Data Management and Analysis Methods
Data Management and Analysis Methods
Data Management and Analysis Methods
29
DATA MANAGEMENT AND ANALYSIS METHODS
u
Texts Are Us
This chapter is about methods for managing and analyzing qualitative data. By qualitative data we mean text: newspapers, movies, sitcoms, e-mail traffic, folktales, life histories. We also mean narrativesnarratives about getting divorced, about being sick, about surviving hand-to-hand combat, about selling sex, about trying to quit smoking. In fact, most of the archaeologically recoverable information about human thought and human behavior is text, the good stuff of social science. Scholars in content analysis began using computers in the 1950s to do statistical analysis of texts (Pool, 1959), but recent advances in technology are changing the economics of the social sciences. Optical scanning today makes light work of converting written texts to machine-readable form. Within a few years, voice-recognition software will make light work
of transcribing open-ended interviews. These technologies are blind to epistemological differences. Interpretivists and positivists alike are using these technologies for the analysis of texts, and will do so more and more. Like Tesch (1990), we distinguish between the linguistic tradition, which treats text as an object of analysis itself, and the sociological tradition, which treats text as a window into human experience (see Figure 29.1). The linguistic tradition includes narrative analysis, conversation (or discourse) analysis, performance analysis, and formal linguistic analysis. Methods for analyses in this tradition are covered elsewhere in this Handbook. We focus here on methods used in the sociological tradition, which we take to include work across the social sciences. There are two kinds of written texts in the sociological tradition: (a) words or phrases generated by techniques for systematic elicitation and (b) free-flowing texts, such as narratives, discourse, and responses to open-ended interview
769
770
questions. In the next section, we describe some methods for collecting and analyzing words or phrases. Techniques for data collection include free lists, pile sorts, frame elicitations, and triad tests. Techniques for the analysis of these kinds of data include componential analysis, taxonomies, and mental maps. We then turn to the analysis of free-flowing texts. We look first at methods that use raw text as their inputmethods such as key-wordsin-context, word counts, semantic network analysis, and cognitive maps. We then describe methods that require the reduction of text to codes. These include grounded theory, schema analysis, classical content analysis, content dictionaries, analytic induction, and ethnographic decision models. Each of these methods of analysis has advantages and disadvantages. Some are appropriate for exploring data, others for making comparisons, and others for building and testing models. Nothing does it all.
Free Lists
Free lists are particularly useful for identifying the items in a cultural domain. To elicit domains, researchers might ask, What kinds of illnesses do you know? Some short, open-ended questions on surveys can be considered free lists, as can some responses generated from in-depth ethnographic interviews and focus groups. Investigators interpret the frequency of mention and the order in which items are mentioned in the lists as indicators of items salience (for measures of salience, see Robbins & Nolan, 1997; Smith, 1993; Smith & Borgatti, 1998). The co-occurrence of items across lists and the proximity with which items appear in lists may be used as measures of similarity among items (Borgatti, 1998; Henley, 1969; for a clear example, see Fleisher & Harrington, 1998).
771
772
most different. The similarity among pairs of items is the number of times people choose to keep pairs of items together (for some good examples, see Albert, 1991; Harman, 1998).
Frame Substitution
In the frame substitution task (DAndrade, 1995; DAndrade, Quinn, Nerlove, & Romney, 1972; Frake, 1964; Metzger & Williams, 1966), the researcher asks the respondent to link each item in a list of items with a list of attributes. DAndrade et al. (1972) gave people a list of 30 illness terms and asked them to fill in the blanks in frames such as You can catch _____ from other people, You can have _____ and never know it, and Most people get _____ at one time or other (p. 12; for other examples of frame substitution, see Furbee & Benfer, 1983; Young, 1978).
Taxonomies
Folk taxonomies are meant to capture the hierarchical structure in sets of terms and are commonly displayed as branching tree diagrams. Figure 29.1 presents a taxonomy of our own understanding of qualitative analysis techniques. Figure 29.2 depicts a taxonomy we have adapted from Pamela Ericksons (1997) study of the perceptions among clinicians and adolescents of methods of contraception. Researchers can elicit folk taxonomies directly by using successive pile sorts (Boster, 1994; Perchonock & Werner, 1969). This involves asking people to continually subdivide the piles of a free pile sort until each item is in its own individual pile. Taxonomic models can also be created with cluster analysis on the similarity data from paired comparisons, pile sorts, and triad tests. Hierarchical cluster analysis (Johnson, 1967) builds a taxo-
Componential Analysis
As we have outlined elsewhere, componential analysis (or feature analysis) is a formal, qualitative technique for studying the content of meaning (Bernard, 1994; Bernard & Ryan, 1998). Developed by linguists to identify the features and rules that distinguish one sound from another (Jakobson & Halle, 1956), the technique was elaborated by anthropologists in the 1950s and 1960s (Conklin, 1955; DAndrade, 1995; Frake, 1962; Goodenough, 1956; Rushforth, 1982; Wallace, 1962). (For a particularly good description of how to apply the method, see Spradley, 1979, pp. 173-184.)
773
TABLE 29.1
Female + +
nomic tree where each item appears in only one group. Interinformant variation is common in folk taxonomies. That is, different people may use different words to refer to the same category of things. Some of Ericksons (1997) clinician informants referred to the highly effective group of methods as safe, more reliable, and sure bets. Category labels need not be simple words, but may be complex phrases; for example, see the category in Figure 29.2 comprising contraceptive methods in which you have to pay attention to timing. Sometimes, people have no labels at all for particular categoriesat least not that they can dredge up easilyand categories, even when named, may be fuzzy and may overlap with other categories. Overlapping cluster analysis (Hartigan, 1975) identifies groups of items where a single item may appear in multiple groups.
Mental Maps
Mental maps are visual displays of the similarities among items, whether or not those
items are organized hierarchically. One popular method for making these maps is by collecting data about the cognitive similarity or dissimilarity among a set of objects and then applying multidimensional scaling, or MDS, to the similarities (Kruskal & Wish, 1978). Cognitive maps are meant to be directly analogous to physical maps. Consider a table of distances between all pairs of cities on a map. Objects (cities) that are very dissimilar have high mileage between them and are placed far apart on the map; objects that are less dissimilar have low mileage between them and are placed closer together. Pile sorts, triad tests, and paired comparison tests are measures of cognitive distance. For example, Ryan (1995) asked 11 literate Kom speakers in Cameroon to perform successive pile sorts on Kom illness terms. Figure 29.3 presents an MDS plot of the collective mental map of these terms. The five major illness categories, circled, were identified by hierarchical cluster analysis of the same matrix used to produce the MDS plot.1 Data from frame substitution tasks can be displayed with correspondence analysis (Weller & Romney, 1990).2 Correspondence analysis scales both the rows and the columns into the same space. For example, Kirchler (1992) analyzed 562 obituaries of managers who had died in 1974, 1980, and 1986. He identified 31 descriptive categories from adjectives used in the obituaries and then used correspondence analysis to display how these categories were associated with men and women managers over time. Figure 29.4 shows that male managers who died in 1974 and 1980 were seen by their surviving friends and family as active, intelligent, outstanding, conscientious, and experienced experts. Although the managers who died in 1986 were still respected, they were more likely to be described as entrepreneurs, opinion leaders, and decision makers. Perceptions of female managers also changed, but they did not become more like their male counterparts. In 1974 and 1980, female managers were remembered for being nice people. They were described as kind, likable, and adorable. By 1986, women were remembered for their courage and commitment. Kirchler interpreted these data to mean that gender stereotypes changed in
774
u
775
the early 1980s. By 1986, both male and female managers were perceived as working for success, but men impressed their colleagues through their knowledge and expertise, whereas women impressed their colleagues with motivation and engagement.
Analyzing Words
Techniques for word analysis include key-words-in-context, word counts, structural analysis, and cognitive maps. We review each below.
Key-Words-in-Context
Researchers create key-words-in-context (KWIC) lists by finding all the places in a text where a particular word or phrase appears and printing it out in the context of some number of words (say, 30) before and after it. This produces a concordance. Well-known concordances have been done on sacred texts, such as the Old and New Testaments (Darton, 1976; Hatch & Redpath, 1954) and the Koran (Kassis, 1983), and on famous works of literature from Euripides (Allen & Italie, 1954) to Homer (Prendergast, 1971), to Beowulf (Bessinger, 1969), to Dylan Thomas (Farringdon & Farringdon, 1980). (On
Although taxonomies, MDS maps, and the like are useful for analyzing short phrases or words, most qualitative data come in the form of free-flowing texts. There are two major types of analysis. In one, the text is segmented into its most basic meaningful components: words. In the other, meanings are found in large blocks of text.
776
Figure 29.4. Correspondence Analysis of the Frequencies of 31 Disruptive Obituary Categories by Gender and Year of Publication
SOURCE: Erich Kirchler, Adorable Woman, Expert Man: Changing Gender Images of Women and Men in Management, European Journal of Social Psychology, 22 (1992), p. 371. Copyright 1992 by John Wiley & Sons Limited. Reproduced by permission of John Wiley & Sons Limited.
the use of concordances in modern literary studies, see Burton, 1981a, 1981b, 1982; McKinnon, 1993.)
Word Counts
Word counts are useful for discovering patterns of ideas in any body of text, from field notes to responses to open2Dended questions. Students of mass media have used use word counts to trace the ebb and flow of support for political figures over time (Danielson & Lasorsa, 1997; Pool, 1952). Differences in the use of words common to the writings of James Madi-
son and Alexander Hamilton led Mosteller and Wallace (1964) to conclude that Madison and not Hamilton had written 12 of the Federalist Papers. (For other examples of authorship studies, see Martindale & McKenzie, 1995; Yule 1944/1968.) Word analysis (like constant comparison, memoing, and other techniques) can help researchers to discover themes in texts. Ryan and Weisner (1996) instructed fathers and mothers of adolescents in Los Angeles: Describe your children. In your own words, just tell us about them. Ryan and Weisner identified all the unique words in the answers they got to that
777
Figure 29.5. Multidimensional Scaling of Informants Based on Words Used in Descriptions of Horror Films
SOURCE: Based on data in Nolan and Ryan (1999).
grand-tour question and noted the number of times each word was used by mothers and by fathers. Mothers, for example, were more likely to use words like friends, creative, time, and honest; fathers were more likely to use words like school, good, lack, student, enjoys, independent, and extremely. This suggests that mothers, on first mention, express concern over interpersonal issues, whereas fathers appear to prioritize achievement-oriented and individualistic issues. This kind of analysis considers neither the contexts in which the words occur nor whether the words are used negatively or positively, but distillations like these can help researchers to identify important con-
structs and can provide data for systematic comparisons across groups.
778
(30 women and 29 men) to describe their most memorable horror film. The researchers identified the 45 most common adjectives, verbs, and nouns used across the descriptions of the films. They produced a 45(word)-by-59(person) matrix, the cells of which indicated whether each student had used each key word in his or her description. Finally, Nolan and Ryan created a 59(person)-by-59(person) similarity matrix of people based on the co-occurrence of the words in their descriptions. Figure 29.5 shows the MDS of Nolan and Ryans data. Although there is some overlap, it is pretty clear that the men and women in their study used different sets of words to describe horror films. Men were more likely to use words such as teenager, disturbing, violence, rural, dark, country, and hillbilly, whereas women were more likely to use words such as boy, little, devil, young, horror, father, and evil. Nolan and Ryan interpreted these results to mean that the men had a fear of rural people and places, whereas the women were more afraid of betrayed intimacy and spiritual possession. (For other examples of the use of word-by-word matrices, see Jang & Barnett, 1994; Schnegg & Bernard, 1996.) This example makes abundantly clear the value of turning qualitative data into quantitative data: Doing so can produce information that engenders deeper interpretations of the meanings in the original corpus of qualitative data. Just as in any mass of numbers, it is hard to see patterns in words unless one first does some kind of data reduction. More about this below. As in word analysis, one appeal of semantic network analysis is that the data processing is done by computer. The only investigator bias introduced in the process is the decision to include words that occur at least 10 times or 5 times or whatever. (For discussion of computer programs that produce word-by-text and word-by-word co-occurrence matrices, see Borgatti, 1992; Doerfel & Barnett, 1996.) There is, however, no guarantee that the output of any word co-occurrence matrix will be meaningful, and it is notoriously easy to read patterns (and thus meanings) into any set of items.
Cognitive Maps
Cognitive map analysis combines the intuition of human coders with the quantitative methods of network analysis. Carleys work with this technique is instructive. Carley argues that if cognitive models or schemata exist, they are expressed in the texts of peoples speech and can be represented as networks of concepts (see Carley & Palmquist, 1992, p. 602), an approach also suggested by DAndrade (1991). To the extent that cognitive models are widely shared, Carley asserts, even a very small set of texts will contain the information required for describing the models, especially for narrowly defined arenas of life. In one study, Carley (1993) asked students some questions about the work of scientists. Here are two examples she collected:
Student A: I found that scientists engage in research in order to make discoveries and generate new ideas. Such research by scientists is hard work and often involves collaboration with other scientists which leads to discoveries which make the scientists famous. Such collaboration may be informal, such as when they share new ideas over lunch, or formal, such as when they are coauthors of a paper. Student B: It was hard work to research famous scientists engaged in collaboration and I made many informal discoveries. My research showed that scientists engaged in collaboration with other scientists are coauthors of at least one paper containing their new ideas. Some scientists make formal discoveries and have new ideas. (p. 89)
Carley compared the students texts by analyzing 11 concepts: I, scientists, research, hard work, collaboration, discoveries, new ideas, formal, informal, coauthors, paper. She coded the concepts for their strength, sign (positive or negative), and direction (whether one concept is logically prior to others), not just for their existence. She found that although students used the same concepts in their texts, the concepts clearly had different meanings. To display the differences in understandings, Carley advocates the use of maps that show the relations between and
779
SOURCE: Kathleen Carley, Coding Choices for Textual Analysis: A Comparison of Content Analysis and Map Analysis, in P Marsden (Ed.), Sociological Methodology (Oxford: Blackwell, 1993), p. 104. Copyright 1993 by . the American Sociological Association. Reproduced by permission of the American Sociological Association.
among concepts. Figure 29.6 shows Carleys maps of two of the texts. Carleys approach is promising because it combines the automation of word counts with the sensitivity of human intuition and interpretation. As Carley recognizes, however, a lot depends on who does the coding. Different coders will produce different maps by making different coding choices. In the end, native-language competence is one of the fundamental methodological requirements for analysis (see also Carley, 1997; Carley & Kaufer, 1993; Carley &
Palmquist, 1992; Palmquist, Carley, & Dale, 1997). Key-words-in-context, word counts, structural analysis, and cognitive maps all reduce text to the fundamental meanings of specific words. These reductions make it easy for researchers to identify general patterns and make comparisons across texts. With the exception of KWIC, however, these techniques remove words from the contexts in which they occur. Subtle nuances are likely to be lostwhich brings us to the analysis of whole texts.
780
Sampling
Investigators must first identify a corpus of texts, and then select the units of analysis within the texts. Selection can be either random or purposive, but the choice is not a matter of cleaving to one epistemological tradition or another. Waitzkin and Britt (1993) did a thoroughgoing interpretive analysis of encounters between patients and doctors by selecting 50 texts at random from 336 audiotaped encounters. Trost (1986) used classical content analysis to test how the relationships between teenagers and their families might be affected by five different dichotomous variables. He intentionally selected five cases from each of the 32 possible combinations of the five variables and conducted 32 5 = 160 interviews. Samples may also be based on extreme or deviant cases, cases that illustrate maximum variety on variables, cases that are somehow typical of a phenomenon, or cases that confirm or disconfirm a hypothesis. (For reviews of nonrandom sampling strategies, see Patton, 1990, pp. 169-186; Sandelowski, 1995b.) A single case may be sufficient to display something of substantive importance, but Morse (1994) suggests using at least six participants in studies where one is trying to understand the essence of
Finding Themes
Themes are abstract (and often fuzzy) constructs that investigators identify before, during, and after data collection. Literature reviews are rich sources for themes, as are investigators own experiences with subject matter. More often than not, however, researchers induce themes from the text itself. There is more than one way to induce themes. Grounded theorists suggest a careful, line-by-line reading of the text while looking for processes, actions, assumptions, and consequences. Schema analysts suggest looking for metaphors, for repetitions of words, and for shifts in content (Agar & Hobbs, 1985). Content analysts have used KWIC to identify different meanings. Spradley (1979, pp. 199-201) suggests looking for evidence of social conflict, cultural contradictions, informal methods of social control, things that people do in managing impersonal social relationships, methods by which people acquire and maintain achieved and ascribed status, and information about how people solve problems. Each of these arenas is likely to yield major themes in cultures. Barkin, Ryan, and Gelberg (1999) had multiple coders inde-
781
Building Codebooks
Codebooks are simply organized lists of codes (often in hierarchies). How a researcher can develop a codebook is covered in detail by Dey (1993, pp. 95-151), Crabtree and Miller (1992), and Miles and Huberman (1994, pp. 55-72). MacQueen, McLellan, Kay, and Milstein (1998) suggest that a good codebook should include a detailed description of each code, inclusion and exclusion criteria, and exemplars of real text for each theme. If a theme is particularly abstract, we suggest that the researcher also provide examples of the themes
boundaries and even some cases that are closely related but not included within the theme. Coding is supposed to be data reduction, not proliferation (Miles, 1979, pp. 593-594). The codes themselves are mnemonic devices used to identify or mark the specific themes in a text. They can be either words or numberswhatever the researcher finds easiest to remember and to apply. Qualitative researchers working as a team need to agree up front on what to include in their codebook. Morse (1994) suggests beginning the process with a group meeting. MacQueen et al. (1998) suggest that a single team member should be designated Keeper of the Codebookwe strongly agree. Good codebooks are developed and refined as the research goes along. Kurasaki (1997) interviewed 20 sanseithird-generation Japanese Americansand used a grounded theory approach to do her analysis of ethnic identity. She started with seven major themes. As the analysis progressed, she split the major themes into subthemes. Eventually, she combined two of the major themes and wound up with six major themes and a total of 18 subthemes. (Richards & Richards, 1995, discuss the theoretical principles related to hierarchical coding structures that emerge out of the data. Araujo, 1995, uses an example from his own research on the traditional British manufacturing industry to describe the process of designing and refining hierarchical codes.) The development and refinement of coding categories have long been central tasks in classical content analysis (see Berelson, 1952, pp. 147-168; Holsti, 1969, pp. 95-126) and are particularly important in the construction of concept dictionaries (Deese, 1969; Stone et al., 1966, pp. 134-168). Krippendorf (1980, pp. 71-84) and Carey, Morgan, and Oxtoby (1996) note that much of codebook refinement comes during the training of coders to mark the text and in the act of checking for intercoder agreement. Disagreement among multiple coders shows when the codebook is ambiguous and confusing. The first run also allows the researcher to identify good examples to include in the codebook.
782
Marking Texts
The act of coding involves the assigning of codes to contiguous units of text. Coding serves two distinct purposes in qualitative analysis. First, codes act as tags to mark off text in a corpus for later retrieval or indexing. Tags are not associated with any fixed units of text; they can mark simple phrases or extend across multiple pages. Second, codes act as values assigned to fixed units (see Bernard, 1991, 1994; Seidel & Kelle, 1995). Here, codes are nominal, ordinal, or ratio scale values that are applied to fixed, nonoverlapping units of analysis. The nonoverlapping units can be texts (such as paragraphs, pages, documents), episodes, cases, or persons. Codes as tags are associated with grounded theory and schema analysis (reviewed below). Codes as values are associated with classic content analysis and content dictionaries. The two types of codes are not mutually exclusive, but the use of one glosscodefor both concepts can be misleading.
Grounded Theory
Grounded theorists want to understand peoples experiences in as rigorous and detailed a manner as possible. They want to identify categories and concepts that emerge from text and link these concepts into substantive and formal theories. The original formulation of the method (Glaser & Strauss, 1967) is still useful, but later works are easier to read and more practical (Charmaz, 1990; Lincoln & Guba, 1985; Lonkila, 1995; Strauss, 1987). Strauss and Corbin (1990), Dey (1993), and Becker (1998) provide especially useful guidance. (For some recent examples of grounded theory research, see Hunt & Ropo, 1995; Irurita, 1996; Kearney et
783
resent individual and small group influences on action. The matrix is designed to help investigators to be more sensitive to conditions, actions/interactions, and consequences of a phenomenon and to order these conditions and consequences into theories. Memoing is one of the principal techniques for recording relationships among themes. Strauss and Corbin (1990, pp. 18, 73-74, 109-129, 197-219) discuss three kinds of memos: code notes, theory notes, and operational notes. Code notes describe the concepts that are being discovered in the discovery of grounded theory. In theory notes, the researcher tries to summarize his or her ideas about what is going on in the text. Operational notes are about practical matters. Once a model starts to take shape, the researcher uses negative case analysis to identify problems and make appropriate revisions. The end results of grounded theory are often displayed through the presentation of segments of textverbatim quotes from informantsas exemplars of concepts and theories. These illustrations may be prototypical examples of central tendencies or they may represent exceptions to the norm. Grounded theory researchers also display their theoretical results in maps of the major categories and the relationships among them (Kearney et al., 1995; Miles & Huberman, 1994, pp. 134-137). These concept maps are similar to the personal semantic networks described by Leinhardt (1987, 1989), Strauss (1992), and DAndrade (1991) (see below).
Schema Analysis
Schema analysis combines elements of the linguistic and sociological traditions. It is based on the idea that people must use cognitive simplifications to help make sense of the complex information to which they are constantly exposed (Casson, 1983, p. 430). Schank and Abelson (1977) postulate that schemataor scripts, as they call themenable culturally skilled people to fill in details of a story or event. It is, says Wodak (1992, p. 525), our schemata that lead us to interpret Mona Lisas smile as evidence of her perplexity or her desperation.
784
From a methodological view, schema analysis is similar to grounded theory. Both begin with a careful reading of verbatim texts and seek to discover and link themes into theoretical models. In a series of articles, Quinn (1982, 1987, 1992, 1996, 1997) has analyzed hundreds of hours of interviews to discover concepts underlying American marriage and to show how these concepts are tied together. Quinns (1997) method is to exploit clues in ordinary discourse for what they tell us about shared cognitionto glean what people must have in mind in order to say the things they do (p. 140). She begins by looking at patterns of speech and the repetition of key words and phrases, paying particular attention to informants use of metaphors and the commonalities in their reasoning about marriage. Quinn found that the hundreds of metaphors in her corpus of texts fit into just eight linked classes, which she calls lastingness, sharedness, compatibility, mutual benefit, difficulty, effort, success (or failure), and risk of failure. Metaphors and proverbs are not the only linguistic features used to infer meaning from text. DAndrade (1991) notes that perhaps the simplest and most direct indication of schematic organization in naturalistic discourse is the repetition of associative linkages (p. 294). He observes that indeed, anyone who has listened to long stretches of talkwhether generated by a friend, spouse, workmate, informant, or patientknows how frequently people circle through the same network of ideas (p. 287). In a study of blue-collar workers in Rhode Island, Claudia Strauss (1992) refers to these ideas as personal semantic networks. She describes such a network from one of her informants. On rereading her intensive interviews with one of the workers, Strauss found that her informant repeatedly referred to ideas associated with greed, money, businessmen, siblings, and being different. She displays the relationships among these ideas by writing the concepts on a page of paper and connecting them with lines and explanations. Price (1987) observes that when people tell stories, they assume that their listeners share with them many assumptions about how the world works, and so they leave out information
785
pure women survive and unmitigated masculinity leads to death (p. 195). The coding of texts is usually assigned to multiple coders so that the researcher can see whether the constructs being investigated are shared and whether multiple coders can reliably apply the same codes. Typically, investigators first calculate the percentage of agreement among coders for each variable or theme. They then apply a correction formula to take account of the fact that some fraction of agreement will always occur by chance. The amount of that fraction depends on the number of coders and the precision of measurement for each code. If two people code a theme present or absent, they could agree, ceteris paribus, on any answer 25% of the time by chance. If a theme, such as wealth, is measured ordinally (low, medium, high), then the likelihood of chance agreement changes accordingly. Cohens (196) kappa, or K, is a popular measure for taking these chances into account. When K is zero, agreement is what might be expected by chance. When K is negative, the observed level of agreement is less than one would expect by chance. How much intercoder agreement is enough? The standards are still ad hoc, but Krippendorf (1980, pp. 147-148) advocates agreement of at least .70 and notes that some scholars (e.g., Brouwer, Clark, Gerbner, & Krippendorf, 1969) use a cutoff of .80. Fleiss (1971) and Light (1971) expand kappa to handle multiple coders. For other measures of intercoder agreement, see Krippendorf (1980, pp. 147-154) and Craig (1981). Reliability concerns the extent to which an experiment, test, or any measuring procedure yields the same results on repeated trials (Carmines & Zeller, 1979, p. 11). A high level of intercoder agreement is evidence that a theme has some external validity and is not just a figment of the investigators imagination (Mitchell, 1979). Not surprisingly, investigators have suggested many ways to assess validity (for reviews of key issues, see Campbell, 1957; Campbell & Stanley, 1963; Cook & Campbell, 1979; Denzin, 1997; Fielding & Fielding, 1986; Guba, 1981; Guba & Lincoln, 1982; Hammersley, 1992; Kirk & Miller, 1986; Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Bernard (1994) argues that, ultimately, the validity of a concept depends on the utility of the device
786
that measures it and the collective judgment of the scientific community that a construct and its measure are valid. In the end, he says, we are left to deal with the effects of our judgments, which is just as it should be. Valid measurement makes valid data, but validity itself depends on the collective opinion of researchers (p. 43). Generalizability refers to the degree to which the findings are applicable to other populations or samples. It draws on the degree to which the original data were representative of a larger population. For reviews of work in content analysis, see Pool (1959); Gerbner, Holsti, Krippendorf, Paisley, and Stone (1969); Holsti (1969); Krippendorf (1980); Weber (1990); and Roberts (1997). Examples of classical content analysis can be found in media studies (Hirschman, 1987; Kolbe & Albanese, 1996; Spiggle, 1986), political rhetoric (Kaid, Tedesco, & McKinnon, 1996), folklore (Johnson & Price-Williams, 1997), business relations (Spears, Mowen, & Chakraborty, 1996), health care delivery (Potts, Runyan, Zerger, & Marchetti, 1996; Sleath, Svarstad, & Roter, 1997), and law (Imrich, Mullin, & Linz, 1995). Classical content analysis is also the fundamental means by which anthropologists test cross-cultural hypotheses (Bradley, Moore, Burton, & White, 1990; Ember & Ember, 1992; White & Burton, 1988). For early, but fundamental, criticisms of the approach, see Kracauer (1953) and George (1959).
Content Dictionaries
Computer-based, general-purpose content analysis dictionaries allow investigators to automate the coding of texts. To build such dictionaries, researchers assign words, by hand, to one or more categories (there are typically 50-60 categories in computerized content analysis dictionaries) according to a set of rules. The rules are part of a computer program that parses new texts, assigning words to categories. Work on content dictionaries began in the 1960s with the General Inquirer and continues to this day (Kelly & Stone, 1975; Stone et al., 1966; Zuell, Weber, & Mohler, 1989). The General Inquirer is a computer program that
787
negative case analysis, see Becker, 1998, pp. 146-214.) Schweizer (1991, 1996) applied this method in his analysis of conflict and social status among residents of Chen Village, China. (For a discussion of Schweizers data collection and analysis methods, see Bernard & Ryan, 1998.) All the data about the actors in this political drama were extracted from a historical narrative about Chen Village. Like classic content analysis and cognitive mapping, analytic induction requires that human coders read and code text and then produce an event-by-variable matrix. The object of the analysis, however, is not to show the relationships among all codes, but to find the minimal set of logical relationships among the concepts that accounts for a single dependent variable. With more than three variables, the analysis becomes much more difficult. Computer programs such as QCA (Drass, 1980) and ANTHROPAC (Borgatti, 1992) test all possible multivariate hypotheses and find the optimal solution. (QCA is reviewed in Weitzman & Miles, 1995.)
788
u
Figure 29.7. Decision Model of Constraints on the Use of Pills or Liquid Medications for Mothers Treating Children with Diarrhea in Rural Mexico
SOURCE: Based on data in Ryan and Martnez (1996).
789
they test it on an independent sample to see if it predicts as well as it postdicts. Typically, EDMs predict more than 80% of whatever behavior is being modeled, far above what we expect by chance. (For more detailed arguments on how to calculate accuracy in EDMs, see Ryan & Martnez, 1996; Weller et al., 1997.) Because of the intensive labor involved, EDMs have been necessarily restricted to relatively simple decisions in relatively small and homogeneous populations. Recently, however, we found we could effectively test, on a nationally representative sample, our ethnographically derived decision models for whether or not to recycle cans and whether or not to ask for paper or plastic bags at the grocery store (Bernard, Ryan, & Borgatti, 1999). EDMs can be displayed as decision trees (e.g., Gladwin, 1989), as decision tables (Mathews & Hill, 1990; Young, 1980), or as sets of rules in the form of if-then statements (Ryan & Martnez, 1996). Like componential analysis, folk taxonomies, and schema analysis, EDMs represent an aggregate decision process and do not necessarily represent what is going on inside peoples heads (Garro, 1998).
Text analysis as a research strategy permeates the social sciences, and the range of methods for conducting text analysis is inspiring. Investigators examine words, sentences, paragraphs, pages, documents, ideas, meanings, paralinguistic features, and even what is missing from the text. They interpret, mark, retrieve, and count. By turns, they apply interpretive analysis and numerical analysis. They use text analysis for exploratory and confirmatory purposes. Researchers identify themes, describe them, and compare them across cases and groups. Finally, they combine themes into conceptual models and theories to explain and predict social phenomena. Figure 29.1 depicts a broad range of analysis techniques found across the social sciences. To
790
conform our presentation with the literature on qualitative methods, we have organized these techniques according to the goals of the investigators and the kinds of texts to which the techniques are typically applied. In this chapter, we focus on the sociological tradition that uses text as a window into experience rather than the linguistic tradition that describes how texts are developed and structured. Texts such as conversations, performances, and narratives are analyzed by investigators from both the sociological and linguistic traditions. Although the agendas of the investigators may differ, we see no reason many of the sociological techniques we describe could not be useful in the linguistic tradition and vice versa. We also distinguish between those analyses associated with systematically elicited data and those associated with free-flowing texts. We argue, however, that these data-analytic pairings are ones of convention rather than necessity. Investigators want to (a) identify the range and salience of key items and concepts, (b) discover the relationships among these items and concepts, and (c) build and test models linking these concepts together. They use free-listing tasks, KWIC, word counts, and the exploratory phases of grounded theory, schema analysis, and EDM to discover potentially useful themes and concepts. Researchers use pile sorts, paired comparisons, triads tests, frame substitution tasks, semantic networks, cognitive maps, content analysis and content dictionaries, and the modeling phases of grounded theory, schema analysis, and EDM to discover how abstract concepts are related to each other. They display the relationships as models or frameworks. These frameworks include formal models that rely on Boolean logic (componential analysis and analytic induction), hierarchical models (taxonomies and ethnographic decision models), probabilistic models (classic content analysis and content dictionaries), and more abstract models such as those produced by grounded theory and schema analysis. Below we describe two important examples of studies in which researchers
791
pulled all the statements that pertained to informants interactions or assessments of other people. He then looked at the statements and sorted them into piles based on their content. He named each pile as a theme and assessed how the themes interacted. He found that he had three piles. The first contained statements in which the informant was expressing negative feelings for a person in a dominant social position. The second was made up of statements emphasizing the others knowledge or awareness. The statements in the third small cluster emphasized the importance of change or openness to new experiences. From this intuitive analysis, Agar felt that his informants were telling him that those in authority were only interested in displaying their authority unless they had knowledge or awareness; knowledge or awareness comes through openness to new experience; most in authority are closed to new experience or change. To test his intuitive understanding of the data, Agar (1983) used all the statements from a single informant and coded the statements for their role type (kin, friend/acquaintance, educational, occupational, or other), power (dominant, symmetrical, subordinate, or undetermined), and affect (positive, negative, ambivalent, or absent). Agar was particularly interested in whether negative sentiments were expressed toward those in dominant social roles. For one informant, Agar found that out of 40 statements coded as dominant, 32 were coded negative and 8 were coded positive. For the 36 statements coded as symmetrical, 20 were coded positive and 16 negative, lending support to his original theory. Next, Agar looked closely at the deviant casesthe 8 statements where the informant expressed positive affect toward a person in a dominant role. These counterexamples suggested that the positive affect was expressed toward a dominant social other when the social other possessed, or was communicating to the informant, knowledge that the informant valued. Finally, Agar (1980) developed a more systematic questionnaire to test his hypothesis further. He selected 12 statements, 4 from each of the control, knowledge, and change themes iden-
792
tified earlier. He matched these statements with eight roles from the informants transcript (father, mother, employer, teacher, friend, wife, coworker, and teammate). Agar then returned to his informant and asked if the resulting statements were true, false, or irrelevant. (In no case did the informant report irrelevant.) Agar then compared the informants responses to his original hypotheses. He found that on balance his hypotheses were correct, but discrepancies between his expectations and his results suggested areas for further research. These examples show that investigators can apply one technique to different kinds of data and they can apply multiple techniques to the same data set. Text analysis is used by avowed positivists and interpretivists alike. As we have argued elsewhere (Bernard, 1993; Bernard & Ryan, 1998), methods are simply tools that belong to everyone.
Whats Next?
We do not want to minimize the profound intellectual differences in the epistemological positions of positivists and interpretivists. We think, however, that when researchers can move easily and cheaply between qualitative and quantitative data collection and analysis, the distinctions between the two epistemological positions will become of less practical importance. That is, as researchers recognize the full array of tools at their disposal, and as these tools become easier to use, the pragmatics of research will lessen the distinction between qualitative and quantitative data and analysis. The process is under wayand is moving fastwith the development of increasingly useful software tools for qualitative data analysis. Useful tools create markets, and market needs create increasingly useful tools. Qualitative data analysis packages (ATLAS/ti, NUDIST, Code-A-Text, the Ethnograph, AnSWR, and others) have improved dramatically over the past few years (Fischer, 1994; Kelle, 1995; Weitzman & Miles, 1995). These products, and
793
Notes
1. MDS displays are highly evocative. They beg to be interpreted. In fact, they must be interpreted. Why are some illnesses at the top of Figure 29.3 and some at the bottom? We think the illnesses at the top are more of the chronic variety, whereas those at the bottom are more acute. We also think that the illnesses on the left are less serious than those on the right. We can test ideas like these by asking key informants to help us understand the arrangement of the illnesses in the MDS plot. (For more examples of mental maps, see Albert, 1991; DAndrade et al., 1972; Erickson, 1997.) (There is a formal method, called property fitting analysis, or PROFIT, for testing ideas about the distribution of items in an MDS map. This method is based on linear regression. See Kruskal & Wish, 1978.) 2. Alternatively, profile matrices (the usual thing-by-variable attribute matrix ubiquitous in the social sciences) can be converted to similarity matrices (thing-by-thing matrices in which the cells contain measures of similarity among pairs of things) and then analyzed with MDS (for step-by-step instructions, see Borgatti, 1999).
References
Agar, M. (1979). Themes revisited: Some problems in cognitive anthropology. Discourse Processes, 2, 11-31. Agar, M. (1980). Getting better quality stuff: Methodological competition in an interdisciplinary niche. Urban Life, 9, 34-50. Agar, M. (1983). Microcomputers as field tools. Computers and the Humanities, 17, 19-26. Agar, M. (1996). Speaking of ethnography (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Agar, M., & Hobbs, J. (1985). How to grow schemata out of interviews. In J. W D. . Dougherty (Ed.), Directions in cognitive an-
thropology (pp. 413-431). Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Albert, S. M. (1991). Cognition of caregiving tasks: Multidimensional scaling of the caregiver task domain. Gerontologist, 31, 726-734. Allen, J. T., & Italie, G. (1954). A concordance to Euripides. Berkeley: University of California Press. Araujo, L. (1995). Designing and refining hierarchical coding frames. In U. Kelle (Ed.), Computer-aided qualitative data analysis: Theory, methods and practice (pp. 96-104). London: Sage. Barkin, S., Ryan, G. W & Gelberg, L. (1999). ., What clinicians can do to further youth violence primary prevention: A qualitative study. Injury Prevention, 5, 53-58. Barnett, G. A., & Danowski, J. A. (1992). The structure of communication: A network analysis of the International Communication Association. Human Communication Research, 19, 164-285. Becker, H. S. (1998). Tricks of the trade: How to think about your research while youre doing it. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Becker, H. S., Geer, B., Hughes, E. C., & Strauss, A. L. (1961). Boys in white: Student culture in medical school. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Berelson, B. (1952). Content analysis in communication research. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Bernard, H. R. (1991). About text management and computers. CAM Newsletter, 3(1), 1-4, 7, 12. Bernard, H. R. (1993). Methods belong to all of us. In R. Borofsky (Ed.), Assessing cultural anthropology (pp. 168-178). New York: McGraw-Hill. Bernard, H. R. (1994). Research methods in anthropology: Qualitative and quantitative approaches (2nd ed.). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira. Bernard, H. R., & Ashton-Voyoucalos, S. (1976). Return migration to Greece. Journal of the Steward Anthropological Society, 8(1), 31-51. Bernard, H. R., & Ryan, G. W (1998). Qualita. tive and quantitative methods of text analysis. In H. R. Bernard (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in cultural anthropology (pp. 595-646). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira. Bernard, H. R., Ryan, G. W & Borgatti, S. ., (1999). Green cognition and behaviors. Re-
794
port submitted to Ford Research Laboratories, Dearborn, MI. Bessinger, J. B. (1969). A concordance to Beowulf. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Bloor, M. (1976). Bishop Berkeley and the adenotonsillectomy dilemma. Sociology, 10, 43-61. Bloor, M. (1978). On the analysis of observational data: A discussion of the worth and uses of inductive techniques and respondent validation. Sociology, 12, 545-557. Bogdan, R. C., & Biklen, S. K. (1992). Qualitative research for education: An introduction to theory and methods (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Borgatti, S. P (1992). ANTHROPAC 4.0. Co. lumbia, SC: Analytic Technologies. Available Internet: http://www.analytictech.com Borgatti, S. P (1998). The methods. In V C. De . . Munck & E. J. Sobo (Eds.), Using methods in the field: A practical introduction and casebook (pp. 249-252). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira. Borgatti, S. P (1999). Elicitation methods for . cultural domain analysis. In J. Schensul, M. LeCompte, S. Borgatti, & B. Nastasi (Eds.), The ethnographers toolkit: Vol. 3. Enhanced ethnographic methods (pp. 115-151). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira. Boster, J. (1994). The successive pile sort. Cultural Anthropology Methods Journal, 6(2), 11-12. Bradley, C., Moore, C. C., Burton, M. L., & White, D. R. (1990). A cross-cultural historical analysis of subsistence change. American Anthropologist, 92, 447-457. Brouwer, M., Clark, C. C., Gerbner, G., & Krippendorf, K. (1969). The television world of violence. In R. K. Baker & S. J. Ball (Eds.), Mass media and violence: A report to the National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence (pp. 311-339, 519-591). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Bulmer, M. (1979). Concepts in the analysis of qualitative data. Sociological Review, 27, 651-677. Burton, D. M. (1981a). Automated concordances and word indexes: The early sixties and the early centers. Computers and the Humanities, 15, 83-100.
795
Burgoon (Ed.), Communication yearbook 6 (pp. 9042D925). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Danowski, J. A. (1993). Network analysis of message content. In W D. Richards & G. A. . Barnett (Eds.), Progress in communication science (Vol. 12, pp. 197-221). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Darton, M. (1976). Modern concordance to the New Testament. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Deese, J. (1969). Conceptual categories in the study of content. In G. Gerbner, O. R. Holsti, K. Krippendorf, W J. Paisley, & P J. Stone . . (Eds.), The analysis of communication content: Developments in scientific theories and computer techniques (pp. 39-56). New York: John Wiley. Denzin, N. K. (1978). The research act: A theoretical introduction to sociological methods (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Denzin, N. K. (1997). Interpretive ethnography: Ethnographic practices for the 21st century. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Dey, I. (1993). Qualitative data analysis: A user2Dfriendly guide for social scientists. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Doerfel, M. L., & Barnett, G. A. (1996). The use of Catpac for text analysis. Cultural Anthropology Methods Journal, 8(2), 4-7. Drass, K. (1980). The analysis of qualitative data: A computer program. Urban Life, 9, 332-353. Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (1992). Resource unpredictability, mistrust, and war: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 36, 242-262. Erickson, P (1997). Contraceptive methods: Do . Hispanic adolescents and their family planning care providers think about contraceptive methods the same way? Medical Anthropology, 17, 65-82. Fan, D. P & Shaffer, C. L. (1990). Use of ., open-ended essays and computer content analysis to survey college students knowledge of AIDS. College Health, 38, 221-229. Farringdon, J. M., & Farringdon, M. G. (1980). A concordance and word-lists to the poems of Dylan Thomas. Swansea, England: Ariel House. Frake, C. O. (1962). The ethnographic study of cognitive systems. In T. Gladwin & W C. . Sturtevant (Eds.), Anthropology and human behavior (pp. 72-85). Washington, DC: Anthropology Association of Washington.
796
Frake, C. O. (1964). Notes on queries in ethnography. American Anthropologist, 66, 132-145. Fielding, N. G., & Fielding, J. L. (1986). Linking data. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Fischer, M. D. (1994). Applications in computing for social anthropologists. London: Routledge. Fjellman, S. M., & Gladwin, H. (1985). Haitian family patterns of migration to South Florida. Human Organization, 44, 301-312. Fleisher, M. S., & Harrington, J. A. (1998). Freelisting: Management at a womens federal prison camp. In V C. De Munck & E. J. . Sobo (Eds.), Using methods in the field: A practical introduction and casebook (pp. 69-84). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira. Fleiss, J. L. (1971). Measuring nominal scale agreement among many raters. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 378-382. Furbee, L. (1996). The religion of politics in Chiapas: Founding a cult of communicating saints. Paper presented at the 96th Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Association, San Francisco. Furbee, L., & Benfer, R. (1983). Cognitive and geographic maps: Study of individual variation among Tojolabal Mayans. American Anthropologist, 85, 305-333. Garro, L. (1998). On the rationality of decision-making studies: Part 1. Decision models of treatment choice. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 12, 319-340. Gatewood, J. B. (1983). Deciding where to fish: The skippers dilemma in Southeast Alaskan salmon seining. Coastal Zone Management Journal, 10, 347-367. George, A. L. (1959). Quantitative and qualitative approaches to content analysis. In I. de S. Pool (Ed.), Trends in content analysis (pp. 7-32). Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Gerbner, G., Holsti, O. R., Krippendorf, K., Paisley, W J., & Stone, P J. (Eds.). (1969). . . The analysis of communication content: Developments in scientific theories and computer techniques New York: John Wiley. Gladwin, C. (1989). Ethnographic decision tree modeling. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Gladwin, H. (1971). Decision making in the Cape Coast (Fante) fishing and fish marketing
797
complex in world folk literature. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Johnson, S. C. (1967). Hierarchical clustering schemes. Psychometrika, 32, 241-253. Kaid, L. L., Tedesco, J. C., & McKinnon, L. M. (1996). Presidential ads as nightly news: A content analysis of 1988 and 1992 televised Adwatches. Journal of Broadcasting Electronic Media, 40, 297-308. Kassis, H. (1983). A concordance of the Quran. Berkeley: University of California Press. Kearney, M. H., Murphy, S., Irwin, K., & Rosenbaum, M. (1995). Salvaging self: A grounded theory of pregnancy on crack cocaine. Nursing Research, 44, 208-213. Kearney, M. H., Murphy, S., & Rosenbaum, M. (1994). Mothering on crack cocaine: A grounded theory analysis. Social Science and Medicine, 38, 351-361. Kelle, U. (1995). An overview of computer-aided methods in qualitative research. In U. Kelle (Ed.), Computer-aided qualitative data analysis: Theory, methods and practice (pp. 1-18). London: Sage. Kelly, E. F., & Stone, P J. (1975). Computer rec. ognition of English word senses. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Kempton, W (1987). Two theories of home heat . control. In D. Holland & N. Quinn (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 222-242). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kirchler, E. (1992). Adorable woman, expert man: Changing gender images of women and men in management. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 363-373. Kirk, J., & Miller, M. L. (1986). Reliability and validity in qualitative research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Kolbe, R. H., & Albanese, J. P (1996). Man to . man: A content analysis of sole-male images in male-audience magazines. Journal of Advertising, 25(4), 1-20. Kracauer, S. (1953. The challenge of qualitative content analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 16, 631-642. Krippendorf, K. (1980). Content analysis: An introduction to its methodology. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Kruskal, J. B., & Wish, M. (1978). Multidimensional scaling. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
798
Kurasaki, K. S. (1997). Ethnic identity and its development among third-generation Japanese Americans. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, DePaul University. Laffal, J. (1990). A concept dictionary of English, with computer programs for content analysis. Essex, CT: Gallery. Laffal, J. (1995). A concept analysis of Jonathan Swifts A tale of a tub and Gullivers travels. Computers and the Humanities, 29, 339-361. Leinhardt, G. (1987). Development of an expert explanation: An analysis of a sequence of subtraction lessons. Cognition and Instruction, 4, 225-282. Leinhardt, G. (1989). Math lessons: A contrast of novice and expert competence. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20(1), 52-75. Leinhardt, G., & Smith, D. A. (1985). Expertise in mathematics instruction: Subject matter knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 247-271. Light, R. J. (1971). Measures of response agreement for qualitative data: Some generalizations and alternatives. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 365-377. Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Lindesmith, A. R. (1968). Addiction and opiates. Chicago: Aldine. (Original work published 1947) Lofland, J., & Lofland, L. H. (1995). Analyzing social settings (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Lonkila, M. (1995). Grounded theory as an emerging paradigm for computer-assisted qualitative data analysis. In U. Kelle (Ed.), Computer-aided qualitative data analysis: Theory, methods and practice (pp. 41-51). London: Sage. MacQueen, K. M., McLellan, E., Kay, K., & Milstein, B. (1998). Codebook development for team-based qualitative research. Cultural Anthropology Methods Journal, 10(2), 31-36. Manning, P K. (1982). Analytic induction. In R. . Smith & P K. Manning (Eds.), Handbook of . social science methods: Vol. 2. Qualitative methods (pp. 273-302). New York: Harper. Martindale, C., & McKenzie, D. (1995). On the utility of content analysis in author attribu-
799
Quinn, N. (1987). Convergent evidence for a cultural model of American marriage. In D. Holland & N. Quinn (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 173-192). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Quinn, N. (1992). The motivational force of self-understanding: Evidence from wives inner conflicts. In R. DAndrade & C. Strauss (Eds.), Human motives and cultural models (pp. 90-126). New York: Cambridge University Press. Quinn, N. (1996). Culture and contradiction: The case of Americans reasoning about marriage. Ethos, 24, 391-425. Quinn, N. (1997). Research on shared task solutions. In C. Strauss & N. Quinn (Eds.), A cognitive theory of cultural meaning (pp. 137-188). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ragin, C. C. (1987). The comparative method: Moving beyond qualitative and quantitative strategies. Berkeley: University of California Press. Ragin, C. C. (1994). Introduction to qualitative comparative analysis. In T. Janowski & A. M. Hicks (Eds.), The comparative political economy of the welfare state (pp. 299-317). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Richards, T. J., & Richards, L. (1991). The NUDIST qualitative data analysis system. Qualitative Sociology, 14, 307-325. Robbins, M. C., & Nolan, J. M. (1997). A measure of dichotomous category bias in free listing tasks. Cultural Anthropology Methods Journal, 9(3), 8-12. Roberts, C. W (1997). A theoretical map for se. lecting among text analysis methods. In C. W . Roberts (Ed.), Text analysis for the social sciences: Methods for drawing statistical inferences from texts and transcripts (pp. 275-283). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Robinson, W S. (1951). The logical structure of . analytic induction. American Sociological Review, 16, 812-818. Romme, A. G. L. (1995). Boolean comparative analysis of qualitative data: A methodological note. Quality and Quantity, 29, 317-329. Roos, G. (1998). Pile sorting: Kids like candy. In V C. De Munck & E. J. Sobo (Eds.), Using . methods in the field: A practical introduction and casebook (pp. 97-110). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira.
800
Rosenberg, S. D., Schnurr, P P & Oxman, T. E. . ., (1990). Content analysis: A comparison of manual and computerized systems. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 298-310. Rushforth, S. (1982). A structural semantic analysis of Bear Lake Athapaskan kinship classification. American Ethnologist, 9, 559-577. Ryan, G. W (1995). Medical decision making . among the Kom of Cameroon: Modeling how characteristics of illnesses, patients, caretakers, and compounds affect treatment choice in a rural community. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Florida, Gainesville. Ryan, G. W (in press). Measuring the typicality . of text: Using multiple coders for more than just reliability and validity checks. Human Organization. Ryan, G. W & Martnez, H. (1996). Can we ., predict what mothers do? Modeling childhood diarrhea in rural Mexico. Human Organization, 55, 472D57. Ryan, G. W & Weisner, T. (1996). Analyzing ., words in brief descriptions: Fathers and mothers describe their children. Cultural Anthropology Methods Journal, 8(3), 13-16. Sandelowski, M. (1995a). Qualitative analysis: What it is and how to begin. Research in Nursing and Health, 18, 371-375. Sandelowski, M. (1995b). Sample size in qualitative research. Research in Nursing and Health, 18, 179-183. Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P (1977). Scripts, . plans, goals, and understanding: An enquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Schnegg, M., & Bernard, H. R. (1996). Words as actors: A method for doing semantic network analysis. Cultural Anthropology Methods Journal, 8(2), 7-10. Schnurr, P P Rosenberg, S. D., Oxman, T. E., & . ., Tucker, G. (1986). A methodological note on content analysis: Estimates of reliability. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 601-609. Schweizer, T. (1991). The power struggle in a Chinese community, 1950-1980: A social network analysis of the duality of actors and events. Journal of Quantitative Anthropology, 3, 19-44. Schweizer, T. (1996). Actor and event orderings across time: Lattice representation and
801
Weller, S. C. (1998). Structured interviewing and questionnaire construction. In H. R. Bernard (Ed.), Handbook of methods in cultural anthropology (pp. 365-409). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira. Weller, S. C., & Romney, A. K. (1988). Systematic data collection. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Weller, S. C., & Romney, A. K. (1990). Metric scaling: Correspondence analysis. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Weller, S. C., Ruebush, T. K., II, & Klein, R. E. (1997). Predicting treatment-seeking behavior in Guatemala: A comparison of the health services research and decision-theoretic approaches. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 11, 224-245. Werner, O., & Schoepfle, G. M. (1987). Systematic fieldwork: Vol. 2. Ethnographic analysis and data management. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. White, D. R., & Burton, M. L. (1988). Causes of polygyny: Ecology, economy, kinship, and warfare. American Anthropologist, 90, 871-887. Willms, D. G., Best, J. A., Taylor, D. W Gilbert, ., J. R., Wilson, D. M. C., Lindsay, E. A., & Singer, J. (1990). A systematic approach for using qualitative methods in primary prevention research. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 4, 391-409. Wilson, H. S., & Hutchinson, S. A. (1996). Methodological mistakes in grounded theory. Nursing Research, 45, 122-124. Wodak, R. (1992). Strategies in text production and text comprehension: A new perspective. In D. Stein (Ed.), Cooperating with written texts (pp. 493-528). New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Wright, K. B. (1997). Shared ideology in Alcoholics Anonymous: A grounded theory approach. Journal of Health Communication, 2(2), 83-99. Yoder, S. (1995). Examining ethnomedical diagnoses and treatment choices for diarrheal disorders in Lubumbashi Swahili. Medical Anthropology, 16, 2112D248. Young, J. C. (1978). Illness categories and action strategies in a Tarascan town. American Ethnologist, 5, 81-97. Young, J. C. (1980). A model of illness treatment decisions in a Tarascan town. American Ethnologist, 7, 106-151.
802
Yule, G. U. (1968). The statistical study of literary vocabulary. Hamden, CT: Archon. (Original work published 1944) Znaniecki, F. (1934). The method of sociology. New York: Farrar & Rinehart.