Object and Space Perception
Object and Space Perception
Object and Space Perception
ScienceDirect
Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex
Research report
INSERM, U1028, CNRS, UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct Team, Lyon, France
University UCBL Lyon 1, France
c
Department of Neuropsychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
d
Behavioral Neurology Unit, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health,
Bethesda, MD, USA
e
Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health,
University of Genoa, Genoa, Italy
f
Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
g
Molecular Neuroscience Department, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA, USA
h
Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA, USA
b
article info
abstract
Article history:
In the 1980s, following Newcombes observations, Ungerleider and Mishkin put forward the
functional subdivision of the visual system into a ventral stream dedicated to object
perception and a dorsal stream dedicated to space perception. Ten years after this dis-
covery, the perception-action model re-defined the dorsal stream as responsible for non-
conscious visual guidance, and most recently a tripartition has been suggested to ac-
count for a variety of visuospatial functions. Here, we investigated the neural un-
derpinnings of object and space perception by combining the administration of the Visual
Object Space Perception (VOSP) battery with a voxel-based lesion symptom mapping
Keywords:
(VLSM) approach in a large sample of patients with penetrating traumatic brain injury
Dorsal stream
(pTBI). First, our results provided new support for the complementary role of both hemi-
Ventral stream
spheres in object recognition. The right lateral occipital complex was found to be critical in
Lateralization
early perceptual discrimination, whereas more anterior temporal and frontal regions in the
VLSM
left hemisphere were found to be critical in more complex forms of object discrimination
Insula
and recognition. Second, our findings confirmed that space perception depended on the
integrity of the right inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and revealed that a network linking
the right IPL with the right premotor cortex was critical for the perception of spatial
relationships in both 2D and 3D representations. Taken together, our results supported the
* Corresponding author. CRNL e ImpAct Team, 16, ave Doyen Lepine, 69676 Bron Cedex, France.
** Corresponding author. Molecular Neuroscience Department George Mason University 4400 University Drive, Mails Stop 2A1, Fairfax, VA
22030, USA.
E-mail addresses: [email protected] (S. Schintu), [email protected] (F. Krueger).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.04.009
0010-9452/ 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
245
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
functional subdivision of the visual system and shed new light on the specific processes
involved along both the dorsal and the ventral streams.
2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1.
Introduction
Years after the what and where hypothesis suggesting a functional partition of the visual system into two streams e a
ventral stream subserving object perception and a dorsal stream
subserving space perception (Mishkin, Ungerleider, & Macko,
1983; Newcombe, 1969; Ungerleider & Mishkin, 1982), new
frameworks have emerged refining this subdivision both
anatomically and functionally. Notably, the perception-action
model defines the dorsal stream as responsible for nonconscious visual guidance of action and the ventral stream
for conscious perception (Goodale & Milner, 1992; Milner &
Goodale, 2006). Recently, Kravitz et al. (Kravitz, Saleem,
Baker, & Mishkin, 2011) suggested a tripartition of the dorsal
stream to account for the variety of visuospatial functions.
Three distinct pathways originating in the posterior parietal
cortex (PPC) mediate different visuospatial abilities: (i) a
parieto-premotor pathway for eye movements, several forms
of visually guided action, and grasping; (ii) a parieto-prefrontal
pathway for top-down control of eye movements and spatial
working memory; and (iii) a parieto-medial temporal pathway
for spatial abilities related to navigation. Likewise, the same
group proposed a refinement of the ventral object representation pathway, which is subserved by distinct cortical and
subcortical structures (Kravitz, Saleem, Baker, Ungerleider &
Mishkin, 2013).
Evidence about hemispheric dominance for object
perception and recognition is controversial. Some neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies point toward a right
hemisphere dominance in object perception (Acres, Taylor,
Moss, Stamatakis, & Tyler, 2009; Konen, Behrmann,
Nishimura, & Kastner, 2011), while others suggest a left
hemisphere
dominance
(Price,
Moore,
Humphreys,
Frackowiak, & Friston, 1996; Sergent, Ohta, & MacDonald,
1992; Stewart, Meyer, Frith, & Rothwell, 2001; Zelkowicz,
Herbster, Nebes, Mintun, & Becker, 1998). These conflicting
findings can be reconciled by the fact that object recognition
involves hierarchically organized processes (Ungerleider &
Haxby, 1994) that depend on either the left or the right
hemisphere. According to this view, the right posterior occipital and temporal regions are specialized for the discrimination of basic features, while more anterior left temporal
regions are specialized for assigning a meaning to objects for
categorization and recognition (De Renzi, 1982).
In contrast, general consensus exists on the prominent role
of the right hemisphere in controlling visuospatial attention
(De Renzi, 1982; Kinsbourne, 1987; McCarthy & Warrington,
1990; Mesulam, 1981; Newcombe, 1969). A series of behavioral experiments have demonstrated a relative right hemisphere advantage for processing relationships between spatial
coordinates (i.e., distance evaluation) (Kosslyn et al., 1989). The
right hemispheres dominance in spatial attention, especially
2.
2.1.
Subjects
2.2.
Neuropsychological assessment and behavioral
analysis
All participants underwent a 5e7 day neuropsychological
assessment. As the experimental measure, we employed the
246
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
2.3.
3.
Results
3.1.
Behavioral results
3.2.
VLSM results
3.2.1.
247
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
Table 1 e Descriptive (mean standard deviations) and inferential statistics for demographic, experimental, and control
measures comparing the lesion group (LG [ 192) with the control group (CG [ 55).
Group
LG
Age (years)
Education (years)
Handedness (R : A : L)
58.27 2.96
14.80 2.49
147 : 6 : 21
19.80 0.61
20.10 4.01
17.70 2.08
19.01 1.83
9.37 1.11
60.99 25.07
52.72 24.92
97.49 5.86
9.38 9.15
53.44 7.53
CG
Demographic Measures
59.00 3.40
15.19 2.47
43 : 4 : 8
Experimental Measures
19.71 1.45
20.13 3.86
17.80 2.73
19.35 1.80
9.69 0.63
Control Measures
65.40 22.91
68.50 21.63
98.83 1.55
11.56 9.66
55.44 4.73
Statistics
t 1.56, p .121
t 1.00, p .316
c2 7.46, p .113
t
t
t
t
t
0.66,
0.04,
0.28,
1.22,
2.01,
p
p
p
p
p
.509
.970
.777
.224
.046
t
t
t
t
t
0.96,
4.22,
1.67,
1.52,
1.86,
p
p
p
p
p
.336
.001
.097
.129
.064
Age: years at the time of VOSP administration; Education: years at the time of VOSP administration; Handedness: R, right-handed; A, ambidextrous; L, left-handed; Pre-injury Intelligence and Post-injury Intelligence (percentile scores) AFQT: Armed Forces Qualification Test for
general intelligence; Token Test: for basic verbal comprehension; BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory-II for depression severity; Boston Naming
Test: for object naming; VOSP: Visual Object and Space Perception for object and space perception.
3.2.2.
Fig. 1 e Lesion Density Overlap Map for pTBI patients. Axial slices (z-coordinates from L38 to D63 in MNI space) illustrating
the number of overlapping lesions at each voxel across the whole population. All analyses were restricted to a minimum
overlap of 4 patients at a given voxel and the color range indicates this overlap, from blue (4 patients) to red (31 patients).
The maximum overlap of 31 patients occurred in frontal areas. The right hemisphere is on the readers left.
248
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
Fig. 2 e Voxel-Based Lesion Symptom Mapping (VLSM) results for A, Silhouette Task; B, Object Decision Task; C, Position
Discrimination Task; D, Cube Analysis Task. For A, B, C, D, all colored regions are critical for the corresponding task
performance (q(FDR) [ .05, minimum cluster size of 10 voxels). Color range displays z-scores, from red (minimum z-score
displayed on the right side) to yellow (maximum z-score). Axial slices display z-coordinates from L38 to D63 in MNI space.
The right hemisphere is on the readers left.
3.2.3.
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
249
Fig. 3 e Conjunction Maps for A, Object perception, and B, Space perception. Lesions resulting from conjunction analyses are
areas of damage associated with space perception and object perception tasks. A, Unique brain regions for the object
decision task. B, Overlapping brain regions for the two space perception tasks (position discrimination and cube analysis).
Axial slices display z-coordinates from L38 to D63 in MNI space. The right hemisphere is on the readers left.
4.
Discussion
The aim of the study was to investigate the neural underpinnings of object and space perception using VLSM
analysis in a large pTBI cohort. Our findings identified distinct
and lateralized brain regions critical for object and space
perception within the left ventral stream and the right dorsal
stream, respectively. These results support the functional
subdivision of the visual system and shed new light on the
specific processes involved along both the dorsal and ventral
streams.
4.1.
250
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
4.2.
The dorsal stream, dedicated to space perception, was originally described as an occipito-parietal circuit projecting from
the early visual cortical areas to the posterior regions of the
parietal cortex (Goodale & Milner, 1992; Ungerleider &
Mishkin, 1982). A new framework has recently been formulated, and describes three different pathways originating from
the PPC that mediate spatial perception and visually guided
actions (Kravitz et al., 2011). Within the dorsal pathway, the
right parietal cortex acts as a fundamental nexus and the large
body of evidence from neglect patients has brought unequivocal support for its role in space perception and visuospatial
attention (Bartolomeo, Thiebaut de Schotten, & Chica, 2012;
De Renzi, 1982; Kinsbourne, 1987; McCarthy & Warrington,
1990; Mesulam, 1981). Contrary to the ongoing debate about
the lateralization of the ventral stream, the right hemispheric
dominance for space perception is well established, and the
study of the neglect patients has largely contributed to this
knowledge (Taylor & Warrington, 1973). Evidence for the
dorsal stream lateralization has also been repeatedly reported
in healthy subjects. For instance, TMS on the right PPC induces neglect-like behavior (Brighina et al., 2002; Fierro et al.,
2000) and enhances ipsilateral detection compared to that
elicited by left hemisphere stimulation (Hilgetag et al., 2001).
In addition, the volume of the longitudinal parieto-frontal
tract identified as the superior longitudinal fasciculus II was
found to be larger in the right hemisphere compared to the left
hemisphere, and this asymmetry correlates with a deviation
toward the left in a line bisection task (Thiebaut de Schotten
et al., 2011).
In line with these findings, we identified a set of brain regions in the right hemisphere necessary for space perception
using two different space recognition tasks, including regions
from the PPC to the precentral gyrus, premotor area (BA 6) to the
postcentral gyrus (BA 2 and 3), and the insula (BA 13). One
critical lesion site associated with space perception impairment
was the right IPL. This region, known to receive vestibular inputs from the cerebellum (Clower, Dum, & Strick, 2005; Clower,
West, Lynch, & Strick, 2001), is strongly connected with somatosensory areas (Lewis & Van Essen, 2000), and maintains
visual somatotopic maps (Ishida, Nakajima, Inase, & Murata,
2009). Maintaining a continuously aligned representation of
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
4.3.
Conclusion
Our findings added novel support for the necessary involvement of a left temporo-frontal network for object perception
and a right parieto-premotor network for space perception.
Even though our results showed a different hemispheric
dominance for both the ventral and dorsal stream, this does
not preclude any possible interaction between the two streams
(Konen & Kastner, 2008; Kravitz et al., 2013; Ungerleider &
Haxby, 1994; Zachariou, Klatzky, & Behrmann, 2013). It is
possible that both our analysis strategy and the specifics of our
sample did not allow us to uncover the structure(s) common to
both visual streams. Given the nature of the lesions in our pTBI
population, brain injuries were not randomly distributed (i.e.,
some brain areas were over- and others under-represented)
and covariation of damage across brain regions cannot be
excluded. As age has been shown to have an influence on most
of the VOSP tasks (Bonello, Rapport, & Millis, 1997), the fact
that our sample included only elderly adults is a limitation, as
well as the chronicity of their brain lesions. In fact, all patients
were studied more than 35 years after the brain injury, and it is
therefore possible that some functional recovering may have
affected our findings. Finally, our lesion data were entirely
based on CT scans which has lower resolution and less capacity to discriminate between grey and white matter
compared to MRI. Despite these limitations, the results from
251
Acknowledgments
The work was supported by the U.S. National Institute of
Neurological Disorders and Stroke intramural research program, and a project grant from the United States Army Medical Research and Material Command administrated by the
Henry M. Jackson Foundation (Vietnam Head Injury Study
Phase III: a 30-year post-injury follow-up study, Grant
DAMD17-01-1-0675). Selene Schintu was supported with
funding from the Henry M. Jackson Foundation, and Fadila
Hadj-Bouziane by the NEURODIS Foundation. The authors are
grateful to all the Vietnam veterans who participated in this
study and the National Naval Medical Center for their support
and provision of facilities, as well as V. Raymont, S. Bonifant,
B. Cheon, C. Ngo, A. Greathouse, K. Reding, and G. Tasick for
their invaluable help with the testing of participants and organization of this study. Note that the views expressed in this
article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect
the official policy or position of the Department of the Navy,
the Department of Defense, nor the U.S. Government. For
further information about the Vietnam Head Injury Study,
contact J. G. at [email protected]. The authors
declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any
commercial or financial relationships that could be construed
as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary data
Supplementary data related to this article can be found at
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.04.009.
references
Acres, K., Taylor, K. I., Moss, H. E., Stamatakis, E. A., & Tyler, L. K.
(2009). Complementary hemispheric asymmetries in object
naming and recognition: a voxel-based correlational study.
Neuropsychologia, 47(8e9), 1836e1843.
Baldo, J. V., Arevalo, A., Patterson, J. P., & Dronkers, N. F. (2013).
Grey and white matter correlates of picture naming: evidence
252
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
c o r t e x 5 7 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 2 4 4 e2 5 3
253
Solomon, J., Raymont, V., Braun, A., Butman, J. A., & Grafman, J.
(2007). User-friendly software for the analysis of brain lesions
(ABLe). Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 86(3),
245e254.
Sterzer, P., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2010). Anterior insula activations
in perceptual paradigms: often observed but barely
understood. Brain Structure & Function, 214(5e6), 611e622.
Stewart, L., Meyer, B.-U., Frith, U., & Rothwell, J. (2001). Left
posterior BA37 is involved in object recognition: a TMS study.
Neuropsychologia, 39(1), 1e6.
Taylor, A. M., & Warrington, E. K. (1973). Visual discrimination in
patients with localized cerebral lesions. Cortex, 9(1), 82e93.
Thiebaut de Schotten, M., DellAcqua, F., Forkel, S. J.,
Simmons, A., Vergani, F., Murphy, D. G. M., et al. (2011). A
lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nature
Neuroscience, 14(10), 1245e1246.
Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of semantic
memory: inferring how from where. Neuropsychologia,
41(3), 280e292.
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F.,
Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical
labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical
parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage,
15(1), 273e289.
Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1994). What and where
in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4(2),
157e165.
Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Analysis of visual
behaviour. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield
(Eds.), Two cortical visual systems. Cambridge: MIT Press.
United States Department of Defense. (1960). Armed forces
qualification test (AFQT-7A) form 1293. Washington, DC: United
States Department of Defense.
Vallar, G., & Perani, D. (1986). The anatomy of unilateral neglect
after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan
correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia, 24(5), 609e622.
Vandenberghe, R., Price, C., Wise, R., Josephs, O., &
Frackowiak, R. S. (1996). Functional anatomy of a common
semantic system for words and pictures. Nature, 383(6597),
254e256.
Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1991). The visual object and space
perception battery (Vol. 4). Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley
Test Company.
Warrington, E. K., & Taylor, A. M. (1978). Two categorical stages of
object recognition. Perception, 7(6), 695e705.
Woods, R. P., Mazziotta, J. C., & Cherry, S. R. (1993). MRI-PET
registration with automated algorithm. Journal of Computer
Assisted Tomography, 17(4), 536e546.
Xu, J., Gannon, P. J., Emmorey, K., Smith, J. F., & Braun, A. R. (2009).
Symbolic gestures and spoken language are processed by a
common neural system. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 106(49), 20664e20669.
Zachariou, V., Klatzky, R., & Behrmann, M. (2013). Ventral and
dorsal visual stream contributions to the perception of object
shape and object location. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1e21.
Zelkowicz, B. J., Herbster, A. N., Nebes, R. D., Mintun, M. A., &
Becker, J. T. (1998). An examination of regional cerebral blood
flow during object naming tasks. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 4(2), 160e166.