Attitudes Domestic Violence Race Gender
Attitudes Domestic Violence Race Gender
Attitudes Domestic Violence Race Gender
3/4, 1999
Charles L. Richman 2
Wake Forest University
Previous research has not extensively add ressed how attitudes toward domes-
tic violence vary between ethnicities and genders. This experiment utilized
domestic violence scenarios with the husband s and wifes ethnicities varied
to form four combinations of European-A merican and African-A merican
couples. Participants were 156 European-A merican (87 female and 69 male)
and 109 African-A merican (73 female and 36 male) undergraduate introduc-
tory psycho logy students aged 18 24 years. Participants read the scenario
and completed 7 questionnaires abo ut their attitudes toward the scenario,
domestic violence, and women. Analyses showed that women relative to men
blamed the husband more for the abuse, sym pathized more with the wife,
and rated the incident as more serious, and African-American participants
sym pathized more with African-A merican victims. In addition, participants
blamed the African-A merican husband less for the abuse than the European-
American husband. Both women and European-A mericans, relative to men
and African-A mericans, had more positive views of women and disappro ved
more strongly of wife beating. This research demonstrates that participant
gender and ethnicity, as well as abuser and victim ethnicity, do have an effect
on attitudes toward domestic violence.
The numbe r of women who are batte red each year by their partne rs is
unknown because of society s perception of domestic violence as a private
1
The authors express their appreciation to Deborah Best, Steve Davis, Anthony Perry, and
the Winston-Salem Police Department for their assistance. This research was conducted as
part of an undergraduate honors thesis by the rst author at Wake Forest University. The
paper is based on a poster presented at the annual meeting of the Southeaste rn Psychological
Association, April 4, 1997, Atlanta, GA. The present research was partially funded by the
Wake Forest University Graduate School and a grant from United Way of Winston-Salem,
Forsyth County to the second author.
2
To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of Psychology, Box 7778
Reynolda Station, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC 27109.
227
matte r, the failure of many victims to report abuse , and the knowledge that
many police of cers and judges dismiss abuse as inconsequential. Estimate s
prepare d by the United States Bureau of Justice (Aug. 1995) sugge st that
at least one million women each year fall victim to the violence of their
husbands or boyfriends. One in ve of those victims experiences three or
more assaults in a six month period. Domestic violence does not discrimi-
nate between ethnicities or geographic locations; European-A merican, Af-
rican-A merican, and Hispanic women in urban, rural, and suburban areas
all experience abuse from their spouse s or boyfrie nds (Greene, Raitz, &
Lindblad, 1989; Lockhart, 1987; US Dept. of Justice, Nov. 1994).
In addition to these shocking estimate s, the recent O. J. Simpson
murde r case has directed the issue of domestic violence to the forefront
of the public s atte ntion. Although domestic violence has been addresse d
extensively by the media and acade mics since the 1970s, it is the Simpson
trial that has highlighted the issues of blame and ethnicity (Kozol, 1995).
According to many academics and the popular press, American society
traditionally has repressed the issue of domestic violence and ignored the
needs of batte red women. Meyers (1994) conte nds that ``news coverage
of batte ring is socially distorte d {and} rooted in assumptions, myths, and
stereotypes that link it to individual and family pathology (p. 48). Many
scholars and journalists agre e that American patriarchal society has relieved
men of much of the responsibility for their abusive acts while blaming
victims and sometimes even condoning abuse . In addition, domestic vio-
lence has been depicted as a private family matte r, and therefore something
not to be interfered with in the public sector (Douglas, 1994; Kozol, 1995;
Minor, 1994; Willis, 1994). Such attitude s and depictions, although less
prevale nt today than twenty years ago, only serve to perpetuate myths and
hinder efforts to eliminate the occurrence of abuse.
The ethnicity of those involved in domestic violence is anothe r one of
society s concerns. The O. J. Simpson case has led Americans to expand their
understanding of domestic violence from gender issues to include racial and
ethnic issues (Kozol, 1995). According to Willis (1994), white Americans
have a ``good black/bad black neurosis that affe cts how they perceive Simp-
son and other abuse rs (p. 8). Whites want ``friendly black heroes, such as
Simpson, while fearing these heroes unde rlying aggression (p. 8; see also
Kozol, 1995). Bruning (1994) points out that ``black Americans seem particu-
larly vexed by the O. J. Simpson affair because in the past black men have
been falsely accuse d by whites; black Americans are caught between de-
fending their ethnicity and condemning wife abuse (p. 11). Stereotype s of the
issues presented by the media merit scienti c investigation to determine how
blaming the victim versus the abuse r in an abusive incident interacts with the
ethnicity both of those involve d and of the observe rs.
Attitudes Toward Domestic Violence 229
Theoretical A pproache s
Prior Research
Theory
weak just-world beliefs are less likely to blame the victims of domestic
violence than those who believe strongly in a just world.
Ethnicity
perceptions could affe ct their attitudes toward wife abuse and toward the
punishme nt that the abuse r should face.
Research concerning the effects of ethnicity on attitude s has been
conducte d more in studie s of rape than in studies of domestic violence. In
the former, African-A merican victims and African-A merican rapists are
believed less and blamed more than Europe an-A merican victims and rapists
(Willis, 1992). Additionally, if the victim, African-Ame rican or Europe an-
American, has been in a relationship with an African-A merican perpetrator,
she is perceived as less truthful. Giacopassi and Dull (1986) assume that
gender and ethnicity are at the root of attitude differences. They state that
``each race and sex is more likely to judge the othe r, whether victim or
offe nder, more harshly (p. 74), because of the tendency to ``reject state -
ments that re ect negative ly on individuals who have characte ristics similar
to themselves (p. 72).
Pierce and Harris (1993) investigate d this theory by varying the eth-
nicity of the husband in their domestic violence scenarios, and hypothesize d
but failed to con rm that the assault would be perceived more seriously if
the abuse r were African-A merican. However, these investigators did not
indicate the ethnicity of the wife in their scenarios, and the majority of the
participants were Europe an-A merican; both factors could have affe cted
the results.
Willis et al. (1996) extended Pierce and Harris research by varying
both the ethnicity of the abuse r and the victim in their scenarios. They
found that the ethnicity of the victim affe cted participants attitude s toward
the incident, rating it as less abusive and the abuse r as less responsible
when the woman was African-A merican, especially when she was in a
relationship with a European-A merican man. Again, however, the respon-
dents were primarily Europe an-Ame rican; these trends need to be investi-
gate d with African-Ame rican participants to determine who is stereotyp-
ing whom.
Hypothe ses
toward the victims. It was also expected that there would be a difference
between ethnic groups, with African-A merican men and women sympathiz-
ing more with African-A merican victims and Europe an-Ame rican men and
women sympathizing more with Europe an-A merican victims. This effect
should be moderate d by the participants gender, with members of one
ethnicity sympathizing more with their own ethnicity, but women (relative
to men) within that ethnicity sympathizing more with the victim than the
abuse r.
METHOD
Participants
Participants were 156 Europe an-A merican (87 female and 69 male) and
109 African-A merican (73 female and 36 male) unde rgraduate introductory
psychology stude nts aged 18 24 years. Ninety-eight percent of the under-
graduate participants were unmarrie d. The participants originated from
various regions of the country; they received course credit for their partici-
pation. Three southe astern unive rsities participated in the study in the
spring and summer of 1996. Thirty-six participants were eliminated from
the data analyse s due to incomplete data or incorrect reporting of the
husband s and/or wife s ethnicity.
Mate rials
Scenario
repeatedly in the facial area with a closed st. Of cer observed female had a
puffe d-up eye, bruised forehead, and bloody nose. Medical attention was
accepted by the female. Female wished to prose cute, and suspe ct was placed
under arrest. Suspe ct was heard calling his wife a ``lazy bitch.
Questionnaires
pants attitudes about the acceptability of wife beating. The authors of the
inventory created it to assess attitude s and beliefs using ve subscale s, but
for the purpose s of this study the scores on the subscale s were combined
to form one total score; the authors found both the subscale s and the
composite scale to have acceptable reliability and validity.
Example s of the 7-point Likert-scale stateme nts include ``sometimes
it is okay for a man to beat his wife and ``women feel pain and no pleasure
when beaten up by their husbands, where 1 was ``strongly agre e, 4 was
``neither agre e nor disagre e, and 7 was ``strongly disagre e. Three ller
items were included. Some items were reverse-score d so that a high compos-
ite score (up to 266) indicate d that wife beating was not acceptable , and a
low composite score (down to 38) indicate d that it was acceptable .
4. The McConahay, Hardee, and Batts (1981) Racism Scale. This scale
was used to assess the degree of participants racial stereotype s and atti-
tude s. The scale is a sound method of assessing people s racial attitude s,
with test-rete st correlations of .93 and .87. There were seven items for each
subscale, all on a 5-point Likert-scale in which 1 indicated ``strongly agre e,
3 indicated ``not sure, and 5 indicate d ``strongly disagre e. Fourte en ller
items were also included.
Sample items include ``it is a bad idea for blacks and whites to marry
one anothe r and ``blacks are getting too demanding in their push for equal
rights. A high composite score on this scale (up to 70) indicate d fewer
negative racial attitudes while a low composite score indicate d more nega-
tive racial attitude s.
5. The Attitudes Toward Wom en Scale Short Form (AWS). The
Spence, Helmreich, and Stapp (1973) AWS was used to determine whether
participants had positive or negative views about women. Spence and Helm-
reich (1978; as cited in Bailey, Less, & Harrell, 1992) designed this scale
to ``assess attitude s toward the rights, roles, and privileges women ought
to have and was not intended to be a measure of global attitude s toward
women (p. 315). Daugherty and Dambrot (1986) found the 25-item scale
to have acceptable alpha (.89) and split-half (.86) reliabilities. Examples of
the 4-point Likert-scale state ments include ``swearing and obscenity are
more repulsive in the speech of a woman than a man and ``a woman
should be as free as a man to propose marriage . In this case a score of 1
indicated ``agre e strongly and a 4 indicate d ``disagre e strongly. Some
items were reverse-score d so that a high composite score (up to 100) indi-
cated positive attitude s toward women, and a low composite score (down
to 25) indicate d negative attitude s.
6. The Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Because of the sensi-
tive and possibly transpare nt nature of the questionnaire s administe red,
this scale was used to determine whether participants were responding in
Attitudes Toward Domestic Violence 235
a socially desirable manner (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964). This scale has
excellent internal consiste ncy and correlates fairly well with other social
desirability scales. Eleven true-false state ments that indicate d social desir-
ability were selected from a total of 33; items that appe ared particularly
relevant to unde rgraduate participants were used in order to obtain a more
accurate rating of social desirability and to reduce the total numbe r of
items to which participants were required to respond.
Example s of such state ments include ``it is sometimes hard for me to
go on with my work if I am not encouraged and ``I m always willing to
admit it when I make a mistake . The more state ments that participants
respond to in a socially desirable manne r, the more likely they are to
respond to other scales in a way that is deemed socially acceptable rathe r
than in a manne r consiste nt with their true opinions. Scores could range
from 0 to 11 on this scale.
Procedure
RESULTS
Opinion of Wife
There were two main effects and three interactions on this scale (refer
to Table II for F statistics) . First, there was a main effect of participant
Table I. Pearson Correlation Matrix of Scenario Opinion Scales and Attitude Scales
Scale WBI Racism IBAWB Incident Husband Wife
WBI .31 b
.46 b
.31 b 2 .45 b
.48 b
Racism .49 b .18 b 2 .15 a .10
IBAWB .16 b 2 .15 a .20 b
Incident 2 .39 b .45 b
Husband 2 .59 b
Wife
p,
a
.05.
p,
b
.01.
Attitudes Toward Domestic Violence 237
Table II. Univariate Analyse s of Scenario Opinion Scales Using a 4-way MANCOVA
Scale Signi cant Effects F df p h 2
gender. Women, compare d to men, had more sympathy and less blame for
the wife ( M 5 24.89 and 23.22, respectively). There was also a main effect
of husband ethnicity, in which participants had more sympathy for the wife
with a Europe an-A merican husband ( M 5 24.55) than with an African-
American husband ( M 5 23.88).
The two-way participant gender by husband ethnicity interaction was
signi cant in the univariate and multivariate analyses (Table II). Women
had the most sympathy for the wife with an African-Ame rican husband,
while men had the least sympathy for the same wife (Table III). Both men
and women rated the wife with the Europe an-A merican husband similarly.
There was also a two-way interaction between participant ethnicity
and wife ethnicity. This interaction must be interpreted cautiously because
it was only signi cant in the univariate analyse s (Table II). It showed
that relative to European-A mericans, African-Ame ricans had the most
sympathy for the African-A merican wife ( M 5 25.04), and the least sympa-
thy for Europe an-Ame rican wife ( M 5 23.36). Europe an-A mericans rate d
Opinion of Husband
There were two main effects on the Opinion of Husband scale (Table
II). There was a main effect of participant gender in which women ( M 5
8.65), relative to men ( M 5 9.55), blamed the husband more and sympa-
thized with him less. There was also a main effect of husband ethnicity, in
which participants blamed the Europe an-A merican husband ( M 5 8.77)
more and sympathized with him less than the African-A merican husband
( M 5 9.27). There were no interactions on this scale, ps . 10.
Opinion of Incident
The Attitude s Toward Women scale had two main effects (see Table
V for F statistics): 1.) Participant gender: compared to men, women held
Table IV. Univariate Analyses of Scenario Opinion Scales Using a 3-way MANCOVA
Scale Signi cant Effects F df p h 2
DISCUSSION
A ttitude Scales
Also consiste nt with the hypothe ses were the ndings that women
relative to men demonstrate d more positive attitudes toward women and
more negative attitudes toward wife beating, especially with the African-
American couple. These ndings serve to con rm previous ndings about
the validity of the scales. The signi cant effects of participant ethnicity
also con rmed hypothese s. Europe an-Ame rican participants, relative to
African-Ame ricans, held more positive views of women and exhibited
stronge r disapproval of wife beating. Also, African-Ame ricans disapproved
least of wife beating with the Europe an-A merican couple; since this nding
was only signi cant univariate ly, it should be interpreted cautiously. Finally,
as expected, African-A merican participants showed evidence of more posi-
tive racial attitude s than European-A mericans, especially when the husband
was African-A merican.
A major limitation to the results concerning attitude scales is the
244 Locke and Richman
groups. More research should be done in this area to determine the nature
and origin of differing attitude s toward and stereotypes of domestic vio-
lence. The more data researchers have on such variable s, the more readily
scholars will be able to support (or fail to support) theories such as the
interpersonal power theory and the feminist theory of domestic violence.
The present data concerning attitude s toward domestic violence is
useful for several reasons: 1.) The knowledge that there are gender and
ethnicity attitudinal differences toward domestic violence should help us
develop educational and intervention programs to meet the needs and
backgrounds of the participants, whether they are police of cers, social
workers, acade micians, counse lors, or batte rers (McKeel & Sporakowski,
1993; Ross & Glisson, 1991; Saunde rs, 1995; Stith, 1990) . 2.) Service provid-
ers need to be aware of their own and society s biases toward minority
groups in order to improve their training and provide better treatme nt for
abuse rs and victims (Williams, 1992). 3.) Awareness about stereotyping can
also be useful in guiding jury selection and rehabilitation programs in
domestic violence court cases (Harris & Cook, 1994). 4.) The negative
stereotypes toward othe rs on the basis of gender and ethnicity may need
to be reduced prior to the removal of accepting attitude s toward wife
abuse . Research that identi es and explains such stereotype s is vital to
their elimination.
REFERENCES
Bailey, W. T., Less, E. A., & Harrell, D. R. (1992). The Attitudes Toward Women Scale
(AWS) and global attitudes toward women. Journal of General Psychology, 119, 315 317.
Baughman, E. (1971). African-Am erican Americans: A psychological analysis. New York:
Academic Press.
Bruning, F. (1994, July 25). Wife beating: A nations obsession. Maclean s, p.11.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe , D. (1964). The appro val motive: Studies in evaluati ve dependence.
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Daugherty, C. G., & Dambrot, F. H. (1986). Reliability of the Attitudes Toward Women
scale. Educational and Psychological Measurem ent, 46, 449 453.
Douglas, S. (1994, August). Blame it on battered women. The Progressi ve, p. 15.
Giacopassi, D., & Dull, R. (1986). Gender and racial differences in the acceptance of rape
myths within a college population. Sex Roles, 15, 63 75.
Greene, E., Raitz, A., & Lindblad, H. (1989). Jurors knowledge of battered women. Journal
of Family Violence, 4, 105 125.
Harris, R. J., & Cook, C. A. (1994). Attributions about spouse abuse: It matters who the
batterers and victims are. Sex Roles, 30, 553 565.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Jean, P. J., & Reynolds, C. R. (1984). Sex and attitude distortion: Ability of females and
males to fake liberal and conservative positions regarding changing sex roles. Sex Roles,
10, 805 815.
246 Locke and Richman
Kozol, W. (1995). Fracturing domesticity: Media, nationalism, and the question of feminist
in uence. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 20, 646 667.
Kristiansen, C., & Giulietti, R. (1990). Perceptions of wife abuse: Effects of gender, attitudes
toward women, and just-world beliefs among college students. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 14, 177 189.
Lenton, R. (1995a). Power versus feminist theories of wife abuse. Canadian Journal of Crimi-
nology, 37, 305 330.
Lenton, R. (1995b). Feminist versus interpersonal power theories of wife abuse revisite d.
Canadian Journal of Criminology, 37, 567 574.
Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundam ental delusion. New York: Ple-
num Press.
Lockhart, L. (1987). A reexamination of the effects of ethnicity and social class on the incidence
of marital violence: A search for reliable differences. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
49, 603 610.
McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It
depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Con ict Resolution, 25, 563 579.
McKeel, A. J., & Sporakowski, M. J. (1993). How shelter counselors views about responsibility
for wife abuse relate to services they provide to battered women. Journal of Family
Violence, 8, 101 112.
Meyers, M. (1994). News of battering. Journal of Communication, 44, 47 63.
Minor, D. (1994, Oct./Nov.). NOW taps Simpson case to ght abuse of women. Public Relations
Journal, pp. 16 17.
Pierce, M., & Harris, R. J. (1993). The effect of provocation, ethnicity, and injury description
of mens and womens perceptions of a wife-battering incident. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 23, 767 790.
Richman, C. L., & Waldrop, A. E. (1998, May). Socio-economic status and perceptions of
domestic violence. Paper presented at the 10th Annual Convention of the American
Psychological Society, Washington, DC.
Ross, M., & Glisson, C. (1991). Bias in social work interve ntion with battered women. Journal
of Social Service Research, 14, 79 105.
Saunders, D. G. (1995). The tendency to arre st victims of domestic violence: A preliminary
analysis of of cer characteristics. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 147 158.
Saunders, D., Lynch, A., Grayson, M., & Linz, D. (1987). The inventory of beliefs about wife
beating: The construction and initial validation of a measure of beliefs and attitudes.
Violence and Victims, 2, 39 57.
Schuller, R., Smith, V., & Olson, J. (1994). Jurors decisions in trials of battered women who
kill: The role of prior beliefs and expert testimony. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
24, 316 337.
Shaver, K. G. (1970). Defensive attribution: Effects of severity and relevance on the responsibil-
ity assigned for an accident. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 101 113.
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1973). A short version of the Attitudes toward
Women Scale (AWS). Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2, 219 220.
Stith, S. M. (1990). Police response to domestic violence: The in uence of individual and
familial factors. Violence and Victims, 5, 37 49.
Tedeschi, J. T., & Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence, aggression, and coercive actions. Washington,
DC.: American Psychological Association.
United States Department of Justice. (1994, November). Violence between intimates. Bureau
of Justice Statistics: Selected Findings, 1 10.
United States Department of Justice. (1995, August). Violence against women: estimates from
the redesigned survey. Bureau of Justice Statistics: Selected Findings, 1 8.
Weitz, R., & Gordon, L. (1993). Images of African-American women among Anglo college
students. Sex Roles, 28, 19 34.
Williams, O. J. (1992). Ethnically sensitive practice to enhance treatment participation of
African-American men who batter. Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary
Human Services, 73, 588 595.
Attitudes Toward Domestic Violence 247
Willis, C. (1992). The effect of sex role stereotype, victim and defendant ethnicity, and prior
relationship on rape culpability attributions. Sex Roles, 26, 213 226.
Willis, C. E., Hallinan, M. N., & Melby, J. (1996). Effects of sex role stereotyping among
European-American students on domestic violence culpability attributions. Sex Roles,
34, 475 491.
Willis, E. (1994, July 5). Good African-Americans and bad African-Americans. Village Voice,
p. 8.