Clinical Psychological Science 2016 Beck 2167702616628523
Clinical Psychological Science 2016 Beck 2167702616628523
Clinical Psychological Science 2016 Beck 2167702616628523
research-article2016
Theoretical/Methodological/Review Article
Abstract
We propose that depression can be viewed as an adaptation to conserve energy after the perceived loss of an investment
in a vital resource such as a relationship, group identity, or personal asset. Tendencies to process information negatively
and experience strong biological reactions to stress (resulting from genes, trauma, or both) can lead to depressogenic
beliefs about the self, world, and future. These tendencies are mediated by alterations in brain areas/networks involved
in cognition and emotion regulation. Depressogenic beliefs predispose individuals to make cognitive appraisals that
amplify perceptions of loss, typically in response to stressors that impact available resources. Clinical features of severe
depression (e.g., anhedonia, anergia) result from these appraisals and biological reactions that they trigger (e.g.,
autonomic, immune, neurochemical). These symptoms were presumably adaptive in our evolutionary history, but are
maladaptive in contemporary times. Thus, severe depression can be considered an anachronistic manifestation of an
evolutionarily based program.
Keywords
depression, unified model, cognitive theory, biological, evolutionary
Received 12/10/15; Revision accepted 12/26/15
A substantial body of research has provided strong support for the cognitive model of depression (Clark & Beck,
1999). Nevertheless, key contributions from a number of
novel biological investigations since the most recent
update of this model (Beck, 2008) have helped expand
our understanding of the links between cognitive and
biological processes involved in depression and in turn
justified the proposal of a unified model of depression
within a cognitive framework. More important, there is a
need for a comprehensive theoretical model that brings
together relatively disparate literatures and accounts and
in doing so highlights emerging consistencies across
findings and perspectives while generating novel insights.
Such an endeavor should help promote integration and
collaboration within the field and in turn the development of more integrative approaches to clinical care
(both of which are still lacking).
A unified model of depression should fulfill a number
of requisites. First and foremost, it should integrate findings from various levels of analysis (e.g., genetic, psychological) into a coherent account. Second, it should fully
Corresponding Author:
Aaron T. Beck, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania,
3535 Market St., Room 2031, Philadelphia, PA 19104
E-mail: [email protected]
Predisposition
Most individuals by and large adapt reasonably well to life
stressors.3 They draw on their own resilience strategies
and problem-solving techniques and can lean on their
social support systems to soften the impact of adverse life
events. However, these strategies are undermined in individuals who have had early traumatic experiences, are
vulnerable because of genetic factors, or both. Consequently, they are at risk for severe depression and other
psychological disorders.4 A critical element in the development of vulnerability to depression is the formation of
depressogenic beliefs about the self, the world, and the
future (i.e., negative cognitive triad; Beck, 1967).
4
Beyond their influence on cognitive development
(e.g., information processing, belief formation; both discussed more later), there is evidence that these sorts of
formative early experiences may disrupt neural development. For example, early life adversity has been linked
with reduced volume of the hippocampus (Rao et al.,
2010), a brain structure that plays a critical role in learning and memory formation (see Squire, 1992) and is
implicated in the neuropathology of depression (see
Campbell & MacQueen, 2004). It is important to note that
this reduction predicts later symptoms of depression
(Rao etal., 2010), and has also been observed in adults
who experienced emotional neglect during childhood
but have not (yet) suffered from severe depression (Frodl,
Reinhold, Koutsouleris, Reiser, & Meisenzahl, 2010).
It is clear, however, that not everyone who experiences adversity in childhood later becomes severely
depressed. One clue to this puzzle came from the landmark finding by Caspi and colleagues (2003) suggesting
that individuals possessing either one or two copies of
the short genetic variant of the serotonin transporterlinked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) experience
higher levels of depression and suicidality following a life
stressor. Furthermore, those who experienced maltreatment in childhood and also carried the 5-HTTLPR short
variant were more likely to become depressed as adults.
This finding was replicated by Kendler, Kuhn, Vittum,
Prescott, and Riley (2005), who demonstrated increased
sensitivity to severe depression in these individuals. Since
then, a number of other studies examining this genetic
polymorphism have yielded consistent findings. In a
sample of adolescents and young adults, the interaction
of 5-HTTLPR genotypes and major interpersonal stress
predicted the onset of severe depression (Vrshek-Schallhorn etal., 2014). Yet another study showed that these
genotypes were associated with more negative appraisals
of stressful life events, which in turn predicted future
depressive symptoms (Conway etal., 2012). The moderating effect of this genetic polymorphism on the link
between stress and depression was confirmed in a recent
meta-analysis (Karg, Burmeister, Shedden, & Sen, 2011;
for a broader review of supporting evidence, see Caspi,
Hariri, Holmes, Uher, & Moffitt, 2010; but for an alternative view, see Risch etal., 2009).
It is important that vulnerability to depression is almost
certainly polygenic (see Flint & Kendler, 2014, for a
detailed discussion and review), and other candidate
polymorphisms have been identified that may play a role
as well. For example, several studies (e.g., Kaufman etal.,
2006; Kudinova, McGeary, Knopik, & Gibb, 2015) have
found that the association between 5-HTTLPR genotypes
and depression is moderated by variants of the brainderived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene (a key neurochemical in neural development and known resilience
Stress reactivity
Biological reactivity to stress also seems to play a critical
role in the pathway from genetic and cognitive predisposition to depression. Dysregulation of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is one of the most consistent
biological correlates of severe depression (see Pariante &
Lightman, 2008; Stetler & Miller, 2011) and may be linked
to serotonergic or noradrenergic dysfunction (given the
important role of these neurotransmitters in HPA activation/regulation; see Dinan, 1996; Tsigos & Chrousos,
2002). Also, numerous studies have found elevated levels
of cortisol in response to stress in depressed individuals
(e.g., Burke, Davis, Otte, & Mohr, 2005; Knorr, Vinberg,
Kessing, & Wetterslev, 2010; Stetler & Miller, 2011). Notably, cortisol reactivity has also been observed in healthy
individuals with the FKBP5 gene minor variant (e.g.,
Ising etal., 2008), carriers of the 5-HTTLPR short variant
(see Miller etal., 2013), and individuals who lost a parent
during childhood (e.g., Tyrka et al., 2008). Over time,
elevated cortisol can lead to neural atrophy (mediated by
glutamate, and particularly in the hippocampus;5 see
McEwen, 2003; Sapolsky, 2000) that could further exacerbate HPA dysregulation (as the hippocampus plays a key
role in HPA feedback inhibition; see Mahar, Bambico,
Mechawar, & Nobrega, 2014) and memory biases (e.g.,
Gerritsen etal., 2012; Young etal., 2012; see Gradin &
Pomi, 2008).
The amygdala, a brain region strongly implicated in
salience detection, emotional processing, and activation
of the HPA axis (Adolphs, 2010; Herman & Cullinan,
1997), seems to play an important role in this stress reactivity and its apparent link with information processing
(see also Disner, Beevers, Haigh, & Beck, 2011). The
extent to which the amygdala is activated by negative
stimuli is directly associated with 5-HTTLPR genotypes
(see Munaf, Brown, & Hariri, 2008), and carriers of the
short variant have been found to show elevated amygdala activation and cortisol responses when attempting
to repair their mood (Gotlib, Joormann, Minor, & Hallmayer, 2008). Likewise, enhanced amygdala reactivity is
associated with childhood maltreatment, independent of
psychiatric status (e.g., Van Harmelen et al., 2013).
In turn, amygdala activation predicts biased recall of
negative information in individuals with a history of
depression (e.g., Ramel etal., 2007), as does functional
connectivity between the amygdala and hippocampus
(e.g., Hamilton & Gotlib, 2008). In short, this biological
reactivity to environmental input/stress may foster greater
affective instability (see, e.g., Thompson, Berenbaum, &
Bredemeier, 2011) and in turn strengthen learning.
Belief formation
In simplified terms, the developmental sequence to predisposition follows the genetic or environmental risk to
negative memories of devaluation as well as negative
evaluations of the self and future. Resulting negative
views coalesce into the negative cognitive triad.
Support for this formulation is provided by the large
number of publications detailing the role of negative selfesteem as a predictor of future depression (see Sowislo &
Orth, 2013), and more recent evidence that the tendency
to experience a decline in self-esteem (shown using ecological momentary assessment) in response to negative
events does as well (e.g., Clasen, Fisher, & Beevers, 2015).
Research using the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS;
Weissman & Beck, 1978) lends further support to this
model. This scale includes items such as if I dont do
well all the time, it means I am a failure. Numerous studies have shown that these attitudes moderate the impact
of stressful life events on depression (e.g., Abela & Skitch,
2007; Abela & Sullivan, 2003; Hankin, Abramson, Miller,
& Haeffel, 2004; Lewinsohn, Joiner, & Rohde, 2001).
These negative attitudes and beliefs seem to result
inimportant and predictable learned patterns of appraising
life experiences/events. For example, using the Attributional Style Questionnaire, Alloy, Abramson, and colleag
ues(e.g., Alloy, Abramson, & Francis, 1999; Alloy, Abramson,
Whitehouse, etal., 1999) have demonstrated that depression-prone people have a tendency to view negative events
as caused by themselves and anticipate enduring negative
consequences. This attributional style prospectively
predicts depressive symptoms (e.g., Hankin et al., 2004;
Nolen-Hoeksema, Girguis, & Seligman, 1986) and has been
linked with maltreatment in childhood (e.g., Gibb, Alloy,
Abramson, & Marx, 2003). In turn, these individuals become
more pessimistic about the future (e.g., Alloy & Ahrens,
1987; Metalsky & Joiner, 1992).
Our general model of depression proneness or predisposition is portrayed in Figure 1. As shown in this figure,
we propose that genetic and experiential risk factors contribute to the development of information processing
biases and biological reactivity to stress. Over time, these
processes can lead to the development of depressogenic
beliefs (i.e., negative views of the self, world, and future),
which in turn further exacerbate processing biases and
stress reactivity. Early negative experiences are also
hypothesized to contribute directly to depressogenic
belief formation.
Pinpointing person-specific
vulnerabilities
Precipitation
Predisposition is not sufficient to cause depression
rather, something must trigger the onset of symptoms.
We propose that the critical element in the precipitation
of depression is the perceived loss of the investment in a
vital resource.
Early Experiences
/Trauma
Information Processing
Biases
Depressogenic Beliefs
i.e., the Negative Cognitive Triad
Genetic Risk
e.g., 5-HTTLPR Short
Variant, FKBP5 Minor
Variant
Biological Stress
Reactivity
e.g., Amygdala,
HPA Axis, Cortisol
Fig. 1. Predisposition to depression. According to our unified model, genetic risk and early experiences/trauma both contribute to the development of information processing biases and biological reactivity to stress. Over time, these tendencies
can lead to the development of the negative cognitive triad (i.e., depressogenic beliefs about the self, world, and future).
In turn, the formation and activation of these beliefs further exacerbate cognitive biases and stress reactivity. Early experiences/trauma are also considered to play a direct role in the formation of depressogenic beliefs.
8
Negative thinking about and interpretations of experiences can be considered proximal cognitive causes of
depression (Hammen & Watkins, 2008). In line with this
formulation, it has been demonstrated that cortisol
changes in response to stressors are closely tied to cognitive appraisals of those events (e.g., Gaab, Rohleder,
Nater, & Ehlert, 2005; see Denson et al., 2009, for a
review).
Conservation of energy
As noted earlier, vital resources such as social relationships
play an important role in helping us meet evolutionarily
derived goals/needs. Thus, following the perceived loss of
a vital investment, we are naturally drawn to compensate
for this loss by limiting all activity not necessary for survival. To implement this conservation strategy/program,
sexual drive, hunger, and parenting are largely extinguished. Under the condition of an expectation of exhaustion of residual energy, an enforced conservation of energy
would permit the individual to survive until the circumstances become more favorable. Of note, similar energy
conservation strategies are observed in other species,
under certain environmental conditions (e.g., amphibians
in cold weather). In fact, the increased incidence of depressive symptoms/episodes during the fall and winter months
(see, e.g., Magnusson, 2000) could be considered in line
with this formulation (see also Davis & Levitan, 2005),6
perhaps suggesting an evolutionarily derived sensitivity of
the depression program to environmental cues signaling
scarcity of various sources of sustenance (e.g., reduced
sunlight). With the development of social behavior at a
later stage of evolution, other members of the social group
assumed a key role in promoting survival. Thus, the same
strategy that conserved energy during food scarcity was
later displaced onto the loss of human resources (see
also Allen & Badcock, 2003).
To the extent that objective circumstances warrant
energy conservation, such behavioral strategies can be
considered adaptive. Similarly, factors that predispose
individuals to depression (e.g., information processing
biases, stress reactivity) can be considered adaptive in
particular environmental situations (e.g., persistent danger or persecution). However, these symptoms and factors likely were more often warranted (and therefore
adaptive) in our evolutionary history than they are in the
contemporary context. Also, we propose that, like other
evolutionarily based programs derived through nature
selection (e.g., the fight-or flight response), the degree of
activation of the depression program varies (concomitant
with the extent of the perceived loss and resulting schema
activation), accounting for symptoms that range from
mild (i.e., dysphoria) to the most severe (i.e., melancholia). As previously noted, mild symptoms may generally
be adaptive even today, in that they can motivate us to
take stock after a devaluing experience (see, e.g., Alloy &
Abramson, 1979; Wakefield & Schmitz, 2013). Conversely,
in their most extreme forms, some symptoms inherently
undermine the individuals prospects for survival and
procreation (e.g., suicidal acts).
Support for the conservation of energy hypothesis
comes from the noted parallel between sickness behaviors of individuals experiencing infection and symptoms
of severe depression (see, e.g., Dantzer, OConnor,
Freund, Johnson, & Kelley, 2008; Durisko et al., 2015).
Evidence for the mobilization of the immune system in
depression is indicated by the presence of proinflammatory immune bodies (cytokines; see Dowlati etal., 2010;
Slavich & Irwin, 2014),7 as well as experimental research
showing that inducing inflammation in humans can cause
severe depressive symptoms (e.g., Capuron & Miller,
10
2004; Harrison etal., 2009). Recent findings suggest that
this immune activation may be driven by stress-induced
endogenous opioids (e.g., Prossin et al., in press). The
physiological components of both infection and depression can be viewed as consequences of the limitation of
the expenditure of energy. Because the immune response
to infections consumes an inordinate amount of energy,
the body is programmed to reduce energy output not
essential for immediate survival (see also Segerstrom,
2007). Thus, loss of appetite, loss of sexual drive, and
generalized fatigue tend to restrict energy-demanding
activities such as foraging for food and engaging in sex.
Activation of the parasympathetic nervous system (which
generally promotes rest and digest) may also play an
important mediating role in these symptoms/behaviors,
as evidenced by research showing that depression
is associated with respiratory sinus arrhythmia (e.g.,
Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg, & Kovacs, 2013), vagal tone
(e.g., Kogan, Gruber, Shallcross, Ford, & Mauss, 2013),
and other biomarkers of parasympathetic activation (see
Lin, Lin, Lin, & Huang, 2011). Finally, emerging evidence
shows that the serotonin system plays a key role in
energy regulation (see Andrews, Bharwani, Lee, Fox, &
Thomson, 2015), suggesting that serotonergic dysregulation may contribute as well.
Although others have highlighted that these sickness
behaviors promote energy conservation, we propose
that common cognitive and emotional symptoms of
depression can be conceptualized within this same functional framework. Perhaps most notably, depressed
mood (and the negative thoughts that accompany it)
promotes withdrawal from people and activities. Also,
the broad cognitive deficits (see McDermott & Ebmeier,
2009) and psychomotor retardation seen in severe
depression may be viewed as a consequence of energy
conservation mechanisms within the brain (typically a
large consumer of the bodys energy). In line with this
proposal, depressed individuals show decreased neural/
metabolic activity in several areas of thebrain (e.g., prefrontal regions; see Drevets, 2000; Mayberg, 1997). Furthermore, there is evidence that depressed individuals
often fail to deactivate the brains default-mode network (which includes the hippocampus) when asked to
perform a task (e.g., Sheline etal., 2009; see Hamilton,
Chen, & Gotlib, 2013), which may also have a net effect
of conserving energy. Diminished communicative behavior observed in depression (e.g., blunted affect; see
Berenbaum & Oltmanns, 1992; Rottenberg, Gross,
Wilhelm, Najmi, & Gotlib, 2002) might be seen as a strategy that prevents unnecessary energy expenditure as
well, in light of concomitant social withdrawal. Finally,
another key factor contributing to inactivity is diminished pleasure in previously valued goals and activities,
as evidenced by research showing muted responses to
pleasurable stimuli in depression (e.g., Pizzagalli etal.,
2009) as well as after experimentally induced inflammation (e.g., Eisenberger etal., 2010). Dopaminergic dysfunction is thought to play a key role in this phenomenon,
and perhaps cognitive and motor disturbances as well
(Nestler & Carlezon, 2006; Willner, 1995), which may be
a biological mechanism for discouraging appetitive
behavior (and thus energy consumption) during stress or
illness. Of note, loss of libido, decreased investment in
progeny, and withdrawal from close relationships (all
vital evolutionary demands) parallel the common precipitating factors of rejection by a lover, loss of an offspring, and public humiliation.
The initial cognitive evolutionary formulation (Beck,
1993) did not attempt to account for atypical symptoms
of depression, such as hypersomnia and increased appetite (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Nevertheless, if conceptualized as behavioral strategies to
replenish energy, these symptoms can be considered in
line with the broader function of energy conservation as
well. Specifically, sleeping more than usual undoubtedly
promotes energy restoration beyond mere inactivity, and
notably is common in response to infection. Similarly,
increased caloric intake should increase energy reserves,
overcoming the fact that some energy is used in the process of consumption (especially if minimal effort/energy
is required to obtain the food). It is interesting that
increased appetite is common in seasonal depression
(APA, 2013; Rosenthal etal., 1984), perhaps reflecting an
evolutionarily derived strategy to compensate for
decreased food availability during winter. Biologically,
some recent evidence suggests that the likelihood of
depression being manifest in traditional versus atypical
forms (e.g., decreased vs. increased sleep and appetite)
reflects the relative balance of the stress and immune
responses within an individual (e.g., Lamers etal., 2013),
but this requires further testing/replication. From a cognitive perspective, we hypothesize that atypical depressive
symptoms may be more common in the absence of
prominent hopelessness (leading to perceptions that current resource scarcity is temporary). We are not aware of
any research that has tested this directly. But notably, in
a recent, large-scale factor analytic study of clinical symptom ratings, hopelessness and atypical symptoms did
load on separate factors (Li etal., 2014). This prediction
is also consistent with lower levels of hopelessness
observed in seasonal depression (Michalak etal., 2002),
which is commonly characterized by certain atypical
symptoms (e.g., hypersomnia; APA, 2013).
11
12
clinical decision making (about diagnosis and treatment)
hinges on determining who is at risk and then how to
intervene to reduce risk and promote resilience.
The proposed links between precipitating factors and
symptoms of depression described earlier are portrayed
in Figure 2. As shown in this figure, depressogenic beliefs
interact with the precipitating stressor(s) to generate negative cognitive appraisals. It is important that both stress
and predisposing beliefs may not be necessary to precipitate such appraisals, and yet can be sufficient. When
there is a perceived loss of a vital investment, cognitive,
emotional, and biological processes are initiated in service of energy conservation. In this sense, the conservation of energy is under cognitive control, and will in turn
be abandoned when the cognitive appraisal changes
from a worldview of scarcity to one of availability of vital
resources. In line with this idea, cognitive appraisals also
play a critical and direct role in mediating the effect of
stress on immune functioning (see Denson etal., 2009).
Controlled by areas of the brain that evolved relatively
later (e.g., prefrontal cortex; see Ochsner & Gross, 2005),
this capacity for cognitive flexibility is generally quite
adaptive for responding to complex or novel situational
demands. And yet, the presence of depressogenic beliefs
can undermine the utilization of this capacity (and more
broadly, strategies for terminating the program). In this
sense, the capacity to develop such beliefs (which may
be unique to humans) creates the potential for chronic
difficulties that can persist even after a stressful situation
has resolved (see Sapolsky, 2004), and in turn bouts of
depression that are prolonged or endogenous. Furthermore, these beliefs (and the schemas in which they are
embedded) are reinforced/strengthened by negative
appraisals, which can promote cognitive and behavioral
rigidity (e.g., reflected in diminished activity within the
brains executive network; see Hamilton etal., 2013). We
return to these topics later when (briefly) discussing ways
to alleviate depression.
13
Depressogenic Beliefs
i.e., the Negative Cognitive Triad
Stressor(s)
Negative Automatic
Thoughts
Autonomic &
Immune Responses
()
()
Sickness Behaviors
e.g., Anergia, Anorexia, Anhedonia
+ Vigilance
14
energy, in response to the perceived loss. Prolonged activation of this program can result in consolidation/reinforcement of depressogenic beliefs coupled with neural
atrophy in key cognitive brain structures, thus exacerbating future risk.
Figure 2 also highlights several factors that seem to
play a key role in symptom course and prognosis, and
thus we argue are important in determining whether the
depression program is maintained or terminated. These
factors are reviewed in the next section, which briefly
discusses recovery or reversal processes.
15
16
Ultimately, a better understanding of the mechanisms involved in these treatments not only would
help us refine them, but also could help foster the
optimization of individual treatment plans (in service of precision medicine).
5. Perhaps due to the dominance of the disease
model, we feel that the topic of resilience has
been relatively understudied. For example, it
would be interesting to carefully study the cognitive and biological characteristics of individuals
who experience significant losses yet due not
become severely depressed, as well as those who
recover quickly without formal treatment (see,
e.g., Charney, 2014). Such work would have
important implications for validating/refining our
unified model, and also direct clinical implications
(see, e.g., Waugh & Koster, 2015).
Author Contributions
A. T. Beck developed the framework of the model and wrote
the initial draft of the manuscript. K. Bredemeier assisted in
reviewing key literatures and updating the model/manuscript.
The authors worked closely together to complete final revisions
and edits, as well as incorporate feedback from colleagues.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Greg Siegle, Connie Hammen,
George Slavich, David A. Clark, Dwight Evans, Stefan Hofmann,
Robert Leahy, Steve Hollon, Rob DeRubeis, Christopher
Beevers, Brandon Gibb, Ken Kendler, Paul Andrews, Michael
Thase, Jay Fournier, Jay Schulkin, Kelly Rohan, Lorenzo
Lorenzo-Luaces, Greg Brown, Abby Adler, Shari Jager-Hyman,
Kelly L. Green, Ben Rosenberg, Elaine Elliott-Moskwa, and
Adam Rifkin for their feedback on our model and preliminary
drafts of the manuscript. Also, we would like to thank Robyn
Himelstein for her assistance with literature reviews.
References
Funding
This work was supported in part by the Fieldstone 1793
Foundation.
Notes
1. It is well established that depressive disorders are more common in females (APA, 2013). Nevertheless, the model does
explicitly not address these gender differences, based on evidence suggesting that the same factors are involved in the etiology of depression for both genders, but some are simply more
common in females (e.g., Hamilton, Stange, Abramson, & Alloy,
2015; Nolen-Hoeksema, Larson, & Grayson, 1999; see NolenHoeksema & Girgus, 1994). Thus, we propose that our model is
applicable to depression in both males and females.
Abela, J. R., & Skitch, S. A. (2007). Dysfunctional attitudes, selfesteem, and hassles: Cognitive vulnerability to depression
in children of affectively ill parents. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 45, 11271140.
Abela, J. R., & Sullivan, C. (2003). A test of Becks cognitive
diathesis-stress theory of depression in early adolescents.
Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 384404.
Adolphs, R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social
cognition? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
1191, 4261.
Aisa, B., Tordera, R., Lasheras, B., Del Rio, J., & Ramirez,
M. J. (2008). Effects of maternal separation on hypothalamicpituitaryadrenal responses, cognition and vulnerability to stress in adult female rats. Neuroscience, 154,
12181226.
17
Allen, N. B., & Badcock, P. B. (2003). The social risk hypothesis of depressed mood: Evolutionary, psychosocial, and
neurobiological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129,
887913.
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency
in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 441485.
Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., & Francis, E. L. (1999). Do negative cognitive styles confer vulnerability to depression?
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 128132.
Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M.E.,
Tashman, N. A., Steinberg, D. L., . . . Donovan, P. (1999).
Depressogenic cognitive styles: Predictive validity, information processing and personality characteristics, and developmental origins. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37,
503531.
Alloy, L. B., & Ahrens, A. H. (1987). Depression and pessimism for the future: Biased use of statistically relevant
information in predictions for self versus others. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 366378.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington,
DC: Author.
Andrews, P. W., Bharwani, A., Lee, K. R., Fox, M., & Thomson,
J. A. (2015). Is serotonin an upper or a downer? The evolution of the serotonergic system and its role in depression and the antidepressant response. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 164188.
Andrews, P. W., & Thomson, J. (2009). The bright side of being
blue: Depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex
problems. Psychological Review, 116, 620654.
Antoni, M. H., Cruess, S., Cruess, D. G., Kumar, M., Lutgendorf,
S., Ironson, G., . . . Schneiderman, N. (2000). Cognitivebehavioral stress management reduces distress and
24-hour urinary free cortisol output among symptomatic
HIV-infected gay men. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 22,
2937.
Barden, N., Reul, J., & Holsboer, F. (1995). Do antidepressants
stabilize mood through actions on hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenocortical system? Trends in Neurosciences, 18, 611.
Beck, A.T. (1963). Thinking and depression: I. Idiosyncratic
content and cognitive distortions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 9, 324333.
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
Press.
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York, NY: International Universities Press.
Beck, A. T. (1982). Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. In P. Clayton & J. Barrett (Eds.), Treatment of
depression: Old controversies and new approaches (pp.
265290). New York, NY: Raven Press.
Beck, A. T. (1993). The descent of man: An evolutionary perspective on major depression. Newsletter of the Society for
Research in Psychopathology, 3, 36.
Beck, A. T. (1996). Beyond belief: A theory of modes, personality, and psychopathology. In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.),
Frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 125). New York, NY:
Guilford.
18
Canli, T., & Lesch, K. P. (2007). Long story short: The serotonin
transporter in emotion regulation and social cognition.
Nature Neuroscience, 10, 11031109.
Cantor, N. (1990). From thought to behavior: Having and
doing in the study of personality and cognition. American
Psychologist, 45, 735750.
Capuron, L., & Miller, A. H. (2004). Cytokines and psychopathology: Lessons from interferon-. Biological Psychiatry,
56, 819824.
Caspi, A., Hariri, A. R., Holmes, A., Uher, R., & Moffitt, T. E.
(2010). Genetic sensitivity to the environment: The case
of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for
studying complex diseases and traits. Focus, 8, 398416.
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W.,
Harrington, H., . . . Poulton, R. (2003). Influence of life
stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the
5-HTT gene. Science, 301, 386389.
Chango, J. M., McElhaney, K. B., Allen, J. P., Schad, M. M.,
& Marston, E. (2012). Relational stressors and depressive
symptoms in late adolescence: Rejection sensitivity as a
vulnerability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40,
369379.
Charney, D. S. (2014). Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience and vulnerability. Focus, 2, 368391.
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Scientific foundations of
cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New York, NY:
John Wiley.
Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Brown, G. K. (1992). Sociotropy,
autonomy, and life event perceptions in dysphoric and
nondysphoric individuals. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
16, 635652.
Clark, D. A., Steer, R. A., Haslam, N., Beck, A. T., & Brown,
G. K. (1997). Personality vulnerability, psychiatric diagnoses, and symptoms: Cluster analyses of the SociotropyAutonomy Subscales. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21,
267283.
Clasen, P. C., Fisher, A. J., & Beevers, C. G. (2015). Moodreactive self-esteem and depression vulnerability: Personspecific symptom dynamics via smart phone assessment.
PLoS ONE, 10, e0129774.
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the
buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310357.
Cohen-Woods, S., Craig, I. W., & McGuffin, P. (2013). The current state of play on the molecular genetics of depression.
Psychological Medicine, 43, 673687.
Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2000). A safe haven: An attachment theory perspective on support seeking and caregiving
in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78, 10531073.
Conway, C. C., Hammen, C., Espejo, E. P., Wray, N. R., Najman,
J. M., & Brennan, P. A. (2012). Appraisals of stressful life
events as a genetically-linked mechanism in the stress
depression relationship. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
36, 338347.
Cristea, I., Huibers, M., David, D., Hollon, S., Andersson, G., &
Cuijpers, P. (2015). The effects of cognitive behavior therapy for adult depression on dysfunctional thinking: A metaanalysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 6271.
Cuijpers, P., Driessen, E., Hollon, S. D., van Oppen, P., Barth,
J., & Andersson, G. (2012). The efficacy of non-directive
supportive therapy for adult depression: A meta-analysis.
Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 280291.
Dalgleish, T., & Watts, F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and
memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical
Psychology Review, 10, 589604.
Dantzer, R., OConnor, J. C., Freund, G. G., Johnson, R. W., &
Kelley, K. W. (2008). From inflammation to sickness and
depression: When the immune system subjugates the brain.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 4656.
Davis, C., & Levitan, R. D. (2005). Seasonality and seasonal
affective disorder (SAD): An evolutionary viewpoint tied
to energy conservation and reproductive cycles. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 87, 310.
DeFronzo, R., Panzarella, C., & Butler, A. C. (2001). Attachment,
support seeking, and adaptive feedback: Implications for
psychological health. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 8,
4852.
Denson, T. F., Spanovic, M., & Miller, N. (2009). Cognitive
appraisals and emotions predict cortisol and immune
responses: A meta-analysis of acute laboratory social stressors and emotion inductions. Psychological Bulletin, 135,
823853.
De Raedt, R., Vanderhasselt, M. A., & Baeken, C. (2015).
Neurostimulation as an intervention for treatment resistant depression: From research on mechanisms towards
targeted neurocognitive strategies. Clinical Psychology
Review, 41, 6169.
DeRubeis, R. J., Siegle, G. J., & Hollon, S. D. (2008). Cognitive
therapy versus medication for depression: Treatment outcomes and neural mechanisms. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 788796.
Dinan, T. (1996). Serotonin and the regulation of hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal axis function. Life Sciences, 58, 16831694.
Disner, S. G., Beevers, C. G., Haigh, E. A. P., & Beck, A. T.
(2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of
depression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 467477.
Dowlati, Y., Herrmann, N., Swardfager, W., Liu, H., Sham, L.,
Reim, E. K., & Lanctt, K. L. (2010). A meta-analysis of
cytokines in major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 67,
446457.
Drevets, W. C. (2000). Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 813829.
Driessen, E., Cuijpers, P., Hollon, S. D., & Dekker, J. J. (2010).
Does pretreatment severity moderate the efficacy of psychological treatment of adult outpatient depression? A metaanalysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78,
668680.
Duncan, L., & Keller, M. (2011). A critical review of the first
10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction
research in psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry,
168, 10411049.
Durisko, Z., Mulsant, B. H., & Andrews, P. W. (2015). An adaptationist perspective on the etiology of depression. Journal
of Affective Disorders, 172, 315323.
Dweck, C. S., & Elliott-Moskwa, E. S. (2010). Self-theories: The
roots of defensiveness. In J. E. Maddux & J. P. Tangney
19
(Eds.), Social psychological foundations of clinical psychology (pp. 136156). New York, NY: Guilford.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive
approach to motivation and personality. Psychological
Review, 95, 256273.
DZurilla, T. J., Chang, E. C., Nottingham, E. J., & Faccini, L.
(1998). Social problem-solving deficits and hopelessness,
depression, and suicidal risk in college students and psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54,
10911107.
Egan, M. F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J. H., Goldberg, T. E.,
Kolachana, B. S., Bertolino, A., . . . Weinberger, D. R.
(2003). The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activitydependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and
hippocampal function. Cell, 112, 257269.
Eisenberger, N. I., Berkman, E. T., Inagaki, T. K., Rameson,
L. T., Mashal, N. M., & Irwin, M. R. (2010). Inflammationinduced anhedonia: Endotoxin reduces ventral striatum
responses to reward. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 748754.
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection
hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and
social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 294300.
Elliott, R., Rubinsztein, J. S., Sahakian, B. J., & Dolan, R. J.
(2002). The neural basis of mood-congruent processing
biases in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59,
597604.
Flint, J., & Kendler, K. S. (2014). The genetics of major depression. Neuron, 81, 484503.
Fournier, J. C., DeRubeis, R. J., Hollon, S. D., Dimidjian, S.,
Amsterdam, J. D., Shelton, R. C., & Fawcett, J. (2010).
Antidepressant drug effects and depression severity:
A patient-level meta-analysis. Journal of the American
Medical Association, 303, 4753.
Frodl, T., Reinhold, E., Koutsouleris, N., Reiser, M., &
Meisenzahl, E. M. (2010). Interaction of childhood stress
with hippocampus and prefrontal cortex volume reduction
in major depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44,
799807.
Gaab, J., Blttler, N., Menzi, T., Pabst, B., Stoyer, S., &
Ehlert, U. (2003). Randomized controlled evaluation of
the effects of cognitivebehavioral stress management
on cortisol responses to acute stress in healthy subjects.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 767779.
Gaab, J., Rohleder, N., Nater, U. M., & Ehlert, U. (2005).
Psychological determinants of the cortisol stress
response: The role of anticipatory cognitive appraisal.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 599610.
Gazal, M., Souza, L. D., Fucolo, B. A., Wiener, C. D., Silva, R.
A., Pinheiro, R. T., . . . Kaster, M. P. (2013). The impact
of cognitive behavioral therapy on IL-6 levels in unmedicated women experiencing the first episode of depression:
A pilot study. Psychiatry Research, 209, 742745.
Gerritsen, L., Rijpkema, M., van Oostrom, I., Buitelaar, J., Franke,
B., Fernndez, G., & Tendolkar, I. (2012). Amygdala to hippocampal volume ratio is associated with negative memory bias in healthy subjects. Psychological Medicine, 42,
335343.
Gibb, B. E., Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Beevers, C. G., &
Miller, I. W. (2004). Cognitive vulnerability to depression: A
20
Hammen, C., Ellicott, A., Gitlin, M., & Jamison, K. (1989).
Sociotropy/autonomy and vulnerability to specific life
events in patients with unipolar depression and bipolar
disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 154160.
Hammen, C., & Goodman-Brown, T. (1990). Self-schemas and
vulnerability to specific life stress in children at risk for
depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 215227.
Hammen, C., Henry, R., & Daley, S. E. (2000). Depression and
sensitization to stressors among young women as a function of childhood adversity. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 68, 782787.
Hammen, C., & Watkins, E. (2008). Depression (2nd ed.). New
York, NY: Psychology Press.
Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Miller, N., & Haeffel, G. J.
(2004). Cognitive vulnerability-stress theories of depression: Examining affective specificity in the prediction of
depression versus anxiety in three prospective studies.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 309345.
Harmer, C. J., Goodwin, G. M., & Cowen, P. J. (2009). Why do
antidepressants take so long to work? A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 195, 102108.
Harrison, N. A., Brydon, L., Walker, C., Gray, M. A., Steptoe, A., &
Critchley, H. D. (2009). Inflammation causes mood changes
through alterations in subgenual cingulate activity and mesolimbic connectivity. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 407414.
Haslam, N., & Beck, A. T. (1994). Subtyping major depression:
A taxometric analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
103, 686692.
Haslam, N., Holland, E., & Kuppens, P. (2012). Categories versus dimensions in personality and psychopathology: A
quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychological
Medicine, 42, 903920.
Hayden, E. P., Dougherty, L. R., Maloney, B., Olino, T. M.,
Sheikh, H., Durbin, C. E., . . . Klein, D. N. (2008). Earlyemerging cognitive vulnerability to depression and the
serotonin transporter promoter region polymorphism.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 107, 227230.
Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and
cognitive therapies. Behavior Therapy, 35, 639665.
Herman, J., & Cullinan, W. (1997). Neurocircuitry of stress:
Central control of the hypothalamopituitaryadrenocortical axis. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 7884.
Hilgard, E. R. (1980). The trilogy of mind: Cognition, affection, and conation. Journal of the History of the Behavioral
Sciences, 16, 107117.
Hollon, S. D., Stewart, M. O., & Strunk, D. R. (2006). Enduring
effects for cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of
depression and anxiety. Annual Review of Psychology, 57,
285313.
Hooley, J. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1989). Predictors of relapse in
unipolar depressives: Expressed emotion, marital distress,
and perceived criticism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
98, 229235.
Horney, K. (1937). The neurotic personality of our time. New
York, NY: Norton.
Horowitz, A. V., & Wakefield, J. C. (2007). The loss of sadness:
How psychiatry transformed normal sorrow into depressive
disorder. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
21
22
and causal mediation components of the hopelessness
theory of depression. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63, 667675.
Michalak, E. E., Wilkinson, C., Hood, K., & Dowrick, C. (2002).
Seasonal and nonseasonal depression: How do they differ?:
Symptom profile, clinical and family history in a general population sample. Journal of Affective Disorders, 69, 185192.
Miller, A. H., Maletic, V., & Raison, C. L. (2009). Inflammation
and its discontents: The role of cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 65,
732741.
Miller, R., Wankerl, M., Stalder, T., Kirschbaum, C., & Alexander,
N. (2013). The serotonin transporter gene-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) and cortisol stress reactivity: A
meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 18, 10181024.
Minnen, A., Wessel, I., Verhaak, C., & Smeenk, J. (2005). The
relationship between autobiographical memory specificity
and depressed mood following a stressful life event: A prospective study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44,
405415.
Miu, A. S., & Yeager, D. S. (2015). Preventing symptoms of
depression by teaching adolescents that people can change:
Effects of a brief incremental theory of personality intervention at 9-month follow-up. Clinical Psychological Science,
3, 726743.
Monroe, S. M., & Harkness, K. L. (2005). Life stress, the kindling hypothesis, and the recurrence of depression:
Considerations from a life stress perspective. Psychological
Review, 112, 417445.
Monroe, S. M., Slavich, G. M., Torres, L. D., & Gotlib, I. H.
(2007). Severe life events predict specific patterns of change
in cognitive biases in major depression. Psychological
Medicine, 37, 863871.
Moreira, F. P., Cardoso, T., Mondin, T. C., Souza, L., Silva, R.,
Jansen, K., . . . Wiener, C. D. (2015). The effect of proinflammatory cytokines in cognitive behavioral therapy.
Journal of Neuroimmunology, 285, 143146.
Munaf, M. R., Brown, S. M., & Hariri, A. R. (2008). Serotonin
transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation:
A meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 852857.
Murphy, F. C., Sahakian, B. J., Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael,
A., Rogers, R. D., Robbins, T. W., & Paykel, E. S. (1999).
Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania
and depression. Psychological Medicine, 29, 13071321.
Musliner, K. L., Seifuddin, F., Judy, J. A., Pirooznia, M., Goes,
F. S., & Zandi, P. P. (2015). Polygenic risk, stressful life
events and depressive symptoms in older adults: A polygenic score analysis. Psychological Medicine, 45, 17091720.
Nestler, E. J. (2014). Epigenetic mechanisms of depression.
JAMA Psychiatry, 71, 454456.
Nestler, E. J., & Carlezon, W. A. (2006). The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biological Psychiatry,
59, 11511159.
Nettle, D. (2004). Evolutionary origins of depression: A review
and reformulation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 81, 91102.
Nezu, A. M. (1986). Cognitive appraisal of problem solving
effectiveness: Relation to depression and depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 4248.
23
24
onset of major depression. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 28, 223243.
Sowislo, J. F., & Orth, U. (2013). Does low self-esteem predict
depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal
studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 213240.
Spitz, R. A., & Wolf, K. M. (1946). Anaclitic depression: An
inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early
childhood, II. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 2, 313342.
Squire, L. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans.
Psychological Review, 99, 195231.
Stetler, C., & Miller, G. E. (2011). Depression and hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal activation: A quantitative summary of four
decades of research. Psychosomatic Medicine, 73, 114126.
Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy;
with happiness, false memory: Mood and the false memory
effect. Psychological Science, 785791.
Stroud, C. B., Davila, J., Hammen, C., & Vrshek-Schallhorn, S.
(2011). Severe and nonsevere events in first onsets versus
recurrences of depression: Evidence for stress sensitization.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 142154.
Sullivan, P. F., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2000). Genetic
epidemiology of major depression: Review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 15521562.
Tanis, K. Q., Newton, S. S., & Duman, R. S. (2007). Targeting
neurotrophic/growth factor expression and signaling for
antidepressant drug development. CNS and Neurological
DisordersDrug Targets, 6, 151160.
Tartter, M., Hammen, C., Bower, J. E., Brennan, P. A., & Cole, S.
(2015). Effects of chronic interpersonal stress exposure on
depressive symptoms are moderated by genetic variation
at IL6 and IL1 in youth. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity,
46, 104111.
Teasdale, J. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition & Emotion, 2, 247274.
Thompson, R. J., Berenbaum, H., & Bredemeier, K. (2011).
Cross-sectional and longitudinal relations between affective
instability and depression. Journal of Affective Disorders,
130, 5359.
Troy, A. S., Wilhelm, F. H., Shallcross, A. J., & Mauss, I. B.
(2010). Seeing the silver lining: Cognitive reappraisal ability
moderates the relationship between stress and depressive
symptoms. Emotion, 10, 783795.
Tsigos, C., & Chrousos, G. P. (2002). Hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal axis, neuroendocrine factors and stress. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 53, 865871.
Tyrka, A. R., Wier, L., Price, L. H., Ross, N., Anderson, G. M.,
Wilkinson, C. W., & Carpenter, L. L. (2008). Childhood
parental loss and adult hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
function. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 11471154.
Van Harmelen, A. L., van Tol, M. J., Demenescu, L. R., van der
Wee, N. J., Veltman, D. J., Aleman, A., . . . Elzinga, B. M.
(2013). Enhanced amygdala reactivity to emotional faces in
adults reporting childhood emotional maltreatment. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 362369.
Vrshek-Schallhorn, S., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., Craske, M. G.,
Griffith, J. W., Sutton, J., . . . Adam, E. K. (2014). Refining
the candidate environment interpersonal stress, the serotonin transporter polymorphism, and gene-environment