals of Occupational Hygiene, 31, 493-504.
AIM: To study SBS symptoms in buildings with different types of ventilation systems. METHOD: Self-reported SBS
symptoms of 4373 office workers were compared between buildings with natural ventilation (11), mechanical
ventilation (7) and air-conditioning (24). RESULTS: It was found that prevalence of self-reported symptoms such as
nasal congestion, dry throat, and headache, was less in naturally and mechanically ventilated buildings.
Drinka, P.J., Krause, P., Schilling, M., Miller, B.A., Shult, P. and Gravenstein, S.(1996) Report of an outbreak:
nursing home architecture and influenza-A attack rates, Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 44, 910- 913.
AIM: To determine factors that might account for a significantly lower attack rate in a newly constructed nursing
building during an epidemic of type A influenza. METHOD: Prospective surveillance with retrospective analysis
was used to assess the attack rate of influenza A in a four-building, long-term care facility for veterans and their
spouses, with an average daily census of 690. RESULTS: An influenza A (H3N2) outbreak was culture-confirmed in
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 80
68 nursing home residents. Building A had significantly fewer culture-confirmed cases than the other buildings.
Building A had a unique ventilation system, more square feet of public space per resident, and does not contain
office space that serves the entire four-building facility. CONCLUSION: Architectural design may influence the attack
rate of influenza A in nursing homes.
Everett W.D. & Kipp, H. (1991). Epidemiologic observations of operating room infections resulting from variations in
ventilation and temperature. American Journal of Infection Control, 19(6):277-82.
AIM: To study the influence of increased ventilation rate on infection rates. METHOD: Wound infection rates of 1998
patients in a publicly funded military facility were measured before, during and after installment of an improved air-
conditioned ventilation system in three operating rooms. RESULTS: Increasing the ventilation rate during summer
months decreased infection rate. CONCLUSION: Increased ventilation rate can protect against infections in summer.
Fisk, W.J., Mendell, M.J., Daisey, J.M., Faulkner, D., Hodgson, A.T., Nematollahi, M. & Macher, J.M. (1993). Phase 1
of the California healthy building study. Indoor Air, 4, 246-254.
AIM: To compare the risk of SBS symptoms in buildings with different types of ventilation systems. METHOD:
Logistic regression models were used to evaluate associations between self-reported SBS symptoms of 880
occupants of twelve public office buildings and features of the buildings, indoor environments, jobs, and personal
factors. Three buildings were naturally ventilated, three were mechanically ventilated, and six were air conditioned.
80 RESULTS: The occupants of the mechanically ventilated and air conditioned buildings had significantly more
symptoms than occupants of the naturally ventilated buildings after adjustment for confounding factors. COMMENT:
Ventilation type was confounded with opening of windows and building age.
Hoge, C.W., Reichler, M.R., Dominguez, E.A., Bremer, J.C., Mastro, T.D., Hendricks, K.A., Musher, D.M., Elliott, J.A.,
Facklam, R.R. & Breiman, R.F. (1994). An epidemic of pneumococcal disease in an overcrowded, inadequately
ventilated jail. New England Journal of Medicine, 331, 643-648.
AIM: To investigate an outbreak of pneumococcal disease that was identified in an overcrowded Houston jail.
METHOD: Risk factors for pneumococcal disease in both a case-control and a cohort study were assessed. Venti-
lation was evaluated by measuring carbon dioxide levels and air flow to the living areas of the jail. The extent of
asymptomatic infection was determined by culturing pharyngeal specimens from a random sample of inmates. Type-
specific immunity was determined with an enzyme immunoassay. RESULTS. Over a four-week period, 46 inmates
had either acute pneumonia or invasive pneumococcal disease due to Streptococcus pneumoniae serotype 12F.
The attack rate was highest among inmates in cells with the highest carbon dioxide levels and the lowest volume of
outside air delivered by the ventilation system . Of underlying medical conditions, intravenous drug use was most
strongly associated with disease. Of 11 case patients tested with the enzyme immunoassay, 9 (82 percent) lacked
preexisting immunity to this strain. CONCLUSIONS. Severe overcrowding, inadequate ventilation, and altered host
susceptibility all contributed to an outbreak of pneumococcal disease in a large urban jail.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 81
Jaakkola, J.J.K., Heinoneon, O.P, & Seppanen, O. (1991). Mechanical ventilation in office buildings and the Sick
Building Syndrome. An experimental and epidemiological study. Indoor Air, 1, 111 -- 121.
EXPERIMENTAL STUDY: AIM: To study the effects of ventilation rate on the sick building syndrome (SBS).
METHOD: A controlled experimental study was carried out in an office building with 2150 employees who frequently
complained about the mechanical ventilation system. The ventilation was shut off in one part of the building and
reduced by 75% and 60% in two areas while leaving one part unaltered as a control. RESULTS: The experimental
reduction of the ventilation rate did not alleviate the symptoms. On the contrary, the reduction of the ventilation rate
caused a slight but statistically significant relative increase in symptoms.
EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDY: A cross-sectional analysis of the baseline data revealed that SBS symptoms did not
associate significantly with the ventilation rate (range 7-70 l/s/person). Temperature, gender, tobacco smoke,
a prior history of allergic diseases, a negative attitude towards the social atmosphere at work were significant
determinants of SBS symptoms.
Jaakkola, J.J. & Miettinen, P. (1995a) Ventilation rate in office buildings and sick building syndrome. Occupational
and Environmental Medicine, 52, 709-714.
AIM: To examine the relation between ventilation rate and SBS symptoms. METHOD: A cross sectional population
based study was carried out among 399 workers from 14 mechanically ventilated office buildings without air
recirculation or humidification in the Helsinki metropolitan area. The ventilation type and other characteristics of 81
these buildings were recorded on a site visit and the ventilation in the rooms was assessed by measuring the airflow
through the exhaust air outlets in the room. A questionnaire directed at workers inquired about the symptoms and
perceived air quality and their possible personal and environmental determinants (response rate 81%). RESULTS:
Logistic regression analysis showed that risk of symptoms was higher in offices with low ventilation rates (below
5 l/s per person) than in offices with high ventilation rates (over 25 l/s per person). The risks for ocular, nasal, skin
and lethargy increased significantly by a unit decrease in ventilation from 25 to 0 l/s per person. CONCLUSION:
The results suggest that outdoor air ventilation rates below the optimal (15 to 25 l/s per person) increase the risk
of SBS symptoms in mechanically ventilated office buildings.
Jaakkola, J.J. & Miettinen, P. (1995b) Type of ventilation system in office buildings and sick building syndrome.
American Journal of Epidemiology, 141, 755-765.
AIM: To examine the role of ventilation type in office buildings as a determinant of self-reported symptoms of
Sick Building Syndrome. METHOD: A cross-sectional study was carried out among 2,678 workers from 41 office
buildings selected randomly from the Helsinki metropolitan area. RESULTS: In logistic regression analysis adjusting
for potential confounders, simple mechanical ventilation (mechanical supply and exhaust ducts) was related to a
higher risk of ocular symptoms, nasal congestion and discharge, pharyngeal symptoms, and lethargy compared
with natural ventilation. Air conditioning was related to a slightly higher risk of symptoms compared with simple
mechanical ventilation. There was no increased risk of SBS symptoms in air-conditioned building with humidification.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 82
Loo, V.G., Bertrand, C., Dixon, C., Vitye, D., DeSalis, B., McLean, A.P., et al. (1996). Control of construction-
associated nosocomial aspergillosis in an antiquated hematology unit. Infection Control and Hospital Epidemiology,
17(6), 360-364.
AIM: To determine the effect of an environmental control program on the incidence of aspergillosis in patients with
leukemia or bone marrow transplants during a construction-associated outbreak. DESIGN: Clinical, microbiological,
and pathological records of 141 patients with leukemia or bone marrow transplants in a university tertiary-care
center were reviewed before and after an outbreak of aspergilosis to determine the presence or absence of
aspergillosis and duration of neutropenia. After the outbreak, wall-mounted portable high-efficiency particulate air
(HEPA)-filter air purifiers were installed, along with the application of copper-8-quinolinolate-formulated paint,
replacement of perforated ceiling tiles with nonperforated type, sealing of all windows, replacement of horizontal,
dust-accumulating blinds with vinyl, opaque, roller shades, and systematic and regular cleaning of surfaces.
RESULTS: Thirty-six cases of nosocomial aspergillosis were diagnosed during this period. The incidence density (ID)
in the preconstruction period was 3.18 per 1,000 days at risk. During construction activity-before the implemen-
tation of a control strategy-the ID increased dramatically to 9.88 per 1,000 days at risk. With infection control
measures implemented and continued construction work, the ID decreased to 2.91 per 1,000 days at risk,
comparable to the preconstruction baseline rate. CONCLUSION: The environmental control strategy may have
played an important role in controlling this outbreak of construction-associated invasive aspergillosis.
82 Mendell, M.J., Fisk, W.J., Deddens, J.A., Seavey, W.G., Smith, A.H., Smith, D.F., Hodgson, A.T., Daisey, J.M. &
Goldman, L.R. (1996). Elevated symptom prevalence associated with ventilation type in office buildings.
Epidemiology, 7, 583- 589.
AIM: To assess relations between ventilation system type and office worker symptoms in a set of U.S. buildings.
METHOD: Twelve public office buildings in northern California meeting specific eligibility criteria were studied in the
summer of 1990: three naturally ventilated, three mechanically ventilated (without air conditioning), and six air-
conditioned buildings. Questionnaire data were collected from 880 workers in selected spaces within the study
buildings. Effect estimates for various ventilation types were adjusted for personal, job, and work place factors
using logistic regression, and alternatively, using a mixed effects model to adjust for correlated responses within
study spaces. RESULTS: Higher adjusted prevalences of most symptom outcomes were associated with both
mechanical and air-conditioned ventilation, relative to natural. With a conservative adjustment for problem building
status, the highest adjusted prevalence odds ratios from logistic regression models were for dry or itchy skin and
lower respiratory symptoms.
Menzies, R., Tamblyn, R.M., Farant, J-P, Hanley, J., Nunes, F., Tamblyn R.T. (1993). The effect of varying levels of
outdoor-air supply on the symptoms of sick building syndrome. New England Journal of Medicine, 328, 821-827.
AIM: To determine the effect of changing the supply of outdoor air in office buildings on self-reported SBS
symptoms. METHOD: Within each of three consecutive two-week blocks, the ventilation systems in four building
were manipulated, in random order, to deliver to the indoor environment an intended 20 or 50 ft3 (0.57 or 1.4 m3)
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 83
of outdoor air per minute per person for one week at a time. Each week, 1546 workers, who were unaware of the
experimental intervention, reported symptoms and the indoor environment was thoroughly evaluated. RESULTS:
Changes in the supply of outdoor air were not associated with changes in the participants ratings of the office
environment or in symptom. CONCLUSION: Increases in the supply of outdoor air did not appear to affect workers
perceptions of their office environment or their reporting of symptoms considered typical of the sick building
syndrome.
Milton, D., Glencross, P. & Walters, M. (2000). Risk of sick-leave associated with outdoor air supply rate,
humidification and occupants complaints. Indoor Air, 10, 212-221.
AIM: To study the relationship between outdoor supply rate and risk of sick leave. METHOD: 1994 sick leave for
3,720 hourly employees of a large Massachusetts manufacturer in 40 buildings with 115 independently ventilated
work areas were analyzed. Corporate records identified building characteristics and IEQ complaints. Ventilation of
outdoor air was rated as moderate or high based on knowledge of ventilation systems and CO2 measurements on
a subset of work areas. Poisson regression was used to analyze sick leave controlled for age, gender, seniority,
hours of non-illness absence, shift, ethnicity, crowding, and type of job (office, technical, or manufacturing worker).
RESULTS: Increased sick leave was found to be consistently associated with lower levels of outdoor air supply. The
effect of ventilation was independent of IEQ complaints and among those exposed to lower outdoor air supply rates
the attributable risk of short-term sick leave was 35%. The cost of sick leave attributable to ventilation at current
recommended rates was estimated as $480 per employee per year. CONCLUSION: These findings suggest that net 83
savings of $400 per employee per year may be obtained with increased ventilation.
Myatt, T.A., Staudenmayer, J., Adams, K., Walters, M., Rudnick, S.N., & Milton, D.K. (2002) A study of indoor
carbon dioxide levels and sick leave among office workers. Environmental Health,1(1):3.
AIM: To explore the relationship between sick leave absences and carbon dioxide (CO2) concentrations in office
buildings due to a low dilution ventilation. METHOD: An intervention study of indoor CO2 levels and sick leave
among hourly office workers employed by a large corporation. Outdoor air supply rates were adjusted periodically
to increase the range of CO2 concentrations. Indoor CO2 concentrations were recorded every 10 minutes and a
CO2 concentration differential was calculated as a measure of outdoor air supply per person by subtracting the 1-3
a.m. average CO2 concentration from the same-day 9 a.m. - 5 a.m. average concentration. The metric of CO2
differential was used as a surrogate for the concentration of exhaled breath and for potential exposure to human
source airborne respiratory pathogens. RESULTS: No association was found between sick leave and CO2
differential. CONCLUSIONS: The CO2 differential was in the range of very low values, as compared with the
recommended maximum differential of 700 ppm. Although no effect was found, this study was unable to test
whether higher CO2 differentials may be associated with increased sick leave
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 84
Norbck, D., Bjrnsson, E., Janson, C., Widstrm m, J., & Boman, G. (1995). Asthmatic symptoms and volatile
organic compounds, formaldehyde, and carbon dioxide in dwellings. Occupational and Environmental Medicine,
52, 388-395.
AIM: To study possible relations between symptoms of asthma, building characteristics, and indoor concentration
of volatile organic compounds (VOCs) in dwellings. METHOD: The study comprised 88 subjects, aged 20-45 years,
from the general population in the Swedish town of Uppsala, selected by stratified random sampling. Room
temperature, air humidity, respirable dust, carbon dioxide (CO2), VOCs, formaldehyde, and house dust mites were
measured in the homes of the subjects. They underwent a structured interview, spirometry, peak expiratory flow
(PEF) measurements at home, methacholine provocation test for bronchial hyperresponsiveness, and skin prick
tests. In addition, serum concentration of eosinophilic cationic protein, blood eosinophil count, and total
immunoglobulin E were measured. RESULTS: Symptoms related to asthma were more common in dwellings with
house dust mites, and visible signs of dampness or microbial growth in the building. Significant relations were also
found between nocturnal breathlessness and presence of wall to wall carpets, and indoor concentration of CO2,
formaldehyde, and VOCs. CO2 exceeded the recommended limit value of 1000 ppm in 26% of the dwellings,
showing insufficient outdoor air supply. CONCLUSION: The results suggest that indoor VOCs and formaldehyde may
cause asthma-like symptoms. There is a need to increase the outdoor air supply in many dwelling, and wall to wall
carpeting and dampness in the building should be avoided.
84 ie, L., Nafstad, P., Botten, G., Magnus, P. & Jaakkola, J.K. (1999). Ventilation in homes and bronchial obstruction
in young children. Epidemiology, 10, 294- 299.
AIM: To assess the role of ventilation rate in homes in the development of bronchial obstruction during the first 2
years of life. METHOD: A matched case-control study based on a cohort of 3,754 newborns in Oslo in 1992-93 that
was followed for 2 years. The case series comprised 172 children with bronchial obstruction, and the control series
was one-to-one matched for date of birth. Ventilation rate and other building characteristics were measured/
collected in home visits, and questionnaires were used to obtain additional information. RESULTS: Risk of bronchial
obstruction was not directly associated with the ventilation rate in liters per second and per person in the homes.
The odds ratios of bronchial obstruction were higher in the low air change than in the high air change group owing
to exposure to environmental tobacco smoke, dampness problems, and the presence of textile wall paper, and
plasticizer-containing surfaces. CONCLUSION: The results are consistent with the hypothesis that low ventilation
rates strengthen the effects of indoor air pollutants.
Asp, A.S.M., A.A., Cannady, P.B., Jr., Morse, P.L., Burton, L.J., & Hammer, P.G. (1986). Efficacy of infection control
measures during a nosocomial outbreak of disseminated aspergillosis associated with hospital construction. Journal
of Infectious Diseases, 153(3), 634-637.
AIM: To study the efficacy of control measures to reduce the risk of infections among patients hospitalized in
institutions undergoing construction. METHOD: A HEPA filter was installed during the outbreak of an Aspergillosis
infection. RESULTS: The outbreak was terminated, despite the continuation of major renovation projects in close
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 85
proximity to patient care areas. The total number of hospital discharges, patients with lymphoreticular malignancies,
and patients receiving chemotherapy did not change significantly during the five-year study period.
Oren, I., Haddad, N., Finkelstein, R., & Rowe, J.M. (2001). Invasive pulmonary aspergillosis in neutropenic patients
during hospital construction: Before and after chemoprophylaxis and institution of HEPA filters. American Journal of
Hematology, 66(4), 257-262.
AIM: To study the effectiveness of chemoprophylaxis and institution of HEPA filters to control a nosocomial outbreak
of invasive pulmonary aspergillosis in acute leukemia patients treated in a regular ward that has only natural
ventilation. METHOD: Chemoprophylaxis with intravenous continuous low-dose amphotericin B was instituted as a
preventive measure after the outbreak of invasive pulmonary aspergillosis. After 18 months a new hematology ward
was opened with an air filtration system through HEPA filters, and a bone marrow transplantation program was
started on the hematology service. RESULTS: During the 18 months after chemoprophylaxis invasive pulmonary
aspergillosis developed in 43% of acute leukemia patients. During the three years after installment of the HEPA
filters, none of the acute leukemia or bone marrow transplantation patients who were hospitalized exclusively in the
hematology ward developed invasive pulmonary aspergillosis, although 29% of acute leukemia patients who were
housed in a regular ward, because of shortage of space in the new facility, still contracted invasive pulmonary
aspergillosis. Overall, 31 patients were diagnosed with invasive pulmonary aspergillosis during almost five years:
74% of patients recovered from invasive pulmonary aspergillosis, and 42% are long-term survivors; 26% of patients
died of resistant leukemia with aspergillosis, but no one died of invasive pulmonary aspergillosis alone. 85
CONCLUSION: During an on-going construction period, an extremely high incidence rate of invasive pulmonary
aspergillosis in acute leukemia patients undergoing intensive chemotherapy was observed. Institution of low-dose
intravenous amphotericin B prophylaxis marginally reduced the incidence rate of invasive pulmonary aspergillosis.
Keeping patients in a special ward with air filtration through a HEPA system eliminated invasive pulmonary
aspergillosis completely.
Skov, P. & Valbjrn, O. (1987). The sick building syndrome in the office environment: The Danish Town Hall study.
Environment International, 13, 339-349.
AIM: To study relationships between indoor climate factors and SBS symptoms in offices. METHOD: Measurements
of the indoor climate were performed in 14 town halls in Greater Copenhagen, Denmark, together with a
questionnaire study and a clinical study of 4369 employees in the town halls and 14 affiliated buildings. The return
rate for the questionnaire study was 80% and the participation rate for the clinical study 77%. RESULTS: The
prevalence of work-related mucosal irritation and of work-related general symptoms in the employees differed highly
between the individual town halls. The lowest prevalences of symptoms were found for the oldest town halls,
whereas there were no statistically significant differences between naturally and mechanically ventilated buildings.
Gender, job category, photoprinting, working with video display terminals, and handling carbonless paper correlated
significantly with the presence of work-related mucosal irritation and general symptoms.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 86
Smedje, G. & Norbck, D. (2000) New ventilation systems at select schools in Sweden - effects on asthma and
exposure. Archives of Environmental Health, 55, 18-25.
AIM: To study the possible impact of improving school ventilation on health and exposure of pupils. METHOD:
Questionnaire data on allergies, asthma, and asthmatic symptoms were obtained in 1993 and 1995 for 1,476
primary- and secondary-school pupils in 39 randomly selected schools. Various exposure factors were measured in
1993 and 1995 in approximately 100 classrooms. In 12% of the classrooms, new ventilation systems were installed
between 1993 and 1995. RESULTS: After the installment of new ventilation systems, the air-exchange rate
increased and the relative humidity and concentration of several airborne pollutants were reduced compared with
classrooms in nonimproved buildings. The reporting of at least one asthmatic symptom and the reporting of more
asthmatic symptoms in 1995 than in 1993 were less common among the 143 pupils who attended schools with
new ventilation systems.
Sundell, J., Lindvall, T., & Stenberg, B. (1994). Associations between type of ventilation and air flow rates in office
buildings and the risk of SBS-symptoms among occupants. Environment International, 20, 239- 251.
AIM: To study the association between type of ventilation and outdoor-air flow rates and SBS symptoms. METHOD:
A cross-sectional questionnaire and field study comprising 160 office buildings, and 260-2649 respondents with
case-controls as well as prevalence comparisons. RESULTS: Measured ventilation rates were higher than required
by most building codes. Increased risk of SBS and elevated prevalence of general SBS symptoms were associated
86 with low outdoor-air flow rates, presence of copying machines in office rooms, and ventilation operating hours less
than 10 h/d.
Wlinder, R., Norbck, D., Wieslander, G., Smedje, G., Erwall, C. & Venge, P. (1998). Nasal patency and biomarkers
in nasal lavage - the significance of air exchange rate and type of ventilation in schools. International Archives of
Occupational and Environmental Health, 71, 479-486.
AIM: To examine the relationships between the ventilation rate and type of ventilation system and objective nasal
measures. METHOD: A standardized investigation, including acoustic rhinometry and nasal lavage, was performed
in the school environment. 234 (84%) of 279 school personnel working 12 randomly selected primary schools
participated in the study. The dimensions of the nasal cavity were measured with acoustic rhinometry. Eosinophil
cationic protein (ECP), myeloperoxidase (MPO), lysozyme, and albumin were analyzed in the lavage fluid. The air
exchange rate and the room temperature were measured in the classrooms. Relationships between nasal measures
and the type of ventilation system were controlled for the type of ventilation, the air exchange rate, room
temperature, age, gender, smoking, atopy, and the urban vicinity of the school. RESULTS: A lower degree of nasal
patency as measured by acoustic rhinometry and increased levels of ECP and lysozyme in nasal lavage were
associated with a lower air exchange rate in the schools. Although mechanically ventilated schools had higher air
exchange rates, they were associated with more nasal symptoms, and nasal mucosal swelling and with increased
lavage levels of ECP and lysozyme as compared with schools with natural ventilation only. In contrast, 12 subjects
working in a school with mechanical displacement ventilation had more patent noses and lower levels of
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 87
inflammatory markers as compared with the personnel in schools with natural ventilation only. CONCLUSION: The
results indicate that both a low air exchange rate and mechanical ventilation systems based on dilution can be
associated with reduced nasal patency and an inflammatory biomarker response of the nasal mucosa among school
personnel.
Wargocki P, Wyon D.P., Fanger P.O. (2004). The performance and subjective responses of call-center operators
with new and used supply air filters at two outdoor air supply rates. Indoor Air, 14 Suppl 8, 7-16.
AIM: To study the performance and responses of call-center operators with new and used supply air filters at
different outdoor air supply rates. METHOD: A 2 x 2 replicated field intervention experiment was conducted in a call-
center providing a telephone directory service: outdoor air supply rate was adjusted to be 8% or 80% of the total
airflow and the supply air filters were either new or had been in place for 6 months. One of these independent
variables was changed each week for 8 weeks. The interventions did not affect room temperature, relative humidity
or noise level. The 26 operators were blind to conditions and each week returned questionnaires recording their
environmental perceptions SBS symptoms. Their performance was continuously monitored by recording the average
talk-time every 30 min. RESULTS: Replacing a used filter with a clean filter reduced talk-time by about 10% at the
high ventilation rate but had no significant effect at the low rate. Increasing the outdoor air supply rate reduced talk-
time by 6% with a new filter in place but increased talk-time by 8% with a used filter in place. The interventions also
had significant effects on some SBS symptoms and environmental perceptions. The present results indicate that
increasing outdoor air supply rate and replacing filters can have positive effects on health, comfort and perfor- 87
mance. CONCLUSION: Increasing outdoor air supply rates may only be beneficial when new filters are installed.
Wargocki, P., Wyon, D.P., Sundell, J.,Clausen, G., & Fanger, P.O. (2000).The effects of outdoor air supply rate in an
office on perceived air quality, Sick Building Syndrome (SBS) symptoms and productivity. Indoor Air, 10, 222-236.
AIM: To examine health and productivity effects of outdoor air supply in an office. METHOD: Perceived air quality,
SBS symptoms and productivity were studied in a normally furnished office space ventilated with an outdoor airflow
of 3, 10 or 30 L/s per person. The temperature of 22 degrees C, the relative humidity of 40% and all other
environmental parameters remained unchanged. Five groups of six female subjects were each exposed to the three
ventilation rates, one group and one ventilation rate at a time. Each exposure lasted 4.6 h and took place in the
afternoon. Subjects were unaware of the intervention and remained thermally neutral by adjusting their clothing.
They assessed perceived air quality and SBS symptoms at intervals, and performed simulated normal office work.
RESULTS: Increasing ventilation decreased the percentage of subjects dissatisfied with the air quality and the
intensity of odor and increased the perceived freshness of air. It also decreased the sensation of dryness of mouth
and throat, eased difficulty in thinking clearly and made subjects feel generally better. The performance of four
simulated office tasks improved monotonically with increasing ventilation rates, and the effect reached formal
significance in the case of text-typing. For each two-fold increase in ventilation rate, performance improved on
average by 1.7%. CONCLUSION: This study shows the benefits for health, comfort and productivity of ventilation
at rates well above the minimum levels prescribed in existing standards and guidelines.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 88
Warner, J.A., Frederick, J.M., Bryant, T.N., Wiech, C., Raw, G.J., Hunter, C., Stephen, F.R., McIntyre, D.A. & Warner,
J.O. (2000) Mechanical ventilation and high-efficiency vacuum cleaning: a combined strategy of mite and mite
allergen reduction in the control of mite-sensitive asthma. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunolology, Part 1, 105,
75-82.
AIM: To investigate the effect of mechanical ventilation and high-efficiency vacuuming on house dust mite (HDM)
numbers and mite allergen reduction in the homes of mite-sensitive asthmatic subjects. METHODS: The homes of
40 HDM-sensitive asthmatic subjects were randomized to receive (1) mechanical ventilation and a high-efficiency
vacuum cleaner (HEVC); (2) mechanical ventilation alone; (3) an HEVC alone; and (4) no intervention. Homes and
patients were monitored for 12 months. Change in absolute humidity, mite numbers, Der p 1 concentrations, lung
function, bronchial hyperresponsiveness, and symptom scores were analyzed. RESULTS: Homes with mechanical
ventilation achieved significantly lower humidity levels than those without, with an associated reduction of mite
numbers and Der p 1 concentrations in bedroom carpets and some other mite sources in the ventilated areas of the
homes. The addition of a vacuum cleaner enhanced this effect. There was a trend for an improvement in histamine
in the patients whose homes were ventilated. CONCLUSION: The use of a mechanical ventilation system in suitable
homes resulted in some reduction in numbers of HDM and Der p 1 concentrations. The addition of an HEVC slightly
enhanced the effect but not sufficiently to see an improvement in symptoms
Zweers, T., Preller, L., Brunekreef, B., & Boleij, J.S.M. (1992). Health and Indoor Climate Complaints of 7043 office
88 Workers in 61 Buildings in the Netherlands. Indoor Air, 2, 127-136.
AIM: To investigate the relationships between the prevalence of work-related health and indoor climate complaints.
METHOD: Information on health, indoor climate complaints and personal and workplace characteristics was
assessed using a questionnaire. A checklist was used to obtain information on building characteristics More than
7000 questionnaires were completed by the regular users of 60 buildings. RESULTS: The prevalence of symptoms
was higher in air-conditioned buildings than in naturally or mechanically ventilated buildings. Some other variables
were also related with most work-related complaints after adjustment for selected management, personal,
workplace and job characteristics. These included gender, work satisfaction in general, presence of allergies and/or
respiratory symptoms, and personal control over temperature at the workplace. No differences were found in
symptom prevalences between buildings with spray and steam humidification. The combination of air-conditioning
and humidification did not lead to further increases in the prevalence of complaints as compared to buildings with
only airconditioning or only humidification.
Quiet
Berg, S. (2001). Impact of reduced reverberation time on sound-induced arousals during sleep. Sleep, 24 (3),
289-292.
AIM: To study the effect of sound-absorbing tiles on sound-induced arousals during sleep. METHOD: EEG-arousals
following specific sound stimuli were studied in 12 subjects during sleep RESULTS: EEG-arousals were significantly
reduced when reverberation time was reduced with sound-absorbing ceiling-tiles. On average reverberation was
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 89
reduced 0.124 seconds at similar sound levels. CONCLUSION: Increased sound absorption, i.e. reduced
reverberation time, may reduce sound-induced sleep fragmentation by contributing to a better acoustic environment.
Hagerman, I., Rasmanis, G., Blomkvist, V., Ulrich, R., Anne Eriksen, C., Theorell, T. (2005). Influence of intensive
coronary care acoustics on the quality of care and physiological state of patients. International Journal of
Cardiology, 98(2), 267-70.
AIM: To evaluate the role of room acoustics on patients with coronary artery disease. METHOD: A total of 94
patients admitted to an intensive coronary heart unit for evaluation of chest pain were included in the study.
Acoustics were altered during the study period by changing the ceiling tiles throughout the CCU from sound-
reflecting tiles (bad acoustics) to sound-absorbing tiles (good acoustics) of similar appearance. Patients were
monitored with regard to blood pressure including pulse amplitude, heart rate and heart rate variability. The patients
were asked to fill in a questionnaire about the quality of the care, and a follow-up of rehospitalization and mortality
was made at 1 and 3 months, respectively. RESULTS: There were significant differences between good and bad
acoustics with regard to pulse amplitude in the acute myocardial infarction and unstable angina pectoris groups,
with lower values during the good acoustics period during the night. The incidence of rehospitalization was higher
for the bad acoustics group. Patients treated during the good acoustics period considered the staff attitude to be
much better than during the bad acoustics period. CONCLUSION: A bad acoustics environment during acute illness
may have important detrimental physiological effects on rehabilitation.
89
Ohrstrom, E., Bjorkman, M. (1983). Sleep disturbance before and after traffic noise attenuation in an apartment
building. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73(3), 877-9.
AIM: To study the effects of installing noise insulating windows on sleep disturbance from traffic noise. METHOD:
Three tenants completed a questionnaire each morning one week before and one week after the insulation of
windows, and body movements during sleep were recorded during these periods. RESULTS: All three participants
demonstrated a decrease in the number of body movements after the windows had been insulated and two reported
improved sleep quality. CONCLUSION: These results suggest that subjectively judged sleep quality as well as
recordings of bed movements are useful tools for evaluating actions to reduce noise.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 90
90
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 91
The new building of what is now known as the University Medical Center Groningen (UMCG)
demonstrates that the Board of Directors believe that architecture really matters. This belief also
triggered the project The Architecture of Hospitals. The aim of this project is to bring the 91
architecture of hospitals back in the center of the architectural debate. It wants to convince
architects of the great artistic and cultural tradition of the hospital as a work of architecture, and
healthcare decision makers of the immense importance of architecture as a factor contributing to
the hospitals core business: promoting peoples physical and mental well-being. The project has
been organized by the Foundation 200 years University Hospital Groningen.
The project The Architecture of Hospitals consists of a series of conferences, publications, and
other initiatives related to hospital architecture and evidence-based design. The main activity is a
major international conference on the architecture of hospitals, to be held April 13-15, 2005 in
Groningen, which is supported by several symposia, preliminary conferences, and workshops. The
first symposium entitled Buildings as ideas: the hospital was held in August 2003 in Groningen
and organized by the K.L. Poll Foundation for Education, Art and Science. This symposium resulted
in two products: the reader The Hospital (K.L. Poll Foundation, 2003), and a video called The
town in the town. Two preliminary conferences held in Groningen, November 2003 and Delft, April
2004, focused on evidence-based design and recent trends in the architecture of hospitals as
healing environments. A third preliminary took the form of a one-week design workshop on the
ultimate future interior of hospitals with participants from several Dutch academies. The results of
this design workshop will be presented at the main conference in April, 2005, and will be exhibited
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 92
in the city of Groningen for several weeks after the conference. The proceedings of the preliminary
conferences and the main conference will be published in the course of 2005.
Further publications of the project include a book on the planning and building of the new University
Hospital Groningen (Van den Noort, 1999), a book on Dutch hospital architecture after the Second
World War (Mens & Tijhuis, 1999), a book on Dutch psychiatric clinics (Mens, 2003), and the
current publication on health impacts of healing environments. In addition, a book is currently being
prepared that analyzes the evolution of housing and (semi-) medical facilities for the elderly.
A final product of the project The Architecture of Hospitals is a television documentary on the role
of the architect as a therapist, directed by Joost van Krieken (date of broadcast on Dutch
television: April 10, 2005).
92
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 93
94
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 95
For more information about Agnes van den Berg, see her website www.agnesvandenberg.nl
Selected publications
Van den Berg, A.E., Koole, S.L & Van der Wulp, N.Y (2003). Environmental preference and 95
restoration: (How) are they related? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 135-146.
Van den Berg, A.E. & Ter Heijne, M. (2004). Angst voor de natuur: Een theoretische en empirische
verkenning [Fear of nature: A theoretical and empirical exploration]. Landschap, 3, 137-145.
Van den Berg, A.E., & Van den Berg, M.M.H.E. (2001). Van buiten word je beter; een essay over
de relatie tussen natuur en gezondheid [Healing from the outside]. Wageningen, Alterra.
Van den Berg, A.E., Vlek, C.A.J. & Coeterier, J.F. (1998). Group differences in the aesthetic evalu-
ation of nature development plans: A multilevel approach. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 18, 141-157.
Koole, S.L., & Van den Berg, A.E. (in press). Lost in the wilderness: Terror management, action
control, and evaluations of nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
P391 Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:35 Pagina 96
96
P391 Omslag Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:37 Pagina 3
P391 Omslag Boekje Agnes Definitief.qxp 31-3-2005 19:37 Pagina 4