The Compassionate Instinct
The Compassionate Instinct
The Compassionate Instinct
By Dacher Keltner
This essay originally appeared on Greater Good, the online magazine of
the Greater Good Science Center at UC Berkeley.
Humans are selfish. Its so easy to say. The same goes for so many
assertions that follow. Greed is good. Altruism is an illusion. Cooperation
is for suckers. Competition is natural, war inevitable. The bad in human
nature is stronger than the good.
These kinds of claims reflect age-old assumptions about emotion. For
millennia, we have regarded the emotions as the fount of irrationality,
baseness, and sin. The idea of the seven deadly sins takes our
destructive passions for granted. Plato compared the human soul to a
chariot: the intellect is the driver and the emotions are the horses. Life
is a continual struggle to keep the emotions under control.
Even compassion, the concern we feel for another beings welfare, has
been treated with downright derision. Kant saw it as a weak and
misguided sentiment: Such benevolence is called soft-heartedness and
should not occur at all among human beings, he said of compassion.
Many question whether true compassion exists at allor whether it is
inherently motivated by selfinterest.
Recent studies of compassion
argue persuasively for a different
take on human nature, one that
rejects the preeminence of selfinterest. These studies support a
view of the emotions as rational,
functional, and adaptivea view
which has its origins in Darwins
Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals. Compassion and
benevolence, this research suggests, are an evolved part of human
nature, rooted in our brain and biology, and ready to be cultivated for
the greater good.
The biological basis of compassion
My work set out to document, for the first time, whether compassion
can be communicated via touch. Such a finding would have several
important implications. It would show that we can communicate this
positive emotion with nonverbal displays, whereas previous research has
mostly documented the nonverbal expression of negative emotions such
as anger and fear. This finding would also shed light on the social
functions of compassionhow people might rely on touch to soothe,
reward, and bond in daily life.
In my experiment, I put two strangers in a room where they were
separated by a barrier. They could not see one another, but they could
reach each other through a hole. One person touched the other on the
forearm several times, each time trying to convey one of 12 emotions,
including love, gratitude, and compassion. After each touch, the person
touched had to describe the emotion they thought the toucher was
communicating.
Imagine yourself in this experiment. How do you suppose you might do?
Remarkably, people in these experiments reliably identified compassion,
as well as love and the other ten emotions, from the touches to their
forearm. This strongly suggests that compassion is an evolved part of
human naturesomething were universally capable of expressing and
understanding.
Motivating altruism
Feeling compassion is one thing; acting on it is another. We still must
confront a vital question: Does compassion promote altruistic behavior?
In an important line of research, Daniel Batson has made the persuasive
case that it does. According to Batson, when we encounter people in
need or distress, we often imagine what their experience is like. This is a
great developmental milestoneto take the perspective of another. It is
not only one of the most human of capacities; it is one of the most
important aspects of our ability to make moral judgments and fulfill the
social contract. When we take the others perspective, we feel an
empathic state of concern and are motivated to address that persons
needs and enhance that persons welfare, sometimes even at our own
expense.
In a compelling series of studies, Batson exposed participants to
anothers suffering. He then had some participants imagine that persons
pain, but he allowed those participants to act in a self-serving fashion
for example, by leaving the experiment.
Within this series, one study had participants watch another person
receive shocks when he failed a memory task. Then they were asked to
take shocks on behalf of the participant, who, they were told, had
experienced a shock trauma as a child. Those participants who had
reported that they felt compassion for the other individual volunteered
to take several shocks for that person, even when they were free to
leave the experiment.
In another experiment, Batson and colleagues examined whether people
feeling compassion would help someone in distress, even when their
acts were completely anonymous. In this study female participants
exchanged written notes with another person, who quickly expressed
feeling lonely and an interest in spending time with the participant.
Those participants feeling compassion volunteered to spend significant
time with the other person, even when no one else would know about
their act of kindness.
Taken together, our strands of evidence suggest the following.
Compassion is deeply rooted in human nature; it has a biological basis
in the brain and body. Humans can communicate compassion through
facial gesture and touch, and these displays of compassion can serve
vital social functions, strongly suggesting an evolutionary basis of
compassion. And when experienced, compassion overwhelms selfish
concerns and motivates altruistic behavior.
Cultivating compassion
We can thus see the great human propensity for compassion and the
effects compassion can have on behavior. But can we actually cultivate
compassion, or is it all determined by our genes?
Recent neuroscience studies suggest that positive emotions are less
heritablethat is, less determined by our DNAthan the negative
emotions. Other studies indicate that the brain structures involved in
positive emotions like compassion are more plasticsubject to
changes brought about by environmental input. So we might think about
compassion as a biologically based skill or virtue, but not one that we
either have or dont have. Instead, its a trait that we can develop in an
appropriate context. What might that context look like? For children, we
are learning some answers.
Some researchers have observed a group of children as they grew up,
looking for family dynamics that might make the children more