Personality A Topical Approach

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Personality: A Topical Approach

Theories, Research, Major Controversies and EmergingFindings


Robert B. Owen
LAWRENCE ERLBAUM ASSOCIATES, PUBLISHERS 1998 Mahwah, New Jersey London
-iiiCopyright 1998 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in
any form, by photostat, microform, retrieval system, or any other
means, without the prior written permission of the publisher.
Lawrence Erbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers
10 Industrial Avenue
Mahwah, New Jersey 07430
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication-Data
Ewen, Robert B., 1940Personality: a topical approach: theories, research, major
controversies, and emerging findings / Robert B. Ewen.
p. cm
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 0-8058-2098-1 (alk. paper)
1. Personality. I. Title
BF698.E88 1998 155.2--DC21
96-53172 CIP
Books published by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates are printed on
acid-free paper, and their bindings are chosen for strength and durability.
Printed in the United States of America
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
-ivContents
Prefaceix
1 Introduction1
2 Freud's Psychoanalytic Theory33
3 Psychoanalytic Theory: Controversies and Emerging Findings62
4 Allport's Trait Theory109
5 Trait Theory: Controversies and Emerging Findings126
6 Cognitive Theory149
7 Rogers' Humanistic/Self Theory174
8 Humanistic/Self Theory: Controversies and Emerging Findings189
9 Skinner's Radical Behaviorism209
10 Behaviorism: Controversies and Emerging Findings230
11 Research Methods251
12 Conclusion275
Glossary285
References303
Index325
-viiPreface
This book is an alternative to, not a replacement for, my textbook on theories of personality (Ewen, 1993). An Introduction to Theories of P
ersonality is still in print, and will remainavailable so long as there are those who wish to use it. The theorist-by-theorist approach allowsth
e student to examine the mysteries of human behavior through the eyes of Freud, Jung, Adler,Horney, and so on, and to obtain a thorough
understanding of each theory. But the topicalapproach also has advantages, as I have learned by writing this book. I have discussed thead
vantages of each approach in the section entitled "Two Approaches to the Study of Personality"in chapter 1.
Because there is a great deal of overlap between these two approaches, I do not expect anystudent to read both books and take both cour
ses. Therefore, I have incorporated a considerableamount of material from An Introduction to Theories of Personality into the present text.
However, there are also substantial differences. The present text devotes considerably moreattention to psychological research, and consi
derably less attention to the more minor andabstruse aspects of various theories. Only four chapters are devoted to individual theories.Psy
choanalytically oriented theory is introduced by a chapter on Freud, trait theory by a chapteron Allport, cognitive theory by a subchapter o
n Kelly, humanistic/self theory by a chapter onRogers, and behaviorism by a chapter on Skinner. Each of these is followed by a chapter (or
subchapter) that deals with relevant research and the work of related theorists (including Jung,Adler, Horney, Fromm, Sullivan, Erikson, Mu
rray, Cattell, Eysenck, Maslow, and Bandura), all ofwhich is organized by topic.
The present text also includes a chapter on research methods. I have placed this chapter at theend because I believe that students will be
tter appreciate this material after they have seen howvarious theories are affected by methodological issues, and because I prefer to begi
n this courseby discussing personality issues rather than numerical issues. However, instructors who disagreemay follow chapter 1 with ch
apter 11 with little or no loss in continuity. As befits a first edition, Ityped every word of the present text on my personal computer, includin
g those sections adoptedfrom An Introduction to Theories of Personality.
Chapters 2 through 11 conclude with a set of study questions designed to encourage criticalthinking. To help achieve this goal (and to stim
ulate discussion), I have included some questionsthat go beyond the chapter material, some that have no clear-cut right or wrong answers
, andsome where my personal viewpoint might be challenged by other psychologists. I suggest thatthese study questions be regarded as
an integral part of each chapter.

I would like to thank Larry Erlbaum for the opportunity to write this book. Larry has been therefor me on several important occasions, and I
am indebted to him for his encouragement andsupport.
-ix1
INTRODUCTION
This book is about one of the most fascinating of all subjects: the human personality. Thetheorists and researchers whose views we will ex
amine often disagree with one another, so anyreader who is seeking a field with clear-cut answers will be disappointed. But if you are intri
guedby the challenge of trying to understand human nature (including your own behavior), and bycomparing and evaluating different and
thought- provoking ideas, you should find the study ofpersonality to be highly rewarding.
THE MEANING OF PERSONALITY
Most of us have an intuitive understanding as to the meaning of personality. (Note: terms inboldface are defined in the Glossary at the
end of this book.) Although there is as yet no oneuniversally accepted definition of personality, most psychologists do agree on certain ge
neralconsiderations.
First of all, personality refers to important and relatively stable aspects of a person's behavior.For example, consider a young woman whos
e personality includes the trait called "painfully shy."She will behave shyly in many different situations, and over a significant period of tim
e. Ofcourse, there may well be exceptions. She may be more outgoing with her family or a close friend,or at her own birthday party. But, m
ost often, her behavior will be consistent: She will havedifficulty dealing with other people, which will continue for months or even years an
d will have asignificant effect on her general well-being.
Most psychologists also define personality as originating within the individual. Gordon Allportput it this way: "Of course the impression we
make on others, and their response to us areimportant factors in the development of our personalities.... [But] what about the solitaryher
mit... or Robinson Crusoe before the advent of his man Friday? Do these isolates lackpersonality because they have no effect on others? [
My] view is that such exceptional creatureshave personal qualities that are no less fascinating than those of men living in human society...
[and that] we must have something inside our skins that constitutes our 'true nature'" ( 1961, p.24).
Personality deals with a wide range of human behavior. Virtually everything about a person--mental, emotional, social, and physical--is incl
uded. Some aspects of personality areunobservable, such as thoughts, memories, and dreams; while others are observable, as is thecase
with overt actions. Personality also includes aspects that are concealed from yourself, orunconscious, as well as those that are conscious a
nd well within your awareness.
-1If we try to summarize these ideas in a single definition, we get the following: Personality refersto important, relatively stable characteristi
cs within the individual that account for consistentpatterns of behavior Aspects of personality may be observable or unobservable, and co
nsciousor unconscious. This is by no means the only acceptable definition of personality; it does reflectthe approach and orientation of the
present text.
THEORIES AND THEORISTS
With regard to the term theory, there is less controversy and greater agreement. A theory is anunproved speculation about reality, one n
ot known to be either true or false. Established factsare often lacking in scientific work, and a theory offers guidelines that will serve us in t
he absenceof more precise information.A theory consists of a set of terms and principles constructed orapplied by the theorist, which are r
eferred to as constructs. Like the author or inventor, thetheorist is a creator (of constructs). And like creators in other disciplines, the the
orist borrowsfrom and builds on the work of his or her predecessors. Freud's well-known constructs of id, ego,and superego are not actual
entities that exist somewhere within your mind; they are termswhich he devised to help explain how our personalities operate. Nor was he
the first to use theterm "id," which was coined by the philosopher Friedrich Nietzsche.It should now be apparentwhy there are often seriou
s disagreements among personality theorists. These theorists sought toexplore a relatively new and unknown world, the human psyche. To
explain their discoveries,therefore, they were forced to develop their own terms and concepts (constructs). And becausethey deal with are
as where the facts are unknown, their theories often differ significantly. Withthis in mind, let us survey some of the important theorists wh
ose work we will examine.
Sigmund Freud: Psychoanalysis
Throughout the course of history, scientists have dealt three great shocks to our feelings of self-importance. Nicolaus Copernicus demonstr
ated that the Earth is not the center of all creation,but merely one of several planets that rotate around the sun. Charles Darwin showed th
athumans are not a unique and privileged life form, but just one of many animal species that haveevolved over millions of years. Sigmund
Freud emphasized that we are not even the masters ofour own minds, but are driven by many powerful unconscious processes (wishes, fe
ars, beliefs,conflicts, emotions, memories) of which we are totally unaware (see Freud, 1917a; 1916- 1917/1966, pp. 284-285).In Freudian
theory (psychoanalysis, which is also the name of the form ofpsychotherapy that he developed), you will find many constructs that expr
ess his belief thatmuch of personality is unconscious and cannot be called to consciousness on demand. You willalso find:
An extremely pessimistic view of human nature that attributes all of our behavior to twoinnate instincts, sexual and destructive, which in
clude the desire for incest and the lust forkilling.
The belief that nothing in the human psyche happens by chance; all mental behavior isdetermined by prior causes.
Constructs designed to explain how personality can become a house divided against itself,including the id, ego, and superego.
Constructs designed to explain how we hide the truth about ourselves from ourselves,including various defense mechanisms.
-2The construct of anxiety, an important and highly unpleasant emotion that is similar tointense nervousness.
The belief that your personality is determined primarily during infancy and early childhood.
An explanation of personality development in terms of psychosexual stages, including theoccurrence of the all-important Oedipus compl
ex.
Various procedures designed to bring unconscious material to consciousness, including freeassociation, dream interpretation, and the an
alysis of resistance and transference.
Biographical Sketch. Sigmund Freud was born on May 6, 1856, at Freiberg, Moravia (nowCzechoslovakia). His parents were Jewish and hi
s father worked as a wool merchant. Freud spentnearly all of his life in Vienna, where his family moved in 1860.
Freud was an excellent student throughout his academic career, receiving his medical degreefrom the University of Vienna in 1881. He wa
s not overly enthusiastic about becoming apracticing physician, a slow route to economic security in those days, and longed for the brillian
tdiscovery that would bring him rapid fame. In 1885 he won a travel grant to study in Paris withJean-Martin Charcot, a noted hypnotist. Ch
arcot was experimenting with the use of hypnosis tocure illnesses that we now regard as psychological, but which he (and his contemporar
ies)believed to be physiological. Intrigued by Charcot's work, Freud returned to Vienna and becamethe colleague of a prominent physician,
Josef Breuer, who was using hypnosis to treat patientswith hysterical behavior disorders (paralysis of a leg or arm, loss of speech, and othe
r symptomsthat had no physical cause).
During 1880-1882, Breuer treated the 21-year-old hysterical patient known as "Anna O." Shesuffered from a veritable museum of sympto
ms: paralyzed limbs, hallucinations, nervouscoughing, sleepwalking, various speech disorders, and even a second personality that livedex
actly one year in the past. Breuer discovered a most unusual way to alleviate these formidabledifficulties. He hypnotized Anna O., and had
her relive each previous occurrence of a symptom inreverse chronological order! This procedure enabled her to release powerful emotions
that shehad been afraid to express at the time (the process of catharsis). Unfortunately, Breuer'ssympathetic care aroused such powerful l
ove from his attractive patient that he became upset,his wife became even more upset, and he dropped the case with considerable embar
rassment.But he had shown that unconscious forces within the mind could cause psychological illness, andcould be brought to light with w
ords and ideas alone (see Ellenberger, 1970, pp. 480-484; Freud& Breuer, 1895/ 1966, pp. 55-82; E. Jones, 1953/ 1963, pp. 142ff).
Freud was most impressed by this demonstration. He worked with Breuer for a decade, and theyco-authored Studies on Hysteria in 1895.
However, Freud eventually found that the hypnoticmethod left much to be desired. Cures were likely to be only temporary, with the patien

tbecoming dependent on the therapist and suffering a relapse as soon as treatment wasdiscontinued. The cathartic removal of a symptom
left the underlying causes unresolved, free tocreate new difficulties. Thus hypnotic therapy acted more like a cosmetic cover-up thansucce
ssful surgery. And some of Freud's patients were unable to go into a trance, partly becausehe wasn't a particularly good hypnotist ( Freud,
1916- 1917/ 1966, pp. 450-451; Freud & Breuer,1895/ 1966, pp. 145ff). For these reasons, Freud abandoned hypnosis (and catharsis) andg
radually developed the form of psychotherapy that has become known as psychoanalysis.
Freud's professional life had many interesting highlights, and also a few major blunders. In 1884,a friend of his suffered an extremely painf
ul illness and became
-3addicted to the morphine that he took as medication. Freud recommended a "harmless"substitute--cocaine--and even published an article
praising the new drug. Unfortunately, cocainealso proved to be highly addictive, and Freud was justifiably criticized. In 1896, Freud announ
cedthat most of his psychoanalytic patients were ill because they had been seduced by immoraladults during childhood. A few years later
he concluded to his chagrin that these incidents wereactually imaginary, and that the unconscious cannot distinguish between memory an
d fantasy.(Interestingly, the current concern with child abuse suggests that Freud's original theory may nothave been as incorrect as he th
ought. Incest may well be more prevalent than is generallybelieved, but is not publicized because of understandable feelings of shame an
d guilt.)
Freud published The Interpretation of Dreams, the cornerstone of his theory, in 1900. Fame wasfar from instant, and this classic took 8 yea
rs to sell all of 600 copies. By now Freud hadcompleted his break with standard medicine, however, and was more self-assured as the lead
erof an established movement. Some of his more controversial theories drew scathing criticism,and he clearly identified with the role of th
e lonely hero struggling against insuperable odds.Nevertheless, the belief that he was ostracized by Vienna for his beliefs is one of the unf
oundedlegends that surround his life. Rather, his position and fame steadily improved (see Ellenberger,1970, p. 450; Freud, 1925/ 1963a,
pp. 44, 91; E. Jones, 1955/ 1963b, pp. 237, 291).
Freud's own life provided him with a great deal of psychological data. He was himself Oedipal,had powerful unconscious hostility toward hi
s father, and was quite close to his mother (who wassome 19 years younger than her husband, and devoted to her "golden Sigi"). Freud be
gan his self-analysis in 1897, probing the inner depths of his own mind with the psychological techniquesthat he developed. He continued
to do so for the rest of his life, reserving the last half hour ofeach day for this purpose.
Personally, Freud was highly moral and ethical. Some found him to be cold, bitter, rejecting, thekind of man who does not suffer fools gladl
y, and more interested in the discoveries to be madefrom his patients than in themselves. Others depicted him as warm, humorous, profo
undlyunderstanding, and extremely kind (e.g., see Ellenberger, 1970, pp. 457-469; E. Jones, 1953/1963; Reik, 1948/ 1964, p. 258; Rieff, 19
59/ 1961; Roazen, 1975/ 1976b; and Schur, 1972). Somecolleagues remained devoted to Freud throughout their lives, while others (includi
ng Breuer,Carl Jung, and Alfred Adler) engaged in acrimonious partings because of their theoreticaldifferences.
Freud married Martha Bernays on September 30, 1886. His letters to his betrothed show him tohave been an ardent and devoted lover, an
d the marriage was for some time a happy one. TheFreuds had six children, three boys and three girls, with the youngest (Anna) becoming
aprominent child psychoanalyst and ultimately assuming the leadership of the Freudianmovement. Although Freud usually declined to pra
ctice his psychological ideas on his ownfamily, he did create a rather bizarre Oedipal situation by psychoanalyzing Anna himself. Nodoubt
due in part to this unusual emotional involvement with her father, she never married,devoted her life to the cause of psychoanalysis, and
eventually replaced Martha as the mostimportant woman in Sigmund's life ( Roazen, 1975/ 1976b, pp. 58-59, 63, 439-440).
In 1909, Freud visited the United States and delivered a series of lectures at Clark University.They were well received, but he left with the i
mpression that "America is a mistake; a giganticmistake, it is true, but none the less a mistake" ( E. Jones, 1955/ 1963, p. 263). World War
Iimpressed on him the importance of aggression as a basic human drive, and the ensuing runawayinflation cost him his life savings (about
$30,000). Fortunately his reputation enabled him toattract English and American patients, who paid in a more stable currency. But his hard
shipswere not yet over.
-4During the last 16 years of his life, Freud was afflicted with an extremely serious cancer of themouth and jaw. This required no fewer than t
hirty-three operations, forced him to wear anawkward prosthesis to fill the resulting gap between what had been the nasal and oral cavitie
s,and prevented him at times from speaking and swallowing, yet he bore this ordeal with hiscustomary stoic courage. Nor did he curtail his
prolific and literate writings, which fill sometwenty-three volumes.
Still one more trial was in store: the Nazi invasion of Vienna in 1938, during which Anna wasdetained by the Gestapo but eventually releas
ed. Freud and his family successfully escaped toLondon, where he was received with great honor. There he finally succumbed to the cance
r onSeptember 23, 1939.
Neo-Freudian Theories
Several prominent personality theorists started out as Freudians. Eventually, however, theirefforts to understand the human personality le
d them to develop constructs that differedsignificantly from Freud's. When Freud refused to accept their proposed changes, their onlyrecou
rse was to abandon psychoanalysis and develop their own theories of personality.
To these theorists, and to serious students of personality theory, the differences between neo-Freudian constructs and Freudian psychoana
lysis are substantial. However, many modernpsychologists disagree. They prefer to focus on the more observable and conscious aspects o
fpersonality, which are much easier to subject to the rigors of experimental research. And so theyregard the theories of Freud and his dissi
dents as fairly similar because all of these theoriesstress the importance of unconscious processes.
A second issue involving neo-Freudian theories has to do with terminology. In order to conveytheir ideas most effectively, and to emphasiz
e their differences with Freud, the neo-Freudiansdevised many new constructs--so many, in fact, that studying each theory is like trying to
learn anew language. Partly for this reason, neo-Freudian theories have had less influence on modernpsychology than has Freudian psycho
analysis. Only certain parts of each theory have enjoyed theprominence accorded to Freud's work.
Therefore, texts such as this (which focus on important topics in the area of personality) typicallydevote less attention to the neoFreudians. The reader who wishes a more detailed discussion ofCarl Jung, Alfred Adler, Karen Horney, Erich Fromm, Harry Stack Sullivan, Er
ik Erikson, andHenry Murray will find it in Ewen ( 1993).
Carl Jung. Carl Jung's quest for information about the human psyche led him to sources thatmany would regard as farfetched: the occult,
studies of extrasensory perception, alchemy, themyth of flying saucers. The result is a theory of personality (called analytical psycholog
y) thatis even more controversial than Freud's, and is easy to dismiss as absurd and unscientific. YetJung regarded himself as an empirical

researcher, possessed a fine mind, read voraciously andacquired an immense store of knowledge, traveled widely in order to study various
races andclasses, and was an esteemed psychotherapist. And some of his ideas have become part of oureveryday language.
Like Freud, Jung regarded unconscious processes as extremely important. Yet he placedconsiderably less emphasis on sexuality as a huma
n motive and posited a more extensive list ofinstincts, including nutrition, power, creativity, and individuation (becoming one's true self, th
eforerunner of the modern concept of self-actualization). Jung argued that we are influenced notonly by prior causes, but also by our goals
and plans for the future. He dispensed with theFreudian constructs ofid, ego, and superego in favor of archetypes, the persona, the shado
w, andothers. He contended that there is
-5a collective unconscious, a storehouse of predispositions to perceive the world in particular waysthat is inherited from previous generation
s, as well as a personal unconscious. Jung introducedthe well-known dimension of introversion-extraversion. And although he agreed that
dreaminterpretation was an excellent way to bring unconscious material to consciousness, hisprinciples and differed markedly from those
of Freud.
Carl Gustav Jung was born on July 26, 1875 in Kesswil, a small village in Switzerland. He was anintroverted and lonely child, deeply preocc
upied with his inner psychic world. From an early agehe experienced visions of the supernatural, such as a faintly luminous figure with a d
etachedhead that appeared to emanate from his mother's bedroom. He soon came to regard himself as "asolitary because I know things a
nd must hint at things which other people do not know, andusually do not even want to know....Loneliness does not come from having no
people about one,but from being unable to communicate the things that seem important to oneself, or fromholding certain views which ot
hers find inadmissible" ( Jung, 1961/ 1965, pp. 18, 42, 356).
Jung became attracted to the fledgling field of psychiatry during his medical studies at theUniversity of Basel, where he received his degre
e in 1900. He became absorbed with the occult,participated in experiments with mediums, and devoured books on parapsychology. In addi
tionto his visions, various experiences seemed to confirm the existence of the supernatural. He madeup a supposedly imaginary story to e
ntertain a group, only to find that he was clairvoyantlyrevealing true and intimate secrets about a man he did not know. And the morning a
fter beingawakened by an extremely sharp headache, he discovered that one of his patients had that nightshot himself in the back of the
head ( Jung, 1961/ 1965, pp. 51, 137-138).
Whereas Freud preferred to limit his practice to neurotics, Jung successfully treatedschizophrenic patients at the famed Burgholzli Psychiat
ric Hospital in Zurich. He remained thereuntil 1909, when he left to concentrate on his growing private practice. In 1903 he marriedEmma
Rauschenbach, who also became his collaborator. The marriage was basically successful,if at times troubled, with the Jungs having four da
ughters and a son (see Stern, 1976/ 1977).
Jung had read The Interpretation of Dreams on its publication in 1900, and began what provedto be a lengthy correspondence with Freud i
n 1906. The two men met a year later, and were socaptivated with each other that they talked continuously for 13 hours! Unfortunately, af
undamental misconception was to destroy their relationship. Freud was seeking disciples whowould carry forth the psychoanalytic banner,
and saw Jung as his crown prince and successor.Jung, on the other hand, regarded his association with Freud as a collaboration that left bo
thmen free to pursue their own theoretical inclinations. So it was inevitable that Jung would viewFreud's steadfast adherence to psychoana
lytic constructs as dogmatism, whereas Freud wouldsee Jung's attempts to develop his own theory as a betrayal.
For some years, Jung supported and defended Freud's ideas. Jung became a psychoanalyst,taught this subject at the University of Zurich,
and served as the first president of theInternational Psychoanalytic Association. But Jung had to be his own man. His analysis of thedelusio
ns and hallucinations of psychotic patients at the Burgholzli had persuaded him of thefrequent occurrence of universal archetypes. When J
ung continued to argue for his own theory,the breach with Freud became irreparable--a trying experience that occasioned two faintingspell
s on Freud's part, and more than a little anguish on Jung's.
The formal parting came in 1913, triggered by a mishap in their correspondence. Jung made a"Freudian slip" in one of his letters, Freud cal
led attention to it in his reply, and
-7Jung responded angrily: "Your technique of treating pupils like patients is a blunder. In this wayyou produce either slavish sons or impudent
puppies...I am objective enough to see through yourlittle trick. You go around sniffing out all the symptomatic actions in your vicinity, thus
reducingeveryone to the level of sons and daughters who blushingly admit the existence of their faults.Meanwhile you remain on top as th
e father, sitting pretty....You see, my dear Professor, so longas you hand out this stuff I don't give a damn for my symptomatic actions... I s
hall continue tostand by you publicly while maintaining my own views, but privately shall start telling you in myletters what I really think o
f you" ( McGuire, 1974, pp. 534-535).
Jung resigned from the International Psychoanalytic Association in 1914 and turned to thesolitude of his home, a large and beautiful edific
e of his own design in Kusnacht (a suburb ofZurich). Here he spent the years from 1913 to 1919 in relative isolation, probing the depths of
hisown unconscious. He conversed with voices from within his psyche, including a female that heinterpreted as his anima and a group of g
hosts that he believed to be souls returning from thedead ( Jung, 1961/ 1965, pp. 170-199). To avoid succumbing to psychosis, Jung forced
himself toretain close ties with his family and patients and scrupulously fulfilled his commitments to theexternal world.
By now Jung was widely admired as an unusually skilled psychotherapist, attracting patientsfrom England and the United States. Despite h
is interest in the occult, he impressed people as apractical man with a firm grip on reality. He was active, vigorous, and jovial, over 6 feet t
all andbroad-shouldered, interested in sailing and mountain climbing as well as scholarly pursuits, agood listener, and a fine conversationa
list. Like Freud, however, Jung's personality wascomplicated and multifaceted. Some saw him as wise, sensitive, and caring. Others viewed
himas cantankerous, womanizing, sarcastic, and highly critical and condescending toward others--especially those who failed to meet his h
igh standards of scholarship (see Brome, 1978; Stern,1976/ 1977, pp. 181-182).

Jung traveled extensively and observed a variety of people and cultures, including the PuebloIndians of New Mexico and tribes in Tunis, Ke
nya, Uganda, and India. In 1944 Jung nearly diedof a heart attack, had a vision of his soul leaving his body, and at first felt bitter disappoin
tmenton returning to life. He also predicted that his doctor would die in his place, which actuallyhappened shortly thereafter.
Jung now became the "wise old man of Kusnacht," with people coming from all over the world tovisit him. His prolific writings fill some twe
nty volumes. Jung died in his Kusnacht home on June6, 1961.
Alfred Adler. Scientific inquiry is supposed to be rational and objective, yet there are timeswhen it resembles a bitter family feud. One su
ch monumental uproar occurred in 1911, when itbecame apparent that the theories of Freud's colleague Alfred Adler were quite different f
romthose of psychoanalysis . An irate Freud "forced the whole Adler gang" to resign frompsychoanalytic circles, and forbade his followers t
o attend any of Adler's conferences.Friendships broke up, wives of the combatants stopped speaking to each other, and members ofopposi
ng factions refused to sit near each other at dinner parties. Psychoanalysts charged Adlerwith plagiarism, and were accused in turn of retai
ning his ideas while expunging his name fromtheir writings ( Ellenberger, 1970, pp. 638-645; McGuire, 1974, p. 447; Roazen, 1975/ 1976b,
pp.184-193).
Although Freud's attacks were excessive, it would seem that he better understood the way tolasting fame. Today Freud is clearly recognize
d as the originator of psychoanalysis; while manyof Adler's significant ideas have been subsumed, without credit, into the theories of other
psychologists.
-82
Freud's Psychoanalytic Theory
Early in Freud's career, three men whom he admired gave him some startling information. JosefBreuer remarked that neurotic behaviors w
ere always concerned with secrets of the marital bed.Jean-Martin Charcot emphatically proclaimed to an assistant that certain nervous dis
orders were"always a question of the genitals," a conversation Freud overheard. And the distinguishedgynecologist Rudolf Chrobak advise
d Freud that the only cure for a female patient with severeanxiety and an impotent husband could not be prescribed: "A normal penis, dos
e to be repeated"( Freud, 1914/ 1967, pp. 13-15; E. Jones, 1953/ 1963, p. 158). Although Freud was somewhatshocked by these radical not
ions and dismissed them from his mind, they later emerged from hispreconscious to form the cornerstone of his theory--one that attribute
s virtually all humanbehavior to the erotic instinct.
HUMAN NATURE AND MOTIVATION
Freud named his theory psycho-analysis. (Most modern writers omit the hyphen.) This term isalso widely used to denote the form of psych
otherapy that Freud originated, but the clinicalpractice of psychoanalysis is only one of its numerous applications.
Instincts and Psychic Energy
Freud's theory stresses the biological causes of human behavior. He concluded that we aremotivated by powerful innate forces, to which h
e gave the name Triebe (instincts, or drives).These instincts become activated when some aspect of the body requires sustenance, as w
hen youneed food, water, or sexual consummation. The activated instinct (need) then produces apsychological state of increased tension (
wish), which you experience as unpleasant.
According to Freud, the basic objective of all human behavior is to achieve pleasure and avoidunpleasure or pain (the pleasure principle,
to be discussed in more detail later in thischapter). So you take action designed to reduce the unpleasant mental tension, which in turnsati
sfies the underlying instinctual need (drive). Thus the primary goal of human pleasure isrealized by means of drive reduction, and the goal
of the instincts is to restore the body to aprevious state of equilibrium. (See Freud, 1911/ 1963c, p. 2, 1916- 1917/ 1966, p. 356; 1926/196
9b, pp. 25-26.) Freud does concede that drive increases may sometimes be pleasurable, as inthe case of excitement during sexual interco
urse, but he regards this as an awkwardcontradiction that cannot readily be reconciled with his theory ( 1924/ 1963h, p. 191).
-333
Psychoanalytic Theory: Controversies andEmerging Finding
Freud had fond hopes of leaving a permanent legacy to humanity: a set of constructs so completeas to need only minor tinkering. But this
was not to be. Numerous creative and insightfulpsychologists have argued that psychoanalytic theory requires major revisions, while other
s haveabandoned Freud's ideas in favor of their own theories.
Challenges to psychoanalytic theory have come from three primary sources: the neo-Freudians,personality theorists of very different persu
asions, and empirical researchers. We will considereach of these sources in the following pages.
HUMAN NATURE AND MOTIVATION
Is Human Nature Malignant or Benign?
According to Freud, we must reluctantly sublimate our innate incestuous and murderous wishesinto less satisfying but more socially accept
able outlets. This leads to conflicts with society andalso to intrapsychic conflicts, as the superego tries to prevent the ego from yielding to i
llicit idimpulses.
Freud's pessimistic conception of human nature is extremely controversial. Virtually all othertheorists have taken a more optimistic view, a
lbeit in varying degrees.
Moderate Approaches
Carl Jung agreed that every personality has its dark side. The shadow includes not only minorweaknesses but also a "positively demonic d
ynamism," from which we turn away in fear ( Jung,1917/ 1972d, p. 30). To Jung, however, this was only part of the story. Among our innate
instinctsare wellsprings of creativity, which can suggest solutions to our problems when the consciousmind becomes bogged down; nutriti
on (hunger and thirst); sexuality; power; activity, whichincludes the love of change, the urge to travel, and play; and becoming one's true
self(individuation) (see Jung, 1917/ 1972d, 1937). Jung also differed sharply with Freud byconcluding that we have an inborn moral natur
e, and an innate religious need:
Man positively needs ideas and convictions that will give meaning to his life and enablehim to find a place for himself in the universe. He c
an stand the most
-624
Allport's Trait Theory
Freud perceived himself as an unraveler of great mysteries. His goal was to guide us through animmense, extremely important, and largel
y unexplored realm: the unconscious aspects of thehuman mind.
Other theorists have followed a markedly different approach. For 30 years, two psychologistswere colleagues at Harvard University. Henry
Murray shared Freud's and Jung's appreciation forthe "wonder world" of the unconscious, and his research probed deeply into our hidden t

houghts,beliefs, and feelings. But Gordon Allport preferred to focus on the surface aspects of personality."If you want to know something a
bout a person," Allport said, "why not first ask him?"
HUMAN NATURE AND MOTIVATION
Instinctual Drives and Needs
Allport agreed that we strive to reduce our innate drives, including hunger, thirst, sex,elimination, the need for oxygen, and the need for sl
eep. "All human beings in all the world dohave drives.... If someone is very hungry, very much in need of oxygen, water, or rest, all otherm
otives fade away until the drive is satisfied" ( Allport, 1961, p. 205).
Instinctual drives are active to some degree throughout our lives and completely dominate themotivational scene of the very young child,
whom Allport regarded as an "unsocialized horror"--excessively demanding, pleasure-seeking, impatient, destructive, and lacking a consci
ence. LikeFreud and Fromm, therefore, Allport argued that we must learn to overcome our inherentnarcissism. "Self-love, it is obvious, rem
ains always active in our natures. [My] theory holds onlythat it need not remain dominant" ( Allport, 1955, pp. 28, 30, 1961, p. 196).
Murray shared Allport's views about our biological drives, though he preferred to use the termneed ( Murray, 1951, p. 267; Murray et al., 1
938; Murray & Kluckhohn, 1953, p. 39). But Murraydid not accept Freud's contention that our goal is to reach some homeostatic end state
where nodrives are active. Instead, Murray argued that we are motivated to achieve the pleasure thataccompanies the reduction of our ne
eds. Thus we readily learn to postpone eating or sex in orderto develop greater levels of tension, and make the subsequent drive reduction
more pleasurable.Murray also posited some twenty psychological needs that result from our biological needs (seeTable 3.1).
-1095
Trait Theory: Controversies and EmergingFindings
Even the most dedicated trait theorist would agree that Allport's list of 4,000-5,000 traits isunmanageable. It seems reasonable to conclud
e that human nature cannot be this diverse, andthat there must be a much smaller number of traits that represent the core of personality.
Some theorists and researchers have focused their attention on selected traits that they regard asimportant. Others have used complicate
d mathematical procedures to try and reduce Allport'slist to a few fundamental traits. We will consider each of these approaches in the pa
ges thatfollow.
SELECTED TRAITS
Introversion-Extraversion
Jung's Theory. Carl Jung attributed individual differences in personality to two processes: thetypical way in which we apprehend internal
and external stimuli, and the characteristic direction(inward or outward) of libido movement.
There are four ways to apprehend stimuli (functions): merely establishing what is there(sensation), interpreting and understanding the me
aning of what you perceive (thinking),evaluating how desirable or pleasant it is (feeling), and forming apparently inexplicable hunchesor c
onclusions without using any of the other functions (intuition). "Sensation tells you thatsomething exists; thinking tells you what it is; feeli
ng tells you whether it is agreeable or not; andintuition tells you whence it comes and where it is going" ( Jung, 1964/ 1968, p. 49). Thinkin
gand feeling are opposites, and are called rational functions because they involve acts of cognitionand judgment. Sensation and intuition a
re also opposites, and these more reflexive functions arereferred to as irrational (meaning nonrational, not pathological). Although everyon
e has theability to use all four functions, there is an inborn tendency for one of them to become dominantover the others.
There are also two directions of libido movement (attitudes). The outward turning of libidotoward the external world is known as extraver
sion, while the flow of libido toward the depthsof one's own psyche is referred to as introversion. Extraverts are
-1266
Cognitive Theory
Human behavior is strongly influenced by what we believe to be true. Prejudiced individuals whothink that other groups (Jews, Blacks, the
United States government) are evil and subhumanhave committed horrendous acts of brutality and terrorism. A student who regards a the
ory ofpersonality as an exciting new approach is likely to learn more than a student who views it ashopelessly confused jargon. If you dete
ct a small lump on your chest, your behavior will be verydifferent depending on whether you interpret this symptom as cancer or as only a
wart.
Shakespeare's Hamlet put it this way, "There is nothing either good or bad but thinking makes itso." Some psychologists have based their
theories on precisely this point of view: they definepersonality primarily, or even entirely, in terms of cognitions.
KELLY'S PSYCHOLOGY OF PERSONAL CONSTRUCTS
Human Nature and Motivation
George Kelly's cognitive theory of personality has many unusual aspects. He preferred to leavevirtually all familiar landmarks behind, inclu
ding even the fundamental concept of motivation:
[In our theory,] the term learning... scarcely appears at all. That is wholly intentional;we are for throwing it overboard altogether. There is
no ego, no emotion, nomotivation, no reinforcement, no drive, no unconscious, no need.... [Thus] the readerwho takes us seriously will be
an adventuresome soul who is not one bit afraid ofthinking unorthodox thoughts about people. ( Kelly, 1955, pp. x-xi)
Kelly defended these radical ideas by pointing out that psychology is a young science, so weshould not expect any theory of personality to
explain a wide variety of behavior. To be useful, atheory must be limited to those aspects of behavior for which it is especially well suited (i
tsfocus and range of convenience). Kelly's psychology of personal constructs wasdesigned for the realm of clinical psychology, and
its primary goal is to help people overcomeproblems with their interpersonal relationships. "If [our] theory works well within this limitedran
ge of convenience, we shall consider our efforts successful, and we shall not be too muchdisturbed if it proves to be less useful elsewhere"
( Kelly, 1955, p. 23).
-1497
Rogers' Humanistic/Self Theory
As we observed in chap. 3, a theory of personality is significantly influenced by the theorist's viewof human nature. At the most negative e
xtreme is Freud, who argued that we must sublimate ourtrue but illicit desires (including incest and murder) into less pleasurable activities
that aresocially acceptable. Jung, Erikson, and Murray took a more moderate position: They agreed thatevery personality has its dark side,
but they concluded that we also possess such constructiveinnate drives as creativity, identity, and mastery of the environment.
Some theorists assume that our inborn potentials are entirely positive, albeit not necessarily thesame for everyone. These theorists conte
nd that we must strive to develop all of our innate drivesand desires, and that psychopathology occurs when we abandon our healthy inne
r potentials inorder to be safe or to satisfy the standards of other people (such as our parents). Because thesetheories emphasize the posit
ive aspects of human nature, they are referred to as humanistic.
If we have at least some positive innate instincts, and if these potentials differ for differentpeople, then each of us must learn to follow our
own inner guidelines in order to bepsychologically healthy. Being a writer and a theorist may be right for me, while you may bestrealize yo
ur unique potentials by becoming a painter, a businessperson, or a professional athlete.Theories that focus on how we perceive ourselves
and becoming one's "true self' are referred to asself theories.
Not all self theories take an entirely positive view of human nature (are humanistic). We willbegin our exploration of this area by considerin
g an important theory that is both a humanisticand a self theory. A discussion of alternative approaches is presented in the following chapt
er.
HUMAN NATURE AND MOTIVATION

Carl Rogers emphasized that only we ourselves can know, and choose, our proper direction inlife. In accordance with this belief, Rogers ori
ginally named his approach client-centeredtherapy. Having subsequently expanded his ideas to include such non- clinical areas asparentin
g and education, he ultimately adopted the broader designation of person-centeredtheory (see Rogers, 1951, p. 7, 1977, p. 5).
Actualization
According to Rogers, we are motivated by a single positive force: an innate tendency to developour constructive, healthy capacities. This i
nherent tendency to actualize our
-1748
Humanistic/Self Theory: Controversies andEmerging Findings
As is the case with psychoanalytic theory, humanistic and self theorists do not always agree aboutthe fundamental aspects of personality.
In this chapter, we investigate some of these issues andcontroversies.
HUMANISTIC THEORY
Is There a Hierarchy of Human Needs?
Abraham Maslow was one of the three original exponents of humanistic psychology. (The othersare Carl Rogers and existential psychologis
t Rollo May.) Like Rogerian theory, Maslow'sapproach is both a humanistic and a self theory.
Deficiency and Growth Motives. For the most part, Maslow shared Rogers' optimistic viewof human nature. Our innate (instinctoid) ne
eds are predominantly benign and include thecapacity for constructive growth, kindness, generosity, and love. Yet Maslow also agreed wit
hErikson that these "instinct-remnants" are very weak and are easily overwhelmed by the far morepowerful forces of learning and culture.
"The human needs... are weak and feeble rather thanunequivocal and unmistakable; they whisper rather than shout. And the whisper is ea
silydrowned out" ( Maslow, 1970b, p. 276).
Apathogenic environment can easily inhibit our positive potentials and evoke hatred,destructiveness, and self-defeating behavior. Maslow
therefore preferred a more eclecticapproach to personality than did Rogers, and advised psychologists to guard against excessivetheoretic
al optimism by acquiring a thorough knowledge of Freudian psychoanalysis:
[My goal is] to integrate into a single theoretical structure the partial truths I [see] inFreud, Adler, Jung... Fromm, Horney, [and others].... Fr
eud is still required reading forthe humanistic psychologist... [yet] it is as if [he] supplied to us the sick half ofpsychology, and we must no
w fill it out with the healthy half.... [Thus] it is alreadypossible to reject firmly the despairing belief that human nature is ultimately andbasi
cally depraved and evil,... [and to conclude that the striving
-1899
Skinner's Radical Behaviorism
Although there are striking differences among the theories that we have discussed in previouschapters, they also have one thing in comm
on. All of them attribute the underlying causes ofhuman behavior to processes that occur within the individual, such as instincts and drive
s(including hunger, thirst, sexuality, destructiveness, self-actualization, striving forsuperiority/mastery, and identity), thoughts, beliefs, em
otions, and unconscious conflicts.
Skinner argued that psychology will never be a true science so long as it attributes our behaviorto unobservable inner causes. He likened
all such approaches to the prescientific fallacies of theancient physicists, who "explained" the laws of gravity by claiming that a falling bod
y acceleratedbecause it became happier on finding itself nearer home. To Skinner, the so-called inner causesof human behavior are useles
s redundancies: To say that an organism eats because it is hungry,attacks because it feels angry, or looks into a mirror because it is narcis
sistic explains nothingwhatsoever, for we are still left with the task of discovering why the organism happened to feelhungry, angry, or nar
cissistic. Psychology can only escape its own dark ages by rejecting theunscientific constructs that pervade personality theory, and by stu
dying how observable behavioris influenced by the external environment:
A causal chain [consists] of three links: (1) an operation performed upon the organismfrom without--for example, water deprivation; (2) an
inner condition--for example,physiological or psychic thirst; and (3) a kind of behavior--for example, drinking....[Therefore], we may avoid
many tiresome and exhausting digressions by examining thethird link as a function of the first. Valid information about the second link ma
y throwlight upon this relationship, but can in no way alter it.... [Thus my] objection to innerstates is not that they do not exist, but that th
ey are not relevant in a [causal] analysis. (Skinner, 1953/ 1965, pp. 34-35)
CLASSICAL VERSUS OPERANT CONDITIONING
Types of Conditioning
Classical Conditioning. In classical conditioning, the appearance of the reward orpunishment does not depend on what the organism
does. Ivan Pavlov ( 1906, 1927, 1928) firstdemonstrated this simple form of learning by placing a dog in a restraint in
-20910
Behaviorism: Controversies and Emer ingFindings
One major effort to bridge the chasm between behaviorism and personality theory is that of JohnDollard and Neal E. Miller. Like Carl Roger
s, Dollard and Miller regarded both clinicalobservation and experimental research as extremely important. Psychotherapy reveals deeplype
rsonal issues that the patient would be unlikely to discuss in the research laboratory, while theresearch laboratory has the advantage of gr
eater scientific rigor. In marked contrast to Rogers,however, the goal of Dollard and Miller was to integrate the best features of two seemin
glyirreconcilable theorists: Freud and Pavlov.
Dollard and Miller's theory is currently regarded as of primarily historical importance, partlybecause these ideas were confined to a single
book published in 1950. Yet it represents anunusual and creative effort to merge two radically different schools of thought, and it hasinflue
nced the work of one well-known and highly regarded behaviorist. Albert Bandura hasmodified behaviorism in an even more eclectic direct
ion, as by emphasizing the importance ofcognitive and personal causes of behavior (such as expectations, beliefs, and thoughts). He alsod
evoted considerable attention to observational (social) learning, which involves instruction andwatching others perform tasks correctly. In f
act, Bandura's version of behaviorism is so eclecticthat it often appears to represent more or less of a return to personality theory. This ch
apterbegins with a brief discussion of Dollard and Miller's theory, which is followed by an examinationof Bandura's work. We will conclude
by investigating some current research in the area of sociallearning theory.
DOLLARD AND MILLER'S THEORY
Dollard and Miller regarded the clinical setting as an unusually rich source of data. "Outside ofpsychotherapy, how many subjects have be
en studied for an hour a day, for five days a week,[and] for from one to three years... [and in a] life situation [that] is vital, [where] the alte
rnativesare years of misery or years of relative peace and success?" ( Dollard & Miller, 1950, p. 4). YetDollard and Miller also shared Skinne
r's belief that the basic principles of human behavior canbe discovered though animal studies in the more rigorous confines of the researc
h laboratory.
-23011
Research Methods
Many things that we once believed to be true have been shown to be incorrect. Much of thisenlightenment has been achieved through scie
ntific research, which is superior to subjectiveopinion in one important respect: It relies on empirical data that can be verified and reproduc
ed.One noted example involves the discoveries of Copernicus and Galileo, which disproved theprevailing egocentric belief that the earth is
located at the center of our universe.

A more recent illustration of the merits of scientific research involves the phenomena known asnear-death experiences. In the last 2 decad
es, numerous reports have claimed to prove theexistence of life after death (such as Betty Eadie's best-selling book Embraced by the Ligh
t). Aperson who is near death becomes aware of leaving his or her body, and this is followed byvarious sublime experiences: proceeding d
own a long dark tunnel, emerging into a brilliant andunearthly light, feeling eternal peace and love, and perhaps even seeing heavenly fig
ures andmeeting God:
Amidst all my pain, I saw a faint light in the distance. As I approached it, my painbegan to disappear. When I finally came out of the dark a
nd into the light, it was themost beautiful thing I had ever seen. I heard chimes, or distant bells, tinkling in thebackground, a beautiful sou
nd I'll never forget. I was gently drawn up and into a greatwhirling black mass. I should have been terrified, but I felt a profoundly pleasant
senseof well-being and healing. Love filled this whirling, moving mass. I have never feltgreater tranquility in my life. The pinpoint of light i
n the distance and the black massaround me took the shape of a tunnel, and I felt myself traveling through it at an evengreater speed, rus
hing toward the light. As I got closer, I saw a man with brilliantgolden light radiating around him. His light blended into mine, and the merg
ing of ourlights was like an explosion of love. His arms were open to receive me. I went to himand received his embrace and said, "I'm hom
e. I'm home. I'm finally home." (Blackmore, 1993, pp. 1-2, 136)
Understandably, such profoundly moving experiences have convinced those who have had themthat there is a heavenly afterlife. They bel
ieve that their soul or spirit has been freed from itsearthly ties, the tunnel is a passageway to the next life, and the bright light emanates f
romheaven. But is this conclusion valid?
Scientific research points to a strikingly different conclusion: The phenomena experienced innear-death experiences can be explained by w
hat happens in the dying human brain. Forexample, there are two kinds of chemical activity that occur in the brain.
-25112
Conclusion
Our journey into the realm of personality has featured a formidable array of constructs andprinciples, many contradictory ideas and heate
d disputes, and some islands of generalagreement. As stated at the outset of this book, personality is not a field with many clearcutanswers. We will therefore close with some general observations about personality theory andresearch.
SOME GENERAL CONCLUSIONS
The theorists and researchers whose work we have discussed have been concerned with manyissues, some of which are summarized in Ta
ble 12.1.
Personality Development and Structure
The Importance of Early Childhood. Although most theorists and researchers rejectFreud's contention that behavior is determined enti
rely by prior causes, virtually all agree thatinfancy and childhood is a time of great importance for personality development.
One major contribution of personality theory has been to identify the ways in which parentscause their children to become pathological: p
ampering, neglect, overprotectiveness,overpermissiveness, frequent anxiety, perfectionism, domination, rejection, ridicule, hypocrisy,inco
nsistent standards, a lack of tenderness and affection, brutality, rigidity, conditional positiveregard, and others. This not only provides usef
ul guidelines for those who seek to become betterparents, but also affords some reassurance to anyone who suffers from psychopathology
. Ifpathological self-defeating behaviors, beliefs, and emotions were learned during childhood, theyhave a logical cause. And this implies th
at there is also a logical remedy, in that relief can beobtained by unlearning them (as through psychotherapy).
Adolescence is also an important period, one that can have a corrective effect on a troubledpersonality or exert a pathogenic influence of i
ts own. However, events during infancy andchildhood undoubtedly play a greater role in shaping one's personality. A person who emergesf
rom childhood with a faulty view of self and others is more likely to behave in misguided andself-defeating ways during adolescence, bring
ing on disapproval and rejection from others andintensifying the sufferer's problems. Conversely, a psychologically healthier child is more l
ikelyto handle the rigors of adolescence successfully.
-275Glossary
Ability Trait ( Cattell) A trait that determines how well we do at reaching a particular goal.
Actualization (Actualizing Tendency) ( Rogers) An innate tendency to develop ourconstructive and healthy capacities; the fundamental hu
man motive.
Adoption Study Determining whether children by birth or adopted children in the samefamily more closely resemble their parents on vario
us personality characteristics.
Agreeableness A trait characterized by being trusting and helpful, as opposed to suspiciousand uncooperative. One of the Big Five person
ality traits.
Alternative Hypothesis The hypothesis that states that the null hypothesis is untrue, andspecifies some other value or set of values for th
e population parameter(s) in question.
Anal Stage ( Freud) The second psychosexual stage, which occurs at about age 1-3 years andinvolves the anus as the primary erotogenic
zone.
Analytical Psychology The name given by Carl Jung to his theory of personality.
Anima ( Jung) The female archetype in man. Predisposes man to understand the nature ofwoman and to behave in sentimental ways.
Animus ( Jung) The male archetype in woman. Predisposes woman to understand the natureof man and to behave in rational ways.
Anticathexis ( Freud) A quantity of psychic energy used by the ego to oppose a dangerous orimmoral cathexis.
Anxiety A highly unpleasant emotion that is similar to intense nervousness. In Freud's theory,anxiety is identified by its source: Realistic a
nxiety is caused by danger in the external world,neurotic anxiety concerns the harm that will result from yielding to an illicit id impulse, a
ndmoral anxiety is caused by acts or wishes that violate the standards of the superego.
Approach-Approach Competition ( Dollard and Miller) A situation wherein an organism mustchoose between two desirable goals.
Approach-Avoidance Conflict ( Dollard and Miller) A conflict caused by a goal that has bothdesirable and undesirable qualities. Moving tow
ard it eventually evokes fear; moving awayfrom it prevents satisfaction.
Archetype ( Jung) A predisposition to apprehend the world that we inherit from ourancestors. A potential to behave in particular ways, not
a specific memory or fact.
Assertive Training A form of behavior therapy that helps a client who is inhibited in certainsituations to reduce anxiety, and gain reinforce
ments, by expressing feelings in an honest andsocially acceptable way.
-285References
Achenbach, T., & Zigler, E. ( 1963). "Social competence and self-image disparity in psychiatricand non-psychiatric patients". Journal of Abn
ormal and Social Psychology, 67, 197-205.
Adler, A. ( 1920/ 1973). The practice and theory of individual psychology. Totowa, NJ:Littlefield, Adams & Co.
Adler, A. ( 1927/ 1957). Understanding human nature. Greenwich, CT: Fawcett.
Adler, A. ( 1929/ 1964a). Problems of neurosis. New York: Harper Torchbooks.
Adler, A. ( 1929/ 1969). The science of living. New York: Anchor Books.
Adler, A. ( 1931/ 1958). What life should mean to you. New York: Capricorn Books.
Adler, A. ( 1933/ 1964b). Social interest: A challenge to mankind. New York: Capricorn Books.
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. ( 1950). Theauthoritarian personality. New York: Harper & Row.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. ( 1978). Patterns of attachment.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Alderfer, C. P. ( 1972). Existence, relatedness, and growth needs in organizational change. NewYork: Free Press.

Alloy, L. B., & Ahrens, A. H. ( 1987). "Depression and pessimism for the future: Biased use ofstatistically relevant information in predictions
for self versus others". Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 52, 366-378.
Allport, G. W. ( 1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt, Rinehart &Winston.
Allport, G. W. ( 1942). The use of personal documents in psychological science (Bull. 49). NewYork: Social Science Research Council.
Allport, G. W. ( 1950). The individual and his religion. New York: Macmillan.
Allport, G. W. ( 1954/ 1958). The nature of prejudice. New York: Anchor Books.
Allport, G. W. ( 1955). Becoming: Basic considerations for a psychology of personality. NewHaven, CT: Yale University Press.
Allport, G. W. ( 1960). Personality and social encounter. Boston: Beacon Press.
Allport, G. W. ( 1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Allport, G. W. ( 1965). Letters from Jenny. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World.
Allport, G. W. ( 1968). The person in psychology: Selected essays. Boston: Beacon Press.
Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. ( 1931/ 1960). A study of values. Boston: HoughtonMifflin.
-303-

You might also like