Personality and Individual Differences: Tara C. Marshall, Katharina Lefringhausen, Nelli Ferenczi
Personality and Individual Differences: Tara C. Marshall, Katharina Lefringhausen, Nelli Ferenczi
Personality and Individual Differences: Tara C. Marshall, Katharina Lefringhausen, Nelli Ferenczi
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 23 January 2015
Received in revised form 14 April 2015
Accepted 21 April 2015
Keywords:
Facebook
Social networking
Social media
Status updates
Big ve
Self-esteem
Narcissism
a b s t r a c t
Status updates are one of the most popular features of Facebook, but few studies have examined the traits
and motives that inuence the topics that people choose to update about. In this study, 555 Facebook
users completed measures of the Big Five, self-esteem, narcissism, motives for using Facebook, and frequency of updating about a range of topics. Results revealed that extraverts more frequently updated
about their social activities and everyday life, which was motivated by their use of Facebook to communicate and connect with others. People high in openness were more likely to update about intellectual
topics, consistent with their use of Facebook for sharing information. Participants who were low in
self-esteem were more likely to update about romantic partners, whereas those who were high in conscientiousness were more likely to update about their children. Narcissists use of Facebook for attention-seeking and validation explained their greater likelihood of updating about their
accomplishments and their diet and exercise routine. Furthermore, narcissists tendency to update about
their accomplishments explained the greater number of likes and comments that they reported receiving
to their updates.
2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
1. Introduction
Why do some people write Facebook status updates that
describe amusing personal anecdotes, whereas others write
updates that declare love to a signicant other, express political
opinions, or recount the details of last nights dinner? Since the
inception of Facebook in 2004, status updates have been one of
its most preferred features (Ryan & Xenos, 2011). Status updates
allow users to share their thoughts, feelings, and activities with
friends, who have the opportunity to like and comment in return.
In spite of the central role of status updates in Facebook use, few
studies have examined the predictors of the topics that people
choose to write about in their updates. The current study took a
step in this direction by examining the personality traits associated
with the frequency of updating about ve broad topics identied
through a factor analytic approach: social activities and everyday
life, intellectual pursuits, accomplishments, diet/exercise, and signicant relationships. We also examined whether these associations were mediated by some of the motives for using Facebook
identied in the literature (e.g., Bazarova & Choi, 2014; Seidman,
2013): need for validation (i.e., seeking attention and acceptance),
self-expression (i.e., disclosing personal opinions, stories, and
Corresponding author at: Division of Psychology, Department of Life Sciences,
Brunel University, Uxbridge UB8 3PH, UK. Tel.: +44 (0)1895 267096.
E-mail address: [email protected] (T.C. Marshall).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.04.039
0191-8869/ 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.
This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
36
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Data was collected from 555 Facebook users currently residing
in the United States (59% female; Mage = 30.90, SDage = 9.19). Sixtyve percent of participants were currently involved in a romantic
relationship, and 34% had at least one child. Fifty-seven percent
checked Facebook on a daily basis, and spent an average of
107.95 min per day actively using it (SD = 121.41). Ninety percent
of participants were recruited through Amazons Mechanical Turk
and paid $1.00 in compensation; the rest were recruited through
web forums for online psychology studies, and received no
compensation.
2.2. Materials and procedure
Participants completed an online survey consisting of demographic questions and the following measures. Cronbachs alpha
coefcients are reported in Table 1.
2.2.1. Big Five personality traits
The 35-item Berkeley Personality Prole (Harary & Donahue,
1994) measures extroversion, neuroticism, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness with 7 items each (1 = Strongly disagree, 5 = Strongly agree).
2.2.2. Self-esteem
The 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965)
measures self-esteem with items such as I feel that I have a number of good qualities (1 = Strongly disagree, 5 = Strongly agree).
37
2
.42
.22
.23
.29
.40
.31
.24
.15
.20
.18
.11*
.04
.14
.16
.24
.23
.18
20.94
5.89
.85
.07
.54
.36
.64
.04
.04
.03
.01
.04
.05
.05
.14
.06
.02
.02
.13
19.42
5.91
.85
.13
.15
.16
.14
.10*
.31
.18
.03
.03
.04
.01
.15
.17
.24
.01
24.79
4.83
.72
.37
.58
.04
.09*
.01
.04
.02
.01
.06
.09*
.04
.13
.12
.14
24.54
4.96
.77
.37
.21
.13
.05
.14
.04
.01
.06
.01
.02
.16
.13
.19
24.47
4.91
.76
.05
.06
.04
.07
.06
.05
.03
.12
.06
.14
.13
.10*
36.85
8.79
.92
.03
.08
.14
.19
.05
.03
.21
.14
.02
.06
.08
3.99
2.88
.73
10
.54
.62
.49
.46
.37
.43
.54
.55
.52
.12
.53
.44
.21
.10
.29
.49
.41
.55
.05
.40
.36
.33
.42
.50
.49
.49
.19
11.75
3.88
.76
8.18
3.17
.75
7.07
2.67
.80
11
.34
.14
.41
.41
.26
.31
.07
3.20
1.59
.76
12
13
.27
.30
.27
.31
.27
.20
2.46
1.17
.19*
.16*
.38
.16*
.26
3.32
1.16
14
15
16
17
.72
.52
.55
.07
.56
.64
.03
.75
.14
20.48
9.27
.85
15.79
6.88
.82
13.71
4.47
.75
18
.08
33.37
11.17
.88
10.53
11.81
2.2.3. Narcissism
The 13-item version of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory
(NPI-13; Gentile et al., 2013) is derived from the original NPI-40
(Raskin & Terry, 1988) and measures three components of trait
narcissism: need for leadership/authority, grandiose exhibitionism,
and entitlement/exploitativeness. Items are rated on a forcedchoice basis, such that one choice represents greater narcissism
and the other less. Higher scores indicate greater narcissism.
2.2.4. Facebook use
Participants reported their number of Facebook friends, how
many days of the week they check Facebook (07 days), how much
time they spend actively using it on days they check it, and how
frequently they update their Facebook status (1 = Never, 9 = 710
times a day).
2.2.5. Topics of status updates
Participants indicated how frequently they write about 20
topics in their Facebook status updates (i.e., verbal descriptions
of their status excluding photos, videos, or emoticons). These
topics were generated by the authors through laboratory group
discussions. Responses were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging
from 1 (Never) to 5 (Very often). To extract common themes across
topics, we conducted principal axis factoring with promax rotation.
This yielded four factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 that
together accounted for 57% of the total variance. Five topics loaded
on the rst factor, which reected social activities and everyday
life (my social activities, something funny that happened to me, my
everyday activities, my pets, sporting events). Four topics loaded on
the second factor, which reected intellectual themes (my views
on politics, current events, research/science, my own creative output
e.g., art, writing, research). Three topics loaded on the third factor,
which reected achievement orientation (achieving my goals, my
accomplishments, work or school). Two topics loaded on the fourth
factor, which reected diet/exercise (my exercise routine, my diet).
Several topics did not meet Tabachnik and Fidells (2007) criteria
that items must have a minimal loading of .32 on a single factor:
three items (my children, my religious beliefs, and quotations or song
lyrics) were below this threshold, and two items cross-loaded (my
38
Table 2
Standardized regression coefcients for the predictors of status update topics, motives for using Facebook, and number of likes/comments.
Predictor
variables
Frequency update
Number of friends
Sex
Age
Extraversion
Neuroticism
Openness
Conscientiousness
Agreeableness
Self-esteem
Narcissism
R2
.42
.12**
.14**
.19
.05
.06
.12**
.02
.07
.03
.14**
.35
.27
.02
.04
.02
.11
.03
.02
.01
.02
.11
.17**
.14
.30
.07
.04
.12*
.11
.09
.04
.06
.04
.17*
.06
.14
.30
.03
.19*
.19*
.02
.02
.01
.23*
.10
.19
.06
.21
.48
.08
.06
.04
.04
.01
.06
.02
.01
.13*
.13**
.31
.38
.15**
.20
.01
.14**
.05
.06
.11*
.01
.01
.02
.31
.42
.17
.08
.01
.11*
.10
.12*
.11*
.02
.01
.02
.32
.09*
.29
.17**
.03
.07
.01
.05
.07
.06
.07
.15**
.21
p < .10.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
(criterion variables), the four motives for using Facebook (mediating variables), and the number of likes and comments received to a
typical update (criterion variable). Predictors included several control variables (frequency of updating ones status, number of
Facebook friends, sex, age) and the traits of interest (Big Five traits,
self-esteem, narcissism). We conducted bootstrap tests of multiple
mediation using Preacher and Hayess (2008) SPSS script to assess
whether the motives for using Facebook mediated the associations
of the personality traits with updating about certain topics. In
these tests, the control variables and other personality traits were
entered as covariates, and the four motives for using Facebook
were entered as multiple mediators.
3.1. Predictors of status update topics and motives for using Facebook
Table 2 reveals support for Hypothesis 1: extraversion was positively associated with updating more frequently about social
activities and everyday life, and with using Facebook to communicate. A further regression analysis showed that the use of Facebook
to communicate predicted the frequency of updating about social
activities and everyday life over and above the control variables
and other personality traits (b = .25, p < .0001). Examination of
the 95% bias-corrected condence intervals (CI) from 1000 bootstrap samples revealed that the positive association of extraversion
with updating about social activities and everyday life was mediated by the use of Facebook to communicate (b = .03, p = .05 (CI:
.003.05)). These results further conrm that extraverts use
Facebook, and specically status updates, as a tool for social
engagement (Ryan & Xenos, 2011; Seidman, 2013).
Hypothesis 2 was only partially supported: neuroticism was not
associated with updating about any of the six topics or with using
Facebook for self-expression, but it was associated with using
Facebook for validation. Indeed, neurotic individuals may use
Facebook to seek the attention and support that they lack ofine
(Ross et al., 2009).
Consistent with Hypothesis 3, openness was positively associated with updating about intellectual topics, and with using
Facebook for information. A further regression analysis showed
that the use of Facebook for information and for self-expression
predicted the frequency of updating about intellectual topics over
and above the control variables and traits (b = .34, p < .0001 and
b = .22, p < .001, respectively). The bootstrap test revealed that
the positive association of openness with updating about
39
4. Conclusions
Taken together, these results help to explain why some
Facebook friends write status updates about the party they went
to on the weekend whereas others write about a book they just
read or about their job promotion. It is important to understand
why people write about certain topics on Facebook insofar as the
response they receive may be socially rewarding or exclusionary.
Greater awareness of how ones status updates might be perceived
by friends could help people to avoid topics that annoy more than
they entertain.
References
Amichai-Hamburger, Y., & Vinitzky, G. (2010). Social network use and personality.
Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 12891295.
Bazarova, N. N., & Choi, Y. H. (2014). Self-disclosure in social media: Extending the
functional approach to disclosure motivations and characteristics on social
network sites. Journal of Communication, 64, 635657.
Buffardi, L. E., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Narcissism and social networking websites.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 13031314.
Buss, D. M., & Chiodo, L. M. (1991). Narcissistic acts in everyday life. Journal of
Personality, 59, 179215.
Campbell, L. C., & Marshall, T. C. (2011). Anxious attachment and relationship
processes: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Personality, 79, 917947.
Carpenter, C. J. (2012). Narcissism on Facebook: Self-promotional and antisocial
behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 482486.
Correa, T., Hinsley, A. W., & de Zuniga, H. G. (2010). Who interacts on the Web? The
intersection of users personality and social media use. Computers in Human
Behavior, 26, 247253.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways ve factors are basic. Personality and
Individual Differences, 13, 653665.
Davenport, S. W., Bergman, S. M., Bergman, J. Z., & Fearrington, M. E. (2014). Twitter
versus Facebook: Exploring the role of narcissism in the motives and usage of
different social media platforms. Computers in Human Behavior, 32, 212220.
Emery, L. F., Muise, A., Alpert, E., & Le, B. (2015). Do we look happy? Perceptions of
romantic relationship quality on Facebook. Personal Relationships, 22, 17.
Emery, L. F., Muise, A., Dix, E. L., & Le, B. (2014). Can you tell that Im in a
relationship? Attachment and relationship visibility on Facebook. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 14661479.
Forest, A. L., & Wood, J. V. (2012). When social networking is not working:
Individuals with low self-esteem recognize but do not reap the benets of selfdisclosure on Facebook. Psychological Science, 23, 295305.
Gentile, B., Miller, J. D., Hoffman, B. J., Reidy, D. E., Zeichner, A., & Campbell, W. K.
(2013). A test of two brief measures of grandiose narcissism: The Narcissistic
Personality Inventory-13 and the Narcissistic Personality Inventory-16.
Psychological Assessment, 25, 11201136.
Gosling, S. D., Augustine, A. A., Vazire, S., Holtzmann, N., & Gaddis, S. (2011).
Manifestations of personality in online social networks: Self-reported
Facebook-related
behaviors
and
observable
prole
information.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 483488.
Harary, K., & Donahue, E. (1994). Who do you think you are? San Francisco: Harper.
Hughes, D. J., Rowe, M., Batey, M., & Lee, A. (2012). A tale of two sites: Twitter vs.
Facebook and the personality predictors of social media usage. Computers in
Human Behavior, 28, 561569.
Mehdizadeh, S. (2010). Self-presentation 2.0: Narcissism and self-esteem on
Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13, 357364.
Murray, S. L., Gomillian, S., Holmes, J. G., & Harris, B. (2015). Inhibiting selfprotection in romantic relationships: Automatic partner attitudes as a resource
for low self-esteem people. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6,
173182.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for
assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior
Research Methods, 40, 879891.
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic
Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890902.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Ross, C., Orr, E. S., Sisic, M., Arseneault, J. M., Simmering, M. G., & Orr, R. (2009).
Personality and motivations associated with Facebook use. Computers in Human
Behavior, 25, 578586.
40
Ryan, T., & Xenos, S. (2011). Who uses Facebook? An investigation into the
relationship between the Big Five, shyness, narcissism, loneliness, and Facebook
usage. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 16581664.
Seidman, G. (2013). Self-presentation and belonging on Facebook: How personality
inuences social media use and motivations. Personality and Individual
Differences, 54, 402407.
Stoughton, J. W., Thompson, L. F., & Meade, A. W. (2013). Big Five personality traits
reected in job applicants social media postings. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
Social Networking, 16, 800805.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston:
Allyn and Bacon.
Tobin, S. J., Vanman, E. J., Verreynne, M., & Saeri, A. K. (2015). Threats to belonging
on Facebook: Lurking and ostracism. Social Inuence, 10, 3142.
Vazire, S., Naumann, L. P., Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Portrait of a
narcissist: Manifestations of narcissism in physical appearance. Journal of
Research in Personality, 42, 14391447.