Dweck - Praise For Intelligence
Dweck - Praise For Intelligence
Dweck - Praise For Intelligence
0022-3514/98/S3.00
34
1984). For example, Dweck and her associates have demonstrated that children who hold performance goals are likely to
sacrifice potentially valuable learning opportunities if these opportunities hold the risk of making errors and do not ensure
immediate good performance (Elliott & Dweck, 1988). That is,
' 'being challenged'' and ' 'learning a lot" are rejected in favor of
"seeming smart" by children who subscribe to a performance
orientation (Mueller & Dweck, 1997). Furthermore, an emphasis on performance goals has been linked to vulnerability to a
maladaptive helpless response to achievement setbacks
(Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Elliott & Dweck, 1988), which is
characterized by negative affect, negative self-cognitions, and
performance impairment in the face of failure (Diener & Dweck,
1978, 1980).
Second, praise for intelligence after good performance may
also directly contribute to children's responses through the development of stable ability attributions for failure. Specifically,
evaluators who praise for ability may teach children that intelligence is a stable trait that is reflected in and can be easily read
from performance. If children carry away this lesson, they may
read low intelligence from poor performance and thus make
ability attributions not only for their successes but also for their
failures.
Whereas individuals who attribute their successes to internal
abilities or traits have been classified as being high in achievement efficacy (cf. Schunk, 1994, 1996) and motivation (cf.
Weiner, 1972, 1985; Weineretal., 1971), individuals who attribute their failures to ability have not been credited with the same
positive strivings. On the contrary, the negative motivational
consequences of attributions that ascribe failures to ability have
been well documented by researchers interested in achievement
(Bell, McCallum, Bryles, & Driesler, 1994; Covington & Omelich, 1984; Dweck, 1975; Jagacinski & Nicholls, 1984; Weiner,
Russell, & Lerman, 1979). In particular, Dweck and her associates have linked ability attributions for failure to helpless responding in the face of failure (Diener & Dweck, 1978, 1980;
Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Elliott & Dweck, 1988). In this study,
we propose that praise for their intelligence, even when it follows
a genuine success, teaches children that they can measure how
smart they are from how well they do. Therefore, if they subsequently do poorly, children may remeasure their ability from
this low performance.
process of their work and the possibilities for learning and improvement that hard work may offer. Because of this emphasis
on their efforts, children may feel able to focus on the development of their skills through the mastery of new material. In
other words, they may orient toward learning goals, which have
been associated with high achievement motivation (cf. Nicholls,
1984) as well as continued displays of persistence, enjoyment,
and good performance in the face of setbacks (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Elliott & Dweck, 1988).
Second, children praised for their hard work may learn to
attribute their performance to effort, which can vary in amount,
rather than to a stable ability. Thus, they will interpret subsequent poor performance as indicating a temporary lapse in effort
rather than as a deficit in intelligence. Attributions that emphasize effort have been correlated with achievement motivation
(Powers, Douglas, Cool, & Gose, 1985) and positive postfailure
striving (Diener & Dweck, 1980; Dweck, 1975; Nicholls, 1976).
Thus, praise for hard work may lead children to display more
adaptive achievement behaviors after failure than children
praised for intelligence.
If intelligence praise has unwanted consequences for children's achievement after failure, what type of praise might result
in resiliency to setbacks? In this research, we compared the
goals and achievement behaviors of children praised for intelligence with those of children praised for effort (i.e., hard work).
Praise for hard work was chosen as a comparison for praise for
intelligence primarily because effort is one of the fundamental
causal ascriptions for achievement outcomes (Weiner, 1972,
1985) and its attributional message was one that was predicted
to enhance achievement motivation. Praise for effort is proposed
to affect children in terms of both their goals and their
attributions.
First, effort-related praise may lead children to focus on the
35
characterized by positive self-cognitions, positive affect, challenge-seeking behaviors, and enhanced performance (Elliott &
Dweck, 1988). Therefore, we hypothesized that children receiving intelligence praise would show less task enjoyment, less
task persistence, and worse task performance after failure than
children praised for effort.
Furthermore, the attributions that children made for their failures were expected to be strongly influenced by praise. In this
study, we expected children praised for their intelligence after
success to explain subsequent failures in terms of this ability.
On the other hand, we expected children praised for hard work
to attribute failure to a lack of effort.
This study also included a control group of children who
received praise without any attributional component. On the
basis of previous goal research (Elliott & Dweck, 1988), we
expected these children to hold learning and performance goals
in approximately equivalent numbers. Further, we expected their
postfailure reactions to fall between those of the two experimental groups.
Method
Participants. A total of 128 fifth graders (70 girls and 58 boys)
participated in this study. Forty-nine percent were from one public elementary school in a small midwestern town, and 51% were from two
public elementary schools in a large northeastern city. Children ranged
in age from 10 to 12 years; their mean age was 10.7 years (SD =
0.6). Fifty percent of the children were Caucasian, 19% were African
American, and 31% were Hispanic. Informed consent for the participation of all children in this and subsequent studies was given by parents,
teachers, and school principals.
Measures. All children were asked to work on three sets of problems, each containing 10 Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1976).
Scores were based on the number of problems solved in each set.
Children's achievement goals were measured after they had worked
on the first set of (success) matrices and received feedback. The measure
was designed to contain a choice of tasks that embodied different goals
and has been used successfully in previous studies (see Dweck & Leggett, 1988).
Three of the choices represented variations of a performance goal in
that they focused on the display of ability: "problems that aren't too
hard, so I don't get many wrong," "problems that are pretty easy, so
I'll do well," and "problems that I'm pretty good at, so I can show that
I'm smart." The fourth choice, "problems that I'll learn a lot from,
even if I won't look so smart," represented a learning goal in that it
emphasized the development of ability over the display of high performance. Three performance goal selections were used to offset the potential social desirability of a learning goal (Leggett, 1986).
After they had made their selections, children were told that their
choices would be granted if there was extra time at the end of the
session, but first they were to work on the experimental tasks that had
been decided before the study began. This explanation was given so that
children selecting different options would not differ in their expectations
of the nature and difficulty of their subsequent tasks. Experimenters
remained blind to children's goal choices because children were asked
to place their response sheets, as well as all subsequent response sheets,
in an unmarked envelope without showing them to the experimenters.
After a second, difficult trial, children were asked to respond to a
series of questions that probed their desire to persist on the problems,
their enjoyment of the problems, their perceptions of the quality of their
performance, and their attributions for poor performance. Children rated
their task persistence, task enjoyment, and performance quality on a
scale from 1 (not at all) to 6 (very much). Task persistence was indexed
36
37
Intelligence Control
Effort
Type of Praise Given
Study 2
Intelligence Control
Effort
Type of Praise Given
Study 3
Intelligence Control
Effort
Type of Praise Given
Figure 1. Proportion of children who selected performance (rather than learning) goals plotted as a
function of the type of praise given.
38
Table 1
Failure Attributions Made After Poor Performance as a
Function of Type of Praise Given
Dependent measure
Type of praise
Study 1
Low ability*
M
SD
Low effort****
M
SD
Intelligence
(n = 41)
Effort
(it = 41)
Control
(n = 46)
16.49,
11.04
9.78b
9.00
13.88,
9.18
4.94,
7.04
11-96,,
8.15
10.58b
8.43
Effort
(it = 30)
Control
(n = 29)
Study 3
Intelligence
(n = 29)
Low ability****
M
SD
Low effort****
M
SD
19.79,
7.18
7.70b
6.20
12.28C
7.43
4.07,
3.43
14.83b
7.70
7.97C
4.87
Study 5
Effort
in = 15)
Control
(it = 15)
20.94,
7.17
1.1%
9.50
12.06,,
8.06
7.13,
5.52
20.06b
11.32
10.06.
6.79
Dependent measure
Type of praise
Intelligence
(n = 16)
Effort
(it = 16)
Control
(n = 16)
16.94,
9.74
7.13k
6.48
13.31,
8.67
7.25,
5.34
20.8U
9.42
5.75.
4.92
Task persistence****
M
SD
Task enjoyment***
M
SD
Intelligence
(it = 41)
Effort
(n = 41)
Control
(n = 46)
3.25.
1.41
4.53 b
1.03
4.3C
1.33
4.11.
1.02
4.89b
0.72
4.52C
0.81
Intelligence
(n = 29)
Effort
(it = 30)
Control
(n = 29)
3.24,
0.83
5.20b
1.00
4.28C
1.29
3.86,
1.01
4.99b
0.55
4.49C
0.94
Intelligence
(n = 16)
Effort
(n = 15)
Control
(n = 15)
3.44,
1.59
4.62b
1.63
4.56b
1.26
3.92,
0.95
5.19,,
0.82
4.90b
0.95
Intelligence
(it = 16)
Effort
(n = 16)
Control
(n = 16)
3.75
1.18
4.63
1.20
4.00
1.03
3.84,
0.74
0.88
4.41 b
0.80
Study 3
Study 6
Low ability**
M
SD
Low effort****
M
SD
Table 2
Ratings of Task Persistence and Enjoyment Made After Poor
Performance as a Function of Type of Praise Given
Study 1
Intelligence
in = 16)
Low ability****
M
SD
Low effort****
M
SD
Task persistence****
M
SD
Task enjoyment****
M
SD
Study 5
Task persistence*
M
SD
Task enjoyment***
M
SD
Study 6
Task persistence
M
SD
Task enjoyment***
M
SD
39
Trial 1
Trial 3
Trial 1
Study 1
Trial 3
Study 3
INTELLIGENCE PRAISE
- - - -
CONTROL PRAISE
EFFORT PRAISE
6.5
Trial 1
Trial 3
Study 5
Trial 1
Trial 3
Study 6
Figure 2. Number of problems children solved before failure (Trial 1) and after failure (Trial 3). Bar
data from Study 6, different tasks were used on THals 1 and 3.
ences were noted between two very different samples of participants. That is, both children from a midwestern school, whose
ethnic makeup was almost entirely Caucasian American, and
children from two northeastern schools, whose ethnic makeup
was predominantly African American and Hispanic, were affected by praise for effort and praise for ability in the same
way. The similar responses of these two divergent populations
provide an internal replication for the findings of Study 1.
Study 2
The results of Study 1 supported our initial predictions that
praise for effort versus praise for intelligence after success leads
children to hold different achievement goals and to display different postfailure responses. However, Study 1 did not fully
investigate the different consequences of these types of praise
40
Method
Participants. A total of 51 fifth graders (26 girls and 25 boys) from
a public elementary school in a large northeastern city participated in
this study. Children ranged in age from 9 to 11 years; their mean age
was 10.5 years (SD = 0.5). T\vo percent of the children were Caucasian,
76% were African American, and 22% were Hispanic.
Measures and procedure. Children were randomly assigned to one
of three categories of praiseintelligence, effort, and controlso that
17 children made up each group. All children worked on the first set of
Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1976) used in Study 1. Achievement goals were again measured after children had received positive
feedback.
Children were also asked to respond to questions from Study 1 that
probed their desire to persist on the problems, their enjoyment of the
problems, and their perception of their performance. However, these
measures were taken after their success on the first problem set. In
addition, expectations of future performance on the problems were measured by responses to the question "How well do you expect to do on
another set of problems like these?"
Study 3
We designed Study 3 to satisfy two main goals. The first aim
was to replicate the finding of Study 1 that praise for intelligence
and praise for hard work had different consequences for children's achievement goals and postfailure responses. The second
aim was to extend the investigation of intelligence praise and
effort praise by examining children's goals and concerns in
several new ways.
What do children care most about after failure: finding out
how to master problems better or finding out how well their
peers performed? If children are more oriented toward a learning
goal, then they should display a greater interest in acquiring
additional strategies so that they can better understand the problems (Butler, 1993; cf. Ruble & Frey, 1991). If, on the other
hand, they are more oriented toward a performance goal, they
should be more interested in knowing the scores of others in
order to assess or validate their ability (Butler, 1993; cf. Ruble &
Frey, 1991). Thus, children praised for effort, who prefer learning goals, were expected to seek information about learning.
Children praised for intelligence, who prefer performance goals,
were expected to seek information related to the performance
of others. This result would also suggest that these children
were truly performance oriented and not simply trying to retain
the experimenter's high opinions of their intelligence by choosing goals that ensured continued good performance.
Next, how do children praised for intelligence versus those
praised for effort represent their task performance to others? To
answer this question, children were asked to report their scores
confidentially to an anonymous child at another school. To the
extent that they had learned that performance reflected their
intelligence, we hypothesized that children praised for their ability would be more likely to misrepresent their performance in
a favorable direction. In contrast, to the extent that children
praised for effort did not see their performance as reflecting
negatively on fundamental aspects of the self, they should have
less reason to distort their performance. Both of these results
would suggest that intelligence praise does not simply teach
41
Method
Participants. A total of 88 fifth graders (48 girls and 40 boys)
participated in this study. Seventy-four percent were from one public
elementary school in a small midwestern town, and 26% were from two
public elementary schools in a large northeastern city. Children ranged
in age from 9 to 11 years; their mean age was 10.3 years {SD = 0.5).
Seventy-four percent of the children were Caucasian, 8% were African
American, and 18% were Hispanic. Informed consent for the participation of all children was given by parents, teachers, and school principals.
Measures and procedure. In addition to the measures used in Study
1, two additional measures were added at the end of the original procedure to test the hypothesis that children praised for ability respond
differently to difficulty than do children praised for effort. A total of 29
children received intelligence praise, 30 received effort praise, and 29
made up the control group.
First, in order to determine whether children praised for ability were
more likely to misrepresent their actual performance after setbacks than
were children praised for effort, their self-reports were assessed. Participants were asked to write a description of the third set of problems for
children in another state. They were asked to state in their descriptions
how many of the third set of problems they had answered correctly. The
accuracy of their score reports was determined by subtracting the number
of problems they claimed to have solved from the actual number they
answered correctly.
In addition, postfailure information seeking was measured to test the
hypothesis that children praised for ability would be more interested in
measuring their performance than in mastering the problems and that
children praised for effort would show the opposite preference. Children
were presented with two identical folders; one contained "interesting
new strategies" for solving the problems (strategy information), and
the other contained the "average scores" of unfamiliar children (performance information). Children were asked to choose only one of the two
folders to read.
42
i.uuE3 Study 3
j
0.75-
0.50-
o
Q.
o
0.25-
Study 4
0.00Intelligence
Control
Effort
43
tion about scores rather than strategies indicates that they focus
on performance in their own right and not just in order to
document their abilities to an evaluator.
Further, the accuracy of children's self-reports of their performance on the third set of problems was measured. The difference between the number of problems that children reported
that they solved to other fifth graders and the number of problems that they actually answered correctly was examined. As
shown in Figure 4, a chi-square analysis revealed significant
group differences on this measure, x 2 ( 2 , N = 88) = 6.61, p <
.05. A full 38% of the children praised for ability misrepresented
their scores to unfamiliar children, whereas only 13% of the
children praised for effort and 14% of the children in the control
condition did so. On average, the children praised for intelligence added 0.45 point (SD = 1.22) to their actual score. All
children's misrepresentations increased their actual scores, suggesting that these inaccuracies were not attributable to random
errors in children's memory of their scores.
This finding suggests that over one third of children praised
for intelligence cared so much about their performance and how
it reflected on them that they lied about their performance to
another child rather than admitting to a lower score. The unwillingness of these children to report their true scores in this way
seems particularly striking when one considers that on this measure, children were reporting their scores to completely unfamiliar children, whom they were certain never to meet.
The tendency of children praised for intelligence to misrepresent their scores offers support for the contention that they indeed learned to measure themselves from their performance in
their own right. Because great care was taken to ensure that
"5
Intelligence
Control
Effort
children believed that adults would never see their score reports,
any overstatements of their performance were unlikely to have
been for the benefit of the experimenter. Instead, it seems plausible to suggest that children praised for intelligence were led to
exaggerate their own scores by some internal judgments that
they had madethat is, the judgment that their performance
was an index of their intelligence. Children praised for hard
work, in contrast, did not necessarily share this type of selfjudgment and so would have felt less pressure to artificially
increase the reports of their scores.
Overall then, the results of this study demonstrated that praising children for intelligence after good performance can backfire
by making children highly performance-oriented and extremely
vulnerable to the effects of subsequent setbacks. Children's performance orientation was indicated by their tendency to sacrifice
learning goals and strategy information in favor of performance
goals and normative information as well as by their tendency
to misrepresent their actual scores after failure. Their vulnerability to setbacks was demonstrated by their relatively low enjoyment, persistence, and performance as well as their low-ability
attributions after failure. Praising children for effort, meanwhile,
appears to allow them to focus on the opportunities for mastery
provided by learning goals and strategy information and to avoid
the vulnerabilities (i.e., low enjoyment, persistence, and performance) that can be associated with task difficulties. Indeed,
praise for intelligence appears to teach children to measure their
ability from their performance on a task in a way that praise
for hard work does not.
Study 4
Study 4 was intended to replicate and extend the findings of
Study 2. We hypothesized that, after a successful performance,
children praised for intelligence would not differ from those
praised for effort or those in the control condition in their task
enjoyment, persistence, expectations, and performance judgments. Also, we hypothesized that children praised for intelligence would endorse a performance goal in greater numbers
than would children praised for effort, as had been found in the
three preceding reported studies.
In Study 4 we also further investigated the contrasting information-seeking behaviors exhibited by children praised for intelligence and those praised for effort in Study 3. We hypothesized that children praised for intelligence would again prefer
information about the performance of others that would allow
them to measure themselves. In this study, however, we measured children's information preferences after their initial successful performance, when they may well have expected to work
on additional problems. In this case, then, a preference for performance-related information would effectively rob children of
useful problem-solving strategies that might improve their performance on later trials. We expected that children praised for
effort would again prefer information about new strategies that
would help them master the tasks.
We also designed Study 4 to extend the findings of the first
three studies by investigating the influence that praise might
have on children's beliefs about the nature of intelligence. How
might praise for intelligence orient children toward an entity
view of intelligence? By judging intelligence exclusively from
44
a small sample of work (e.g., ""feu got a high score; you must
be smart"), an adult might imply that children's intelligence is
an internal, perhaps stable, trait that readily displays itself in
performance. That is, the feedback may convey that intelligence
is a stable dimension that can be reliably measured.
Praise for hard work, on the other hand, does not appear to
carry this message of intelligence as a fixed trait to children.
Instead, because this type of praise emphasizes effort as playing
a fundamental role in achievement, it is likely to orient children
toward the development of their abilities. This focus is consistent
with an incremental view that characterizes intelligence as
something that can be developed.
Method
Participants. A total of 51 fifth graders (29 girls and 22 boys) from
a public elementary school in a large northeastern city participated in
this study. Children ranged in age from 9 to 11 years; their mean age
was 9.9 years (SD = 0.5). Two percent of the children were Caucasian,
69% were African American, and 29% were Hispanic. Seventeen children were assigned to each feedback group.
Measures and procedure. The first part of this study was designed
to replicate Study 2. Then, children's implicit theories of intelligence
were measured in order to investigate how praise might influence children's beliefs about the fixedness of intelligence. Specifically, children
were asked to rate a statement taken from the Implicit Theory Scale
(">fou have a certain amount of intelligence and really can't do much
to change it") on a scale from one (not at all true) to six (very true)
(see Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995, for a review).
Next, participants' information-seeking preferences were measured as
in Study 3. Children were asked to choose to read either a folder containing strategy-related information or one containing performance-related information. However, in this study the assessment was made after
the first (success) trial and not after a failure task.
0.0
Intelligence
Control
Effort
Method
Participants. A total of 46 fifth graders (26 girls and 20 boys)
participated in this study. Twenty-six percent were from one public
elementary school in a small midwestern town, and 74% were from two
45
46
A one-way ANO\5\ revealed a significant difference in children's postfailure task persistence, F(2, 45) = 3.16, p = .05.
Children praised for intelligence were less likely to want to
persist on the problems after setbacks (M = 3.44, SD = 1.59)
than were children praised for effort (M = 4.62, SD = 1.63);
children in the control condition (M = 4.56, SD = 1.26) closely
resembled those in the effort condition. Follow-up t tests revealed significant differences between the intelligence condition
and the effort, *(30) = -2.09, p < .05, and control, t(30) =
2.22, p < .05, conditions but no difference between the effort
and control conditions, f(30) = 0.12, ns.
A second one-way ANOV\ disclosed a significant difference
in children's task enjoyment after setbacks, F(2, 45) = 8.64, p
< .005. Children praised for intelligence (M = 3.92, SD 0.95) enjoyed the tasks less than did children praised for effort
(M = 5.19, SD = .82); again, children in the control condition
(M = 4.90, SD = .95) fell in between the other two groups.
Follow-up t tests yielded significant differences between the
intelligence condition and the effort, ?(30) = 4.07, p < .001,
and control, t(30) = -2.92, p < .01, conditions but not between
the effort and control conditions, r(30) = 0.82, ns.
A third one-way AN0\A did not initially reveal significant
differences in children's performance change between the first
and third problem sets, perhaps because of the smaller sample
size of Study 5 than of Study 1 and Study 3, F(2, 45) =
2.13, ns. However, a planned comparison revealed significant
differences in the performance of children praised for intelligence and those praised for effort from the first to the third
problem set, t(30) = -2.10, p < .05, even though children's
performance did not differ on the first set of problems, z(30)
= 0.21, ns. Intelligence-feedback children's scores on the
problems given after failure dropped an average of 0.50 (SD =
2.16) from their scores on the first set of problems (Figure
2c). Children in the effort condition, however, improved their
prefailure scores by 1.00 (SD = 1.86) problems. Children in
the control group improved their scores slightly (M = 0.38, SD
= 2.16). Two additional t tests did not yield significant differences between the performance of children in the effort and
control conditions, ?(30) = 0.88, ns, or between that of children
in the intelligence and control conditions, f(30) = 1.15, ns.
As found in several of the preceding studies, a fourth oneway ANOV\ revealed no significant differences in how the three
groups perceived their performance after two problem sets, F(2,
45) = 0.59, ns.
Overall, the results of this study support the hypothesis that
the contrasting responses to failure demonstrated by children
praised for effort and those praised for ability were not caused
by children's reactions to the judgments of a single experimenter. Children praised for intelligence after success displayed
more negative responses to subsequent failure than did children
praised for effort, even when the experimenter who administered
the failure feedback was blind to the children's experimental
condition and previous performance.
Furthermore, because children were not asked to explicitly
state their achievement goals before they worked on the second
problem set and encountered failure, their statements of these
preferences could not in any way have influenced their later
expectations or reactions. It seems increasingly likely that children's different responses to failure after praise for effort and
Study 6
We designed Study 6 primarily to examine yet another alternate interpretation of the findings of Studies 1 through 5. Earlier,
we proposed that the two types of praise led children to interpret
their performance in markedly different ways. We hypothesized
that praise for intelligence would teach children to use their
performance on an intellectual task as a measure of intellectual
ability. In contrast, we proposed that praise for effort would
lead children to emphasize the degree to which they worked on
a task when explaining their performance.
However, an alternate explanation for the differences found in
the preceding studies is that children learned that the particular
experimental task on which they worked measured ability and
not that performance on intellectual tasks, in general, measures
ability. For instance, it is possible that children praised for ability
after working on the first set of problems believed that the
experimental task was actually designed to serve as an intelligence test. This belief could account for the heightened desire
to do well, manifested in their endorsement of performance
goals after praise. Furthermore, for these children, subsequent
poor performance on the same task would have seemed to be a
real measure of low intelligence. This interpretation could have
resulted in the decreased subsequent motivation and performance that they displayed in the preceding studies. Children
praised for hard work, meanwhile, were not likely to view the
experimental task as being diagnostic of their intelligence, perhaps allowing them to avoid experiencing decrements in motivation and achievement.
We designed this study to assess the viability of this explanation by determining whether children praised for ability did
indeed acquire a general tendency to judge their intelligence
from their performance or whether they did so only for the task
on which their intelligence was praisedthe "diagnostic task."
After children worked on the initial task and were praised for
their intelligence, they were introduced to a novel task of unknown diagnosticity on which they encountered challenge. We
then assessed children's responses to their poor performance on
this new task. Because we initially hypothesized that children
praised for ability learn to make general judgments about their
skills from their performance, we expected these children to
display reactions similar on the novel task to those documented
in the preceding studies after failure. Similarly, we expected
children praised for effort to display the resiliency to setback
that they demonstrated in the preceding studies.
An additional aim of Study 6 was to examine whether the
effect of contrasting forms of praise on children's beliefs about
the nature of intelligence noted in Study 4 would be noted not
only right after the praise, but also later on, after setbacks. In
Study 4, children's theories about the malleability of intelligence
were measured only after a successful performance. It could
reasonably be argued that children who received a positive judgment of their intelligence from an adult experimenter might have
had a greater interest in defining intelligence as fixed (thereby
claiming a permanent positive judgment of this characteristic)
than might children praised for effort. If this were true, greater
endorsement of the entity view would not be expected in the
face of poor performance, because children would no longer
have any vested interest in believing that a low measurement of
ability reflects a fixed trait.
In this study, we used two methods to measure children's
theories of intelligence after they experienced a setback. First,
as in Study 4, children rated their agreement with a statement
about the nonmalleability of intelligence. In addition, they were
asked to define intelligence in an open-ended question. Because
we hypothesized that praise does indeed convey information on
the nature of intelligence (and does not simply motivate children
to adopt a belief that serves their interests), we expected to
replicate the finding of Study 4 that children praised for intelligence agreed with a statement that described intelligence as
fixed to a greater extent than did children praised for effort. We
also wished to determine whether children praised for intelligence after success would also define the nature of intelligence
more in terms of an ability or trait than would children praised
for effort.
Method
Participants. A total of 48 fifth graders (23 girls and 25 boys)
participated in this study. Eighty-one percent were from a public elementary school in a small midwestern town, and 19% were from a public
elementary school in a large northeastern city. Children ranged in age
from 10 to 12 years; their mean age was 10.8 years (SD = 0.6). Eightyfour percent of the children were Caucasian, 8% were African American,
and 8% were Hispanic.
Measures and procedure. Much of the basic procedure outlined in
Study 1 was used in this study. However, after the first set of Standard
Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1976), children in this study were asked
by the same experimenter to work on different kinds of problems for 4
min. These problems were taken from the revised Minnesota Paper Form
Board Test (Likert & Quasha, 1970), which requires individuals to
"perceive fragmented percepts as wholes" (Lezak, 1983) and which is
quite distinct in appearance from Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices.
The first set of these problems contained extremely difficult items,
whereas the subsequent set contained items of moderate difficulty. Children's scores were again based on the number of problems that they
solved.
Participants' desire to persist on the problems, their enjoyment of the
problems, their attributions for failure, and their judgments about the
quality of their performance after setbacks were assessed as in Study 1.
In addition, children's beliefs about the nature of intelligence were
measured as in Study 4. These beliefs were also assessed when children
were asked to define intelligence by completing the sentence "I think
intelligence is . . ." Two coders who were blind to the experimental
condition the children were in categorized children's responses to this
open-ended question according to the presence (coded 1) or absence
(coded 0) of two elements: their use of terms that emphasized the more
malleable or motivational components of intelligence (e.g., effort and
knowledge) and their use of terms that emphasized the trait-like nature
of intelligence (e.g., ability and smartness).
Results and
Discussion
47
48
children praised for intelligence continued to show greater dec rements in enjoyment and performance than did children praised
for effort. They also continued to attribute their failure to a lack
of ability to a greater degree than did children praised for effort.
Thus, children who were told that their high performance was
caused by high intelligence appeared to continue to read their
ability from subsequent performance, even on an unrelated task,
whereas children praised for effort did not appear to reach the
same conclusion. The differential effects of praise for hard work
and praise for intelligence were therefore not limited to only
one experimental task; the attributional messages conveyed by
praise generalized to children's experiences with a new set of
problems.
Definitions of intelligence. Figure 5 shows that a significant
difference was obtained for children's endorsement of an entity
versus an incremental theory of intelligence in this study, F(2,
45) = 4.41, p < .05. As in Study 4, children in the intelligence
condition clearly endorsed an entity theory, affirming the fixed
nature of intelligence (M = 4.13, SD = 1.20), whereas children
in the effort and control conditions distinctly preferred an incremental view vis-a-vis the malleability of intelligence (M = 2.56,
SD = 1.55, and M = 2.94, SD = 1.84, respectively). Followup t tests showed significant differences between the intelligence
condition and both the effort, f(30) = 3.19, p < .005, and
control, f(30) = 2.16, p < .05, conditions. No differences were
noted between the effort and control conditions, 1(30) =
-0.62, ns.
These findings demonstrate that children praised for intelligence after a success are led to endorse statements that describe
intelligence as a fixed trait that is not subject to development,
even after they have experienced setbacks on a problem-solving
task.
In a related vein, on the open-ended question, children in
the effort and ability conditions were found to differ in their
definitions of intelligence. The two raters showed 94% agreement on their coding of malleable or motivational terms (e.g.,
knowledge and effort) and 83% agreement on their coding of
trait terms (e.g., smartness and ability); differences were resolved through discussion.
A chi-square comparison between children's use of malleable
or motivational terms for intelligence (e.g., "studying hard,"
"trying your best," and "how much you know") revealed a
significant difference between the intelligence and effort conditions, x 2 ( 2 , N = 48) = 5.81, p = .05. Whereas 56% of children
praised for effort used these terms (e.g., "It is to work hard")
to describe the nature of intelligence, only 25% of children
praised for ability did so. The responses of children in the control condition resembled those of children in the intelligence
condition; only 23% of them explained intelligence in terms of
knowledge or effort.
However, although the differences were in the predicted direction, a chi-square analysis did not reveal significant differences
between the three groups in terms of their use of trait terms
(e.g., "It is smartness") to define intelligence, x 2 ( 2 , N = 48)
= 2.23, ns.
Still, the results of the open-ended descriptions offer some
corroboration for children's endorsement of the statement about
the nature of intelligence. Children praised for ability after good
performance were found to be somewhat more likely to later
49
Alternative Explanations
Although the encouragement of children with low achievement levels through the use of praise for their ability has received
50
51
52
improve self-esteem in low-income elementary school children. Journal of School Psychology, 22, 155-163.
Powers, S., Douglas, P., Cool, B. A., & Gose, K. F. (1985). Achievement
motivation and attributions for success and failure. Psychological Reports, 57, 751-754.
Raven, J. C. (1976). Standard progressive matrices. Oxford, England:
Oxford Psychologists Press.
Ruble, D. N., & Frey, K. S. (1991). Changing patterns of comparative
behavior as skills are acquired: A functional model of self-evaluation.
In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary
theory and research (pp. 79-113). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schunk, D. H. (1983). Ability versus effort attributional feedback: Differential effects on self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 848-856.
Schunk, D. H. (1994). Self-regulation of self-efficacy and attributions
in academic settings. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.),
Self-regulation of learning and performance: Issues and educational
applications (pp. 75-99). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schunk, D. H. (1996, April). Attributions and the development of selfregulatory competence. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association, New \fork.
Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation: From mechanism to cognition. Chicago: Markham.
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation
and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548-573.
Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M.
(1971). Perceiving the causes of success and failure. Morristown,
NJ: General Learning Press.
Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1979). The cognition-emotion
process in achievement-related contexts. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 37, 1211-1220.