Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programs Czilia Loibl The Ohio State University Emily Haisley formerly of Yale University Lauren Jones Cornell University George Loewenstein Carnegie Mellon University Preliminary findingsJuly 2012 T hough saving is important to many families in poverty, it can appear to be an insurmountable task in the context of a day-to-day struggle to make ends meet. Additionally, there are a host of psychological factors (which affect all income groups) that impede wealth development and seem almost designed to sabotage the motivation to save. Hyperbolic time discounting leads to present-biased preferences that weigh the pleasures of current consumption over the pleasures of deferred consumption. Procrastination and naivety about this tendency contribute to peoples belief that they will save tomorrow even if they are not saving today. Loss aversion, first proposed by Kahneman and Tversky (1979), suggests that people weigh the out-of-pocket loss of consumption more heavily than an equivalent future gain in consumption provided by matching funds. The IDA program interventions of this research project have been designed with these factors in mind to help participants overcome these barriers to saving success. Experiment 1: The importance of timing Myopic decision making refers to the tendency to make individual decisions without considering the aggregate consequences of making the same decision multiple times. The peanuts effect describes the undervaluing of small dollar amountsboth gains and losses (Markowitz 1952; Prelec and Loewenstein 1991; Weber and Chapman 2005). Taken together, these two behavioral regularities account for the popularity of rent-to-own agreements, where small payments are made over an extended period of time, even though the final sum is higher than the one-payment retail price. A way to implement this insight into the design of IDA programs is to increase the frequency of deposits by changing the deposit schedule from the typical monthly schedule to a twice-monthly schedule. An additional benefit of having a twice-monthly deposit schedule is that it imposes more deadlines to save and counters procrastination. Procrastination can undermine even the best intentions to save. Since procrastination decreases as deadlines near, we would expect reductions in consumption to be greatest close to the deposit deadline. Thus, imposing more frequent deadline may help to decrease overall consumption. H1: Savings deposits will be higher when deposit goals are twice-monthly compared to monthly. Experiment 2: The importance of accountability Accountability has been shown to affect numerous psychological phenomena, such as social perception, behavior attribution, attitude formation and change, and consumer preference (see Lerner and Tetlock 1999 for review). Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 2 Typical IDA program structure does little to hold participants accountable for making savings deposits. Although case managers share IDA bank statements quarterly, feedback is random and frequently delayed by several weeks. We propose utilizing an automated phone system to provide frequent and timely feedback. Operating in much the same way as a credit card or bank system, the phone system provides IDA savers with a short, motivational message before and/or after the deposit deadline. This simple manipulation increases the salience of accountability on all four of the dimensions outlined by Lerner and Tetlock (1999): (1) the expectation of being observed; (2) identifiability; (3) the expectation that performance will be assessed by another; and (4) the expectation that one will have to give reasons for actions. H2: Savings deposits will be greater when accountability is high. Experiment 3: The effect of probabilistic incentives Empirical findings demonstrate that people do not treat probabilities linearly when making decisions under uncertainty, but rather systematically overweigh small probabilities (Gonzalez and Wu 1999; Kahneman and Tversky 1979). This bias accounts for the willingness to pay large premiums in state lotteries and insurance, both of which involve overweighing the small probability of a sudden large increase or decrease in wealth, respectively. By extension, lottery-linked incentives to save will be overvalued, producing a greater bang for the buck compared to guaranteed incentives, such as the IDA match, given identical net payouts. Additionally, lottery-based incentives introduce entertainment and suspense to the routine of savings. Such incentives have been utilized by commercial banks and microfinance institutions outside of the U.S. to encourage low-income savings (Guilln and Tschoegl 2002; Ashraf et al. 2003). In a typical arrangement, monthly drawings are held for cash and prizes, and customers get one lottery ticket for every $X they have on deposit for the duration of the month. These accounts typically draw customers from the lower end of the income distribution and frequently have reduced interest rates to offset the costs of prizes. In a sense, the lottery distributes a portion of the interest. A similar structure is implemented for the IDA program, where a portion of participant is distributed by a lottery that is linked to participant deposits. Lottery odds are structured so that net winnings, coupled with the guaranteed match, equal traditional match rates. The automated phone system is utilized to notify lottery winners as well as provide motivational encouragement to continue saving. H3: Savings deposits will be greater when the match rate is partially distributed by a lottery compared to when the match rate is fixed. Experiment 4: The effect of increasing sequences This experiment responds to the finding that people like increasing sequences (Loewenstein and Prelec 1993). Understanding choices between sequences is an important aspect of intertemporal decision making. We examine whether savers differ in their attitudes about the future with this small change to the savings program. H4: Savings deposits will be greater when the match rate is increasing over time in the savings program compared to when the match rate is fixed. Our test bed is the Individual Development Account (IDA) program, a matched savings program. The federal government established this program in 1998 with the Assets for Independence Act. It provided funds for community-based savings programs to match, most commonly, $2 for every dollar saved (acf.hhs.gov/ programs/ocs/afi). The Assets for Independence (AFI) program is appropriating $24 million annually, funding the programs implementation at nearly 2,000 community organizations across the nation. Since program inception in 1998, more than 70,000 matched savings accounts, so called Individual Development Accounts or IDAs, were opened through AFI program, which is the largest federally funded IDA program. Previous studies Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 3 have shown that successful program graduates are more likely to continue saving, accumulate assets, and establish retirement and investment accounts (Loibl et al. 2010; Loibl and Red Bird 2009; Loibl, Kraybill, and DeMay 2011). Taken together, these interventions result in three minor adjustments to current IDA programs: 1. Adjustment of deposit timing to weekly, monthly, or quarterly, 2. Implementation of a phone system through which participants receive notification of upcoming deadlines and lottery winnings, and 3. Redistribution of match money as the match rate is determined, in part, by a lottery or a changing match rate. 2. EXPERIMENTAL RESULTS 2.1 Frequency of deposits Experiment 1 about the importance of timing savings deposits was successful. Savers who were asked to make two deposits per month, every other week, save about one-third more than those who continued with the regular monthly deposit schedule ($510 vs. $372). In addition, regression analysis shows that the amount of each deposit is significantly higher for the twice-monthly savers. They tend to save about $42 per month, while the monthly savers only save about $24. At our two research sites for this intervention, the savers were in their mid-thirties, the majority African- American, full-time working women, many of whom had engaged in post-secondary education. On average, at least one child was in every household, but many were single-adult headed. The average household income was in the low $20,000s. Cumulative, average savings deposits since consent, over 28 months, including trend lines The graph illustrates the growing divide between those who were asked to deposit twice per month (red line) vs. the monthly savers (blue line). The average, cumulative savings of the twice-monthly savers were $510, compared to $372 of the monthly savers. We collected data at our two sites for 22 months. Note: Treatment: Reminder & accountability call at 15th and 30th of each month; Control: Reminder & accountability call at 30th of each month; N=101 Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 4 2.2 Accountability Our findings from our second experiment document the effectiveness of a simple program intervention: reminder and accountability calls at deposit deadline. We have several interesting results: Descriptive analysis shows that reminder calls work best. Savers who receive these short, motivating calls a few days prior to the deposit deadline save an average of $848 during 24 months of data collection. Counterproductive effects of the accountability call render the combination reminder & accountability call somewhat ineffective. Savers in this condition saved about as much as those who received no calls, when adding up savings deposits. Regression analysis indicates that savers who receive any combination of reminder and accountability calls are more likely to make a deposit and tend to deposit more on each deposit deadline than those who do not receive calls. Savers in the treatment groups save about $54 per month, while no-call group participants only save at a rate of about $32 per month. At our four research sites for this intervention, the saver demographics were similar to typical IDA programs and our two increasing frequency research sites. Savers were in their mid-thirties, the majority African- American, full-time working women, many of whom had engaged in post-secondary education. On average, at least one child was in every household, but many were single-adult headed. The average household income was in the low $20,000s. Cumulative, average savings deposits since consent, over 28 months, including trend lines The graph illustrates the growing divide between those who received reminder calls (red line) and no calls (light blue line) or reminder-and-accountability calls (dark blue line). We collected data at our four sites from 16 to 28 months. Note: Treatment 2: Reminder & accountability call at 30th of each month; Treatment 1: Reminder call at 30th of each month; Control: Standard savings program, no calls; 86 savers; N = 335 Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 5 2.3 Lottery As expected, the lottery intervention was very popular among savers; however, the we cannot yet identify differences in savings outcomes. The descriptive analysis shows that the lottery group and the control group deposit similar amounts of savings, $1,902 and $2,072 or about $65 and $70 per month, respectively. We find, however, that savers in the lottery condition are more likely to deposit regularly, even accounting for their bi-monthly deposit deadline; an average of 14 deposits compared to 11 deposits during the data collection period. This finding supports the motivating nature of the lottery intervention. Our research site for the lottery intervention reflects the characteristics of Hispanic families in Los Angeles. Savers were in their early forties, the majority Hispanic, full-time and part-time working women, many of whom had engaged in post-secondary education. On average, at least one child was in every household in traditional two-parent households. The average household income of $15,000 was below the federal poverty guidelines, which is $19,090 for a family of three. Cumulative, average savings deposits since consent, over 39 months, including trend lines: The graph illustrates that savings deposits between lottery group and control group are very similar. In fact, the control group (blue line) accumulated slightly higher savings than the lottery group (red line). Note: Guaranteed match: $1 match for $1 saved; additional 1 in 5 chance to receive $3 match for $1 saved; additional 1 in 100 change to receive $15 match for $1 saved; twice-monthly deposit deadline on 15th and 30th of month; reminder and accountability call at each deposit deadline; N=79 2.4 Increasing match With only 12 months of data collection, the positive effect of an increasing match rate is already becoming visible. Savings deposit data show that those who will be switched from a 2:1 to a 4:1 match rate halfway to their savings goal feel already encouraged to save. After only 12 months of data collection, when only few savers have actually experienced the match increase, we see a slight effect of the increasing-match intervention on the amount saved in the IDA program. Those in the treatment group saved an average of $595 since consent, compared to $569 for the control group. Regression analysis confirms that participants in the increasing-match rate condition tend to deposit more in any given month, although they are not more likely to make their deposits. Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 6 At our research site for this intervention, the saver demographics reflect the characteristics of low-income families in Oregon. Savers were in their late thirties, the majority white or Hispanic, full-time and part-time working women, many of whom had engaged in post-secondary education. On average, at least one child was in every household, but many were single-adult headed. The average household income of the savers was not shared by this site. Cumulative, average savings deposits since consent, over 12 months, including trend lines: The graph illustrates that the prospect of a 4:1 savings match in the future (red line) encourages higher savings deposits, almost right from the start of the program as compared to the control group who receives the standard 3:1 savings match. Note: Treatment group: Reminder call at the end of each month and a match rate that doubles from $2:$1 to $4:$1 when half of the savings goal is reached, 74 IDA participants; Control group: Reminder calls at the end of each month, 34 IDA participants; N=74 3. INSIGHTS FROM PHONE INTERVIEWS WITH STUDY PARTICIPANTS We contacted study participants at our four longest-running sites to inquire about their experience with the new features of the IDA program. This is a practice common in experimental field research because it informs the data analysis and helps explain the quantitative findings. We were able to conduct the interview with 133 savers on the phone (42% response rate, N=314). 1) What did you like about participating in the IDA program? We started the interviews by inquiring about their overall satisfaction with the IDA program. Key responses, listed in the following paragraph, indicate the many positive experiences of savings program participants that have been described in the literature extensively (Sherraden 2000; Sherraden et al. 2004): the generous match rate, the intensive financial education, the welcoming community of fellow savers, the contact to professionals, and the one-on-one financial management training and credit repair. Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 7 It gave me the chance, for the first time, to save money and not go into it if situations came up. It was a backup. Normally, if you have a savings account, you dip into it when things get tight. IDA doesnt allow you to do that, so it was good. The wonderful opportunity it presents to a person in my situation who needs help to make something out of their lives. Being a single mom and an entrepreneur, it wasnt adding up for me. IDA helped me to do both. It was hard for me to have funds to be a good provider and pursue my dreams as an entrepreneur. I couldnt do both. IDA helped me get the money to start a business. I liked the information about banking and how to get my credit together. It was very useful. I was able to do research and send letters to repair my credit. I am working on getting some loans and they gave me some useful information for that. I liked everything about it. I have it set up so it comes out of payroll directly. It is continuous. Love the matching, saving to buy a home. They offer workshops and provide information. One of the workshops guys provided information on insurance and that was very helpful talking about homeowners insurance. Another workshop lady there talked about banking. 2) What did you not like about participating in the IDA program? Our second question in the interview inquired about the challenges of savings program participation. The federal funding of the program imposes a number of rigorous elements, such as the uses of the funds (home purchase, post-secondary education, small business development), at least 10 hrs. of financial education, or program termination after three missed deposits. Below are selected responses to this question. If you miss a month or two months you are out of the program. It would have been nice if the matching could have been used to pay off credit card debt. I realize that may go against what the program is about, but I got a jump on savings that I didnt have before. The class hours are difficult to fit with my work hours. 3) What prevented you from saving as much as you might have liked? The third question in the interviews was directed toward the actual savings behavior and the challenges associated with making the recommended monthly deposit. The responses reflected the many demands on tight budgets of low-income families. Responses include: There was always a bill to pay; I didnt always think I had an option to save. My car went out on me and I had to spend money on that. I was spending money the wrong way. They taught me how to save but I made wrong choices. 4) Did you have other pressing needs that prevented you from saving as much as you would have liked? The follow-up question addressed the topic of unexpected, unplanned expenses. They are the main reason for program dropout, which is especially high during Christmas time. About two-third of program families have children and they need the savings for Christmas gifts, a topic that was addressed many times during the interviews. Responses include: Christmas shopping hurt my savings. I am trying to pay several debts such as mortgage and credit card debt. Normal bills, groceries, and stuff for school. Unexpected things that came up, sometimes irresponsibility and overspending. 5) How did family/friends help/made it more difficult? Many low-income families rely on family and friends for providing financial assistance in emergency situations, which has been documented regularly in the federal Survey of Consumer Finances analysis and Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 8 reports (Fisher and Montalto 2011; Bucks et al. 2009). On the other hand, family and friends may be skeptical about the savings program and hesitant to use mainstream banking services. Responses to this question include: My brother provided a good example as far as savings and my mother reminded me how important it is not to have credit card debt. I have two sons. If I wasnt able to buy something they wanted, like a game or a bike, they understood. They understood the goal. They saw that I needed to save money so if they had extra money they would help me buy things that I needed. Skepticism. No one believed the matching; they thought I was participating in a scam. 6) What was good about lottery and/or calls? After the introductory, warm-up questions about the IDA program and savings behavior, we asked about our interventions. The first in this series of three questions inquired about the positive experiences with the calls and/or the lottery. The question was worded to match the particular treatment group at each site. Savers responded in the following ways: With the lottery match, there was a possibility of getting extra money. The calls kept me alert. The lottery match is like a light at the end of the tunnel. It motivates me to stay in the program. I doubt that I would have been able to keep with it if there wasnt that incentive. It kept it fresh in my mind, and making small deposits over an amount of time gave me more of a chance to win the lottery. They were simple and to the point. They come a few days before the end of the month, so you have time to make the deposit. 7) What was bad about lottery and/or calls? The second question in this series of three questions inquired about the features of the interventions that didnt work for the savers. Complaints addressed the number of calls to be overwhelming and the preference for other media, such as text message, letters, or a live person making the reminder call. Only two commented here about the lottery feature. Key responses to this question include: You guys called a lot. Sometimes I would get two phone calls in a day. They were only once a month. It would be helpful if it was twice a month. I would get the call at the end of the month and not be able to make the deposit right away so I would miss the deadline for the month. It could be overwhelming when you are busy, almost like a bill collector. Text message might have been better than a phone call. Additional Comments Final comments by interviewees were highly positive about the savings program: I think it is a really great idea. You are not just handing money to people; you are giving them some financial structure. I love it, it is so helpful. I think its a great thing that helps people from lower incomes to get a piece of the American dream. The matching savings are a godsend. That is the reason I dont have any business loans. I was able to save enough with the help of the matching. Thank the people that provide the program. The educational component is extremely important. It is valuable to understand the economics, and just the financial literacy part of it is as important as the money because knowledge is power. It makes you think and gives you the tools that are needed. Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 9 4. CONCLUSIONS This project addresses the predictors of saving in the lives of low-income families in several novel and significant ways. First, this research presents the first controlled, systematic study of how savings program characteristics affect outcomes for low-income savers. Our approach to collecting data over multiple years, from the date when people entered the savings program until its completion, provides us with an understanding of how savings behavior develops over time and how it responds to our interventions. As a result, our data provide robust predictions of long-term savings behavior, as compared to shorter-term studies that use similar features in the savings context. Second, it helps us better assess the strategy of using decision biases or vices that normal undermine decision quality to, instead, improve it. The twice-monthly deposit feature tests the use of peoples tendencies to underweight small dollar amounts as a tactic to increase savings. Similarly, the lottery interventions utilizes the tendency to underweight small probabilities. The reminder and accountability calls shed light on the importance of accountability as a psychological process that facilitates savings. Further, the design of these three experiments allows us to parse out the relative contribution of each psychological mechanism. Finally, it is the first research to develop best practice recommendations for managing and designing IDA programs based on concepts of behavioral economics. Once data collection is completed in spring 2013, the analysis will allow us to draw robust, causal inferences regarding the effect of each of our interventions. Field experimentation is critical here, not only to ensure that an intervention is effective but also to safeguard against unintended consequences before results are implemented on a wider scale. OUR PARTNER SITES The Community Financial Resource Center (CFRC) This agency in the S. Figueroa corridor of Los Angeles is partnering with the United Way of Greater Los Angeles for offering the IDA program (www.cfrcla.org). CFRC is a single-agency provider of IDAs, serving a mostly Hispanic neighborhood in Los Angeles. Contact: Lindsay Moore, United Way of Greater Los Angeles and Veronica Lopez, CFRC CASA of Oregon (CASA) Located in Sherwood, Oregon, at the outskirts of Portland (www.casaoforegon.org), CASA is one of the nations largest, statewide provider of IDAs. It serves the state of Oregon with its so-called Valley Individual Development Accounts (VIDA) Program, which is offered through over 40 partner agencies across Oregon. Contact: Maggie Reilly Capital Area Asset Builders (CAAB) This large, single-agency IDA agency is located in the center of Washington, D.C. (www.caab.org). It is serving the working poor of the D.C. area with its matched savings programs. Contact: Miriam Savad Convenant Community Capital Corporation (Convenant) This large, single-agency IDA program is located in the heart of Houston, Texas (www.covenantcapital.org). It serves the Houston area with its so-called Smart-Savings Program. Contact: Heather Daron Kentucky Domestic Violence Association (KDVA) A multi-site IDA agency serving Kentucky (www.kdva.org), about 20 of their network agencies participated in the research project. These agencies provide programs and housing for victims of domestic violence. The IDA program is part of its Economic Justice Project. Contact: Michelle Fiore Beyond Housing This agency is located in the heart of St. Louis, Missouri (www.beyondhousing.org) and is one of the sites implementing the IDA program for the United Way of Greater St. Louis. Contact: Debbie Irwin, United Way of Greater St. Louis and Eric Zegel, Beyond Housing Copyright 2012, The Ohio State University Testing Strategies to Increase Saving and Retention in IDA Programspage 10 Ohio State University Extension embraces human diversity and is committed to ensuring that all research and related educational programs are available to clientele on a nondiscriminatory basis without regard to age, ancestry, color, disability, gender identity or expression, genetic information, HIV/AIDS status, military status, national origin, race, religion, sex, sexual orientation, or veteran status. This statement is in accordance with United States Civil Rights Laws and the USDA. Keith L. Smith, Associate Vice President for Agricultural Administration; Associate Dean, College of Food, Agricultural, and Environmental Sciences; Director, Ohio State University Extension; and Gist Chair in Extension Education and Leadership. For Deaf and Hard of Hearing, please contact Ohio State University Extension using your preferred communication (e-mail, relay services, or video relay services). Phone 1-800-750-0750 between 8 a.m. and 5 p.m. EST Monday through Friday. Inform the operator to dial 614-292-6181. Visit Ohio State University Extensions web site Ohioline at: http://ohioline.osu.edu Connecticut Department of Labor (CT DOL) The Connecticut Department of Labor is administering the IDA program for the state of Connecticut. It implements the Connecticut IDA Initiative and the Housing Trust Fund IDA Initiative in Connecticut. CTE, responsible for the Clearinghouse function for the Department of Labor, provides technical assistance and training to program operators. Six partner agencies implemented our research program. Contact: Lisa Arends, Connecticut Department of Labor and Marie Hawe, CTE Oakland Livingston Human Service Agency (OLHSA) Located in Pontiac, Michigan (www.olhsa.org), OLHSA is a network agency provider of IDAs. It is implementing the IDA program for Oakland and Livingston County communities in Michigan. Contact: Heidi Henderson REFERENCES Ashraf, Nava, N. Gons, Dean S. Karlan, and Wesley Yin. 2003. A review of commitment savings products in developing countries. Asian Development Bank: Economics and Research Department Working Paper Series 45:137. Bucks, Brian K., Arthur B. Kennickell, Traci L. Mach, and Kevin B. Moore. 2009. Changes in U.S. family finances from 2004 to 2007: Evidence from the Survey of Consumer Finances. Federal Reserve Bulletin 95 (February):A1A56. Fisher, Patti J., and Catherine P. Montalto. 2011. Loss aversion and saving behavior: Evidence from the 2007 U.S. Survey of Consumer Finances. Journal of Family and Economic Issues 32 (1):414. Gonzalez, Richard, and George Wu. 1999. On the shape of the probability weighting function. Cognitive Psychology 38 (129166). Guilln, Mauro F., and Adrian E. Tschoegl. 2002. Banking on gambling: Banks and lottery-linked deposit accounts. Journal of Financial Services Research 21 (3):219231. Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47 (2):263291. Lerner, Jennifer S., and Philip E. Tetlock. 1999. Accounting for the effects of accountability. Psychological Bulletin 125 (2):255275. Loewenstein, George E., and Drazen Prelec. 1993. Preferences for Sequences of Outcomes. Psychological Review 100 (1):91108. Loibl, Czilia, Michal Grinstein-Weiss, Min Zhan, and Beth Red Bird. 2010. More than a penny saved: Long-term changes in behavior among savings program participants. Journal of Consumer Affairs 44 (Spring):98126. Loibl, Czilia, David S. Kraybill, and Sara Wackler DeMay. 2011. Accounting for the role of habit in regular saving. Journal of Economic Psychology 32:581592. Loibl, Czilia, and Beth Red Bird. 2009. Survey of former IDA program participants: How do they fare? Journal of Extension 47 (6):6RIB3. Markowitz, Harry M. 1952. The utility of wealth. The Journal of Political Economy 60:151158. Prelec, Drazen, and George Loewenstein. 1991. Decision making over time and under uncertainty: A common approach. Management Science 37:770786. Sherraden, Margaret S., Amanda Moore McBride, Stacie Hanson, and Lissa Johnson. 2004. Short-term and long-term savings in low-income households: Evidence from Individual Development Accounts. Journal of Income Distribution 13 (3/4):7697. Sherraden, Michael. 2000. From research to policy: Lessons from Individual Development Accounts. Journal of Consumer Affairs 34 (Winter):159181. Weber, Bethany J., and Gretchen B. Chapman. 2005. Playing for peanuts: Why is risk seeking more common for low-stakes gambles? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 97 (1):3146. This research was supported by a grant from the FINRA Investor Education Foundation. All results, interpretations and conclusions expressed are those of the authors alone, and do not necessarily represent the views of the FINRA Investor Education Foundation or any of its affiliated companies. No portion of this work may be reproduced, cited, or circulated without the express written permission of the authors.