Analysis of Music Education Objectives in Learning Domains: Sciencedirect
Analysis of Music Education Objectives in Learning Domains: Sciencedirect
Analysis of Music Education Objectives in Learning Domains: Sciencedirect
com
ScienceDirect
Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104
5th World Conference on Learning, Teaching and Educational Leadership, WCLTA 2014
Abstract
The purpose of the study was to find out the extent to which Slovenian general education teachers follow the principles of a
balanced music objectives planning in the prevailing affective, psychomotor and cognitive domains. The research sample
involves 372 Slovenian student and current general education teachers who, working in pairs, prepared 186 lesson plans for
music education. The research results showed that the majority of the musical objectives planned in this study, pertained to the
psychomotor domain, followed by the cognitive objectives and by the least represented objectives of the affective domain.
2015
2015The
TheAuthors.
Authors.Published
Publishedby by Elsevier
Elsevier Ltd.Ltd.
This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
Peer-review under responsibility of Academic World Education and Research Center.
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Peer-review under responsibility of Academic World Education and Research Center
Keywords: affective, psychomotor and cognitive domains; balanced music education planning; general education teachers.
1. Introduction
Music education concerns those aspects of an individuals development which (even) in modern society remain
overlooked. Research findings confirm that learning in and through music stimulates self-confidence, emotional
sensitivity, social skills, team work, relaxation, measures of intelligence, concentration, literacy, fine motor co-
ordination (see review in Hallam, 2010), personal relationships (Portowitz et al., 2009), social skills (Hallam, 2001;
Spychiger, 2001; Brand, 2008), self-esteem within a music context (Portowitz et al., 2009), reading ability and math
ability (Scripp, 2003), creativity (Wolff, 1992; Greene, 1995; Hallam, 2010), spatial reasoning (Rauscher et al.,
1997; Scripp, 2003), memory, self-expression (Scripp, 2003), critical thinking, problem solving (Eisner, 1985;
Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Stevenson and Deasy, 2005), etc. In spite of numerous research findings, which confirm
1877-0428 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Peer-review under responsibility of Academic World Education and Research Center
doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.04.069
96 Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104
positive effects of music attendance on academic achievements, the research field is still open for identification of
various factors which could influence non-musical benefits. But it is also clear that no study had found that
participation in music programs diminishes students performance at school or their academic achievement (Costa-
Giomi 2004, p.141).
Although we agree that it may be self-defeating to use non-musical benefits as an argument for music lessons
(Schellenberg, 2005, p.13), it is important to consider music programmes potentials for the teaching and learning
of many skills and concepts beyond the arts themselves (Elster, 2001, p.3). Therefore we conclude, similarly to
other authors (Sharp et al., 2000; Taggart et al., 2004; Eurydice, 2009; Bamford, 2009), that music programmes
should be given a greater importance in school curriculum, and also that music and other arts instruction should be
included in standard school curricula because of their intrinsic merits (Schellenberg, 2005, p.13).
In this context the important question in music education is what knowledge and skills should be acquired and for
what purpose? The answers touch upon the dimensions of holistic learning which occur in mutual connections
between learning domains. In different learning situations, a pupil should develop abilities to experience, learn and
express music, which has to happen as an integrated experience including the emotional-social, motor and cognitive
nature of learning.
The objectives of music education in different areas of development can be classified according to various
taxonomy models (Bloom, 1956; Krathwohl et al., 1964; Simpson, 1966; and many others, an overview of which is
provided in: Anderson et al., 2001) which pursue education behaviour in progressive steps. All these models share
the idea that the taxonomy is perhaps best viewed as a conceptual framework that can be used within virtual and
philosophical framework aiding the necessary transition from curriculum to instruction (Anderson et al., 2001, p.
241). This chapter will refer to the taxonomies for cognitive domain according to revision of Blooms taxonomy
(Anderson et al. 2001) with categories: Remember, Understand, Apply, Analyze, Evaluate, Create; affective domain
according to Krathwohl et al. taxonomy (1964) with categories: Receiving, Responding, Valuing, Organization,
Characterization; and psychomotor domain according to Kiblers et al. taxonomy (1970) with categories: Gross
Body Movements, Finely Coordinated (manipulation and visual motor coordination), Non-Verbal (coomunicating
feelings and attitude), Speech Behaviours. Classification of musical objectives in cognitive, affective and
psychomotor domains enable detection of complex aspects of musical development which might be neglected or
over-emphasised within musical education, and reminds us of the necessity to take into account higher objectives
when planning the music education process. Music education, just as any other subjects, does not allow for partiality
and disregard of individual learning domains. The interaction among the processes of emotive, motivational,
cognitive and motor domains represents the very basis for a general as well as musical development.
Similarly to other authors (Harrow, 1972; Horne, 1980; Portowitz et al., 2009), we wish to point out the holistic
aspect that needs to be taken into account despite separate representations of objectives according to taxonomies, as
we must consider that an individual is not simply a sum of his or her parts (Dewey, 1972). In this sense, the
development of musical values requires interaction between affective and cognitive domains, the development of
singing and playing skills presupposes interaction between psychomotor and cognitive domains, the development of
the sense of aesthetics requires interaction of affective, cognitive and psychomotor domains, etc. There is a relation
of reciprocity among individual learning domains, as the level of interest, feelings, views and values in connection
with music predetermines the level of musical activity of pupils, which influences the quality of their musical
knowing and in return enhances and deepens their attitude towards music.
According to the current Slovenian Syllabus for music education in primary schools (2011), music education is
based on teaching-objective and development-process models of planning with emphasises on the importance of
teaching/learning process as well as its achievements. The teaching/learning process in music education should
therefore equally consider the learning domains in the affective, psychomotor and cognitive area. The description of
the music teaching concept itself for the lower classes (1st, 2nd, 3rd class with two 45 minute lessons per week) of
primary school, points out the importance of emotional, motivational and social aspects of music teaching and
learning. In upper classes (4th and 5th class with 1,5 music lessons per week and form 6th to 9th class with 1 music
Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104 97
lesson per week) of primary school, it also indicated the interaction among all learning domains, as well as gradual
upgrading of musical experience into musical knowing.
In order to achieve these dimensions of musical teaching/learning, good planning and implementation of music
education are necessary. These require the teachers musical and didactic competence. Our thesis is that only a
musically developed teacher who is willing to accept, learn and implement the professional and didactic
particularities of the subject can successfully fulfil music education objectives. Research shows that the quality of
teaching has a decisive impact on the quality of pupils musical achievements (Hallam, 2001; Spychiger, 2001;
Rauscher, 2008).
3. Research
Research on the competence of general education teachers of the arts shows that teachers lack self-confidence
and artistic knowledge (Slosar, 1995; Holden and Button, 2006; Kertz-Welzel, 2008; Bamford, 2009; Taggart et al.,
2004; Eurydice, 2009), which is reflected in the field of music in an unsatisfactory level of musical skills and in
lower teacher confidence in music compared to other subjects (Holden and Button, 2006). In the Slovenian pre-
school and early primary-school period, music education does not pursue the objectives of holistic development in
affective, psychomotor and cognitive learning domains (Denac, 2002). Problems point to a lack of musical and
didactic knowledge, necessary to stimulate higher levels of music teaching and learning in all mentioned domains.
Based on the problems described, the following research questions were formed:
What is the extent of musical objectives planned in Slovenian lesson plans in the prevailing affective, psychomotor
and cognitive domains?
Are there any differences between Slovenian student teachers and current general education teachers with respect
to the extent of musical objectives planned in the prevailing learning domains of affective, psychomotor and
cognitive development?
2.2. Methodology
The research was carried out in 2010 at the Faculty of Education, University of Ljubljana. It involves a sample of
372 student teachers and current general education teachers who, working in pairs, prepared 186 lesson plans for
music education.
Data were collected by analysing 186 lesson plans for music education in the first four classes of Slovenian
primary school (pupils aged between 6 and 9). 120 lesson plans (64.5 %) were prepared by student teachers (full-
time students of primary education) and 66 (35.5 %) by current general education teachers (part-time students of
primary education).
.
98 Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104
Table 1. Number (f) in percentage (f %) of analysed lesson plans prepared by student teachers or current teachers
T f f%
ST 120 64.5 %
CT 66 35.5 %
For data processing, frequency distribution of variables (f, f %) were used along with some descriptive statistics
(arithmetic mean, standard deviation). For the comparison of arithmetic means of two groups the Independent-
Samples Test/t-test for Equality of Means was used.
3.1. The representation of musical objectives in the prevailing affective, psychomotor and cognitive domains
Of the total 2142 musical objectives analysed in 186 lesson plans, 523 (24%) were planned in the affective
domain, 889 (42%) in the psychomotor domain and 730 (34%) in the cognitive domain.
Table 2. Number (f) in percentage (f %) share of musical objectives according to appurtenance to the prevailing affective, psychomotor and
cognitive domains.
LD f f%
AF 523 24%
PM 889 42%
Legend: LD learning domain, AF affective domain, PM psychomotor domain, COG cognitive domain
Further on, we examined the number of objectives by individual domains teachers planned per each lesson plan.
Table 3. Average number of musical objectives by the prevailing affective, psychomotor and cognitive domains planned per lesson plan.
Legend: LD learning domain, AF affective domain, PM psychomotor domain, COG cognitive domain
Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104 99
On average, teachers planned 11.48 objectives per lesson plan. This relatively high number of objectives results
from the fact that the objectives are very complex and can often be attributed to taxonomic categories of more than
one learning domain. For example, the objective pupil gives an aesthetic evaluation of a song sung can be
attributed to both, the affective and the cognitive domain, because evaluation requires learning response at the
emotional and motivational as well as at the cognitive level. Complex dimension of musical objectives could be
illustrated also by the objective pupil listens carefully to a music piece as attentiveness includes the emotional and
motivational as well as cognitive level of response. Often the objectives also include several achievements planned,
e.g.: the objective pupil listens carefully to faster and slower sounds and expresses them through movement
involves the affective and cognitive domains (listen carefully), as well as the affective and psychomotor domains
(express through movement); the musical objective pupil expresses the comprehension of pitch by motion
includes the affective and psychomotor domains (expressing by motion) as well as cognitive domain
(comprehension of pitch). Due to the complex dimension of musical objectives as well as overlapping of learning
domains one objective could be attributed to more than one learning domain. In our study that was also the reason
for higher number of musical objectives per lesson plan.
We tried to classify the objectives according to the prevailing component of learning development, however, it
turned out that they often simply cannot be attributed to a single taxonomic category, as the learning domains tend to
overlap and condition each other (Horne, 1980). The analysis of objectives showed that the majority of the music
education objectives teachers planned, pertained to the psychomotor domain of development (4.75 per class plan),
followed by the cognitive domain (3.92 objectives per class plan) and the affective domain (2.81 per class plan).
These results show a positive switch to active approaches to music teaching: given the high share of objectives
pertaining to the psychomotor domain we can conclude that Slovenian general education teachers are aware of how
important active acquisition of a learning experience through motor response to sound is, so they consciously plan
active musical experience for children, thus enabling them to develop their musical thinking, as musical thinking is
evidenced in musical action (Juntunen and Westerlund, 2001, p. 206). Music teaching and learning requires a
holistic learning response, including in the physical-motor area. Gardner (1983, p. 123) says that music is best
thought of as an extended gesture a kind of movement or direction that is carried out, at least implicitly, with the
body, as young children certainly relate music and body movement naturally (ibid). This speaks in favour of a
methodical approach, combining voice, hand and body, which is also supported by studies (Oblak, 1987; Denac,
2002) which confirmed the frequent motor response, especially in young children, to music, particularly one with a
distinctive rhythm as well as their improvement in fine motor skills, locomotor performance, and performance
accuracy (see review in Hallam, 2010). That is why several music education approaches (Orff, Dalcroz, Kodaly,
etc.) use gesture as a form of musical thinking. Elliott (1995, p. 103) also points out that if the body is in the mind,
then it makes perfect sense that the kinds of moving involved in music making are essential to improving
musical understanding.
The results also show that the least attention is dedicated to the affective learning domain, which proves that
Slovenian general education teachers are not adequately aware of the importance of emotional and social learning
factors. In music education, affective objectives significantly determine the processes of performing, listening and
creating which, as the primary goals of every music teaching-learning situation, are to enable students to achieve
self-growth, self-knowledge and musical enjoyment (Elliott, 2003, p. 54).
It is also necessary to consider that pupils can express and recognise various basic emotions like happiness,
sadness, anger and fear in music on the base of music parameters such as melody, tempo, timbre, rhythm, dynamic,
harmony etc. (Adachi and Trehub, 1998, 2000, 2004; Morton and Trehub, 2007; Hailstone et al., 2009) and their
combinations (Juslin et al., 2006). Research shows (Spychiger, 2001; Hallam, 2001; Sicherl - Kafol, 2001; Denac,
2002; Brand, 2008; Portowitz et al., 2009; Hallam, 2010) that musical activity enhances pupils interest, mutual
cooperation, acceptance of peers, responsibility for success, self-criticism, positive feelings, self-confidence and the
ability to feel the interpretation, which are all factors important for the internalisation of musical values and musical
meanings. Research findings (Schellenberg et al., 2007) also confirm that exposure to different types of music
effects changes in emotional state (intensity of the felt emotion) and mood (positive or negative emotion). Therefore,
100 Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104
the development of musical dispositions depends considerably on the pupils interest in musical activity and on a
stimulating learning environment which includes the family atmosphere and the social and economic circumstances.
It can be concluded that the processes of experiencing music are the basic starting point for music
teaching/learning and that appropriate teaching methods and a positive learning atmosphere are necessary to
encourage pupils positive attitude towards music.
The results of musical objectives in the prevailing affective, psychomotor and cognitive domains also show that
the proportion of the objectives for the cognitive domain was higher (3.92 per class plan) than the proportion of the
objectives for the affective domain (2.81 per class plan). We believe this is so, because the objectives in the
cognitive domain are easier to plan and make operational, on the basis of the cognitive taxonomy (Anderson et al.,
2001), and also easier to observe and check. So it is not surprising that so far cognitive objectives have received
most attention by both theory and practice. However, there is a threat that only cognitive achievements of music
lessons are pursuit, while the other learning domains tend to be neglected. A research by Denac (2010) indicates a
similar situation. It shows that even in Slovenian pre-school music education planning the objectives of the
cognitive domain prevail.
3.2. Difference Between Student Teachers and Current General Education Teachers in Terms of the Representation
of Musical Objectives in the Prevailing Affective, Psychomotor and Cognitive Domains
Table 4 shows the arithmetic means and standard deviations between student teachers and current general
education teachers in the presence of objectives planned in the prevailing affective, psychomotor and cognitive
domains.
Table 4: Average number of musical objectives per lesson plan for student teachers and current teachers in the prevailing affective,
psychomotor and cognitive domains.
Standard
LD Group n Mean
deviation
Legend: LD learning domain, AF affective domain, PM psychomotor domain, COG cognitive domain,
ST student teacher, CT current teacher
The analysis of the average share of musical objectives pertaining to the prevailing domains shows certain
differences between student teachers and current general education teachers in all three learning domains in favour
of current general education teachers. On the whole, current general education teachers planed more objectives
(12.68 per lesson plan) than student teachers (10.82 per lesson plan). Current general education teachers also pay
more attention to the principles of balanced objective planning, especially as regards the cognitive (4.85 objectives
per lesson plan) and psychomotor (4.79 objectives per lesson plan) and to a lesser extent also the affective domain
(3.05 objectives per lesson plan). With student teachers, on the other hand, the numbers of objectives planned vary
Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104 101
more considerably among the learning domains (affective: 2.68 objectives per lesson plan, psychomotor: 4.72
objectives per lesson plan and cognitive: 3.42 objectives per lesson plan).
Based on these results we presume that Slovenian current general education teachers, who have more experience
in the teaching practice, are more aware of the importance of the balanced planning of objectives of different
learning domains, which is a significant factor of a quality music education in the context of holistic approach.
Table 5. Difference between student teachers and current teachers in terms of the amount of objectives by individual learning domains.
Legend: LD learning domain, AF affective domain, PM psychomotor domain, COG cognitive domain
Based on the value of the t-coefficient and the level of its statistical importance it is possible to determine which
variables are the ones where statistically significant differences occur between the results of student teachers and
those of current general education teachers. The analysis of those results shows that there are statistically significant
differences in favour of current general education teachers in planning cognitive objectives (t = -7.447, sig. = 0.000).
Statistically significant differences can also be noticed in the planning of affective objectives (t = -3.101, sig. =
0.002) and in the planning of all objectives together (t = -6.314, sig. = 0.000). The results of arithmetic means
already showed that there were no statistically significant differences between student and current general education
teachers in planning objectives in the psychomotor domain, which was also confirmed indirectly by the t-test (t = -
0.356, sig. = 0.723).
We can conclude that Slovenian current general education teachers better respect the principles of a balanced
musical objectives planning as they give more weight to cognitive and affective objectives as well as objectives
planning in all learning domains together than student teachers. On the other hand, we found no statistically
significant difference between both study groups in planning of psychomotor objectives so we can summarize that
both, student teachers and current general education teachers in this study population are aware of the importance of
active acquisition of music learning experience through motor response. In this sense they follow the principle of
curricullum-as-practicum and the fact that our musical knowledge is in our actions: our musical thinking and
knowing are in our musical doing and making (Elliott, 1995, p. 56).
The comparison of both study groups indicate the influence of teaching practice which gives advantage to current
general education teachers who have more didactic knowledge due to years of music teaching. If we consider that
the relationship between teaching methods and learning objectives is reciprocal (Marenti Poarnik, 2000; Hus,
Kordigel Aberek, 2011) then we can conclude that current general education teachers have more experiences in
realization of learning objectives in all learning domains by using different teaching methods. On the other hand,
student teachers showed the impact of their study programme which highlights teaching methods for enactive
musical teaching and learning. The results in both groups in this study indicate the need for higher awareness of
affective objectives in music teaching.
5. Conclusion
Planning and implementation of music education require realisation of musical objectives in interaction of the
affective, psychomotor and cognitive domains. The main focus is on holistic development, even though we use
distinct terms for various domains in order to better understand the complex dimensions of musical learning. In line
102 Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104
with the holistic approach we plan musical objectives according to the prevailing learning domains, while keeping in
mind that they exist together in the intertwined network (Portowitz et al., 2009) of individual's feelings, thinking and
actions and they cannot be separated.
It is difficult to generalise, given the limited research sample in this study. Nevertheless, we can sum up that
Slovenian general education teachers are aware of the importance of activity-based music education planning. The
research results showed that the majority of the musical objectives planned in this study, pertained to the
psychomotor domain, followed by the cognitive objectives and by the least represented objectives of the affective
domain. We also found out that current general education teachers dedicate more attention to the musical objectives
of the cognitive and affective domains than student teachers, but as far as the psychomotor domain is concerned,
there are no significant differences between these two groups. The above facts indicate that Slovenian current
general education teachers respect the principles of balanced objective planning to a greater extent than student
teachers, which indicates positive effects of professional experience that teachers gain from years of music teaching
practice.
The fact that the smallest number of objectives planned pertained to the affective domain shows that general
education teachers in this study are not aware enough of the importance of emotional and social learning factors. If
we agree that the reason behind this might be either the fact that we do not think about this learning domain clearly
enough, or that we lack an effective model on which to base these objectives (Horne, 1980, p. 189), or perhaps
communication of emotions involves tacit knowledge that is difficult to convey from teachers to student (Juslin et
al., 2006, p.79), than we can conclude that teachers in this study are either not aware enough of curriculum demands
which emphasise the process of experiencing music as the basic starting point of music teaching/learning, or they
lack professional knowledge. Implementation into teaching practice of objectives in affective domain indicated in
the Slovenian Syllabus for music education (2011, p.5), such as: encouraging experiencing and expressing music
through musical activities, arousing curiosity and developing interest in and active approach to music, involvement
in various forms of musical activities, development of a positive attitude towards national and world musical
heritage, development of sensitivity and tolerance towards different musical cultures, requires teachers musical
and didactic competence. It can be concluded that realization of musical objectives, especially those in the affective
learning domain, calls for the use of appropriate teaching strategies and other didactic elements as well as positive
learning atmosphere to encourage pupils positive attitude towards music.
In this context we should not forget that music teachers are always perceived as role models and in this sense
they co-shape childrens motivation for music education and music in general (Rotar Pance, 1999, p. 39).
Therefore, the key factors of successful teaching and fulfilment of the planned learning objectives also include the
teachers interest in music education and their motivation for teaching it. Music teachers have to be good musicians,
curious, open to new ideas, critical and reflective towards their own development and towards the development of
their pupils. Only in this way a teacher can grow as an expert and continuously deepen their expert knowledge and
interest, as well as sensitivity for aesthetic values. This brings us to the conclusion that only a personally and
professionally competent teacher can fulfil the planned teaching/learning objectives and translate them into practice.
Last but not least, the research results also proved that the musical objectives of the affective area can be
planned and identified. It is necessary to emphasise that the objectives in the affective domain are equally important
as the objectives of other learning domains. Even though the emotional-social domain is not easily measurable, this
does not mean that it should be avoided. Furthermore, the importance of musical objectives related to the affective
learning domain is based on the influence that music training has on recognising and expressing emotions in music,
as it is consistent with evidence that accuracy at identifying emotions in music can improve with expertise
(Hailstone et al., 2009, p. 2149). If we also consider a study by Horne (1980), which indicate that teachers and
parents are aware of the role of the affective and social learning factors and even attribute them a greater importance
than to the cognitive objectives, then teachers should raise the quality of their music teaching in this area.
The problems of higher levels of music teaching/learning in this study population are connected with the fact
that general education teachers didactic knowledge, which is necessary to stimulate balanced learning development,
is insufficient. Successful music education planning should devote more attention to the balanced music objective
planning in all learning domains. In comparison to the psychomotor and cognitive domain there exists no similar
awareness for affective musical outcomes so it is necessary to promote the emotional and social aspects of musical
learning.
Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104 103
The findings of this study underline the need for a quality training system, including permanent professional
training, for general education teachers in Slovenia aimed at raising their awareness of emotional and social nature
of learning in terms of developing their competences for balanced music education planning.
The results of this study opened up possibilities for further research, that could examine whether the musical
objectives of affective, psychomotor and cognitive learning domains are mutually connected and to what extent their
possible interaction could influence a balanced learning development.
References
Adachi, M. and Trehub, S. E. (1998). Childrens Expression of Emotion in Song. Psychology of Music 26 (2), 133153.
Adachi, M. and Trehub, S. E. (2000). Decoding the Expressive Intentions in Childrens Songs. Music Perception, 18 (2), 213224.
Adachi, M. and Trehub, S. E. (2004). Perceiving emotion in childrens songs across age and culture. Japanese Psychological Research, 46 (4),
322336.
Anderson, L. W., Krathwohl, D. R., Airasian, P. W., Cruikshank, K. A., Mayer, R. E., Pintrich, P. R., at al. (2001). A Taxonomy for Learning,
Teaching, and Assessing A Revision of Blooms Taxonomy of Educational Objectives. New York: Longman.
Bamford, A. (2009). The Wow Factor: Global research compedium on the impact of the arts in education. Berlin: Waxmann Verlag.
Bloom, B.S. (1956). A taxonomy of educational objectives (Handbook 1, Cognitive domain). New York: David McKay Company.
Brand, E. (2008). Improving interpersonal communication through music. In S. Malbran and G. Mota (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd
International Society of Music (pp. 71-79). Bologna.
Costa-Giomi, E. (2004). Effects of Three Years of Piano Instruction on Childrens Academic Achievement, School Performance and Self
Esteem. Psychology of Music, 32, (2/139), 139-152.
Csikzentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York, NY: Harper Collins.
Denac, O. (2002). Glasba pri celostnem razvoju otrokove osebnosti [Music in Holistic Development of a Childs Personality]. Ljubljana:
Zavod Republike Slovenije za olstvo.
Denac, O. (2010). Teoretina izhodia nartovanja glasbene vzgoje v vrtcu [Theory of planning Music Education in Kindergarten].
Ljubljana: Debora.
Dewey, J. (1972). The Early Works: 1882-1898. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.
Eisner, E. W. (1985). The educational imagination: on the design and evaluation school programs. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Elliott, D. (1995). Music Matters: A New Philosophy of Music Education. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Elliott, D. (2003). Music Education in the 21st Century. Diskussion Musik-pdagogik, 3, 49-56.
Elster, A. (2001). Learning Through the Arts: Program Goals, Features, and Pilot Results. International Journal of Education & the Arts, 12,
7.
Eurydice (2009). Arts and Cultural Education at School in Europe Curricula and initiatives. Brussels: EACEA P9 Eurydice.
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: A theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.
Greene, M. (1995). Releasing the imagination. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Inc.
Hailstone, J. C., Omar, R. and Henley, S. M. D. (2009). Its not what to play, its how you play it: Timbre affects perception of emotion in
music. The quarterly Journal of Experimental psychology, 62 (11), 2141-2155.
Hallam, S. (2001). The power of music: A study commissioned by The Performing Right Society. London: PMRS.
Hallam, S. (2010). The power of music: Its impact on the intellectual, social and personal development of children and young people.
International Journal of Music Education. 28 (3), 269-290.
Harrow, A., J. (1972). A Taxonomy of the Psychomotor Domain: A Guide for Developing Behavioral Objectives. New York: David McKay
Company.
Holden, H. and Button, S. (2006). The teaching of music in the primary school by the non-music specialist. British Journal of Music
Education, 23 (1), 23-38.
Horne, S. E. (1980). Behavioural Objectives in the Affective Domain: a new model. British Educational research Journal, 6 (2), 189-196.
Hus, V., Kordigel Aberek, M. (2011). Questioning as a mediation tool for cognitive development in early science teaching. J. Balt. sci.
educ.10 (1), 6-16.
Juntunen, M. L. and Westerlund, H. (2001). Digging Dalcroze, or, Dissolving the Mind-Body Dualism: philosophical and practical remarks on
the musical body in action. Music Education Research, 3 (2), 203-214.
Juslin, P. N., Karlsson, J., Lindstrom E., Friberg, A. and Schoonderwaldt, E. (2006). Play it Again With Feeling: Computer Feedback in
Musical Communication of Emotions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12 (2), 79-95.
Kertz-Welzel, A. (2008). The Implementation of the National Standards for Music Education in American Schools, 115-124 /online/. Retrieved
on 15th February 2014 from http://www.zfkm.org/sonder08-kertzwelzel.pdf.
Kibler, R., J., Barker, L., L., and Miles, D., T. (1970). Behavioral Objectives and Instruction. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Krathwohl, D. R., Bloom, B. S., and Masia, B. B. (1964). Taxonomy of Educational Objectives. The Classification of Educational Goals.
Handbook II: Affective Domain. New York: David McKay Company, Inc.
Marenti Poarnik, B. (2000). Psihologija uenja in pouevanja [Psychology of learning and teaching]. Ljubljana: DZS.
Morton, J. B. and Trehub, S. E. (2007). Childrens judgements of emotion in song, Psychology of Music, 35 (4), 629-641.
Oblak, B. (1987). Ustvarjalno uenje v glasbeni vzgoji na stopnji razrednega pouka osnovne ole. (Doktorska disertacija). [Creative Learning
104 Barbara Sicherl Kafol et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 186 (2015) 95 104
in Music Education at the Primary Education Level. Doctoral disertation]. Ljubljana: Akademija za glasbo v Ljubljani.
Portowitz, A., Lichtenstein, O., Egorova, L. and Brand, E. (2009). Underlying mechanisms linking music education and cognitive
modifiability. Research Studies in Music Education, 31 (2), 107-128.
Rauscher, F. (2008). An empirical investigation of the effects of music instruction on cognition. In B. LeCarpentier (Ed.), Evaluating the
impact of arts and cultural education (pp. 302-310). Paris: Centre Pompidou.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., Levine, L. J., Wright, E. L., Dennis, W. R., and Newcomb, R. (1997). Music training causes long-term
enhancement of preschool childrens spatial-temporal reasoning abilities. Neurological Research, 19, 1-8.
Rotar Pance, B. (2006). Motivacija, klju h glasbi [Motivation the Key to Music]. Nova Gorica: Educa, Melior.
Schellenberg, E., G. (2005). Music Lessons Enhance IQ: A reply to Black and Stelle. The Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice, 4 (2),
10-13.
Schellenberg, E., G., Nakata, T., Hunter, P., G., and Tamoto, S. (2007). Exposure to music and cognitive performance: Test of children and
adults. Psychology of Music, 35 (1/5), 5-19.
Scripp, L. (2003). Critical links, next steps: an evolving conceprtion of music and learning in public school education. Journal for Learning
Through music /online/. Retrieved on 1st March 2014, from http://music-in-education.org/articles/2-AF.pdf.
Sharp, C. and Le Mtais, J. (2000). The Arts, Creativity and Cultural Education: An International Pespective. London: Qualifications and
Curriculum Authority.
Sicherl-Kafol, B. (2001). Celostna glasbena vzgoja [Holistic Music Education]. Ljubljana: Debora.
Simpson, E. J. (1966). The Classification of Educational Objectives, Psychomotor Domain. Research Project at the Univesity of Illinois.
Slosar, M. (1995). Dejavniki uspenosti razrednih uiteljev pri glasbeni vzgoji na razredni stopnji osnovne ole. (Doktorska disertacija)
[Music Education Performance Factors for Primary Education Teachers. Doctoral Thesis]. Ljubljana: Akademija za glasbo v Ljubljani.
Spychiger, M. (2001). Understanding musical activity and musical learning as sign processes: Toward a semiotic approach to music education.
The Journal of Aesthetic Education, 35 (1), 53-68.
Stevenson, L. M., and Deasy, R. J. (2005). Third space, when learning matters. Washington, DC: Arts Education Partnership.
Syllabus for music education in primary school (2011). Uni nart: program osnovnoolskega izobraevanja, Glasbena vzgoja 2011 [Syllabus:
Primary school programme, music Education 2011]. Ljubljana: Ministrstvo za olstvo, znanost in port: Zavod Republike Slovenije za
olstvo.
Taggart, G., Whitby, K., and Sharp, C. (2004). Curriculum and Progression in the Arts: An international Study. Final report. London:
Qualifications and Curriculum Authority.
Wolff, K. L. (1992). The effects of general music education on the academic achievement, perceptual-motor development, creative thinking,
and school attendance of first-grade children, Assential Advocacy Resources for Music: American Music Conference /online/. Retrieved on
15th February 2014 from http://amc-music.com/pdf/essential-advocacy-resource.pdf.