Mating 2
Mating 2
Mating 2
by
Susana Lucia Kugeares
2002
Committee:
____________________________________
Michael J. Telch, Supervisor
____________________________________
David M. Buss, Co-supervisor
____________________________________
Patrick K. Randall
____________________________________
Cindy Meston
____________________________________
Devendra Singh
____________________________________
Frank Wicker
by
Susana Lucia Kugeares, B.A., M.A.
Dissertation
Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of
the University of Texas at Austin
in Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements
for the Degree of
Doctor of Philosophy
Dedication
To my family
Acknowledgements
Without question, the undertaking of this dissertation project has been the
most grueling but most rewarding task of my life thus far. Even years ago when I
was only entering graduate school (before the turn of this century), I recognized
the enormous fortitude and courage this project would require of me and the
commensurate pride that I would feel if I accomplished it. My premonitions
turned out to be true. But what I had not predicted was how great the contributions
from others would be in order for this project to move from birth to completion,
like a child that took a village to raise it.
Part of that village was in Austin, when I started this project. Thanks goes
to Dr. Judy Langlois who generously let me use faces from her research on
attractiveness to pilot my study, and to Jasper Smits, whose help on little things
was actually quite big. Thanks also to Dr. Jan Kamphuis, one of the most gifted
writers I have met, for his invaluable editorial comments on earlier drafts (and for
using the word elegant to describe my study). I play back that tape and have not
forgotten your help. On this project and in the development of my professional
confidence in general, I will always owe you at least a little thanks for the seeds
you planted.
The UT Psychology Department has been blessed with some of the most
capable and skilled computer support staff that I have come across in any setting
v
vi
vii
reaching goals, his passion for his work, and his ability to transmit that excitement
to others. I hope that our paths continue to cross in the future, and if your lecture
invitations in the future ever lead you to Greece (or Tampa), let me know.
To Dr. Mike Telch, my committee chair and now a friend, I owe enormous
thanks. This dissertation simply would not have happened without his mentorship.
He took me on despite how novice I was about research and never flagged in his
support. His guidance with research design, statistical knowledge, and
conceptualization was superior. His intellectual creativity made it fun and his
enthusiasm reassured my at my deepest doubts. I cannot overstate how grateful I
am for his expertise, patience, and hospitality. Mike, as you said, it is a shame we
began working together so late in the game, but I hope that collaboration and
contact continues on.
The last of my Austin village were Penny Frohlich, Amanda Gregory,
Tracy Sloan, and Kevin Larson, who provided fun during the wee hours of the
morning still working in the computer lab, an ear for venting frustrations and
laments, and rationality during the inevitable crises that arose in this process and
innumerable other things that good friends do that help keep one motivated and
sane. Thanks for making my graduate school years the most fun and stimulating
ones of my life.
viii
As the dissertation process extended, for me, beyond state lines, I owe
certain thanks to folks in Florida as well. Thanks to Dr. Art Rosenblatt, the Chief
of Psychology at the Tampa VA hospital, to whom I owe being gainfully
employed without yet being dissertated. Drs. John Schinka and Glenn Curtiss
helped me walk my first statistical steps and, as true believers in science, directly
pointed out the flaws in my study. Both astute researchers and statisticians, they
were also great fun in meetings and I hope future research opportunities arise for
more of the same. Drs. Karen Nicholson, Sid Davis, and Ron Gironda have earned
my respect as well-rounded psychologists and my gratitude for their support of me,
each in his/her own unique way. My co-intern and now colleague at the VA, Dr.
Jennifer Duchnick, has been a support from day one, feeling joyful when I met yet
another goal and angry too when I met another obstacle. Her quiet determination
is an example to be followed. Thanks for accompanying me on this trek.
Help and support can come in many forms and my family provided most of
them. However, I want to especially thank my mother for being my library
connection. Her love of library research compensated for my lack of it and was
invaluable to me. My sister Zoe was a support from afar, and thanks to my father
who provided a cushion to fall on if I needed it and was always there to remind me
that I had already eaten the cow and only the tail remained. Sometimes, there is
no wisdom like that of the Greeks. I owe my sister, Christina, enormous thanks
ix
for so many things. Through the most chaotic times, she was a constant with
support as well as practical help, keeping my refrigerator stocked, my kitchen ratfree, and my clocks ticking. She was proud of me even during my discouraged
moments and was purely elated at the high points. I could not have asked for a
better cheerleader in my corner. Thank you all for believing in me.
Publication No.______________
this, the mating anxiety mechanism was hypothesized to estimate the likelihood of
rejection by a potential mate by calculating the discrepancy between their
respective levels of desirability (Mate Value Discrepancy). Gender differences in
mating anxiety were also expected based on documented sex differences in
preference for physical attractiveness (PA) and social status/financial resources
(S/R) in a mate. A fictitious dating service was created in which participants
viewed mate profiles of four types: high PA-high S/R, high PA-low S/R, low PAhigh S/R, and low PA-low S/R. A 2 (Profile PA) x 2 (Profile S/R) x 3 (Replicates
for each mate type) x 2 (Participant Gender) design, with 2 covariates (self-rated
PA and self-rated S/R) was used to test the effects of profile and participant
characteristics on Anxiety, Likelihood of Rejection (LR), and Likelihood of
Asking the individual for a date (LA). Hierarchical Linear Modeling was used to
test the predictions about mate value discrepancy (MVD) and LR on Anxiety.
MVD had a highly significant effect on Anxiety and on LR. LR had a significant
effect on Anxiety as predicted, but did not mediate the effect of MVD on Anxiety.
A gender differences in anxiety were found in the effect of profile status/resources
on anxiety, but not for other profile or participant characteristics as expected. The
implications of an unexpected sex difference (men having greater anxiety than
women) are discussed. The results suggest strong support a domain-specific
approach and the position that human emotions serve adaptive functions.
xii
Table of Contents
List of Tables................................................................. xvii
List of Figures.................................................................... xviii
List of Appendices..................................................................... xix
Chapter 1: Introduction
1.1 Examining Social Anxiety Through an Evolutionary Lens..... 1
1.2 The Adaptationist Perspective of the Human Mind..... 3
1.3 An Evolutionary View of Emotional Disorders....... 6
1.3.1 Proper Classification of Dysfunction.... 6
1.3.2 Misconstrual as Dysfunction......................... 7
1.4 The Function of Social Anxiety....... 8
1.4.1 A Proximal Theory: Self-Handicapping Strategy...... 9
1.4.2 An Evolutionary Account: Submissiveness Signal Hypothesis. 11
1.4.3 An Evolutionary Account: Social Exclusion Model.. 15
1.4.4 A Review of the Proposed Functions of Social Anxiety....... 20
1.5 A Review of Relevant Dating Anxiety Research......... 21
1.5.1 Physical Attractiveness and Social Anxiety......23
1.5.2 Social Skill Deficits and Social Anxiety.................. . 24
1.5.3 Cognitive Interference and Social Anxiety .. 26
1.5.4 Dysfunctional Self-Evaluations and Social Anxiety.... 27
1.5.5 The Self-Presentational Model..............28
1.5.6 A Review of the Features of Dating Anxiety 31
Chapter 2: Present Study
2.1 Mate Value Hypothesis: A Context-Specific Theory of
xiii
Social Anxiety. 34
2.1.1 Mate Value Discrepancy and Likelihood of Rejection
(Hypothesis 1).... 36
2.1.2 Gender Differences in Mating Anxiety (Hypothesis 2).... 37
2.2 Specific Contributions..................................................38
Chapter 3: Method
3.1 Overview of Experimental Design....................................... 39
3.2 Participants....................................................................... 39
3.2.1 Exclusionary Criteria.... 40
3.2.2 Participant Characteristics. 40
3.3 Stimuli: Date Profiles.. 44
3.3.1 Physical Attractiveness in Profiles 44
3.3.2 Status/Resources in Profiles.. 45
3.4 Measures...................................................... 47
3.4.1 Participant Measures: Mating-Relevant Variables...47
3.4.2 Participant Measures: Anxiety and Personality Variables... 48
3.4.3 Profile Measures... 51
3.5 Procedure......................................................................... 53
3.6 Steps to Enhance Manipulation Integrity. 54
3.7 Experimental Conditions......................................................55
Chapter 4: Statistical Analyses
4.1 Sample Size and Power........................................................... 56
4.2 Analytic Strategy................................................. 56
4.2.1 Hierarchical Linear Modeling.. 57
4.3 Specific Analyses................................................. 58
xiv
xvi
List of Tables
xvii
List of Figures
xviii
List of Appendices
xix
CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION
adaptive problems that existed recurrently over long spans of prehistoric time.
This is assumed because of the long span of time necessary to accumulate, bit by
bit, each additional modification and feature that results in complexly designed
adaptations. As a result, a given psychological mechanism may not have adaptive
consequences in the modern environments, if the environmental context relevant
to the mechanism has significantly changed. This requires that adaptations be
distinguished from adaptiveness (Symons, 1992). Second, all observed structures
and behavioral phenomena need not be adaptations. A given structure may be a
byproduct of an adaptation, that is a non-purposeful consequence of an adaptation
much like heat is an incidental byproduct of a light bulbs original lightproducing design. However, a byproduct can only be understood in relation to the
functionally designed adaptation that produces it. It must be remembered that any
emotion, including mating anxiety, may be merely a byproduct of some other
adaptation.
Since features of psychological mechanisms evolved to solve particular
adaptive problems, and those problems were specific to context, a psychological
mechanisms function, if one exists, can only be understood as it relates to the
ancestral human context of the adaptive problem it was designed to solve and in
which it is currently activated. By identifying adaptations one carves the
phenotype at its natural, functional joints (Symons, 1992, p. 140).
1997), pregnancy sickness (Profet, 1987), and allergies (Profet, 1991) are among
the physiological defenses for which adaptive benefits are suggested. If anxiety
similarly serves an adaptive defensive function, there may be detrimental
consequences to blocking it with medications (Nesse, 1990).
Last, socially nonconforming behavior can sometimes be labeled as
dysfunction when it might be the functional and proper output of an evolved
psychological mechanism. Psychopathy is one example. Despite its harmful
effects on social groups and even, in some ways, on the individual himself (e.g.,
incarceration), recent evolutionary theories view psychopathy as a frequencydependent adaptation a coordinated system of emotions, behaviors, motivations,
cognitions, and physiological reactions that promote a short-term sexual strategy
and a cheating social lifestyle (Harpending & Sobus, 1987; Mealey, 1995). We
will now turn our attention to social anxiety in particular.
their ability to create the positive social impressions they desire, and, therefore,
seek alternate strategies of self-presentation that, at least, minimize the negative
implications on themselves of failure (Snyder, et al., 1985). Snyder and
colleagues proposed that when an individual expects a high likelihood of failure
in a social situation yet avoidance is not possible, an anxious self-presentation is
employed to deflect failure from being attributed to a true lack of ability or to
other shortcomings. By impeding successful social performance, the social
anxiety serves as a ready excuse in the event that a social failure occurs
(Snyder, et al., 1985, p. 971). In addition, if the opposite occurs (that is social
success), the implications of evaluation by others of the individuals abilities are
even more positive than without the impediment at all. The authors hypothesize
that only individuals who feel a threat to an important dimension of their selfesteem (either intelligence, ability, or attractiveness) will use a self-handicapping
strategy in this way.
Indeed, individuals reported more social anxiety only in situations where
anxiety could be used to explain poor performance but not in situations where it
was precluded as an explanation (Snyder, et al., 1985). Other research found that
when anxiety was precluded as an explanation for failure, subjects reported other
explanations for poor performance such as lack of effort (Smith, Snyder, &
Handelsman, 1982). They interpreted this result as further evidence of the
10
11
animals, natural selection pressures are stronger for appearing able to win a
physical confrontation than for actual success in confrontations. The theory does
not state explicitly but implies that the appearance is associated with the actual
ability to win a physical confrontation. Hence, selection pressures led to the
evolution of signals of dominance and submissiveness because they were reliable
indicators of the likelihood of success in a physical confrontation (Ohman, 1987).
Ohman offered several pieces of evidence to support his claim that
socially aroused fear is distinct from fear evoked by other stimuli. He reported
that facial and behavioral expressions of fear differ when triggered in different
contexts. For instance, anxiety induced by the presence of a predator is more
automatic and is geared more for physical escape than socially related anxiety.
He also noted similarities between the behaviors associated with social anxiety in
humans and indicators of submissiveness in other animals, such as crouching,
freezing, and making submissive facial expressions. In non-human primates,
dominance conflicts frequently arise during mating and feeding interactions and
when meeting strangers. He also suggested that, in non-human primates,
dominance hierarchies are equally important for both males and females in
determining which individuals have access to limited resources (such as food and
mates). If these situations present opportunities for dominance conflicts in
humans as well, and if social anxiety serves the function proposed by the SSH,
12
then humans should feel social anxiety in the same contexts that involve
competition for social dominance as other animals: mating, feeding, and meeting
strangers.
Unfortunately, Ohmans (1987) SSH stops short of proposing specific
benefits that a display of submissiveness confers on the signal sender, nor does it
posit what danger(s) from the signal receiver submissiveness displays stave off.
The SSH might suggest that one possible benefit to the sender of a
submissiveness signal might be eliciting sympathetic reactions rather than
hostility from dominant others when the individual might not otherwise possess
the requisite qualities or skills to be dominant himself (or herself). Ohmans
theory implies that certain kinds of individuals (or any individual in a particular
type of social encounter) become motivated to exhibit a signal of submissiveness.
But, it fails to specify what could be thought of as design features of the
mechanism that governs submissiveness displays. For example, are the primary
triggers for displaying social anxiety located within the individual (e.g., small
physical size)? within the interactional partner (e.g., higher social status or
aggressive demeanor)? or in some other feature of the surrounding environment
(e.g., scarcity of a particular resource, thereby intensifying competition)?
Ohman predicts that social anxiety in humans should begin to manifest
itself during the developmental periods when individuals start to become sensitive
13
14
primates, one would infer that the SSH holds dominance hierarchies to be equally
important to human males and females. It follows that the SSH would predict no
gender differences in social anxiety that could not be accounted for by the
proposed social dominance effect.
15
16
abnormal, or incompetent should arouse the most social anxiety. It is not made
clear how being unattractive risks group expulsion. The SEM only states that
unattractiveness makes a person less desirable as an interactional partner
(Baumeister & Tice, 1990, p.169). Furthermore, the importance of attractiveness
does not follow from the SEMs premise that group living evolved in response to
the benefits to survival, since attractiveness is clearly more related to reproduction
than to survival.
How does incompetence lead to group exclusion? The SEM leaves this
vague, but it seems plausible that the model might view it as related to the first
threat to social exclusion, failures to contribute adequately to a groups
functioning.
How likely is social deviance to result in social exclusion? Evidence from
chimpanzee behavior suggests that deviant behavior can provoke a combination
of fear and hostility from other group members and does, indeed, lead to
exclusion from social interactions (Goodall, 1986). Observations of chimpanzees
suggests that excluding individuals who display abnormal behavior may be
adaptive for survival since abnormal behavior (e.g., limping or paralysis) is often
the result of disease. Therefore, excluding abnormal individuals from social
interactions could protect against contagion. It is possible that similar decision
17
18
19
individuals avoid those situations and behaviors that they feel anxious about,
those in which they anticipate being rejected or evaluated negatively. But, its
authors make no attempt to explain any possible adaptive function of avoidance.
They state that avoidance keeps the individual safe from acute anxiety, but
acknowledge the costs of possible chronic loneliness and lost opportunities to
build social bonds.
20
attempt to capture with a single mechanism the function of social anxiety in all
social contexts.
21
22
23
24
Social skill deficits have been consistently found among dating anxious
individuals. Relative to men with high frequencies of dates, low-dating men more
frequently leave pauses of longer than 10 seconds in their conversations, talk for
less total time, and introduce more negative topics into conversations. Their
female conversational partners are also less likely to respond positively to their
verbalizations than to those of men with low anxiety. Women with low dating
frequency are generally similar to low dating frequency men on this micro-level
of social skills. They make less eye contact, talk for less total time, and leave
more pauses of four seconds or longer than high frequency dating women do.
Another social skill relevant to dating is how to ask for a date, specifically the
directness of ones approach. Women who ask for a date directly are judged less
positively by others than women who ask indirectly (Muehlenhard, Koralewski,
Andrews, & Burdick, 1986). This suggests that demonstration of subtle social
skills can influence how attractive one appears to others.
Deficits in men and women on such subtle social behaviors as these can
lead to dating failures and to an increased likelihood of developing anxiety about
dating interactions. With respect to understanding what causes social anxiety,
these studies suggest that a lack of certain qualities or skills that would make an
individual desirable might activate social anxiety.
25
26
Other research suggests that social anxiety does cause an attentional shift
onto the interaction partner but in a distorted and skewed way. Hope and
Heimberg (1990) reported that high anxious individuals have better recall for
negative evaluative comments made by interaction partners than positive ones.
High anxious individuals also anticipate more negative evaluations by others and
interpret neutral feedback more negatively (Hope & Heimberg, 1990). These
findings suggest the attention of a socially anxious individual becomes restricted
to the interaction partners evaluation of them. Whether social anxiety leads to
focusing attention onto the self or onto the partner in a skewed manner, valuable
information about the partners characteristics on which social decisions can be
base is missed.
27
28
29
30
but this is not implied by the SPM. Third, novel situations where others
expectations are ambiguous or where general rules of behavior are unclear should
arouse the motivation to impress. All of these situations that trigger a desire to
impress others should also elicit social anxiety.
Schlenker and Learys (1982) second requisite component for arousing an
anxious response is doubt about ones ability to create the desired impression.
The SPM identifies low self-esteem and poor social and communication skills as
the root of low performance outcome expectancies.
31
judgments of attractiveness (Udry & Eckland, 1984) which suggests that poorer
social skills may detract from attractiveness and, in turn, increase social anxiety.
Rather than being the result of social skill deficits, social anxiety may
cause apparent skills deficits, or at least poor social performance, through
cognitive interference. This interference occurs through a shift in attention onto
the self or onto cues of negative feedback from the interaction partner, resulting in
missing important positive information about the interaction and the partner.
Anxiety also interferes with the accurate appraisal of social interactions,
such as interpreting neutral feedback more negatively, remembering more
negative than positive feedback, and anticipating more negative evaluation from
others. These attentional shifts and distorted appraisals can lead to poor social
performance, lower evaluations by others, and more anxiety. Anxiety-producing
dysfunctional appraisals also include expecting social performance failures,
having unattainably high standards for social performance, and underestimating
ones social skill, social performance, and others positive evaluations.
Schlenker and Learys (1985) Self-Presentational Model conceptualizes
social anxiety as the product of high motivation to appear desirable mixed with
doubt about ones ability to successfully create that impression. The motivation
to impress may be driven by an evolved desire to be maximally included in social
groups (Leary, 1990), and is proposed to be highest when the interaction partner
32
33
34
35
36
signals of availability and receptivity or interest, may also influence the likelihood
of rejection but are beyond the scope of this investigation. The following
predictions were made about Mate Value Discrepancy (MVD).
Prediction 2: For both men and women, Likelihood of Rejection (LR) was
expected to be higher for negative MVD scores (i.e., participant desirability was
lower than potential mates desirability); LR was expected to be lower when
MVD score was positive (i.e., when participant desirability exceeded potential
mates desirability).
Prediction 3: Based on the hypothesized association between Likelihood of
Rejection and Anxiety, a relationship similar to Prediction 2 was expected
between MVD and Anxiety.
Prediction 4: Finally, it was speculated that Likelihood of Rejection mediates the
relationship between MVD and Anxiety.
37
that different levels of participants own PA and S/R would lead to gender
differences in anxiety. The following specific predictions were made about the
effect of profile characteristics and participant characteristics on anxiety.
Prediction 5: Men were expected to show greater anxiety than women when
Target Physical Attractiveness level was high than low.
Prediction 6: Women were expected to show greater anxiety than men when
Targets Status/Resources level was high than low.
Prediction 7: Men were expected to show a steeper increase in anxiety as selfrated Status/Resources decreased.
Prediction 8: Women were expected to show a steeper increase in anxiety as selfrated Physical Attractiveness decreased.
38
CHAPTER 3: METHOD
3.2 Participants
Participants were recruited from a large pool of introductory psychology
students at the University of Texas at Austin, for whom participation fulfilled part
of the course requirements. Since this population is active in mate-seeking, it was
considered an appropriate population on which to test hypotheses about mating
anxiety at the stage of attaining a mate (see Ohman, 1987 for similar
assumptions). Individuals were eligible to participate if they were not involved in
an exclusive dating relationship at the time of the study, were interested in
meeting new people, and spoke English as their first language.
39
40
41
Women
n
%
122
49.2%
n
126
Men
%
50.8%
23
89
4
6
18.9%
73.0%
3.3%
4.9%
10
94
15
7
7.9%
74.6%
11.9%
5.6%
84
14
13
5
5
68.9%
11.5%
10.7%
4.1%
4.0%
64
27
19
6
9
50.8%
21.4%
15.1%
4.8%
7.2%
115
0
7
94.3%
0%
5.8%
115
6
2
91.3%
4.8%
1.6%
2
55
20
12
17
15
1.6%
45.1%
16.4%
9.8%
13.9%
12.3%
8
82
13
6
8
9
6.3%
65.1%
10.3%
4.8%
6.3%
7.1%
47
45
24
4
38.5 %
36.9 %
19.7 %
3.2 %
57
42
19
6
45.2 %
33.4 %
15.1 %
4.8 %
111
11
91.0 %
9.0 %
87
39
69.0 %
31.0 %
82
36
67.2 %
29.5 %
82
40
65.1 %
31.7 %
4
29
45
34
10
3.3 %
23.8 %
36.9 %
27.9 %
8.2 %
4
21
34
45
22
3.2 %
16.7 %
27.0 %
35.7 %
17.5 %
5
31
63
23
4.1 %
25.4 %
51.6 %
18.9 %
2
31
70
23
1.6 %
24.6 %
55.6 %
18.3 %
42
43
6.70
1.96
16.13
12.84
4.19
3.03
2.31
1.88
240
244
141
245
248
248
248
248
248
248
248
--
Desirability variables
Self-rated Physical Attractiveness
Self-rated Status/Resources
Self-rated Overall Desirability
Waist:Hip Ratio
1.38
1.29
1.31
--
7.31
5.11
1.31
12.01
1.78
1.94
1.04
0.73
SD
122
122
122
122
117
120
73
122
122
122
122
122
6.08
6.28
6.59
0.78
7.80
1.76
15.90
15.66
4.14
2.20
2.14
1.85
Women
1.32
1.16
1.26
.009
8.95
2.52
1.19
13.81
1.75
1.52
0.98
0.77
SD
Anxiety measures
DAS (range = 23-161)
247 82.38 24.82
122
84.03 23.48
LSAS-F (range = 0-72)
248 22.55 11.35
122
24.49 11.61
LSAS-A (range = 0-72)
248 21.06 11.68
122
22.40 11.58
STAI-S (range = 20-80)
248 35.21 11.21
122
37.53 11.97
STAI-T (range = 20-80)
248 39.28 10.47
122
42.01 10.82
SEQ (range = 10-40)
248 33.10
5.57
122
32.12
5.77
Note. LTM = Long-term mate; STM = Short-term mate; DAS = Dating Anxiety Survey; LSAS =
Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (Fear & Avoidance Subscales); STAI = State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory (State & Trait subscales); SEQ = Self-Esteem Questionnaire; BIDR = Balanced
Inventory of Desirable Responding (SDE = Self-Deceptive Enhancement, IM = Impression
Management).
***p .001, **p .01, *p .05.
5.92
6.16
6.49
--
Measure
Dating history variables
# past dating partners
# past sex partners
Age 1st intercourse (yrs.)
Longest relationship (mos.)
Degree seeking LTM
Degree seeking STM
Level of interest
Level of motivation
Total sample
Table 2. Means, Standard Deviations, and t-test Comparisons for Gender on Dating history,
Desirability, and Anxiety Variables
126
126
126
126
126
126
126
126
126
123
123
124
68
123
126
126
126
126
80.80
20.66
19.77
32.96
36.63
34.06
5.76
6.04
6.40
0.87
5.60
2.15
16.40
10.04
4.25
3.84
2.48
1.90
Men
26.04
10.80
11.68
9.96
9.43
5.23
1.42
1.41
1.36
.006
5.12
6.74
1.39
9.12
1.82
1.96
1.06
0.70
SD
1.02
2.69**
1.78
3.27***
4.18***
-2.77**
1.86
1.45
1.16
-9.55**
2.37*
-0.60
-1.94
3.76***
-0.47
-7.37**
-2.59**
-0.56
44
Faces were selected from a pilot study in which 21 male and 21 female
undergraduate introductory psychology students rated 36 male and 37 female
faces acquired from various hairstyle magazines. Each face was rated by
opposite-sex and same-sex participants on estimated age, attractiveness, and
sexiness. Ratings of the latter two characteristics were made on a scale from1,
not at all attractive or sexy, to 9, extremely attractive or sexy. Based on
means of opposite sex raters only (listed in Table 3), the three male and three
female faces with the highest PA ratings were selected for the high PA profiles,
and the three males and females with the lowest Attractiveness means were used
for the low PA profiles. This resulted in 24 final profiles, 12 profiles of women
and 12 of men.
45
46
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
3
Replicate
Mate Type
1.71 (1.27)
1.29 (0.94)
2.86 (1.33)
2.33 (1.63)
2.05 (1.25)
1.33 (0.94)
7.48 (1.63)
7.67 (1.39)
7.14 (1.64)
7.38 (1.54)
6.86 (1.71)
8.00 (1.47)
1.48 (0.83)
2.24 (1.25)
3.05 (1.49)
2.05 (1.12)
2.29 (1.27)
2.00 (1.10)
7.14 (1.21)
7.29 (1.06)
7.76 (1.08)
7.14 (1.32)
7.38 (1.23)
7.48 (1.06)
Attractiveness
Women
Men
(n = 21)
(n = 21)
M (SD)
M (SD)
1.24 (0.77)
1.10 (0.79)
2.14 (1.51)
1.95 (1.34)
1.33 (0.96)
1.10 (0.85)
6.48 (1.92)
7.00 (1.49)
7.19 (1.71)
Men
(n = 21)
M (SD)
1.43 (0.60)
2.05 (1.10)
2.71 (1.64)
1.71 (0.83)
1.86 (0.94)
1.67 (0.80)
6.91 (1.46)
7.52 (1.25)
8.10 (0.95)
7.52 (1.31)
7.29 (1.37)
7.43 (1.37)
Sexiness
7.24 (1.93)
7.14 (1.85)
8.10 (1.54)
Women
(n = 21)
M (SD)
Face Characteristic
Table 3. Mean Attractiveness and Sexiness Ratings from Pilot Study for the Faces Selected for the Profile Stimuli
sales representative, security guard, and postal clerk. Current educational level
and area of study (if applicable) were generated to logically correspond to the
occupations. The same 12 sets of S/R information (current educational level,
career goal, level of ambition, college major if applicable, and current
automobile) were used for male and female profiles. They were randomly
assigned to the faces.
3.4 Measures
3.4.1 Participant Measures: Mating-Relevant Variables
Demographics measure. This measure, entitled Love Letters Market
Research Survey, was created for this study and is included as Appendix D. It
measured participant age, ethnicity, year in college, past dating history, current
relationship status, level of interest and motivation to meet new people, and
sexual orientation. Filler items were also included to promote believability of the
fictitious dating service.
Dating Preferences and Beliefs. This questionnaire consisted of 34
items frequently used in human mating research, which assess various aspects of
desirability (or mate value) such as physical attractiveness, social status, financial
resources, intelligence, and desirability as a short-term mate (STM) and a longterm mate (LTM). It also assessed participants degree of seeking a LTM or STM
47
and which type of mate they typically find. Items were rated on 7-point or 9-point
Likert scales; two items were forced choice. Although these items have been used
in numerous other studies to measure mate value, no psychometric data is
available.
Waist-to-Hip Ratio (WHR). WHR was calculated by dividing the
circumference of the narrowest point of the waist by the circumference of the
widest point of the buttocks or hips. This measurement is highly correlated with
health and physical attractiveness in men (Singh, 1994) and women (Singh,
1993), and also fertility in women. Each participant measured his or her WHR
using a standard flexible tape measure and a mirror, privately in a nearby
restroom. Participants were given both written and verbal instructions on how to
measure their WHRs, as shown by Appendix B.
48
Contact, Active Intentions for Dating, and Dating Interactions. Support for
construct and concurrent validity have also been shown (Calvert, Moore, &
Jensen, 1987).
Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale-Self Report Version (LSAS; Liebowitz,
1987). This self-report measure of fear and avoidance of social situations is based
on the original LSAS clinician-administered interview. The LSAS is commonly
used in pharmacological treatment studies for social phobia (Liebowitz, 1993) and
has been shown to have good internal consistency and test-retest reliability
(Heimberg, Horner, Juster, Safren, Brown, Schneier, & Liebowitz, 1999). The
self-report version has shown high correlations with the LSAS interview version
(rs = .78 to .85) and high internal consistency (rs = ..79 to .95; Baker, Heinrichs,
Kim, & Hofmann, 2002). The questionnaire asks participants to rate their fear
and avoidance of 24 social interaction and performance-oriented situations, thus
created two subscales of 24 items each. Items are rated on a 4-point scale, from 0,
none for Fear or never for Avoidance, to 3, severe for Fear or Usually for
Avoidance. The title Social Anxiety and Avoidance Survey was used in this
study.
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene,
1970). This is a widely used, self-report inventory for the assessment of state and
trait anxiety. It contains 2 subscales of 20 items each rated on a 4-point Likert
49
scale. Items depict positive and negative feelings. The state anxiety subscale
(STAI-S) measures the intensity of various current feelings, from not at all to
very much so. The trait anxiety subscale (STAI-T) measures how often
participants generally experience various feelings, thoughts, and attitudes, from
almost never to almost always. For this study, the subscales were entitled
How You Feel (Present) or (General). Scores can range from 20 to 80. Both
subscales have demonstrated good psychometric properties, including a high
internal consistency of about .90 (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970).
Participants completed the STAI-S twice, first to establish a baseline anxiety level
and, second, after completing the fictitious dating service, to assess postexperiment anxiety. At the post-assessment, the measure was entitled Reactions
Survey.
Self-Esteem Questionnaire (SEQ; Rosenberg, 1965). This 10-item scale
assesses global self-esteem. Items are rated on a scale of 1-4 for a range of 10-40.
The SEQ has been shown to have high internal consistency (.92; Rosenberg,
1965) as well as good test-retest reliability (r = .85).
Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR; Paulhus, 1994).
This 40-item inventory, renamed the Personal Decisions & Responses Scale for
this study, measures the tendency to endorse socially desirable responses in selfreport. It consists of two subscales: Self-Deception scale (SDS) and Impression
50
51
52
participants were asked if they already [knew] any of the Love Letters members
and to what extent [they] believed this dating service was in Austin.
Participants rated the items on a 0%-100% probability scale. This measure was
entitled Opinion Survey and is included as Appendix H.
3.5 Procedure
Participants were run in same sex groups of 2-8. They were seated at
every other seat, which decreased likelihood of participants using social support
to dispelling their anxiety. The test session had two sections. Participants were
led to believe that only the first section of the session was an experiment and that
the second part was not part of the study. The explanation given to participants is
included as Appendix A.
Section 1 lasted about one hour and was led by one same-sex
experimenter. After the session was explained, participants completed the Dating
Preferences and Beliefs measure, SEQ, DAS, BIDR, LSAS, STAI, and then took
their WHR measurements individually in a nearby restroom.
Section 2 of the session, the dating service, lasted 1-2 hours and was led
by two opposite-sex experimenters, who presented themselves as representatives
of the fictitious Love Letters dating service. Participants were each given a
notebook of 12 profiles, Interest Letter, and envelope with fictitious return
53
address and logo, and were given a folder containing the Welcome to Love
Letters page, Market Research Survey, 12 Profile Assessment Surveys, Date
Choice measure, instruction page for completing the Interest Letter, and STAI
post-test (Reactions Survey). After completing these measures and the Interest
Letter, each participant had his or her photo taken, deposited the sealed Interest
Letter in a designated box, and completed the Opinion Survey. The photo was
included to arouse more real-life anxiety about being evaluated by a potential
date.
Once all participants were finished, the group was debriefed and informed
about the deceptive aspects of the study. They were also asked to refrain from
discussing the deception with other potential study participants. After participants
departed, experimenters together completed an Anxiety and Belief Evaluation
Form on each participant to document behavioral indicators of anxiety and degree
of belief.
54
the testing room with all materials; 2) the latter two experimenters introduced
themselves as Love Letters representatives and wore shirts with a Love Letters
company logo on them; 3) the two representatives behaved in a professionally
detached manner, as if unfamiliar, with the first section experimenter; 4)
participants were told that staying for the Dating Service part of the session was
voluntary and would not affect their credit; 5) all Section 2 data collection sheets
included the Love Letters logo; 6) the Welcome to Love Letters instruction page
(see Appendix C) was the first page in the data collection folders.
55
56
57
58
59
this was the case, a piecewise regression (a 2-piece linear function that has a
breakpoint at which the slope changes significantly) was tested for its goodness of
fit against the linear model, using a chi square statistic. No specific predictions
had been laid out about the influence of participant characteristics (i.e., Level 2
variables) on the Level 1 relationships of interest. However, when significant
variation was found among individual slopes in the Level 1 models, additional
exploratory analyses were conducted at Level 2. Participant variables tested at
Level 2 included gender, self-esteem (SEQ), socially desirable response style
(BIDR), various anxiety measures (DAS, LSAS, STAI), and mating-relevant
factors (SOI, WHR, participant self-rated MV, and degree seeking a LTM or
STM).
60
reduced. Provided the preceding analyses of LR and Anxiety, MVD and LR, and
MVD and Anxiety were found to be significant, LR was added to the Level-1
model of MVD and anxiety to test the mediation hypothesis.
61
CHAPTER 5: RESULTS
62
63
3.22
3.68
Perceived Status
Women
Men
5.13
5.20
0.12
0.12
0.11
0.10
6.90
6.76
2.74
3.25
Perceived Resources
Women
Men
0.15
0.15
0.13
0.13
0.11
0.11
2.01
2.74
2.15
2.48
Measure
Perceived PA
Women
Men
0.10
0.10
High S/R
M
SD
Low S/R
M
SD
Low PA
5.25
5.69
3.68
4.19
6.15
7.15
0.13
0.13
0.12
0.11
0.13
0.13
7.45
7.33
7.93
7.78
0.10
0.10
0.10
0.10
0.12
0.12
High S/R
M
SD
7.12
7.46
High PA
Low S/R
M
SD
Profile Type
Table 4. Means and Standard Errors on Manipulation Check Variables for Men and Women
The profiles of women were rated as slightly more attractive than the
profiles of men, F (1, 236) = 27.06, p < .001 (M = 4.96, SE = 0.08 and M = 4.36,
SE = 0.80, respectively). Male and female profiles were rated similarly with
respect to financial resources and social status.
64
Perceived PA
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
LL1 LL2 LL3
Fin. Resources
Replicates
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
LL1
LL2 LL3
Social Status
Replicates
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
LL1
LL2 LL3
Replicates
65
.04, and Likelihood of Approach ratings, F (8, 228) = 6.28, p < .001, than male
replicates. These Replicate findings support the inclusion of replicates in the
design to represent each Mate Type since participants anxiety, estimated
likelihood of being rejected, and estimated likelihood of asking for a date are not
equivalent across prospective mates. Since Replicate was not a key variable of
interest, the effect of Replicate was not examined in further analyses.
66
67
32.60
31.52
4.35
9.21
Likelihood of Rejection
Women
Men
Likelihood of Approach
Women
Men
0.99
0.98
1.88
1.85
1.40
1.36
4.67
12.67
34.37
34.72
6.90
12.68
1.11
1.10
1.92
1.90
1.48
1.43
High S/R
M
SD
6.78
10.58
Measure
Anticipated Anxiety
Women
Men
Low S/R
M
SD
Low PA
24.19
43.98
44.20
49.84
34.18
44.15
2.18
2.15
1.78
1.76
2.38
2.30
37.59
51.23
47.71
50.05
50.83
50.93
2.49
2.46
1.76
1.74
2.57
2.49
High S/R
M
SD
High PA
Low S/R
M
SD
Profile Type
9.36, p = .002, and PA x S/R, F (1, 223) = 71.21, p < .001. The interaction of PA
x Gender was not significant, contrary to prediction.
With respect to the conditional effect of S/R, men and women did not
differ significantly in their reported anxiety to high S/R profiles; but in response
to low S/R profiles, women had significantly lower anxiety than men, F (1, 237) =
11.14, p = .001. The significant PA x S/R interaction indicated that when profile
PA was high, participants anxiety was significantly greater for high S/R profiles
relative to low S/R profiles, F (1, 232) = 101.96, p < .001. However, under
conditions of low PA, the effect of profile S/R level on anxiety did not reach the
level of statistical significance (p = .068). That is, when participants were
examined as a whole, S/R level did not significantly affect anxiety when profile
PA was low.
The significant 3-way interaction of PA x S/R x Gender, F (1, 223) =
22.20, p < .001 helped further clarify the effect of S/R level on anxiety to low PA
profiles. The 3-way interaction is illustrated in Figure 2. Under conditions of low
profile PA, anxiety in women was not significantly influenced by the S/R level of
the profile (p = .824), whereas anxiety in men was significantly higher when
profile S/R level was high, F (1, 121) = 5.72, p = .018. In the high profile PA
condition, both women (F (1, 114) = 78.73, p < .001) and men (F (1, 118) =
24.39, p < .001) showed significantly higher anxiety to high S/R profiles than low
68
S/R profiles. However, as evident in Figure 2, S/R level led to a bigger difference
in anxiety within the high PA condition for women than men. That is, when PA
was high, womens anxiety to low S/R profiles was much lower than to high S/R
profiles.
Anxiety (0 - 100)
Women (n = 109)
60
50
40
High PA
Low PA
30
20
10
0
Low S/R
High S/R
Anxiety (0 - 100)
Men (n = 116)
60
50
40
High PA
Low PA
30
20
10
0
Low S/R
High S/R
69
70
71
Lik.of Rejection
Women (n = 116)
55
45
High PA
Low PA
35
25
Lik.of Rejection
Men (n = 119)
55
High PA
Low PA
45
35
25
72
73
Lik.of Approach
Women (n = 117)
40
High PA
Low PA
30
20
10
0
Lik.of Approach
Men (n = 120)
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
High PA
Low PA
74
50
40
Men
Women
30
20
10
Lo PA
Hi PA
Lo S/R
Hi S/R
Participant Characteristic
75
76
77
Anxiety
100
75
Linear
50
Piecewise
25
0
-25
-8 -6 -4 -2
.001 after breakpoint). For men, however, the change in slope of anxiety as a
function of MVD occurred at a larger negative MVD value (a breakpoint of 5),
where Profile desirability was much lower than the participants desirability ( = 1.38, t = -1.35, p = .177 before breakpoint; = 8.514, t = 20.07, p < .001 after
breakpoint). Below the breakpoint, the effect of MVD on mens anxiety was nonsignificant.
Anxiety
100
75
Women
Men
50
25
0
-25
-8
-6
-4
-2
Since a direct test of the effect of MVD on Anxiety above and beyond the
effects of Profile-MV or Participant-MV could not be conducted in HLM, two
additional analyses were performed. First, the effects of Profile-MV and
Participant-MV were tested in independent models. Both variables had a
significant effect on anxiety, shown in Figure 8 below. As one would expect,
greater anxiety resulted from higher levels of Profile-MV ( = 8.12, t = 31.15, p <
79
Anxiety
75
Profile MV
50
Participant MV
25
0
-25
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Next, a Level 2 model was used to test whether the effect of Profile-MV
on Anxiety differed depending on the level of Participant-MV. This was
conceptually similar, but not identical, to the interaction of the two variables in
MVD. The result was marginal but non-significant ( = -0.38, t = -1.71, p = .09).
However, when men and women were analyzed separately, a significant effect
was found for women ( = - 0.92, t = -3.03, p = .003), shown in Figure 9. For
women, higher Profile MV led to greater anxiety when self-rated MV was lower.
Participant-MV did not significantly influence the effect of Profile-MV on
Anxiety for men ( = 0.05, t = 0.20, p = .841).
80
Profile-MV on Anxiety
Women
Men
10
5
0
2
Participant Self-Rated MV
Since significant individual variation was found above and below the
breakpoint, exploratory analyses of several participant characteristics (Level 2
variables) were performed to examine their potential moderating influence. Slope
coefficients () and t-ratios with Bonferroni-corrected significance levels are
listed in Table 6. Higher scores on the DAS and LSAS-Fear scale heightened the
effect of MVD on Anxiety. No other Level 2 variables showed significant
moderating effects. Interestingly, Level 2 effects were found only for the slope
above the breakpoint for the sample and not below.
Although gender itself did not moderate the MVD-Anxiety relationship,
gender differences were found in the effects of other Level 2 variables. Table 7
includes slope coefficients () and t-ratios for these Level 2 effects by gender. As
81
82
1
-0.536
0.006
0.009
0.004
0.052
0.033
-0.008
-0.011
0.001
0.194
-0.121
0.184
-0.250
4.926
se
0.842
0.018
0.041
0.036
0.040
0.046
0.091
0.032
0.033
0.349
0.348
0.256
0.218
11.686
t-ratio
-0.636
0.359
0.232
0.107
1.288
0.711
-0.083
-0.330
0.025
0.555
-0.346
0.716
-1.148
-0.422
2
-0.375
0.058
0.091
0.073
0.066
0.090
-0.140
-0.060
-0.047
0.287
0.257
-0.263
0.310
-20.30
se
0.748
0.016
0.030
0.032
0.037
0.032
0.067
0.027
0.029
0.279
0.283
0.222
0.190
9.963
t-ratio
-0.501
3.628*
3.045*
2.278
1.814
2.810
-2.074
-2.198
-1.655
1.029
0.905
-1.184
1.632
-2.038
Above breakpoint
Note. DAS = Dating Anxiety Survey; LSAS = Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale,
Fear and Avoidance subscales; STAI = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, State and
Trait subscales; SEQ = Self-Esteem Questionnaire; BIDR = Balanced Inventory
of Desirable Responding, SDE = Self-Deceptive Enhancement, IM = Impression
Management; PA = Physical Attractiveness; S/R = Status/Resources; LTM =
Long-term Mate; STM = Short-term Mate; WHR = Waist-to-Hip Ratio.
*p .05.
Fixed Effect
Gender
DAS
LSAS-F
LSAS-A
STAI-S
STAI-T
SEQ
BIDR-SDE
BIDR-IM
Participant PA
Participant S/R
Seek LTM
Seek STM
Gender x WHR
Below breakpoint
83
0.008
0.115
0.124
0.086
0.133
-0.369
-0.050
0.060
0.610
0.030
34.09
DAS
LSAS-F
LSAS-A
STAI-S
STAI-T
SEQ
BIDR-SDE
BIDR-IM
Participant PA
Participant S/R
WHR
** p .01; * p .05.
-0.025
-0.095
-0.110
0.004
-0.072
0.206
0.016
0.009
0.618
0.285
-4.564
DAS
LSAS-F
LSAS-A
STAI-S
STAI-T
SEQ
BIDR-SDE
BIDR-IM
Participant PA
Participant S/R
WHR
Fixed Effect
0.058
0.090
0.082
0.101
0.114
0.317
0.078
0.077
0.652
0.664
26.089
0.026
0.061
0.066
0.058
0.065
0.118
0.045
0.053
0.591
0.624
6.298
0.134
1.285
1.523
0.855
1.165
-1.165
-0.644
0.778
0.936
0.046
1.307
-0.933
-1.550
-1.658
0.074
-1.112
1.743
0.362
0.169
1.045
0.456
-0.725
0.053
0.088
0.060
0.064
0.061
-0.034
-0.057
-0.046
0.491
0.081
-20.20
0.064
0.110
0.097
0.086
0.154
-0.308
-0.068
-0.033
-0.707
-0.061
-7.527
0.019
0.039
0.040
0.053
0.045
0.086
0.035
0.030
0.280
0.291
6.338
0.020
0.036
0.041
0.041
0.038
0.080
0.037
0.043
0.384
0.427
6.146
2.810*
2.260
1.497
1.202
1.366
-0.396
-1.642
-1.537
1.754
0.278
-3.186*
3.235*
3.041*
2.344
2.083
4.046**
-3.84**
-1.850
-0.775
-1.842
-0.143
-1.225
Above breakpoint
se
t ratio
Below breakpoint
se
t ratio
had been observed for the total sample, no Level 2 variables had a significant
effect on the MVD-Anxiety relationship below the breakpoint for men or women.
For women, higher trait anxiety (STAI-T), higher dating anxiety (DAS), higher
fear of social situations (LSAS-F), and lower self-esteem scores (on SEQ)
increased the degree to which positive MVD scores led to higher anxiety. For
men, dating anxiety (DAS) was a significant moderator as it was for women. In
addition, lower WHR in men was associated with a steeper slope after the
breakpoint in the effect of MVD on Anxiety.
84
Likelihood of Rejection
50
25
0
-8 -6 -4 -2 0
Coefficient
se
t ratio
Gender
DAS
LSAS-F
LSAS-A
STAI-S
STAI-T
SEQ
BIDR-SDE
BIDR-IM
Participant PA
Participant S/R
G x STAI-T
G x WHR
0.707
0.006
0.036
0.025
0.032
0.052
-0.069
-0.018
-0.014
-0.172
0.041
-0.0002
-12.97
0.428
0.008
0.020
0.017
0.019
0.021
0.040
0.015
0.012
0.147
0.172
0.044
6.547
1.651
0.698
1.790
11.491
1.673
2.512
-1.734
-1.204
-1.137
-1.169
0.241
-0.005
-1.982
*p .05.
85
Anxiety
100
75
50
25
0
0
25
50
75
100
Likelihood of Rejection
Significant individual variation existed (2 = 501.98, 195 df, p < .001) and
was explored with additional Level 2 analyses. Table 9 shows coefficients and
significance levels for these analyses. Significant moderating effects on the LRAnxiety relationship were found for fear of social situations (LSAS-Fear; =
0.009, t = 3.01, p = .003) and for trait anxiety (STAI-Trait; = 0.009, t = 2.97, p =
.003). At higher scores on each of these scales, higher LR estimates led to greater
86
Coefficient
se
t ratio
Gender
DAS
LSAS-F
LSAS-A
STAI-S
STAI-T
SEQ
BIDR-SDE
BIDR-IM
Participant PA
Participant S/R
G x DAS
G x LSAS-F
G x LSAS-A
G x STAI-T
G x WHR
-0.026
0.003
0.009
0.007
0.006
0.009
-0.008
-0.004
-0.001
0.025
0.033
0.001
0.005
0.003
-0.006
-1.055
0.072
0.001
0.003
0.003
0.003
0.003
0.006
0.003
0.002
0.027
0.026
0.003
0.006
0.006
0.007
1.019
-0.361
2.323
3.006*
2.495
1.651
2.970*
-1.296
-1.357
-0.243
0.908
1.294
0.192
0.811
0.536
-0.885
-1.036
*p .05.
significant both above and below the breakpoint ( = 2.49, t = 5.73, p < .001
below breakpoint; = 9.39, t = 25.84, p < .001 above breakpoint). To test for
partial mediation, a comparison was made of the 95% confidence intervals for the
MVD-Anxiety slopes and the slopes after LR was added. Considerable overlap
was found. These results suggest that LR did not function as a full or partial
mediator of the MVD-Anxiety relationship.
LR
p < .001
p < .001
MVD
Anxiety
p < .001
88
CHAPTER 6: DISCUSSION
The aim of the present study was to test specific design features of a
mating context-specific model of social anxiety. The design features included
characteristics of potential mates, characteristics of participants, and cognitive
appraisals made by participants (e.g., estimates of the likelihood of rejection and
mate value discrepancy) that may underlie differences in anxiety. Before
reviewing the major findings, the degree to which participants believed the
deceptive aspects of the procedure and the extent to which the profile stimuli
successfully represented the mate types deserve comment.
89
90
The stimuli created for this study were designed to represent four types of
mates, which varied on the characteristics of physical attractiveness (low and
high) and social status and resources (low and high). Participants perceptions of
the profiles on the manipulated characteristics showed very high consistency with
the mate type manipulations. The physical attractiveness manipulation accounted
for 90% of the variance in perceptions of profile attractiveness and the
status/resources manipulation accounted for 88% of the variance in perceived
financial resources. The information used to represent high and low financial
resources, which had not been tested before, can be considered valid and useful
for future studies. Similarly, the faces used in the profiles had greater ecological
validity (believability) for a dating service methodology than other faces used in
attractiveness research (e.g., Langlois, Ritter, Roggman, & Vaughn, 1991;
Langlois, Roggman, Casey, & Ritter, 1987). Perceived social status showed a
more unexpected and interesting result.
In designing the profiles, social status was considered conceptually closely
related to financial resources, as is obvious by the use of a single manipulated
variable called status/resources. Therefore, it was thought that information
representing high or low financial resources (e.g., current automobile, future
occupation such as physician or delivery truck driver) would also convey the
impression of high or low social status. The results of the manipulation check
91
92
93
rejected by a potential mate was significantly associated with level of anxiety and
with discrepancy in mate value. However, likelihood of rejection estimates did
not mediate the effect of mate value discrepancy on anxiety. The implications of
these findings and additional unexpected findings are discussed below.
94
95
not been examined as they relate to anxiety. The present finding is the first
empirical evidence that (non-pathological) social anxiety is more strongly driven
by characteristics of the interaction partner than characteristics of the subject.
96
may change in situations where mating potential exists, at least for men. This is
consistent with the point made by others that certain environmental criteria must
be present for a properly functioning psychological mechanism to be activated
(Tooby & Cosmides, 1990; Nesse, 1990) as is the case with physiological
mechanisms (e.g., calluses) and supports the context-specificity of social anxiety.
In light of the tests described above, the gender difference found in overall
anxiety to the stimuli likely represents a true sex difference in proneness to
experiencing anxiety in mating situations. Sex differences are precedented in
other domains of anxiety (Shear, Feske, & Greeno, 2000; Yonkers & Kidner,
2002), but by and large women show higher anxiety than men across a number of
anxiety domains: agoraphobic anxiety and panic (Bekker, 1996), generalized
anxiety, post-trauma anxiety and avoidance (Fullerton, Ursano, Epstein, Crowley,
Vance, Kao, Dougall & Baum, 2001), fear to blood/injury (Kleinknecht, 1988)
and to small animals, and even general social anxiety (e.g., Dell'Osso, Saettoni,
Papasogli, Rucci, Ciapparelli, Di Poggio, Ducci, Hardoy, & Cassano, 2002).
Although not representative of a disorder, the present sex difference in anxiety in
mating initiation contexts is the first known to this author to show a higher level
of anxiety in men than women. This finding, unique in the anxiety literature, also
underscores the value of a context-specific approach to conceptualizing and
investigating psychological mechanisms (Tooby & Cosmides, 1990; Tooby &
97
98
rather than an aversive anxiety that would lead to avoidance. This has
implications on the understanding of the function of mating anxiety and is
discussed in a later section.
Women and men were different in how profile status and resources
affected their anxiety. Both showed greater anxiety when a profile was highly
attractive and high in status and resources. However, women also showed the
reverse. That is, a highly attractive potential mate who had low status/resources
elicited a much lower level of anxiety in women than in men. These results
provided support for the expectation, in Prediction 6, that womens anxiety would
be more sensitive to differences in level of status/resources of potential mates.
However, the specific prediction that high S/R in the potential mate would lead to
greater anxiety in women than in men was not borne out. Rather, low profile S/R
led to significantly less anxiety in women than in men. Studies of the effect of
status and resources are limited to their influence on mate preferences and
desirability, and have consistently shown that higher status/resources in a mate is
more important to women than to men (e.g., Buss & Schmitt, 1993). Since
anxiety in the current context appears to track desirability, this finding may help
fine-tune our understanding of womens desire for status/resources. Women may
have a relatively stronger dislike for low status/resources in a mate than a desire
for high status and resources.
99
100
the present study, the relationship of desirability as a mate and anxiety had not
been examined. This could be related to the significant relationship of selfdeception and anxiety, and the issue of distorted yet healthy positive selfperceptions. Participants in this study rated themselves as higher than the average
in their level of desirability as a mate. In addition, higher self-deception on the
BIDR-SDS scale was associated with lower anxiety in men and in women. As
higher self-deception has been linked with positive self-esteem (Paulhus, 1991),
the present sample can be thought of normal individuals who likely maintain a
healthy degree of positive distortion, or illusory self-efficacy, about their assets,
abilities, and their mating desirability.
No support was found for the predicted gender differences for self-judged
physical attractiveness and status/resources. It was expected (Prediction 7) that
men would show higher anxiety than women when self-rated status/resources
were low. We also expected women to show higher anxiety than men when selfrated attractiveness was low (Prediction 8). The results indicated that men and
women were influenced to the same degree by their self-rated level on these
characteristics. Previous work with clinical populations found no gender
difference in the influence of negative self-appraisals on anxiety in subjects with
high dating anxiety (Clark & Arkowitz, 1975; Schlenker & Leary, 1985). The
present finding extends this observation to non-pathological levels of anxiety.
101
102
lower point, i.e., in response to prospective mates who were much lower in
desirability than did women. Womens mating anxiety, on the other hand,
appeared to become activated when the desirability of the prospective mate was
closer to her own.
Mens lower MVD breakpoint fits conceptually with the more general
finding of higher mating initiation anxiety in men than women. That is, when
opportunities for mating exist, men not only experience higher anxiety than
women but do so for a broader range of potential mates with respect to level of
desirability. In other words, men cast a wider net in the mating pool and are,
therefore, anxious to a broader range of potential mates than women. This greater
anxiety could help alert men to the resulting greater (or more frequent) possibility
of rejection.
The lower breakpoint for men may also be related to a relaxation of
mating standards in certain contexts, which leads to a broader range of potential
mates being acceptable to men (Buss, 1994). The fact that this loosening of
standards occurs in short-term mating for men makes one wonder whether the
dating service methodology of this study (or real dating services) pulled for shortterm mating psychology more than long-term mating strategies. Only a fraction
of participants (25%) rated themselves high in currently seeking a short-term mate
103
104
105
106
and greater number of sex partners (e.g., Singh, 1994). Self-rated desirability was
associated with number of past sex partners only for men and not for women. In
addition, WHR heightened the effect of MVD on anxiety in men but not women.
To speculate, other factors may govern whether or not and how frequently
individuals become involved in new mating relationships. For instance,
individuals with less desirable WHRs may find themselves seeking new mates
because they lose their mates more often whereas those with desirable WHRs may
find themselves in new relationships because more opportunities or offers by new
mates arise for them. If this were the case, the effect of WHR on dating and
sexual history could remain obscured without examining the reasons that a new
mate was found. Alternatively, self-rated desirability may developmentally
mediate the effect that WHR has on number of dating partners. Desirable WHR
may increase self-confidence, which is attractive to potential mates in its own
right (Buss, 1990), and may increase ones frequency of approaching mates or
ones receptivity to mating offers which then results in greater dating experience.
These possibilities remain speculative, however, until more research in this area
can be done. Nevertheless, this study indicated that WHR is significantly
associated with subjective perceptions of ones own desirability as a mate. In
order to expand on the present study, further research could also examine the
effect of WHR in the potential mates on dating anxiety in participants.
107
108
The present study did not address the issue of social incompetence
relevant to the Social Exclusion explanation of social anxiety (Baumeister & Tice,
1990), but has implications on the Social Exclusion Model that deserve comment.
As reviewed earlier, the SEM asserts that anxiety occurs after a social blunder has
been made that could lead to group exclusion. In the present study, individuals
reported (and demonstrated) anxiety before any blunder had been made. The
SEM gives no rationale to expect that individuals would be more likely to commit
a social blunder with highly attractive individuals. Yet, anxiety was greater for
imagined interactions with highly attractive individuals, suggesting that some
variable other than the likelihood of committing a serious social blunder was
driving anxiety. Perhaps, a serious social blunder is one that leads to the loss of
desirable mating opportunities, consistent with Busss (1990) argument? If so,
the SEM likely points in the correct general direction of the function of social
anxiety, but falls short by inadequate specificity and operationalization of its
concepts.
According to the Self-Handicapping Strategy, individuals should become
anxious when they feel a threat to an important dimension to their self-esteem
(Snyder, et al., 1985). It follows that an individual with lower self-esteem should
be more apt to feel threatened and, therefore, would feel socially anxious more
often or more intensely. The present data provide little support for this theory.
109
110
111
112
during mate initiation as well. For instance, the incidental finding that physical
attractiveness had a stronger influence on perceptions of social status than did
financial resources may be the case in young adults at the peak of their
reproductive value, whereas older adults may base their perceptions of social
status more heavily on characteristics linked to financial status. Whether older
age is associated with changes in how strongly men and women desire certain
traits in a mate (other than preferred age in a mate; Buunk, Dijkstra, Kenrick, &
Warntjes, 2001) have not been researched. However, it is reasonable to suspect
such differences, which could in turn influence which factors drive anxiety in
older adults when initiating mating.
113
likelihood of asking the prospective mate to meet, which may be evidence against
the hypothesis that mating anxiety will lead to avoidance. The data contained few
high scores for Likelihood of Rejection, which prevents this question from being
answered adequately without further study. Speculatively, a higher likelihood of
avoidance may be associated with anxiety only when the mating anxiety
mechanism is dysfunctional. Hence, the frequency of avoidance could be a key
discriminator between a functional and dysfunctional mating anxiety mechanism.
It is also possible that a critical threshold in anxiety exists beyond which
avoidance becomes the most likely behavioral output of the mechanism, but
below which a recruitment of resources (e.g., attentional, behavioral, effort) that
would boost the chances of success during the mating attempt is the most likely
output. Alternatively, the mating anxiety mechanisms solution to the costs of
rejection may most often be moderate anxiety which triggers increased interest
and effort, whereas avoidance is a low frequency output because the costs of not
trying to attain a mate are much higher than the costs of being rejected by one.
Further research is needed to address these questions by presenting participants
with a broader range of rejection likelihoods and by examining behavioral
responses during mating attempts that may indicate better preparation or
performance associated with various levels of anxiety. Other factors may also
influence the likelihood of rejection besides relative desirability or the
114
attractiveness and status of the potential mate. These might include signals of
availability, receptivity, or interest. Further research on these factors may help
elucidate the lesser importance of likelihood of rejection that was found in this
sample under these circumstances.
Similar research with older populations is necessary in order to determine
whether the present findings are unique to young adults at the peak of their
reproductive value. Do older men and women face different adaptive problems in
seeking a mate that the younger group studied here does not face? Might the
anxiety-provoking features of prospective mates carry different relative weights
for men and women seeking a mate at an older age? Besides loss of youth, older
adults are more likely to have children by a previous mate. How might this
influence mating anxiety? These questions, which have clinical implications as
well, can only be answered by further research using older populations. More
direct tests of the competing evolutionary accounts of social anxiety, suggested in
earlier sections, are also theoretically valuable avenues for further investigation.
Additional speculation is possible regarding other ancestrally-relevant
adaptive problems that mating anxiety may have been designed to solve. That is,
anxiety during mating initiation may address the costs associated with rejection
but may also have helped prevent or minimize other costs possibly associated
with approaching someone as a potential mate. For instance, the present study
115
found that more highly attractive potential mates aroused higher anxiety. Using
traditional societies today as an analog, ancestral females who were attractive
likely became mated at a young age. Therefore, the chances of an attractive
young woman being already mated was higher than for a less attractive young
woman. This could have meant that approaching a highly attractive woman
would risk retaliation from her high status or powerful mate. In the reverse,
women may have risked paying certain costs if they approached, or even were
approached by, an attractive potential mate. It has been shown that women place
more emphasis on physical attractiveness in a short-term mate (Buss, 1994).
Therefore, if a mated woman approached or was approached by an attractive man,
her mate (if aware of this interaction) might infer that she had short-term mating
interests in the competitor male and might inflict costs on her such as increased
mate-guarding, restriction of her behaviors, and even violence (Shackelford &
Buss, 2000). This adaptive problem for women could drive womens anxiety
during mating initiation, more so than mens.
116
117
118
Appendix B.
INSTRUCTIONS FOR WAIST AND HIP MEASUREMENTS
Take this sheet, a pencil, and a tape measure to the nearest restroom (the session facilitators will
show you where it is).
To Measure WAIST
To Measure HIPS
Note: Clothes add inaccuracy to the measurements, so be sure the tape measure is pressed
only against your skin, and not your clothing, while measuring.
WAIST MEASUREMENT:
Step 1: Stand facing the mirror and find the narrowest part of your torso (your waist).
Step 2: Place the tape around your waist, flat against your skin with centimeters showing.
Step 3: Take the measurement at the end of a normal exhale, with the tape held snugly against the
skin, but NOT pressed into your flesh.
Step 4: Take your waist measurement three times, for maximal accuracy. Each time, measure the
narrowest part of your waist. Record the measurements in centimenters (to the nearest 0.5 cm) here:
WAIST: (1) _______ cm
(2) ________ cm
(3) ________ cm
HIP MEASUREMENT:
Step 1: Face away from the mirror. Look over your shoulder and locate the area of maximum width
across your buttocks. Be sure your clothes are low enough to not interfere with the measurements.
Step 2: Place the tape measure snugly around your hips at their widest point, as in the diagram
do not press into your flesh or leave it loose.
Step 3: Take three measurements of your hips. Each time, measure the widest point of your
buttocks. Record the measurements in the centimeters (to the nearest 0.5 cm) here:
HIPS: (1) _______ cm
(2) ________ cm
120
(3) ________ cm
Appendix C.
121
Appendix D.
122
123
124
125
126
REFERENCES
Abed, R. T. (1998). The sexual competition hypothesis for eating disorders.
British Journal of Medical Psychology, 71, 525-547.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV). Washington DC: Author.
Baker, S. L., Heinrichs, N., Kim, H. J., & Hofmann, S. G. (2002). The Liebowitz
Social Anxiety Scale as a self-report instrument: a preliminary psychometric
analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 701-715.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
statistical considerations. Journal of Social and Personality Psychology, 51,
1173-1182.
Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1990). Anxiety and social exclusion. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 165-195.
Bekker, M. H. J. (1996). Agoraphobia and gender: A review. Clinical
Psychology Review, 16, 129-146.
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical lineal models:
Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W., & Congdon, R. T., Jr. (1996). HLM:
Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling with the HLM/2L and HLM/3L
Programs. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary
hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1-49.
Buss, D. M. (1990). The evolution of anxiety and social exclusion. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 196-201.
127
128
129
Glasgow, R., & Arkowitz, H. (1975). The behavioral assessment of male and
female social competence in dyadic heterosexual interactions. Behavior
Therapy, 6, 488-498.
Goodall, J. (1986). Social rejection, exclusion, and shunning among the Gombe
chimpanzees. Ethology and Sociobiology, 7, 227-236.
Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Shebilske, L. J., & Lundgren, S. R.
(1993). Social influence, sex differences, and judgments of beauty: Putting
the interpersonal back in interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality &
Social Psychology, 65, 522-531.
Harpending, H. C., & Sobus, J. (1987). Sociopathy as an adaptation. Ethology
and Sociobiology, 8 Suppl, 63-72.
Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new
perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 78, 81-91.
Heimberg, R. G., Horner, K. J., Juster, H. R., Safren, S. A., Brown, E. J.,
Schneier, F. R., & Liebowitz, M. R. (1999). Psychometric properties of the
Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. Psychological Medicine, 29, 199-212.
Hope, D. A., & Heimberg, R. G. (1990). Dating anxiety. in Harold Leitenberg
(Ed.) Handbook of Social and Evaluative Anxiety (pp. 217-246). New York:
Plenum Press.
Jaremko, M. E., Myers, E. J., Daner, S., Moore, S., & Allin, J. (1982).
Differences in daters: Effects of sex, dating frequency, and dating frequency
of partner. Behavioral Assessment, 4, 307-316.
Kenrick, D. T., & Sheets, V. (1993). Homicidal fantasies. Ethology &
Sociobiology, 14, 231-246.
Kleinknecht, R. A. (1988). Specificity and psychosocial correlates of
blood/injury fear and fainting. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 26, 303-309.
130
131
Ohman, A. (1986). Face the beast and fear the face: Animal and social fears as
prototypes for evolutionary analyses of emotion. Psychophysiology, 23, 123145.
Ohman, A. (1987). Evolution, learning, and phobias: An interactional analysis.
in D. Magnusson & A. Ohman (Eds.) Psychopathology: An interactional
perspective (pp. 143-158). Orlando: Academic Press.
Paulhus, D. L. (1986). Self-deception and impression management in test
responses. In A. Angleitner & J. S. Wiggins (Eds.), Personality assessment
via questionnaire (pp.142-165). New York: Springer.
Paulhus, D. L. (1991). Enhancement and denial in socially desirable responding.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 307-317.
Paulhus, D. L. (1994). Balanced inventory of desirable responding: Reference
manual for BIDR Version 6. Unpublished manuscript, University of British
Columbia.
Pilkonis, P. A. (1977). The behavioral consequences of shyness. Journal of
Personality, 45, 596-611.
Price, J. (1998). The adaptive function of mood change. British Journal of
Medical Psychology, 71, 465-477.
Profet, M. (1987). The evolution of pregnancy sickness as protection to the
embryo against Pleistocene teratogens. Evolutionary Theory, 8, 177-190.
Profet, M. (1991). The function of allergy: Immunological defense against
toxins. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 66, 23-62.
Rosemberg, F. K., Garcia. J. H., & Perez S., L. (1998). Staying well in an ailing
society. The Behavior Therapist, 21, 174-175.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
133
134
Smith, R. E., & Sarason, I. G. (1975). Social anxiety and the evaluation of
negative interpersonal feedback. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 43, 429.
Smith, T. W., Snyder, C. R., & Handelsman, M. (1982). On the self-serving
function on an academic wooden leg: Test anxiety as a self-handicapping
strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 314-321.
Snyder, C. R., & Smith, T. W. (1982). Symptoms as self-handicapping
strategies: The virtues of old wine in a new bottle. In G. Weary & H. L.
Mirels (Eds.), Integrations of clinical and social psychology (pp. 104-127).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Snyder, C. R, Smith, T. W., Augelli, R. W., & Ingram, R. E. (1985). On the selfserving function of social anxiety: Shyness as a self-handicapping strategy.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 970-980.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
Press.
Stroebe, W. (1977). Self-esteem and interpersonal attraction. In S. W. Duck
(Ed.), Theory and practice in interpersonal attraction (pp.79-104). London:
Academic Press.
Symons, D. (1992). On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human
behavior. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.) The adapted
mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 137-159).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Thornton, B., & Moore, S. (1993). Physical attractiveness contrast effect:
Implications for self-esteem and evaluations of the social self. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 474-480.
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional
adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and
Sociobiology, 11, 375-424.
135
VITA
Permanent address:
137