Identifying Information That Promotes Belt-Positioning Booster Use
Identifying Information That Promotes Belt-Positioning Booster Use
Identifying Information That Promotes Belt-Positioning Booster Use
1. Report No.
Identifying Information That Promotes Belt-Positioning Booster Use Volume I: Summary and Findings
7. Authors
July 2008
6. Performing Organization Code
The Center for Injury Research and Prevention The Childrens Hospital of Philadelphia 34th and Civic Center Blvd. Philadelphia, PA 19104
12. Sponsoring Agency Name and Address
DTNH22-01-C-05845
13. Type of Report and Period Covered
Office of Behavioral Safety Research National Highway Traffic Safety Administration 1200 New Jersey Avenue SE. Washington, DC 20590
15. Supplementary Notes 16. Abstract
Many parents with low educational attainment prematurely graduate their children to seat belt restraint rather than use belt-positioning booster seats. This study aimed to identify interventions that promoted booster seat use among this population. Focus groups were used to elicit factors contributing to booster seat nonuse, which informed subsequent intervention development. A first phase (12 focus groups, n=107) identified parents perceived barriers, benefits, and threats relating to booster seats. These findings were used to identify existing and create new interventions. A second phase (16 focus groups, n=142) elicited parents reactions to these interventions and provided parents with belt-positioning booster seats and education on their use. Lack of education and fear of injury were the primary barriers to booster seat use. Parents were motivated by interventions that provided clear, concrete messaging relating to use. Parents favored the intervention that presented a real story detailing a childs severe injury that could have been prevented with appropriate restraint. At follow-up, parents credited this intervention with motivating booster seat use most often. Although parents cited their childrens lack of comfort and noncompliance as barriers to use, they were not as motivated by interventions that addressed these barriers. Effective intervention programs can be created by identifying and addressing factors that contribute to a populations intention to use belt-positioning booster seats. In addition, successful programs must use messages that motivate the target population by addressing their perceived threats to booster seat nonuse.
17. Key Words 18. Distribution Statement
motor vehicle safety, child safety seat, booster seat, qualitative research, focus groups
19. Security Classif. (of this report)
This report is free of charge from the NHTSA Web site at www.nhtsa.dot.gov
21. No. of Pages 22. Price
Unclassified
Unclassified
Acknowledgements
We would like to acknowledge Debra Dean and Teresa Koenig of Westat, Inc., for expert conduct of focus groups within the project, and Mary Aitken of the Little Rock Injury Free Coalition for Kids (IFCK), Michael Gittelman of the Cincinnati IFCK, and Benjamin Selassie of the Baltimore IFCK for their efforts in recruiting for the focus groups. We would also like to acknowledge Alexandra Winski for executing the follow-up interviews and Rachel Cohen for analyzing data for this project.
ii
Glossary
BPB CHOP CPS CRS FARS IFCK NASS NHTSA PAPM PCPS PI PIF PSA BB&T
Belt-Positioning Booster Seat The Childrens Hospital of Philadelphia Child Passenger Safety Child Restraint System Fatality Analysis Reporting System Injury Free Coalition for Kids National Automotive Sampling System National Highway Traffic Safety Administration Precaution Adoption Process Model Partners for Child Passenger Safety Principal Investigator Participant Information Form Public Service Announcement Barriers, Beliefs, and Threats
iii
Executive Summary
Motor vehicle crashes remain a leading cause of injury among children in the United States (NHTSA, 2006). Use of appropriate restraints in motor vehicles is an effective strategy for reducing the risk of injury and death to child passengers in a motor vehicle crash (Arbogast, Durbin, Kallan, & Winston, 2003; Elliott, Kallan, Durbin, & Winston, 2006; NHTSA, 2003). Although child restraint use for children under 8 years old in the United States has increased since 1998 (Arbogast, Durbin, Kallan, & Winston, 2004), children between the ages of 4 and 8 years continue to be at highest risk for inappropriate restraint by seat belts alone (Fact and Trend Report, 2006). Among this age group, children of parents with a high school education or less were 27% more likely to be inappropriately restrained than those of parents with higher educational attainment (Winston, 2006). The reasons for low appropriate restraint use among these at-risk populations were unclear. In order to inform future efforts to increase belt-positioning booster seat (BPB) use among this population, this study focused on identifying reasons for booster seat nonuse for children (age 4 through 8) of parents with a high school education or less. While children 4 through 8 were the target population for booster seat use, parents were recruited into the study who had children as young as age 3, as their children would presumably be making the transition from a child restraint into a booster seat in the near future. The Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1991) formed the theoretical foundation for the study. According to Ajzen and Fishbein, behavior is preceded by a positive intention to perform the behavior, which, in turn, is informed by the perceived benefits, barriers, and threats to performing that behavior. According to this theory, to promote booster seat use it is necessary to encourage positive intentions toward booster seat use. This can be done by overcoming the parents perceived barriers to booster seat use, highlighting parents perceived benefits to booster seat use, and reducing parents perceived threats to booster seat use. Therefore, this research aimed to: 1. identify factors that influence parents current child restraint use behaviors and intentions for future use, and 2. test interventions that address these factors as a means to promote appropriate restraint use behaviors, particularly the use of booster seats. This multi-site study used focus groups to elicit contributing factors to booster seat nonuse, which informed subsequent intervention development. A first phase (12 focus groups, n=107) identified parents perceived barriers, benefits, and threats relating to belt-positioning booster seats. These findings were used to identify existing and create new interventions. A second phase (16 focus groups, n=142) elicited parents reactions to these interventions and provided parents with beltpositioning booster seats and education on their use. Lack of education and fear of injury were the primary barriers to belt-positioning booster seat use. Parents were motivated by interventions that provided them with clear, concrete messaging relating to use. Parents favored the intervention that presented a real story detailing a childs severe injury that could have been prevented with appropriate restraint. At follow-up, parents credited this intervention with motivating booster seat use most often. Although parents cited their childs lack of
iv
comfort and noncompliance as barriers to use, they were not as motivated by interventions that addressed these barriers. Effective intervention programs can be created by identifying and addressing factors that contribute to a populations intention to use belt-positioning booster seats. In addition, successful programs must use messages that motivate the target population by addressing their perceived threats to booster seat nonuse.
vi
Table of Contents
1. Introduction ........................................................................................... 1
1.1 Study background ..............................................................................................................1 1.2 Literature review: Previously identified at-risk parent driver populations........................1 1.3 Literature review: Identification of at-risk child passenger populations ...........................3 1.4. Study goals .......................................................................................................................4
2. Methods .................................................................................................. 5
2.1 Theoretical foundation for methods...................................................................................5 2.2 Phase 1: Formative Research .............................................................................................9 2.3 Phase 2: Development......................................................................................................14 2.4 Phase 3: Evaluation..........................................................................................................19 2.5 Follow-up interviews: Assessing behavior change..........................................................26 2.6 Additional Considerations ...............................................................................................27
3. Results .................................................................................................. 28
3.1 Phase 1: Formative Research ...........................................................................................28 3.2 Phase 3: Evaluation..........................................................................................................35
4. Discussion ............................................................................................ 45
4.1 Key finding: Lack of knowledge as the primary barrier to use .......................................45 4.2 Using perceived threats to create motivation toward behavior change ...........................45 4.3 Sending strong, educational messages through targeted channels and media .................46 4.4 Designing interventions for targeted at-risk populations.................................................46 4.5 Additional findings ..........................................................................................................47 4.6 Study limitations ..............................................................................................................48 4.7 Future work......................................................................................................................48 4.8 Implications......................................................................................................................48
5. References ........................................................................................... 49
vii
1. Introduction
1.1 Study background
Motor vehicle crashes remain a leading cause of death and acquired disability among children in the United States (NHTSA, 2006). Use of appropriate child restraints is an effective strategy for reducing the risk of injury and death to child passengers in a motor vehicle crash (Elliott et al., 2006; NHTSA, 2003; Arbogast et al., 2003). Appropriate restraint use is categorized into four stages or steps, based on the weight and height of the child passenger (NHTSA, 2005): Step One: Rear-facing infant child safety seats from birth until children have reached age 1 and are at least 20 pounds; Step Two: Forward-facing child safety seats for children at least 20 pounds to at least 40 pounds; Step Three: Belt-positioning booster seats for all children who have outgrown their child safety seats until they are 49 tall; Step Four: Seat belts for children taller than 49.
While seat belts are better than no restraint at all, appropriate restraint use along with riding in the rear seat have been found to decrease the risk of injury for children under age 12 (Durbin, Chen, Smith, Elliott, & Winston, 2005); more specifically, booster seats have been demonstrated to reduce the risk of consequential injuries by 59% for 4- to 7-year-old children when compared to seat belt restraint. Booster seats virtually eliminated seat belt syndrome in these children (Arbogast et al., 2003). Although child restraint use has increased significantly since 1998 (Arbogast et al., 2004) and is currently at its highest level in history (Glassbrenner, 2003), many children still travel in motor vehicles restrained inappropriately, incorrectly, or not at all. Recent data show that 72.6% of restrained children less than 80 lbs. are inappropriately or incorrectly restrained and 11.8% of child passengers less than 80 lbs. are not restrained at all (Decina et al., 2005). The use of appropriate child restraint is an area where behavioral intervention for both parent drivers and child passengers is essential to increase compliance. Recent interventions have been found to change behavior pertaining to booster seat use in select populations; however, in order to facilitate this behavioral change among those least likely to use appropriate restraints further action must be taken.
Hanfling et al., 2000; Agran et al., 1998; Russell et al., 1994; Margolis et al., 1992). Children 4 to 8 years old, in particular, represent a challenging population in regard to child restraint. Many of these children have outgrown child safety seats designed for younger passengers and frequently ride unrestrained or are prematurely placed in adult seat belt systems (Winston, 2006; Decina & Lococo, 2004; Ramsey et al., 2000; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2000; Decina & Knoebel, 1997). As of 2000, 83% of children 3 to 8 years old are prematurely graduated to seat belts rather than being restrained in a child safety seats or belt-positioning booster seats; children age 2 to 5 who are inappropriately restrained in seat belts are 3.5 times more likely to sustain injuries, particularly head injuries, than children in child safety seats or belt-positioning booster seats (Winston et al., 2000). Data from the Partners for Child Passenger Safety (PCPS) study suggest that the use of appropriate restraints is improving. belt-positioning booster seat use among 4- to 8-year-old children, the targeted population for their use, increased significantly from 4.6% to 13% between December 1998 and December 2000, representing an annual increase of 74%. The greatest age-specific increase in use occurred among 4-year-olds, rising from 14% to 34% over the two-year period, representing an annual increase of 80% (Durbin et al., 2001). More recent data indicate appropriate restraint use continues to be on the rise, and parents are becoming ever more aware of the dangers associated with placing children under age 13 in the front seat of a motor vehicle. From 1999 through 2005, use of appropriate restraint increased for all children through the age of 8 from 51% to 73%. In addition, during the same time period, appropriate restraint use among children age 4 to 8 tripled from 15% to 54%. Specifically, beltpositioning booster seat use among this age group grew from 4% to 36%. Similarly, front seating for children 4 to 8 declined from 19% in 1999 to 6% in 2005 (Partners for Child Passenger Safety, 2006). In light of the high prevalence of inappropriate restraint of child passengers in motor vehicles, particularly among certain populations of children between the ages of 4 and 8, this study sought to identify ways to increase booster seat use in at-risk populations. Through the use of focus groups grounded in behavior change theory, this study defined the beliefs of these populations concerning child restraint, crashes and injuries, and the barriers the parent drivers face in implementing appropriate restraint practices. Several interventions were examined for their potential in effecting behavior change, and data were collected to identify key components necessary in child passenger safety interventions to increase belt-positioning booster seat use in the study populations.
This study targeted White, African American, and Hispanic parents with a high school education or less who routinely drive with children 3 to 8 years old (this target age range differs slightly from the traditional age range for booster seat use (4 to 8 years old); this choice is explained in Section 2.2.2.1) A first wave of focus groups was guided by a trans-theoretical behavior change model and aimed to identify beliefs about, and barriers to, appropriate restraint use. Themes culled from these focus groups were grouped into advantages to booster seat use, barriers to their use, perceived threats, and people who influence child restraint decisions. The themes informed the design and selection of interventions that address the concerns of the target audiences with the hope of leading to use of booster seats. The second wave of focus groups involved ranking the themes followed by review and discussion of interventions, focusing on which messages facilitated behavioral change. The selection of interventions was based on group rankings of themes in order to determine interventions of relevance to the group. Through this method, this study was able to identify and describe parents: child restraint use beliefs, including potential benefits of and threats incurred by using child restraint systems; child restraint use barriers; suggested intervention strategies to improve child restraint use; and insights on methods for implementation of these strategies.
As part of this study, the research team also assessed whether or not differences existed between several different populations of parent drivers. These study findings will provide a foundation for the development of primary injury risk-reduction interventions used to promote proper child restraint use behaviors among diverse populations.
2. Methods
2.1 Theoretical foundation for methods
The Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, 1991), as depicted in Figure 2.1, forms the theoretical foundation for the study design. This theory is built on the assumption that a persons intention to perform a behavior predicts his behavior and these intentions result from the persons beliefs about the behavior. The three types of beliefs that form the theory are: Beliefs about the likely outcomes of the behavior and the evaluations of these outcomes (behavioral beliefs that form favorable or unfavorable attitudes about the behavior; e.g., benefits and disadvantages); Beliefs about the normative expectations of others and motivation to comply with these expectations (normative beliefs that result in perceived social pressure to perform the behavior, or subjective norm); and Beliefs about the presence of factors that may facilitate or impede performance of the behavior and the perceived power of these factors (control beliefs about personal control or
ability to perform the behavior, also known as behavioral control; e.g., threats and barriers). Therefore, the more favorable the attitude and subjective norm and the greater the perceived control, the more positive should be the intention to perform the behavior.
Subjective Norm
Intention
Behavior
In order to apply this theory to research design, the behavior under investigation has to be clearly defined: a specific action by a specific target population under specific circumstances. For this study, the behavior under investigation was the use of booster seats for children 4 to 8 years old (action) by parents who do not consistently use booster seats for their children (population) on every trip (circumstance). (While children 4 to 8 were the target population for booster seat use, parents were recruited into the study who had children as young as 3, as their children would presumably be making the transition from child restraints into booster seats in the near future [see sections 2.2.2.1 and section 2.4.1.1]). According to the Theory of Planned Behavior, interventions that reduce perceived barriers and threats to, and enhance benefits of, booster seat use will promote positive intentions and therefore promote the consistent use of booster seats. Behavior change was further defined according to the Precaution Adoption Process Model (PAPM), used to categorize current behavior according to stages of progress towards action (Weinstein & Sandman, 1992). In this study, six stages were identified: 1) booster seat nonuse and unaware of booster seats; 2) booster seat nonuse and aware of booster seats but with no plans to use; 3) booster seat nonuse but aware of booster seats, plans to use but has not yet used one; 4) booster seat used at least once; 5) booster seat used most of the time; 6) booster seat nonuse extinguished as demonstrated by telling others about the importance of booster seats.
Therefore, this study was conducted in three phases (Figure 2.2) with the associated goals: Phase 1: Formative Research Goal 1: Define the target populations that are at highest risk for nonuse of booster seats for children. Within the target populations, define the current use and intended future use of booster seats, and the determinants of these intentions (attitudes benefits and disadvantages; subjective norm; behavioral control threats and barriers).
Goal 2:
Phase 2: Development Goal 1: Identify existing interventions and create new interventions that reduce perceived barriers and threats to and enhance benefits of booster seat use for the target populations.
Phase 3: Evaluative Research Goal 1: Elicit target population reactions to interventions and any changes in intentions to use booster seats. Measure changes in booster seat use behavior.
Goal 2:
The Institutional Review Board of The Childrens Hospital of Philadelphia approved the protocol for this study, along with all instruments and scripts.
Define Behavior
PHASE 1
PHASE 2
PHASE 3
Educational attainment: high school education or less; Household income: under $20,000; Race/ethnicity representation: diverse, including African American, White, and Hispanic populations; and Geographic representation: diverse, including suburban, urban, and rural.
would receive an honorarium for participation. Potential candidates were excluded from participating in this study if they either (1) failed to meet the inclusion criteria as set forth in section 2.2.2.1, (2) were known to have participated in a focus group within three months prior to the start of this study, or (3) were employees of one of the market research firms that were used to recruit participants for this study. Subjects were precluded from participating in more than one focus group. Qualifying individuals were assigned a focus group date, based on their restraint use, race or ethnicity and sex. These participants received follow-up telephone calls reminding them of their commitment 24 hours prior to the discussion date. Local chapters of the Injury Free Coalition for Kids (www.injuryfree.org, IFCK) partnered with the subcontractor to recruit focus group participants. For more information on IFCK see Appendix 3.1. Each local IFCK chapter provided historical information on the populations in its community and suggested specific locations for holding the sessions. IFCK personnel also assisted in distributing recruitment flyers and in advertising the focus groups in local sites. The final locations and demographics for the focus groups are shown in Table 2.1. Table 2.1: Location and demographic composition of each Phase 1 focus group Location
Prince George's County, MD (Pilot Test) Little Rock, AR
Demographic Segments
One session African American mothers Two sessions with Spanish-speaking Hispanic mothers Two sessions with African American mothers
Cincinnati, OH
Two sessions with African American mothers Two sessions with White mothers
One session with Spanish-speaking Hispanic mothers One session with Spanish-speaking Hispanic fathers One session with African American fathers
2.2.3 Phase 1 focus groups: Determining current booster seat use, intended future use, and determinants of future use
Goal 2: Define the current use of booster seats, intended future use, and determinants of intentions to use (of target populations) The Phase 1 focus groups sought to identify and describe participants current use and intended future use as well as perceived child restraint use barriers, benefits and threats. To do this, six key topics framed the discussion:
Child passenger safety messages - the prior knowledge relating to child restraint systems, received and held by the participants;
11
People who matter - the participants trusted sources of information relating to child passenger safety; Perceived barriers - the obstacles that bar participants from using child restraint systems; Perceived benefits - the advantages that promote participant child restraint systems use; Perceived threats - the concerns faced by participants when driving in a vehicle with children; Control beliefs - the extent of control the participants perceived themselves as having over selecting, accessing, and using child restraint systems for their children.
These topics are each reflected question types in the Phase 1 Moderators Guide (Appendix 1.3). The moderators guide was used as a framework for each discussion; however, the discussions were not limited to topics in the guide. This was done to ensure that each discussion was free flowing, and allowed the participants to explore the issues that were most relevant to their lifestyles. The guide broke the time allotted to the groups into five segments: the introduction, an icebreaker, discussion topics and probes, the card sort exercise, and a brief closing. The timing and order of these segments is detailed in Table 2.2. Table 2.2: Timing and flow of Phase 1 focus groups Topic
Introduction Icebreaker Discussion Topics and Probes Child's reaction CPS messages and knowledge Perceived threats and barriers Perceived benefits Prior restraint use Current restraint use Card sort exercise Summary and closing TOTAL TIME
Time Allowed
10 min 10 min 60 min 10 min 10 min 10 min 10 min 10 min 10 min 15 min 5 min
100 min
As participants arrived, a member of the subcontracted agencys staff greeted them. The Participant Consent Form (Appendix 1.4) and the Participant Information Form (PIF; Appendix 1.5) were distributed at this time. The PIF asked questions covering general demographic information (age, race, sex, etc.), as well as information specific to the participants driving behaviors. Participants were encouraged to fill out the PIF upon arrival. Once all participants had arrived, the consent form was read aloud. Participants were briefed on the format and duration of the group, and were provided the ground rules for the discussion. The discussion then began with an icebreaker. Here, participants were asked to state their names and the ages of their children. This provided a segue into the discussion topics. The discussion was divided into six topics (Childs Reaction to Restraint, CPS Messages and Knowledge, Perceived 12
Threats and Barriers, Perceived Benefits, Prior Restraint Use, and Current Restraint Use), each of which included three to seven questions. Questions in the moderators guide were chosen to elicit participants current use; intended future use; and perceived child restraint use barriers, benefits, and threats. In the discussion, questions from each topic were covered in each group; however, since these questions were used as a guide for the discussion, and not as a script, not all questions were covered in every group. Each discussion concluded with a semi-quantitative method known as card sort. The card sort was used to gain further insight into the relative importance of specific issues related to child restraints and their use (ease of use, cost, comfort, etc.). With this method, the participants choose the best combination of child restraint attributes that reflect their experience. Rather than weighing each attribute individually, this technique allows participants to provide the best description of their experience by choosing multiple attributes simultaneously. The card sort, described in the moderators guide for this phase (Appendix 1.3), asked participants to select the attributes that best described their experiences with child restraints from a list of multiple phrases. Each attribute, shown in Table 2.3, was printed on a separate card, and a set of all 24 cards was provided to each participant. The participants were asked to place the cards they selected into an envelope. The focus group moderator then collected the envelopes and conducted the remaining portion of the conversation based on the selections of the participants. The discussion following the card sort was based on the selections that the participants made. The moderator reviewed the cards that were placed in the envelopes, and questioned participants about the good and bad outcomes of using a booster seat with their child passengers. Details on these questions and all others in the Phase 1 focus groups can be found in the Moderators Guide, (Appendix 1.3). Table 2.3: Child restraint attributes listed in the card sort exercise Child Restraint Attributes Used in the Card Sort
Comfortable Safe Easy to use Light weight Easy to move to another car Cheap Good features Child can see out of window Child likes Big kid seat Keeps child under control Easy to put in car Uncomfortable Unsafe Hard to use Heavy Hard to move to another car Expensive No features Too bulky Child dislikes Baby seat Child can easily get out Hard to put in car
13
2.2.4 Analysis: Delineating relevant barriers, benefits and threats and trusted sources
The analytic goal was to generate lists of barriers, benefits, and threats to booster seat use among the target populations and delineate their trusted information sources and preferred delivery channels for messages. These lists served to inform Phases 2 and 3 of the research. After each focus group and in-depth discussion, the moderator drafted an initial summary of the findings for review by the project team (field notes that contained the key themes). The focus group discussions were transcribed for analysis; discussion groups conducted in Spanish were translated into English. Transcripts and the field notes were reviewed for common themes and new ideas that emerged. A rubric (Appendix 1.6) was constructed based on the identified themes, and organized according to the Theory of Planned Behavior, as benefits, barriers, and threats to booster seat use. One investigator then coded statements within the transcripts into the relevant thematic areas from the outline. This was done using N6 software (QSR International, Doncaster, Victoria, Australia). Once each of the transcripts was coded, a report summary generated by N6 was used to select noteworthy quotes. These selections were then included in a summary report. Single reports were generated for each demographic group in each city. All researchers who observed the focus groups reviewed the summaries and made comments, ensuring that the reports were representative of the groups. The researchers and the moderator discussed all comments to reach consensus. In situations where consensus could not be reached, all possible interpretations were included in the summary document. To supplement this transcript data, participant answers to the questions on the PIF were used to establish a demographic profile of the study population for Phase 1. For categorical variables, frequencies were calculated. For descriptive analyses of continuous variables, e.g., participant age and number of children, the mean, median, mode, and range were obtained. Descriptive analyses were also performed on the participants selections from the card sort exercise. To do this, the number of participants who selected each statement was first quantified. Data from 14 participants were removed from the data set as their card selections included statements that were contradictory (e.g., selecting both expensive and inexpensive). These modified quantities were further defined by determining the number of participants who selected each statement from each sex, race/ethnicity, and type of CRS used. Pearsons chi-square test was used to determine any significant differences between these groups (i.e., females versus males).
14
2.3.2 Identifying interventions that target relevant barriers, benefits and threats
Each intervention was presented to the research team, comprised of members from NHTSA, the IFCK, the subcontracting firm and the Childrens Hospital of Philadelphia to evaluate which interventions targeted the relevant benefits, barriers, and threats. Interventions were eliminated that had unclear or non-relevant messages. Interventions were also eliminated that used a medium that the participants in Phase 1 cited as being ineffective, such as flyers. Four interventions or intervention programs were selected for evaluation by the participants in Phase 3. Three targeted booster-seat-aged children: the IFCK of Austins The Buckleteers; the Cinderella public safety announcement by NHTSA, the Walt Disney Company, and the Ad Council; and Weiner/Seaman Productions Riding with the Big Green Snake. A fourth intervention targeted Latino parents and was created by the University of Washingtons Harborview Injury Center: the Abrocha Tu Vida campaign. Table 2.4 describes these interventions and highlights the barriers and threats they address.
2.3.3 Creating new pilot interventions to address new ideas/themes from Phase 1
New pilot interventions were created to supplement the chosen existent interventions. The goals of these new interventions were to inform, persuade, remind, and provide benefit to the target audience who received the message. Five key elements were considered in creating the interventions: the desired behavior (action), the target audience (receiver), the intended message, the source (sender) who delivers the message, and channel by which the message is delivered (Allen, 1999). For this study, the target behavior was defined as booster seat use by parents for their children age 4 to 8. The target audience was defined by determining the population of parents at highest risk for nonuse of booster seats for their children (see Section 2.2.1). The intended message and trusted sources for the message were determined from analysis of focus group discussions with the target audience in which themes emerged that encompassed issues of importance to the audience (see
15
Section 2.2.3). The source serves to translate the theme into a message that will be received well and be understood by the target audience. Given these factors, set criteria were used to explore presentation formats to deliver messages for behavior change. When completed, the intervention must: engage the target audience; hold their interest for the duration of the delivery; and inspire a desired action.
It is important to take into account that the target audience is receiving countless messages in a given day, some of which may contradict the message delivered. Therefore, an effective message should be meaningful to the population (placed in a context that is relevant), address specific barriers and threats to accomplishing the behavior, highlight benefits that are meaningful and provide practical solutions to overcoming the barriers and threats. A successful message will move an audience one step closer to achieving the desired behavior (see Section 2.1). The appeal of the message can be rational (communicate rational benefits of the behavior and assume that the target audience will make the rational decision to adopt the behavior); emotional (gain the attention of the target audience by generating emotions that will motivate them to adopt the behavior); or moral (discourage undesirable behavior and direct audiences to the right behavior). Recent research in these appeals distinguishes between the emotions that they arouse and the coping mechanisms that they mediate (Miller et al., 1995). Appeals that generate fear can paralyze the audience while those that generate guilt and regret can promote behavior change by providing the necessary coping strategies (the preventive behavior) that will avoid the guilt and regret. The channel chosen for message delivery was the one that best targets and reaches the target population. Television, radio, and word-of-mouth were most often cited by the target audiences. See Section 3.1.2.2 for more results about the channels for safety information described by the target audiences. Three messages were identified as being relevant to the target populations but were not clearly addressed by previously existing interventions: importance of booster seat laws, CPS (particularly booster seat) education (around injuries that are prevented), and booster seats as part of good parenting in the car. As a result, the research team created new pilot interventions to convey these messages. These interventions, summarized in Table 2.5, were each designed to deliver a single message
16
Table 2.4: Summary of the interventions selected in Phase 2 Campaign Title Sponsor(s)
Harborview Injury Prevention and Research Center, Latino Kids Safety Coalition, and State Farm Insurance
Materials
Four radio commercials in Spanish
Target population
Spanish-speaking parents and their children 4 to 8 years old
Mode of Distribution
Regional radio
Targeted barriers
Resistance of family members No one enforcing law Lack of info
Childcare centers School health classes Educational programs Community centers Doctors offices
Childcare centers School health classes Educational programs Community centers Doctors offices
Cinderella Interventions
Child resists restraint Booster is a baby seat Lack of info Resistance of family members
17
Table 2.5: Summary of the interventions designed in Phase 2 Campaign Title Designer
The Center for Injury Research and Prevention at The Childrens Hospital of Philadelphia
Materials
Two radio PSAs in English
Targeted barriers
Resistance of family members No one enforcing law Lack of info
The Center for Injury Research and Prevention at The Childrens Hospital of Philadelphia
Four-minute video
Childcare centers School health classes Educational programs Community centers Doctors offices National television Previews to movies
Avoid regret
The Center for Injury Research and Prevention at The Childrens Hospital of Philadelphia
Five-minute video
Childcare centers School health classes Educational programs Community centers Doctors offices National television Previews to movies
Lack of info
18
2.3.3.2 Target Message: Belt-positioning booster seat education (focusing on injuries that are prevented)
Participants in Phase 1 conveyed a lack of awareness about the added safety benefit provided by a booster seat (as opposed to a seat belt). They expressed interest in hearing these stories from real parents. In response, a true story intervention titled Avoid Regret was designed. This video intervention described a fathers regret that his daughter had not used a booster seat, as she had been severely injured in a crash. He spoke of how his wife was fatally injured, and how his two daughters in the back seat survived. His four-year-old child, restrained in a child safety seat, suffered only very minor injuries. His seven-year-old child, seated next to the four-year-old, but restrained in a seat belt alone, suffered severe injuries resulting in brain damage. The narrator in the video, the father, described life after the crash with a disabled daughter and how and why proper restraint in a booster seat would have reduced the severity of her injuries.
2.3.3.3 Target Message: Belt-positioning booster seats as part of good parenting in the car
A final barrier described by the participants was the challenge of parenting in the car. Children removed their restraints, distracted the driver, and in general misbehaved, highlighting a key threat that parents feared getting into a crash with their child in the car. Parents felt ill equipped to manage their childs behavior in the car. Although interventions were available to explain the benefits of restraint use to children, no programs were found that helped parents use discipline and restraint in booster seats to enforce better behavior in the car. A video was created, titled Safer for Kids, Easier for You, that illustrated parenting techniques for controlling children in the back seat. In this video, a trusted source, a pediatrician, described what the parents could do to improve the situation. Among the suggested techniques was the use of booster seats to ensure children were comfortable in their restraints, and would therefore stay in place in the vehicle.
19
Figure 2.5 outlines the steps taken in Phase 3 to obtain these goals. Figure 2.5: Phase 3 objectives
Deliver Interventions and Elicit Reactions Measure changes in BB&T and intention
20
Demographic Segments
One session with African American mothers Three sessions with English speaking Hispanic mothers Two sessions with African American mothers Two sessions with African American fathers
Little Rock, AR
Two sessions with African American mothers Two sessions with English speaking Hispanic mothers
Cincinnati, OH
Two sessions with African American mothers Two sessions with white mothers
2.4.2 Phase 3 focus groups: Evaluating interventions that address relevant determinants
The Phase 3 focus groups were designed to achieve two objectives: 1. elicit the reactions of designated target populations to selected interventions, and
21
2. determine if the selected interventions motivated any changes in intentions to use booster seats. A focus group moderators guide was constructed to achieve these goals. Four key topics provided a framework for the group: the effect of key barriers, and threats on current behavior, the strength of targeted intervention messages, the identity of appropriate intervention sources, channels, and audiences (see section 2.3.3), and the propensity of selected interventions to change behavior.
As in Phase 1, the moderators guide (Appendix 2.3) was used as a framework for each focus group discussion. Discussion was again free-flowing, allowing participants to explore the issues that were most relevant to their lifestyles. The guide broke the groups into five segments: the introduction, an icebreaker, a list ranking exercise and an accompanying discussion, the viewing and discussion of four interventions, and a brief closing. The timing and order of these segments is detailed in Table 2.7. Table 2.7: Timing and flow of Phase 3 focus groups Topic
Introduction Icebreaker List ranking exercise and discussion Delivering interventions and eliciting reactions Selected intervention 1 Selected intervention 2 Safer for Kids, Easier for You intervention Avoid Regret intervention Summary and closing TOTAL TIME
Time Allowed
5 min 10 min 20 min 80 min 20 min 20 min 20 min 20 min 5 min 120 min
As in Phase 1, a member of the subcontracted agencys staff greeted participants as they arrived. A Participant Consent Form (Appendix 2.4) and the Participant Information Form (Appendix 2.5) were distributed upon arrival. These documents were available in both English and Spanish, depending on the language the participants preferred. The PIF asked questions covering general demographic information (age, race, sex, etc.), as well as, information specific to the participants driving behaviors and intentions toward booster seat use (for more information on the PIF and other questionnaires distributed in Phase 3, see section 2.4.2.4). Participants were encouraged to fill out the PIF upon arrival. Once all participants had arrived, the consent form was read aloud. Participants were briefed on the format and duration of the group, and were provided the ground rules for the discussion.
22
2.4.2.1 The list ranking exercise: Prioritizing relevant benefits of and barriers and threats to booster seat use
Analysis of the Phase 1 focus groups generated lists of benefits, barriers and threats to booster seat use and preferred sources and channels for safety information. The first task of the focus groups in the Evaluation phase was then to prioritize these lists. This served to determine which issues were of greatest relevance to each group of focus group participants. Four different lists (Appendix 2.7), each composed of an average of 13 items, were printed on poster paper and also read aloud to overcome literacy barriers: perceived threats, or Bad things that I worry about when I drive with my children in the car; perceived benefits, or Good things that might happen if my child is in a booster seat; perceived barriers, or Things that might make it hard for me to use a booster seat for my child; and people who matter, or People or places whose opinions about booster seats matter to me."
Each list was color coded (benefits = green, barriers = red, threats = orange, people who matter = blue), and participants were given five round stickers in each of the four colors. The participants were asked to place their stickers next to the statements that most closely represented their experiences, using the green stickers with the green list, the blue stickers with the blue list, etc. Participants were encouraged, but not required, to use all of their stickers. They were also allowed to place more than one sticker on an item that they felt strongly about. The focus group moderators, in addition to personnel from the Childrens Hospital, were present and available to answer questions concerning the lists. Following the list ranking, the participants were asked to discuss why they had chosen the items that they had. The participants were also asked if they believed any topics were missing from the lists. The most highly ranked items on the lists were then identified and used to select interventions to be shown to the group. For example, if a group ranked the barrier relating to the law or police officers highly, an intervention addressing the law would be presented. For more information on the interventions shown in this study see Section 2.4.2.2. This activity served a dual purpose. It served to validate whether the populations selected for Phase 3 had similar concerns regarding booster seat use as those in Phase 1. However, the list-ranking exercise also served to prioritize the issues of greatest importance to the target population. By identifying the most important issues, the study team was able to identify which interventions would best fit the needs of the focus group participants.
23
For more information on the barriers, benefits, and threats that each of these interventions addresses see Table 2.4 and 2.5. The remaining two interventions, which addressed potential injury and parental tips, were designed by CHOP in response to a perceived deficit in existing interventions and were shown to all groups (see Sections 2.3.1-3 and Table 2.5). Each intervention was assessed via focus group discussion using the same series of questions shown in the Phase 3 Moderators Guide (Appendix 2.3). These questions were based on theories of social marketing, and sought to identify whether each intervention created intention and motivation to engage in the targeted behavior. The purpose of these discussions was to determine which interventions best addressed the barriers and threats and highlighted the benefits of booster seat use. By selecting interventions that specifically addressed the issues important to the participants, the study was able to directly address the effectiveness of the intervention itself, without being solely focused on the relevance of the intended message. Each intervention was evaluated using the same set of questions. This was done to assure that each intervention was evaluated on the same basis. The effectiveness of these questions was evaluated in the four pilot groups run at the start of this phase. For more information on the segmentation of the groups in Phase 3, see Table 2.6.
24
prior to this time. Participants were required to sign a separate consent form (Appendix 2.11) to participate in the giveaway. Each participant electing to receive a booster seat was required to listen to a 15-minute educational presentation that detailed how to properly use the booster seat. The script for this giveaway and presentation is shown in Appendix 2.12. Certified child passenger safety technicians from the local IFCK chapter did the training. Following the presentation, participants were allowed to ask questions of the technician. This booster seat giveaway eliminated the barriers of access to booster seats that the participants might have faced, including cost and lack of knowledge about where to buy a booster seat. By eliminating these barriers, the research team was able to examine the effectiveness of the intervention messages, as none of the interventions shown in the group were able to overcome the cost or inaccessibility barriers alone. The effectiveness of the giveaway in changing the participants intention to use booster seats was evaluated through a follow-up telephone interview (see section 2.5). Results from these interviews, also funded by the contractor, will be published separately.
25
result of viewing and discussing the interventions; and further changes in attitudes and intentions after receiving a booster seat. In addition, immediate changes in knowledge were assessed.
26
members of the CHOP research team, asked a series of questions, detailed in the follow-up interview script (Appendix 2.15). Phone calls were recorded using a telephone pickup microphone and a digital recorder to allow for separate assessment of adherence to the protocol. Results from these interviews will be published separately, by the contractor.
3. Results
3.1 Phase 1: Formative Research 3.1.1 Study sample: Demographic PIF data
Demographic data describing the study sample was taken from the Phase 1 PIF (Appendix 1.5), administered to focus group participants prior to the focus group discussion. A total of 107 participants from one pilot group and 11 focus groups were included in Phase 1 of this study (Table 3.1). The plurality of parent drivers who participated in Phase 1 were African American (46.7%), female (86.9%), age 26 to 30 (35.5%), and married (51.4%). Many participants were employed outside of the home (45.8%), with 63.3% of those employed working full time, and 44.9% working in a service-oriented position. The Phase 1 participants answered several questions relating to their driving behaviors (Table 3.2). The majority of participants drove a vehicle (77.6%). Of these parent drivers, 51.2% drove cars Almost one-half of driven vehicles (47.6%) were model years 1996 to 2000. The participants also provided information on their driving behaviors with child passengers (Table 3.3). Each participant had between 1 and 6 children, with 3 being the mean number of children per participant household. Of those children 3 to 6 years old, the mean age of the youngest child was 4.2 years. Most participants rode in a vehicle with their child or children 3 to 6 almost every day (73.8%). On these rides, 46.7% of respondent said they were always the driver. Independent of who drove the vehicle 79.4% of participants said that they alone made the decision concerning where their child sat in the car and 85.0% made the decision concerning the type of restraint used for the child or children. Of the 107 participants, 89 (83.2%) had used booster seats for their children at some point. Additional questions were asked in the PIF (Appendix 1.5, Questions 15 -18) relating to the type of restraint currently used and the type of restraint intended for future use. These questions were completed by less than one-third of the participants. As a result, no conclusive results could be drawn on the current and future restraint patterns of the Phase 1 Participants.
28
% of sample
29
% of sample
30
Table 3.3: Driving characteristics of Phase 1 participants when accompanied by child passengers n=
Frequency riding in a vehicle with youngest child in the past 3 months (N = 107) Almost every day A few times a week A few times a month Frequency respondent was the driver (N = 107) Always Sometimes Rarely Never Person who usually decides where child sits in the vehicle (N = 107) Respondent Spouse/partner Child Respondent and spouse Other Person who usually decides type of CRS (N = 107) Respondent Spouse/partner Child Respondent and spouse Ever used booster seat for child (N = 107) No Yes Never heard of until now Blank Number of children living with respondent Mean number: 3 children Range: 1-6 children Age of youngest child between 3 and 6 years Mean age: 4.2 years Mode age: 4 years Range: 3 to 6 years Mean age: 6.1 years Age range: 0 to 19 years
% of sample
79 22 6 50 30 4 23
85 10 4 5 3 91 9 1 6 14 89 2 2
79.4 9.3 3.7 4.7 2.8 85.0 8.4 0.9 5.6 13.1 83.2 1.9 1.9
31
information. Members of the Hispanic groups also cited the radio as an important source of information. Magazines and newspapers were mentioned; however, not all participants agreed that these were good vehicles to get messages about child passenger safety to parents.
33
variables had more variation, with 64.3% of participants selecting the card marked cheap, and 51.2% electing the card representing good features. Cards related to the childs attitude drew a similarly divided reaction, with 69% choosing that the child likes card, and 25% choosing the child thinks its a baby seat card. Non-paired variables, such as child can easily get out and the seat is too bulky, showed similar variability, with 45.2% and 66.7%, respectfully.
35
% of sample
36
% of sample
37
% of sample
38
Table 3.7: Driving characteristics of Phase 3 participants when accompanied by child passengers n=
Frequency riding in a vehicle with youngest child in the past 3 months (n = 142) Almost every day A few times a week A few times a month Blank Frequency respondent was the driver (n = 142) Always Sometimes Never Blank Person who usually decides where child sits in vehicle (n = 142) Respondent Spouse/partner Child No one Other Blank Person who usually decides type of CRS (n = 142) Respondent Spouse/partner Child Respondent and spouse Blank Ever used booster seat for child (n = 142) No Yes Never heard of until now Blank Number of children living with respondent Mean number: 2.5 children Range:1- 12 children Age of youngest child between 3 and 8 years Mean age = 4.4 years Mode age = 4 years Range = 3 to 8 years 22 113 6 1 15.5 79.6 4.2 0.7 127 7 3 2 3 89.4 4.9 2.1 1.4 2.1 125 5 6 2 2 2 88.0 3.5 4.2 1.4 1.4 1.4 85 39 16 2 59.9 27.5 11.3 1.4
% of sample
119 17 5 1
39
3.2.2 Focus group discussion: Qualitative transcript analysis 3.2.2.1 List ranking discussion
Participants selected barriers, benefits, threats and people that were most relevant to their lives. These selections were then discussed briefly. Appendix 2.8 reports the top five selections from each group, and Appendix 2.9 summarizes selected statements from the focus group transcripts on the relevance of each highly ranked barrier, benefit, threat, and person who matters.
3.2.2.1.1 Top 5 threats: Bad things that I worry about when I drive my children in the car
Threats of similar type were ranked highly among all groups. Every group ranked my child might get seriously hurt in an accident within the top five. Participants in each group identified this as a threat they thought about often when they drove with their children. Most groups rated either I will get pulled over by the cops, or I might have to pay a ticket highly. These concerns stemmed primarily from hearing stories from other parents, or having been pulled over themselves. Participants in each group cited the resultant fine as a strong motivator to restrain their child properly; however, many also cited that money should not be a primary factor in their behavior, repeatedly stating that they restrain their children to keep them safe.
3.2.2.1.2 Top 5 benefits: Good things that might happen if I use a booster seat
Parents in each group ranked items relating to their childs safety highly. Benefits such as My child will feel safe and snug in a booster seat or I will know that my child is safe when he is in a booster seat were ranked in the top five in all groups. Participants reiterated their emphasis on the safety of their children when commenting on these selections; parents noted that the benefit of increased safety was paramount to any other possible benefit. As a result, parents considered any other benefits, such as their child liking the restraint, as a bonus. Perceived benefits incurred by the child (My child will feel like a big kid in a booster seat and My child can see out the window when he is in a booster seat), were highly ranked in many groups. Parents stated that these benefits were important to them, as it increased their childrens willingness to use booster seats. These benefits were crucial to many parents, especially White parents, who struggled to get their children to use proper restraint. In contrast, African American participants often noted that their childs approval or disapproval had no effect on their choice of child restraint.
3.2.2.1.3 Top 5 barriers: Things that might make it hard to use a booster seat
Participants in most groups were hesitant to select items on this list, noting that there were no factors that completely barred them from keeping their child safe. As a result, less than half of the participants in Phase 3 placed stickers on this list. Discussion showed that parents felt strongly that 40
nothing would stop them from keeping their children safe; however, some did concede and note several things that made it harder to use a booster seat for their children. Of the barriers discussed in the groups, the most commonly identified was the effect of the child passengers behavior on restraint use. Several participants in each group noted that their children, although comfortable in a booster seat, just did not want to be restrained at all. In many groups, a subset of participants disagreed with this, stating that their children often reminded other passengers to put on a seat belt. As a result, the effect of the child passengers opinion was divided between those parents whose children liked their booster seat, and those parents whose children did not. Another factor that made using booster seats hard for many parents was lack of room in their vehicle. African American participants, as well as those participants in Little Rock most often noted this. The participants agreed that a booster seat took up a lot of room in a car, and made it difficult to restrain multiple children properly.
3.2.2.1.4 Top 5 information sources: People and places whose opinions I trust
Participants across all sites, sexes, and racial and ethnic groups agreed that they trusted their own opinions about booster seats the most. Parents in all groups emphasized this, noting that although they may accept advice from other sources; their own feelings about what keeps their child safe supersede all others. The most highly ranked outside sources included: government officials, physicians and the police. Family members and friends also ranked highly, with participants noting that most of their information on child passenger safety is received by word of mouth.
immediately, despite their previous barriers. Parents also thought that this intervention would positively affect their childrens views on safety, noting that they would show the video to even their youngest children if they were given the opportunity. Parents in all groups cited that this intervention was particularly eye-opening because it answered all of their questions about booster seats, while also speaking to their fear of a crash. Each group spoke at length about the crash, speaking about their fears as well as stories they had heard. Parents frequently asked for copies of the video, hoping to show it to their families and friends.
3.2.2.2.6 Intervention: Its the Law radio commercials (by the Childrens Hospital of Philadelphia [English] and Harborview Injury Research Center [Spanish])
A difference of opinion existed between participant groups concerning the English-language radio commercials. Overall, African American and White participants agreed that the commercials would not reach as large an audience as interventions aired on television. These parents also noted concern for their childs safety should be enough of a motivation without the added consequence of a ticket. In contrast, Hispanic participants were very receptive to the information presented in the Spanishlanguage radio commercials. They felt that there was a lack of commercials promoting child safety and were happily surprised by the amount of detailed information addressing age-appropriate restraint included. Some parents believed, though, that ticketing was unfair due to ignorance of the law on the part of immigrants and the associated cost of a booster seat.
43
Table 3.8: Changes in participant intention toward booster seat use as a result of Phase 3 focus groups % of sample PIF PDF
Likely booster use next 2 months (N = 142) Very likely Somewhat likely Somewhat unlikely Very unlikely Don't know Blank Could get child to use booster seat (N = 142) Strongly agree Somewhat agree Neither agree nor disagree Somewhat disagree Strongly disagree Blank High-backs look safe (N = 142) Strongly agree Somewhat agree Neither agree nor disagree Somewhat disagree Strongly disagree Blank No-backs look safe (N = 142) Strongly agree Somewhat agree Neither agree nor disagree Somewhat disagree Strongly disagree Blank Children will like boosters (N = 142) Strongly agree Somewhat agree Neither agree nor disagree Somewhat disagree Strongly disagree Blank 57.7 17.6 0.7 12.0 9.2 2.8 78.2 9.2 3.5 0.7 2.8 5.6 65.5 21.1 9.2 0.0 0.0 4.2 22.5 38.7 16.2 9.9 7.7 4.9 43.7 34.5 10.6 4.9 2.1 4.2 87.3 4.9 0.7 0.7 4.9 1.4 90.1 6.3 1.4 0.0 0.0 2.1 87.3 7.0 3.5 0.0 0.7 1.4 34.5 35.9 12.0 4.2 9.2 4.2 74.6 17.6 3.5 2.1 0.7 1.4
The last two statements were eliminated from analysis, as the participants responses indicted that they had inferred that this was a trick question, assuming that children should not use seat belts alone. Each of the remaining questions showed an increase in the percentage of parents who strongly agreed and several showed a decrease in the percentage of parents who strongly disagreed. Focus group interventions and discussion specifically served to improve participants opinions of the safety of the high-back booster seat; however, many parents were still hesitant to fully commit that 44
no-back boosters were safe. This smaller percentage of parents who strongly agreed that low-back boosters were safe (compared to those who strongly agreed that high-back booster seats were safe) serves to validate the data, as mostly high-back boosters were included in the interventions shown.
4. Discussion
For parents with limited educational attainment (high school education or less), lack of knowledge about injury consequences of inappropriate restraint was the major barrier to booster seat use. Parent participants endorsed a simple direct message that reinforced the life-saving benefits of restraint use and described the injuries that could be prevented with booster seat use. These parents reported positive changes in attitudes and intentions as a result of the information acquired from this message.
Lack of knowledge was the most prevalent barrier across groups differing in sex, race, and ethnicity and was cited frequently by participants as their primary barrier to use. This result demonstrates that previous intervention tactics have not fully educated the targeted at-risk populations concerning appropriate child restraint, particularly restraint of 4- to 8-year-old children in booster seats. The parents targeted in this study were representative of differing racial, ethnic, sex, and geographic groups; however, all participants were of low educational attainment (achieving a high school diploma or less), representing 46.1% of United States residents over the age of 25 (United States Census Bureau, 2006). Research has shown that education for these populations, particularly health education, must be concrete and personal (Riley et al., 2006; Bass, 2005). It is important not to simply provide information to these populations, but to truly educate them. This education involves providing knowledge coupled with messages that motivate the target parents, shown through channels and media that the parents trust and respect.
children in the car. A clear educational message toward appropriate restraint use would therefore be strengthened by being paired with a message confronting the possibility of injury resulting from a child being improperly restrained in a vehicle.
4.3 Sending strong, educational messages through targeted channels and media
Using media and spokespeople who are trusted and well received by the targeted audience can further strengthen health messages. This study found that differing populations are receptive to different forms of media. For example, Hispanic participants noted that they listened to the radio frequently; whereas African American and White participants gave the most credence to information conveyed by television, noting they did not listen to the radio frequently. All participants emphasized that printed media, such as newspapers and magazines, were a much less effective means of communicating health messages. While African American and White participants cited the Internet as a trusted source, Hispanic participants noted that they rarely accessed the Internet for information regarding their childs safety. These differences demonstrate that messages, if conveyed through an ineffective media source, could be received poorly or lost completely. Finally, the choice of spokesperson is critical to success. In this study, all participants agreed on a set of spokespeople they would trust to relate messages on child safety. The list included: other parents, doctors (and other medical personnel), and police officers. Participants also noted that their children would respond best to a child spokesperson. These two findings revealed a common theme: effective messaging should be conveyed by members of the target community - people with whom the population identifies with as a result of shared experience.
advice from a trusted source, such as another parent, through a frequented media source, such as television. These suggestions were used to create three interventions, including one that detailed the injuries a child could incur from being prematurely restrained in a seat belt in the event of a crash. This intervention addressed each of the factors raised by the participants in Phase 1, and was extremely well received by the participants of Phase 3. This intervention used an actionable emotion (Kahn et al., 2006; Taylor, 1997; Zeelenberg, 1999), regret, coupled with education to correct participants 46
previous misconceptions and promote booster seat use. It was cited by participants as both educational and motivational. Parents felt that this intervention alone had changed their intention to use a booster seat as it provided them with the knowledge necessary to use a booster seat properly, in addition to the motivation to overcome their additional barriers. This response demonstrates that interventions targeted toward the needs of a specific population are effective in motivating change in behavioral intention.
discounted cost. In addition to these types of programs, a preliminary message relating to the cost of a booster seat would be necessary for this population, prior to any supplementary education.
4.8 Implications
Education that provides parents with clear knowledge translated from research but put in the context of a true story was found to change intention relating to booster seat use. Parents noted that a more graphic message, detailing specific injuries that could result from improper restraint, would not scare them from using seat belts, but would motivate them to use booster seats. These findings support the design of this study as an effective means to determine the barriers, benefits, and threats experienced by a population, and in designing interventions to target these factors. Use of focus groups was found to be an effective medium to elicit the concerns faced by parents relating to child restraint. These focus group discussions were supported by quantitative data, documenting participants change in intention over time.
48
5. References
1. Agran, P. F., Anderson, C. L., & Winn, D. G. (1998). Factors associated with restraint use of children in fatal crashes. Pediatrics, 102(3), E39. 2. Ajzen, I. (1991). The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 50, 179-211. 3. Allen, G. (1999). Marketing, from http://ollie.dcccd.edu/mrkt2370/Chapters/ch9/9commun.htm 4. Arbogast, K. B., Durbin, D. R., Kallan, M. J., & Winston, F. K. (2003). Effect of vehicle type on the performance of second generation air bags for child occupants. Annual Proceedings/Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine, 47, 85-99. 5. Arbogast, K. B., Durbin, D. R., Kallan, M. J., & Winston, F. K. (2004). Evaluation of pediatric use patterns and performance of lap shoulder belt systems in the center rear. Annual Proceedings/Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine, 48, 57-72. 6. Baker, S. P., Braver, E. R., Chen, L. H., Pantula, J. F., & Massie, D. (1998). Motor vehicle occupant deaths among Hispanic and black children and teenagers. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med, 152(12), 1209-1212. 7. Bass, L. (2005). Health literacy: implications for teaching the adult patient. Journal of Infusion Nursing, 28(1), 15-22. 8. Braver, E. R. (2003). Race, Hispanic origin, and socioeconomic status in relation to motor vehicle occupant death rates and risk factors among adults. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 35(3), 295-309. 9. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2000). Motor-vehicle occupant fatalities and restraint use among children aged 4-8 years-United States, 1994-1998. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep, 49(7), 135-137. 10. Decina, L., Lococo, K., & Block, A. (2005). Misuse of Child Restraints: Results of a Workshop to Review Field Data Results. DOT HS 809 851. Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 11. Decina, L. E., & Knoebel, K. Y. (1997). Child safety seat misuse patterns in four states. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 29(1), 125-132. 12. Decina, L. E., & Lococo, K. H. (2004). Misuse of child restraints. DOT HS 809 671. Washington DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 13. Durbin, D. R., Kallan, M. J., & Winston, F. K. (2001). Trends in booster seat use among young children in crashes. Pediatrics, 108(6), E109. 14. Durbin, D.R., Chen, I., Smith, R., Elliott, M.R., & Winston, F.K. (2005). Effects of Seating Position and Appropriate Restraint Use on the Risk of Injury to Children in Motor Vehicle Crashes. Pediatrics, 115(3), 305-309. 15. Ebel, B. E., Koepsell, T. D., Bennett, E. E., & Rivara, F. P. (2003). Too small for a seat belt: predictors of booster seat use by child passengers. Pediatrics, 111(4 Pt 1), e323-327.
49
16. Eby, D. W., & Kostyniuk, L. P. (1999). A statewide analysis of child safety seat use and misuse in Michigan. Accid Anal Prev, 31(5), 555-566. 17. Elliott, M. R., Kallan, M. J., Durbin, D. R., & Winston, F. K. (2006). Effectiveness of child safety seats vs. seat belts in reducing risk for death in children in passenger vehicle crashes. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160(6), 617-621. 18. Funk, D. L., McErlean, M., & Verdile, V. P. (2003). Parental report of child restraint device use in an emergency department population. J Emerg Med, 24(3), 247-251. 19. Glassbrenner, D. (2003). The use of child restraints in 2002. Research Note. DOT HS 809 555. Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 20. Hanfling, M. J., Mangus, L. G., Gill, A. C., & Bailey, R. (2000). A multifaceted approach to improving motor vehicle restraint complicance. Inj Prev, 6(2), 125-129. 21. Henley, N., & Donovan, R. J. (2003). Death Anxiety and Threat Appeals: Toward a Practical Application in the Context of Health Promotion. Omega, 46(3), 225-239. 22. Hletko, P. J., Hletko, J. D., Shelness, A. M., & Robin, S. S. (1983). Demographic predictors of infant car seat use. Am J Dis Child, 137(11), 1061-1063. 23. Istre, G. R., McCoy, M. A., Womack, K. N., Fanning, L., Dekat, L., & Stowe, M. (2002). Increasing the use of child restraints in motor vehicles in a Hispanic neighborhood. Am J Public Health, 92(7), 1096-1099. 24. Kahn, B. E., Luce, M. F., & Nowlis, S. M. (2006). Debiasing Insights From Process Tests. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(1), 131-137. 25. Kostaridou, S., Anastasea-Vlachou, K., Sotiropoulou, F., Panagopoulou, G., Panagopoulou, M., Papathanasiou-Klontza, D., et al. (1997). Car transportation conditions of preschool children: use of children's car safety seats. Acta Paediatr, 86(2), 192-195. 26. Laflamme, L., & Engstrom, K. (2002). Socioeconomic differences in Swedish children and adolescents injured in road traffic incidents: cross sectional study. BMJ, 324(7334), 396-397. 27. Lane, W. G., Liu, G. C., & Newlin, E. (2000). The association between hands-on instruction and proper child safety seat installation. Pediatrics, 106(4 Suppl), 924-929. 28. Lee, J. W., Fitzgerald, K., & Ebel, B. E. (2003). Lessons for increasing awareness and use of booster seats in a Latino community. Inj Prev, 9(3), 268-269. 29. Margolis, L. H., Wagenaar, A. C., & Molnar, L. J. (1992). Use and misuse of automobile child restraint devices. Am J Dis Child, 146(3), 361-366. 30. Miller, D. T., & Taylor, B. R. ( 1995). Counterfactual thought, regret, and superstition: How to avoid kicking yourself. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 305-331). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 31. Miller, T. R., Spicer, R. S., & Lestina, D. C. (1998). Who is driving when unrestrained children and teenagers are hurt? Accid Anal Prev, 30(6), 839-849. 32. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2003). Traffic safety facts 2002: children. DOT HS 809 620. Washington , DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration.
50
33. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2005). Improving the Safety of OlderChild Passengers. DOT HS 809 953. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 34. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2006). Race and Ethnicity in Fatal Motor Vehicle Traffic Crashes 1999 - 2004. DOT HS 809 956. Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 35. Niemcryk, S. J., Kaufmann, C. R., Brawley, M., & Yount, S. I. (1997). Motor vehicle crashes, restraint use, and severity of injury in children in Nevada. Am J Prev Med, 13(2), 109-114. 36. Partners for Child Passenger Safety. (2006). Fact and Trend Report. 37. Ramsey, A., Simpson, E., & Rivara, F. P. (2000). Booster seat use and reasons for nonuse. Pediatrics, 106(2), E20. 38. Riley, J. B., Cloonan, P., & Norton, C. (2006). Low health literacy: a challenge to critical care. Critical Care Nursing Quarterly, 29(2), 174-178. 39. Rogers, R. W., & Mewborn, C. R. (1976). Fear appeals and attitude change: effects of a threat's noxiousness, probability of occurrence, and the efficacy of coping responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(1), 54-61. 40. Russell, J., Kresnow, M. J., & Brackbill, R. (1994). The effect of adult belt laws and other factors on restraint use for children under age 11. Accid Anal Prev, 26(3), 287-295. 41. Stiles, M. C., & Grieshop, J. I. (1999). Impacts of culture on driver knowledge and safety device usage among Hispanic farm workers. Accid Anal Prev, 31(3), 235-241. 42. Taylor, K. (1997). A Regret Theory Approach to Assessing Consumer Satisfaction. Marketing Letters, 8(2), 229-238. 43. United States Census Bureau. (2003). S1501: Educational Attainment. 2006 American Community Survey. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Commerce: Economics and Statistics Administration. 44. Vaca, F., Anderson, C. L., Agran, P., Winn, D., & Cheng, G. (2002). Child safety seat knowledge among parents utilizing emergency services in a level I trauma center in Southern California. Pediatrics, 110(5), e61. 45. Verreault, R., Stulginskas, J., Keyl, P., Read, J., & Pless, I. B. (1982). Use of automobile seat restraints by children in two Canadian cities. Can Med Assoc J, 126(10), 1163-1168. 46. Vivoda, J. M., Eby, D. W., & Kostyniuk, L. P. (2004) Differences in safety belt use by race. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 36(6), 1105-1109. 47. Wagenaar, A. C., Molnar, L. J., & Margolis, L. H. (1988). Characteristics of child safety seat users. Accid Anal Prev, 20(4), 311-322. 48. Webb, G. R., Sanson-Fisher, R. W., & Bowman, J. A. (1988). Psychosocial factors related to parental restraint of pre-school children in motor vehicles. Accid Anal Prev, 20(2), 87-94. 49. Weinstein, N., & Sandman, P. (1992). A model of the precaution adoption process: Evidence from home radon testing. Health Psychology, 11, 170-180. 50. Winston, F. K. (2006). Parent driver characteristics associated with sub-optimal restraint of child passengers. Traffic Injury Prevention. 51
51. Winston, F. K., Durbin, D. R., Kallan, M. J., & Moll, E. K. (2000). The danger of premature graduation to seat belts for young children. Pediatrics, 105(6), 1179-1183. 52. Zeelenberg, M. (1999). Anticipated Regret, Expected Feedback and Behavioral Decision Making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 93-106. 53. Zempsky, W. T., Isaacman, D. J., Sullivan, K. M., & Sipp, N. (1996). Child restraint device use in patients leaving a children's hospital. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med, 150(12), 1284-1287.
52