Playing by Ear Suzuki Method Full
Playing by Ear Suzuki Method Full
Playing by Ear Suzuki Method Full
In the 1930s, the violinist Shinichi Suzuki experimented with a new method of teaching
music to very young children and he became convinced that the best way to learn to play a
musical instrument was to follow a process similar to the learning of ones own native language
(Suzuki, 1969, 1981, 1986, 1989). Later known as the mother-tongue approach, the method is
based on the principle that by immersing young children in music, mainly by having them listen
repeatedly to the pieces they will learn to play on their instrument, their musical abilities would
unfold in the most natural way. The idea that in the initial stage a child should learn to play by
ear instead of relying on note reading was in sharp contrast to the more common practice of the
time (Landers, 1984). But when Suzukis young Japanese students were heard, first in a film
presented in the United States in 1958, then during a tour in 1964, the quality of their
performance was for many a testimony of the success of this method (Herman, 1981). Many
influential musicians and dedicated music teachers became advocates of this approach (Bigler &
Lloyd-Watts, 1979; Hargrave, 2010; Herman, 1981; Kataoka, 1985; Kendall, 1978; Koppelman,
1978; Powell, 1988; Starr & Starr, 1983). Since then, the Suzuki method has grown to a worldwide movement (Bigler and Lloyd-Watts, 1979, p. 1) and has became one of the leading music
1
methods in North America. In view of its popularity and considering that tens of thousands of
students are now learning music through the Suzuki method (Suzuki Association of the
Americas, 2010), we are fully justified in undertaking an analysis of one of the basic principles
of this methodear playing.
Defining the mother-tongue approach
It is interesting to look at how Suzuki (1989) came to associate the concept of the mothertongue approach to music learning. He explains that he was first astonished by the fact that
children everywhere in the world were speaking in their own language; moreover, they did this
fluently, which required a very high level of proficiency (p. 19). Since all children of normal
intelligence spontaneously learn to speak their language, he believed that there must be a secret;
and it must be training. He observed that indeed, all children . . . are brought up by a perfect
educational method: their mother tongue, and he wanted to find out if he could apply this
method to other faculties (1969, p. 10). He studied very closely how a baby learns to speak and
tried to work out some method according to these basic rules (1989, p. 38). Suzuki adopted as
a model the mother-tongue system of language learning . . . defined its attributes and applied
them to music study (Schneiderman, in Comeau, 1998, p. 6).
When applying the mother-tongue approach to music teaching, the concept of immersion
comes first. Suzuki noted that children are surrounded by language sounds from birth, and he
reasoned that if children were surrounded by musical sounds to the same degree, they would
develop an equally remarkable ability in music (Bigler and Lloyd-Watts, 1979, p.1). It is often
noted that, through listening, the children absorb unconsciously the language of music just as
they absorb the sounds of their mother tongue (Powell, 1988, p. 7). Listening is thus the most
basic element of the method, for when one listens repeatedly, the music enters the mind; and the
more thoroughly it is internalized, the easier it is to reproduce (Kataoka, 1985, p.13). This
immersion is done through the use of recordings.1 Young children repeatedly get to hear the
pieces that they are going to learn on their musical instrument. The importance of repetition2 is
strongly emphasized: children listen to the recordings of their music over and over again
(Bigler and Lloyd-Watts, 1979, p. 6); students . . . become familiar with this selected
repertoire through many, many listening repetitions (Taggart, in Comeau, 1998, p. 33); and
children learn by repeated listening to the music they are about to study just as babies listen to
the sounds of language heard about them on a daily basis (Liccardo, in Comeau, 1998, p. 33).
So the child is introduced to the instrument through playing by ear; he should know the melody
well before trying it out on the keyboard. No printed music is used until the student has mastered
basic playing skills: Wait to teach [reading] until an appropriate age and time. Until that time, I
think its more important to develop the ear so that children listen to and judge their own sound.
(Suzuki, 1993, p. 12)
Research problem
Ear playing is at the core of the Suzuki method and Suzuki teachers endorse this
approach. Suzukis principles and application of ear-playing are often presented in non peer
review music education magazines.3 However, the Suzuki method is rarely discussed in scholarly
writings. Fewer than 20 PhD dissertations have been written on it, and they can be classified into
1
It is sometimes suggested that the Suzuki method was made possible by the advancement of technology: until
recently, a system based on listening was not possible because the supporting technology did not exist [but now]
tape recorders and/or other means of making recorded music [are] easily and widely accessible . . . . Dr. Suzuki had
the vision and wisdom to utilize modern technology and thereby changed and improved the way music is learned
and taught (Bigler and Lloyd-Watts, 1979, p. 5).
2
When well-known Suzuki piano teachers were asked how much listening is required (Comeau, 1998, p. 35), their
answers ranged from one hour a day (for Adams, Liccardo, Powell) to three (Schneiderman and Williams) and four
hours a day (Fest and Harrel).
3
The American Suzuki Journal is a quarterly publication of the Suzuki Association of the Americas for teachers and
parents, and it discusses at great length the various components of this method; the Music Educators Journal lists
112 articles on the Suzuki method, while the Journal of Music Teacher Education, Clavier Companion and the
American Music Teacher each have a few articles.
four main topics: 1) curriculum issues,4 2) new applications,5 3) comparative analysis,6 and 4)
experimental investigation.7 It is very difficult to find papers on this method in scholarly
journals.8 We have been unable to find any studies that provide a critical analysis of this popular
approach that parallels music learning with first-language acquisition. Suzuki developed his
teaching principles through his own intuitions and experience, but few researchers have since
investigated whether the initial process of playing by ear is supported by existing theoretical and
empirical literature. This paper will address this gap in the research, particularly in the context of
piano learning and teaching. First a quick historical overview of pedagogues and educators that
have promoted ear-playing will help to put the Suzuki method into perspective. Then strong
evidence supporting the importance of ear playing in the early stages of learning will be
presented. Lastly, in addressing two criticisms linked to ear-playing, we will argue that while
there are problems with the development of aural training, there are no reasons for concern with
regard to music reading.
Advocates of ear playing
Following in a long tradition of instrumental instruction, teachers tend to emphasise pitch
notation and reading skills, and most current method books are designed to teach note reading
The development of a lesson plan sourcebook (Hwang, 1995), of a teachers guide (Lee, 1992), of a reading course
(Lo, 1993), of a comprehensive curriculum (Romeo, 1986), of a program combining Waldorf and Suzuki (Smolen,
2000), and the description of home practice sessions (ONeill, 2003).
5
Adapting the Suzuki method for art education (Arimitsu, 1982), for the bassoon (Schwalje, 2008), for a mixed
method for cello students (Lee, 2007), for American and European piano pedagogical materials (Rutledge, 1983),
for an alternative piano group class approach (Williams, 2000), for a program in Israel (Menczel, 1997).
6
Investigating violin technique in the Suzuki Method and other pedagogies (Perkins, 1993), cello technique in the
Suzuki Method and other pedagogies (Lee, 2001), different pedagogical methodologies for the clarinet (Sperti,
1970).
7
Measuring the perceptual/cognitive listening development between Suzuki trained and traditionally trained
students (Moorhead, 2005), the attention and perseverance behaviours of preschool children enrolled in Suzuki
lessons and others involved in preschool activities, (Scott, 1987), the effect of Suzuki instruction and early
childhood music aptitude (Stamou, 1998), the effect of different incidental listening experiences (Chang, 1999).
8
Brief mention of the Suzuki method (often not relevant except for mentioning the existence of the Suzuki method)
was found in 34 articles in the British Journal of Music Education and 56 articles in the International Journal of
Music Education.
right from the beginning. There are however prominent educators who have promoted the
development of ear playing before introducing notation. Even in the early 1700s, Couperin
(1716/1974) in his teaching manual LArt de toucher le clavecin was recommending that
students be introduced to keyboard playing by ear: One should not begin to teach notation to
children until after they have a certain number of pieces in their hands. It is next to impossible,
while watching their book, for their fingers not to become disarranged and twisted . . . moreover,
memory is formed much better in learning by heart. (1974, p. 32). In the same century, the
philosopher Jean-Jacques Rousseau (1762/1979), also a musician stressed a learning sequence
that favoured sound-before-sign: The intuitive experience and enjoyment of music should come
first . . . . A good deal of traditional music education has worked deductively: the formal rules
have been taught in the abstract, for example, through verbal description of written notation,
rather than in the practical context of making the sounds themselves (1979, p. 215). Wellknown 20th century pedagogues have promoted similar approaches. The American piano teacher
Abby Whiteside stated that the only safe beginning for a music student is to play by ear. To
believe this completely, one need only observe the ease and accuracy of those students who
began in that manner. The skill they develop is never duplicated by those who learned the notes
first and built up a coordination depending on the eye (Whiteside, 1997, p. 165). Canadian
professor Marc Durand (1996) finds it essential that music learning focus first on sound, for a
strong connection must be established between the ear and the instrument before the eye
connection is developed. American teacher Stanley Schleuter (1997) developed his theory of
instrumental learning based on his observation of language acquisition: Children gain
vocabulary and verbal facility over a long time period through speech alone and without a
symbol system . . . . Music learning should follow the same basic sequence of events for
language learning (p. 21). He believes that music readiness [should] occur first so that students
have something to express musically with instruments and only then does notation take on
musical connotations (p. 23). His rationale is very similar to Suzukis, but his teaching
approach is somewhat different: the student must first establish a vocabulary of tonal and
rhythmic patterns through singing, clapping and counting exercises, concurrent with the
development of musical instrument skills. Application of language models of learning have also
been of particular interest to jazz teachers because this form of performance demands
improvisational skills. The linguist Barry Velleman (1978) developed several recommendations
for jazz educators and he suggested that a large part of the training be spent on drilling
improvisational patterns without reference to written materials. He emphasized the need for
students to model patterns of sounds after hearing the instructor and he stressed that: ear
training should precede music reading (p. 29).
It is obvious that many prominent pedagogues have valued ear-playing and that all of
these have had their own strategies for applying this principle. However, it is Suzuki who has
had the greatest impact, spreading ear playing to thousands of beginning music students all over
the world. His systematic approach is well suited for young children and the impressive results
he achieved contributed to the popularity of learning to play by ear. What, however, is the
supporting evidence for promoting ear playing?
Support for Ear Playing
Central to the debate surrounding ear-before-eye or sound-before-sign sequence is the
premise that sensory and motor experiences should always precede the learning of a concept and
the use of symbols. Childrens ability to read music is not the problem; as Tommis and Fazey
(1999) have shown, children as young as three can develop a basic understanding of the pitch
component of musical notation and relate this to the piano keyboard. Methods for preschoolers
such as the Kelly Kirby Kindergarten Piano Method (Kelly-Kirby, 1939) and Music for Young
Children (Balodis, 1993, 1996) have been very successful in teaching youngsters how to read
music and play the piano from a simple score. The question is whether music reading is the best
way to start a musical instrument.
Over half a century ago, the psychologist James Mainwaring (1941, 1947, 1951) made
the observation, while comparing musical and linguistic skills, that the ability to speak and
understand ones own language precedes the acquisition of the ability to read and write it, and
yet musical education frequently begins by inverting the sequence: Instead of learning first how
to produce . . . the sounds . . . and later being taught to associate a symbol with the sound he can
immediately and unconsciously reproduce, the child is taught to associate the symbol with an
activity, such as the depression of a particular key, and not with the resultant sound (1941,
p. 206). This progression follows the following scheme: recognition of a symbol, then automatic
motor response followed by an unexpected sound (1941, p. 208). When this method is adopted,
the association which becomes mechanized is that between a visual symbol and a manipulatory
action (1951, p. 201). Mainwaring promoted the sound-action relationship where the visual
symbol evokes an image of the sound and stimulates the necessary action. The correct sequence
of teaching is then as follows: recognition of symbol, image of sound represented, kinaesthetic
manipulatory reaction, production of expected sound (1941, p. 214). Mainwaring favoured
playing by ear, which he defined as an acquired skill that reproduced directly on an instrument a
recalled musical experience (1951, p. 201), for he believed that to think in sounds . . . is . . .
of fundamental importance in the development of musicianship (1941, p. 208) and he felt that
the ability to play by ear . . . is, in fact . . . genuinely a criterion of real musicianship
(p. 210).
Twenty-five years later, Kochetvitsky (1967) would make similar observations, based
this time on the structure and function of the central nervous system. Through his analysis of the
auditory stimulus, the conditioned reflex and the conditioned response, Kochetvitsky showed
what he called the extreme importance of establishing a connection between the auditory and
the motor system at the very beginning of music study, and later between the visual, the auditory
and the motor systems. He deplores the fact that piano lessons traditionally follow this sequence:
visual impression
motor act is rarely heard since there is not time for listening: the next note must be found and
played (p. 30). He recommended that the initial period be devoted to tone production, with full
attention given to tone quality, kinaesthetic sensations and form of movement. Students are given
simple tunes to play by ear, forcing them to hear inwardly the sounds they want to reproduce.
This approach allows the development of the following schema: auditory stimulus (inwardly
heard tone)
auditory
perception and evaluation of the actual sound (p. 30). The auditory stimulus calls forth the
movement which produces the sound and the result of the motor action is immediately checked
by the ear and evaluated. This link must always be observed in performance as well as in
practice. The introduction of note symbols should come only when this link is strongly
established; every sign should represent an element already experienced aurally. Then once
notation has been learned, it is the teachers task to watch carefully to make sure that hearing
inwardly is always the leading and controlling element (p. 31). The motor response should not
become a direct reaction to a visual stimulation, but should always go through the auditory
system and only then promote the motor reaction. The printed note signs first excite the cells of
the visual region of the cortex, are transmitted to the auditory region, and only then . . . promote
the corresponding motor response (p. 28). The chain of reactions is always guided by the sound:
visual stimulus: the note sign
of motor act
anticipation
information, it can be pointless to expose them to the complex variety of technical skills needed
to play an instrument while at the same time asking them to read and comprehend notation.
Children focusing on reading notation may have few cognitive resources left to devote to
manipulating their instrument and listening to what they are playing (p. 106).
Bamberger (1996, 1999) brings forward another important factor to consider when
teaching beginners. Listeners, even novice ones, do not perceive music on a note-to-note basis,
but through structurally meaningful entities such as motives and phrases (1999, p. 49). Even
young children naturally focus their attention on these units of perception and only with
further effort do they move on to the notes (p. 49). Teaching music should follow the same
gestalt principle of sound organization. Instead of asking students to focus on the smallest,
isolated objects, the individual notes, with no context or functional meaning (p. 50), students
should learn music by experiencing the playing of meaningful musical patterns and phrases.
Only after they have had considerable experience with larger musical entities, should single
notes be studied in isolation. Otherwise, teachers are asking beginner students to put aside their
most natural way of experiencing music. When notation is introduced too early, students struggle
over individual notes, sometimes playing so slowly and hesitantly that they have no concept of
the piece they are trying to perform. Playing by ear is a good way to ensure that learning is
always musically meaningful.
Concerns about ear training
There is strong support for ear playing in the early stages of music learning, but what can
we say about the common criticisms directed at the Suzuki method: 1) poor aural skills in spite
of the focus on listening and 2) poor reading skills in the absence of note reading in the early
stages of learning?
10
Suzuki teachers (Powell, in Comeau, 1998) insist that their method develops good aural
skills: Suzuki students ears tend to be wonderful because it is a listening-based approach (p.
71). Listening is constantly emphasised as students learn to listen to themselves when they
play (p. 71). The method insists on cultivating good tone quality where students demonstrate
listening refinement and sensitivity to slight gradation and variation [in sound quality]
(Schneiderman, in Comeau,1998, p. 72). It is often acknowledged that Suzuki students play
musically due to their well developed listening capabilities (Herman, 1981; Powell, in Comeau,
1998). However, a clear distinction must be made between developing the skills to listen to ones
own playing and acquiring strong mental representations of the musical properties of pitch,
rhythm and harmony. The latter is known as ear training and implies both a cognitive
understanding of the musical elements that are heard when music is being performed and the
mental ability to experience sound recollections when written symbols are read from a musical
score. A look at how aural skill is defined in Edwin Gordons music learning theory and a review
of how ear training is introduced in popular music program will help us understand what might
constitute legitimate concerns regarding the capacity of the Suzuki method to develop good eartraining skills.
Edwin Gordon (1984, 2001, 2003, 2004), 9 like Suzuki, looked at the process of language
development as a means of understanding musical learning. He observed that a child first listens,
and after much repetition begins to repeat what he has heard. After a period of imitation, the
child begins to associate words with what they stand for, and then individual words are grouped
There are two other publications that will not be discussed here but are worth mentioning in the context of this
study. Grunow, Gordon and Azzara (2000) developed a sound-before-sight method book, Jump Right In: The
Instrumental Series. Based on Gordons Music Learning Sequence (1984), it emphasizes playing by ear prior to and
while learning to read music. As a continuation of that series, Lowe and Gordon published in 2004, Music Moves for
Piano, a piano series also based on Gordons music learning theory and designed to develop audiation and keyboard
performance skills.
11
into sentences to communicate thoughts. It is only after these initial stages have been well
mastered that the child will receive instruction on how to read and write. In order to match these
natural stages, Gordon developed a set of sequential levels of learning that includes five stages of
discrimination: aural/oral, verbal association, partial synthesis, symbolic association and
composite synthesis. The aural/oral experience is at the core of Gordons approach. Since a child
learns to speak from listening, music learning should also begin with patterns of sound. This is
accomplished through rote learning of tonal and rhythmic patterns using neutral syllables.
Gordon stressed the importance of accumulating a vocabulary of melodic and rhythmic patterns,
rather than acquiring knowledge of individual notes. At the second stage, an appropriate label is
given to each pattern. Syllables are used to identify pitch and Gordon developed a rhythmic
language to label the different rhythm patterns. Through verbal association, students learn tonal
and rhythmic solfge. At the partial synthesis level, teachers use tonal and rhythmic activities to
make sure that students can recognize tonality and meter. When this recognition is achieved, a
student is ready to learn musical symbols through reading and writing, and the symbolic
association level is reached. It should be noted that students must have mastered the aural/oral
and verbal association stages before music symbols are introduced in order to ensure that they
will always be able to hear internally the music written in notational form.
Gordon introduced the concept of audiation to explain the process that takes place when
one hears music silently through recall. Through this process, mental hearing happens, even
though no physical sound is present. But audiation is more than just a musical form of auditory
imagery. It is a cognitive process by which the brain gives meaning to musical sounds, just like
thinking gives meaning to speech. Audiation necessarily implies music comprehension and this
is achieved through aural training and verbal association where, in the initial stages, each tonal
12
and rhythm pattern is attached to a syllable name. This association is a key element of good ear
training abilities.
The Yamaha method (Lancaster, 1984-85; Wagner, 1985) is an example of another
curriculum centred on ear training and musicianship development. According to the Yamaha
Music Foundation (2003), their research has shown that young students are developing aural
skills much more rapidly than other skills required in music lessons, like manual dexterity. They
also found that it is difficult for young children to read music and play at the same time. So,
instead of teaching music reading first and instructing them to learn pieces through note reading,
each new song is taught through solfge using proper syllables (do, re, mi), and this experience
of singing a song in fixed do is then easily transferable to keyboard playing. The sequence of
learning begins with listening, then imitating with the singing voice, followed by the attachment
of syllable names, then the presentation of music notation and finally the performance on the
keyboard. This approach helps students to internalize the music they are learning; it stimulates
and cultivates musical responsiveness to sound as it establishes a strong connection between
written signs and aural representation. In this progression, it is not enough to memorize a sound
pattern and to reproduce it on an instrument; a student needs to acquire a musical vocabulary of
tonal and rhythm syllables to insure that notation is strongly link to a mental sound
representation.
The Suzuki method works differently and does not take into consideration the
development of such skills as audiation or sight-signing. Suzuki trainer Marilyn Taggart
(Comeau, 1998), agrees that Suzuki teachers dont really do sight-singing per se. The ear
13
training seems to take care of itself because of the listening (p. 71).10 Nowhere in Suzukis
writings are these skills ever addressed. The mother-tongue progression follows a different
sequence of learning. It starts with strong aural training as students listen repeatedly to a series of
songs. When these songs are well integrated, students recall the patterns from memory and
attempt through trial and error to find the right notes on the instrument, thus the expression
playing by ear. The learning progression moves from the aural stage to the actual playing on
an instrument. A direct link is established between pitch and rhythm patterns registered in the
memory and the reproduction of these patterns on a musical instrument. In contrast to Gordons
audiation or Yamahas solfge, an important component is missing: pitch and rhythm patterns
are not labelled before performance happens on the instrument.11 The progression bypasses any
form of conceptualisation of basic musical patterns. The fact that Suzuki students listen
repeatedly to their recordings and learn to play the piece by ear is no guarantee that they will
develop a good cognitive comprehension of how music is organised or that any internal aural
representation will be activated when music notation is later introduced.
There are other issues surrounding the development of ear training in the Suzuki method
that are worth mentioning. Interestingly enough, methods that teach music to very young
children often claim that they produce a high percentage of students with perfect pitch. While
there is no scientific evidence that methods like Yamaha or Kelly Kirby Kindergarten Piano
Method are in fact developing perfect pitch, it is still revealing that teachers from these methods
10
A few Suzuki teachers mention doing sight-singing or dictation, but according to Comeaus interviews (1998),
what is being done never goes beyond the introductory level. More importantly, these teachers are the exception and
not the norm; Taggarts comment is certainly more representative of most Suzuki teachers.
11
It is interesting to note that her dissertation, Medford (2003) developed a program that combines certain elements
of the Kodly and Orff approach with the Suzuki method. Repertoire is first introduced through solfge, using
Kodlys syllables and hand signals, and only then is a student asked to play the pieces on his instrument. A
masters thesis by Krigbaum (2005) applied Gordons music learning theory to Suzuki training by developing an
audiation-based approach for Suzuki violin instruction. And finally, Kitts masters thesis (1993) looked into the
benefit of introducing Gordons rhythmic learning sequence to Suzuki piano students.
14
are noticing that ability among many of their students, but no such trend seems to have been
observed by Suzuki teachers. Nowhere in the literature, even in magazine like the American
Suzuki Journal, do we see any testimony that this method contributes to developing perfect pitch,
although this method is used with students that are precisely at the critical age for developing
such a skill. This could possibly be explained by the fact that this method does not attempt to
develop an association between a specific sound and its syllable name, and no labelling of aural
experience is introduced, something that is essential in the development of perfect pitch.
Another interesting point in this debate is linked directly to the development of ear
playing. Although the Suzuki method requires that students listen repeatedly to their recordings
and then find the notes on their instrument, the actual process for playing by ear is ambiguous
and not clearly outlined in any of the literature12. Krigbaum (2005) points out that Suzuki himself
never offers a concrete process for how this learning should happen. That confusion often results
in Suzuki students being taught to perform their pieces by rote, a process much closer to
imitation than to playing by ear13. The teacher or the parent demonstrates while the child
observes, then imitates. In certain cases, a child is taught step-by-step, note-by-note, how to
perform a piece of music through demonstration and verbal instruction (p.77). Although
students are performing without notation, they engage in a more passive process of imitation that
does not reflect the ability to play by ear. The learning sequence where a student is searching for
12
There are in fact very few teaching resources that offer concrete suggestions on how to actually teach ear playing.
In Jump Right In, the sound-before-sign method book by Grunow, Gordon and Azzara (2000), and in Lowe and
Gordon (2004) Music Moves for Piano, the authors recommend that students first be taught to sing certain songs by
rote, then be invited to perform those songs on their instrument, with no further instruction on how to make this
happen.
13
Adopting the definition in McPherson (2005), Musco (2010) provides a clear distinction between playing by ear,
a performance of pre-existing music learned aurally without the aid of notation (p. 49), rote learning, which
entails aural processes but may also involve verbal or visual hints (p. 50) and modeling, a powerful tool for
learning (p.50) where the students get to imitate the teachers demonstrations. She then points out that many
teaching materials promoting a sound-before-sight approach actually provide rote learning activities as opposed to
ear playing ones. She also concedes that existing research often makes it difficult to distinguish ear playing from
rote learning as the treatment protocols often mix the two.
15
the notes of a song on his instrument through a recall process of memorized patterns might
actually not be well understood by many Suzuki teachers and in the end, the dominant sequence
of teaching might rely more on demonstration and rote learning then on actual ear playing, a
process that would generate little ear training. Also, once Suzuki students learn to read music,
they start to depend on their music books to learn new pieces. They are still required to listen to
their recordings, but the visual information from their music books often becomes their main
guide. It could be argued that at that point, students often stop developing any form of aural
skills.
Concerns about reading skills
Poor music reading has been the most criticized aspect of the Suzuki method. Teachers
who depend on music notation to teach beginners have seriously questioned the absence of note
reading in the early stages of music learning, suggesting that students might never reach an
acceptable level of reading proficiency since they learn to rely so heavily on their ears rather than
their eyes (How Teachers View, 1996; Musco, 2001; Hargrave, 2003; Garson, 2005;). Suzuki
teachers do not agree with this critique, but they nevertheless have recognized that Suzuki
students often have the reputation of being poor readers (Ballance, 2009; Erbin, 2009).
Unfortunately, few studies have looked specifically at the effect of learning to play by ear on
reading skills (Musco, 2010). However, when reviewing existing empirical research, it is
possible to identify certain trends that clearly suggest there is no reason to be concerned.
Three studies have tested groups of instrumental students to find out if there is a link
between the ability to play by ear and sight reading. Luce (1958, 1965) looked at the relationship
between students performance in sight-reading and their ability to reproduce short musical
phrases by ear. A group of 98 high school instrumentalists were tested on original sight-reading
16
and ear-playing tests and the results indicate a significant relationship (r =.50, p
.01) between
the two skills. She concluded that instrumental music education should include both, music
reading and playing by ear. McPherson (1993) developed a theoretical model that outlines five
distinct types of musical performance: sight-reading, performing rehearsed music, playing from
memory, playing by ear, and improvising. Using a sample of high school instrumentalists, he
wanted to clarify the relationship between these five musical skills. His findings show a positive
correlation of r=.40 (and of .55 among the upper group of participants) between the ability to
play by ear and the level of proficiency in sight-reading. His study also suggests that playing by
ear contributes to overall musical growth and provides more enjoyable and meaningful learning.
Bernhard (2004) investigated the effects of singing and playing melodies by rote in beginning
band students. Statistics revealed a significant relationship of r =.67 between playing by ear and
sight-reading. The results of these studies suggest that skills in playing by ear correlate with
skills in music reading.
A number of other studies have looked at the impact of using a sound/aural approach to
teach music students and the results are fairly consistent (Musco, 2010). No negative effect has
been observed on sight-reading ability when aural modeling is used, and in some cases, there is
evidence of the effectiveness of an aural approach to improve sight reading skills. An earlier
study (Musco 2006) involves learning melodies by ear in order to play in an unfamiliar key. The
results suggest that playing by ear may contribute to skill development, but most interestingly for
us, both the experimental group (aural approach) and the control group improved significantly in
sight-reading. In other words, learning songs by ear has the same positive impact on sight
reading as a note-based approach. Smith (2006) examined the effect of playing songs by ear on
the musical performance of middle-school instrumental music students. Subtest measurements
17
included music reading, aural response and ear-tune performance. Though trends in the data
could be identified, none proved to be statistically significant. But it was clear that students who
learned to perform by ear did not show a decrease in reading skill. The author concluded that it
goes against prevailing attitudes that a students skill in reading music will deteriorate if they
spend time learning and performing songs by ear.
Haston (2004) assessed the effectiveness of teaching beginning wind instrumentalists
using a sound-before-sight approach. The experimental group received an aural/modeling
emphasis (singing while fingering their instruments, play-by-ear activities, call and response, and
playing from printed music) and the other group had a visual emphasis (playing only from
printed music). The aural/modeling group scored higher on sight-reading posttests, though not
significantly. There were clearly no statistically significant differences between the sight-reading
abilities of wind instrumentalists taught with an aural/modeling emphasis and those taught with a
visual emphasis. The author concluded that teaching with an aural/modeling emphasis does not
hamper students music performance skills, and may in fact aid them.
Sperti (1970) adapted certain aspects of the Suzuki method to the teaching of the clarinet
and conducted experiments to test the effectiveness of two different pedagogical approaches: one
favouring playing by ear and the other one focusing on note reading. Both groups received class
instruction using the same instructional material, but with different pedagogical procedures. The
control group received 32 hours of established teaching practices based on note reading. The
experimental group received 16 hours of lessons based on a comprehensive listening program
and the use of rote teaching, after which they got an additional 16 weeks of score reading
instruction. Both groups were tested for sight-reading performance and for subjective elements
of performance like tone quality, technique and interpretation. The achievement of the subjects
18
in the experimental group was significantly superior to the control group in all categories of
performance. This clearly showed that not only was there no negative impact on the ability to
read music with the group of students who first learned to play the clarinet by ear, but they
achieved superior results in sight-reading.
Fincher (1983) evaluated the impact of rote playing upon sight-reading skill development
in group classes of beginning adult piano students. This study attempted to answer long-held
beliefs by piano teachers that a student will learn to sight read faster if he does not hear the
pieces played in advance, which will lead to playing by ear instead of developing reading skills.
The experiment was conducted with four classes of beginning adult piano students where two
experimental groups learned to play by rote before seeing the printed page and two control
groups learned through reading only. The rote students listened several times to a piece to grasp
its aural image, then tried to play by imitation; only after several attempts of playing by ear, was
the printed page introduced and the student continued to learn the piece by reading. Results
indicate that the aural learning approach dramatically affected sight-reading skills in a positive
way as the students from the ear-playing groups scored much higher in both pitch-reading and
rhythm-reading. This study suggests that playing the melody by rote during the prestudy
procedure enhanced the impact on sight-reading skill development.
Glenn (1999) compared two methods of teaching strings to sixth-grade beginning
students over a full school-year period. One emphasized rote instruction in the early stages and
the other was notation-centred from the start. Results indicate that students in the ear-playing
approach performed as well as the students in the notation-based method in all performance tests,
including sight-reading. Interestingly enough, students who received the extensive period of rote
instruction demonstrated a significantly higher rate of continuation: 70% pursued lessons after
19
the test year compared to only 32% in the notation-centred class. Also, the rote-learning group
reported being more motivated to play music than the other group. Glenn recommended that
students should get to a level where technical gestures have become automatic when they play
before being introduced to musical notation.
Studies looking into the effect of playing by ear on the development of music reading
skills may not be numerous, but all the experimental investigations that have been reviewed here
show evidence that playing by ear does not have a negative impact on reading abilities, and
might have a positive effect.
Conclusion
It was interesting to note that Shinichi Suzuki developed the mother-tongue approach at
the same time as many other music educators and researchers were debating similar ideas. The
period was particularly favourable for associating music learning and language development and
many educators came to the conclusion that music should first be learned through ear playing.
This paper has shown that there is strong evidence supporting the value of playing by ear when
first learning a musical instrument; the sequence should proceed from sound to symbol so
students develop the ability to think in sound. However, it is also clear that playing by ear does
not guarantee that when musical symbols are introduced, students will automatically be able to
inwardly hear and comprehend notation. The importance of labelling sounds is an important step
to insure good aural skills. This might be even more essential for students who are learning an
instrument like the piano where pitch is a given and where the temptation to learn by rote is very
strong because of the complexity of playing with both hands. There is real concern that Suzuki
students could develop poor aural skills. On the other hand, there is no evidence supporting the
belief that beginners who are taught by ear will never reach the same level of reading proficiency
20
as students who are introduced to notation at their initial lessons. There are no empirical studies
that have ever demonstrated that sound-before-sight instruction harms students abilities to read.
While some studies indicate no effect on music reading abilities, other research shows that
playing by ear improves them.
It is clear that more research is needed to develop a better understanding of the effects of
the mother-tongue approach as applied in the Suzuki method. Several researchers discussed
above (Fincher, 1983; Glenn, 1999; Haston, 2004; Musco, 2006; Sperti, 1970) developed their
own experiments where they provided music lessons through an aural-modeling approach and
measured its impact on various skills. But no studies have been conducted so far that have
specifically assessed how well Suzuki students can hear notation inwardly before reproducing it
on an instrument. It would be interesting to evaluate the mental process followed by Suzuki
students once reading has been introduced. Are they progressing from the visual stimulus to the
proper movement action (like any conventional student) or do they progress from the visual
stimulus to the auditory stimulus (inwardly heard tone), then the anticipation of motor act and
finally the movement action? There has also been no study to evaluate how many Suzuki
students are actually learning new pieces by ear, through a process of trial of error, and how
many are learning through rote learning where they observe and repeat the same action.
Assuming that Suzuki students do learn to play by ear in the early stage of music lessons, there
have been no studies to demonstrate if those students retain that ability after musical reading is
introduced. Once reading has been superimposed on the process of playing by ear, if the ability
to play by ear is not continued and the relationship sound-action is not consistently emphasized,
it could be replaced by the symbol-action relationship. It would be interesting to evaluate 1) to
21
what extent Suzuki students keep developing ear-playing skills and, 2) whether they have greater
facility than non-Suzuki students with skills associated with ear playing, like improvisation.
22
References
Arimitsu, T. (1982). The Suzuki Method of Instruction Applied to Art Education for Young
Children in America. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 0550189)
American Music Teacher. (2011). Retrieved February 2011.
http://www.mtna.org/Publications/AmericanMusicTeacher/tabid/296/Default.aspx
American Suzuki Journal. (2011). Retrieved February 2011. http://suzukiassociation.org/journal/
Ballance, R. M. (2009). Lessons in note reading from a tenor viol experience. American Suzuki
Journal, 37(4), p. 54.
Balodis, F. (1993). Sunshine. Kanata, ON: Music for Young Children.
Balodis. F. (1996). Sunrise. Kanata, ON: Music for Young Children.
Bamberger, J. (1996). Turning music theory on its ear. International Journal of Computers and
Mathematical Learning, 1(1), 33-55.
Bamberger, J. (1999). Learning from the children we teach. Bulletin for the Council for Research
in Music Education, 142, 48-74.
Bernhard, H. C., II. (2004). The effects of tonal training on the melodic ear playing and sight
reading achievement of beginning wind instrumentalist. Contributions to Music
Education, 31(1), 91-107.
Bigler, C. G. and Lloyd-Watts, V. (1979). Studying Suzuki Piano: More than Music. Athens,
OH: Senzay.
British Journal of Music Education. (2011). Retrieved February 2011.
http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayJournal?jid=BME
23
Chang, D.-L. (1999). The effects of different incidental listening experiences on performance
achievement and developmental music aptitude of first-grade Suzuki violin and non-violin
students. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 9948078)
Clavier Companion. (2011). Retrieved February 2011. http://www.claviercompanion.com/
Comeau, G. (1998). 10 Teachers Viewpoints on Suzuki Piano. Ottawa, ON: CFORP.
Couperin, F. (1716/1974). LArt de toucher le clavecin. (M. Halford, Trans.). Van Nuys, CA:
Alfred Publishing Co. (Original work published 1716).
Durand, M. (1996). Approche psycho-pdagogique de lenseignement du piano. (Piano pedagogy
course given at Universit de Montral).
Erbin, Z. (2009). A mother-tongue approach to reading. American Suzuki Journal, 37(4), p. 5559.
Fincher, B. J. (1983). The effects of playing the melody by rote during the pre-study procedure
upon sight-reading skill development of beginning class piano students. Retrieved from
ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 8403971)
Garson, A. (2005). Suzuki myths. American Suzuki Journal, 33(3), p. 56-58.
Glenn, K. A. (1999). Rote vs. note: The relationship of working memory capacity to performance
and continuation in beginning string classes. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital
Dissertations. (AAT 9927738)
Gordon, E. E. (1984). Learning Sequences in Music: Skill, Content, and Patterns. Chicago, IL:
G.I.A. Publications.
Gordon, E. E. (2001). Preparatory Audiation, Audiation, and Music Learning Theory: A
Handbook of a Comprehensive Music Learning Sequence. Chicago, IL: GIA
Publications.
24
Gordon, E. E. (2003). A Music Learning Theory for Newborn and Young Children. Chicago, IL:
GIA Publications.
Gordon, E. E. (2004). The Aural-Visual Experience of Music Literacy: Reading and Writing
Music Notation. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications.
Grunow, R. F., Gordon, E. E., & Azzara, C. D. (2000). Jump Right In. Chicago, IL: GIA
Publications.
Hargrave, C.W. (2003). A Suzuki reading method. American Suzuki Journal, 31(2), p. 36-38.
Hargrave, C.W. (2010). Suzuki piano in the Americas: Our roots. American Suzuki Journal,
38(2), 16-18.
Haston, W. A. (2004). Comparison of a visual and an aural approach to beginning wind
instrument instruction. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 3132535)
Herman, E. (1981). Shinichi Suzuki: The Man and His Philosophy. Secaucus, NJ: Warner Bros.
How Teachers View Playing by Ear. (1996, September). Clavier, 35(7), 6-9.
Hwang, L.-C. (1995). The development of lesson plan sourcebook for Suzuki piano school.
Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 9539820)
International Journal of Music Education. (2011). Retrieved February 2011.
http://ijm.sagepub.com/
Journal of Music Teacher Education. (2011). Retrieved February 2011. http://jmt.sagepub.com/
Kataoka, H. (1985). Thoughts on the Suzuki Piano School. Princeton: Birch Tree Group Ltd.
Kelly Kirby, M. B. (1939). The Kelly Kirby Kindergarten Piano Method. Oakville, ON: F. Harris
Music.
Kendall, J. D. (1978). The Suzuki Violin Method in American Music Education. Secaucus, NJ:
Warner Bros.
25
Kitts, W. L. (1993). The effects of Edwin Gordon's rhythmic learning sequence on private
traditional piano and Suzuki piano students. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital
Dissertations. (AAT 1356576)
Kochevitsky, G. (1967). The Art of Piano Playing. Secaucus, NJ: Summy-Birchard.
Koppelman, D. (1978). Intoducing Suzuki Piano. San Diego, CA: Dichter Press.
Krigbaum, C. D. (2005). The development of an audiation-based approach to Suzuki violin
instruction based on the application of Edwin E. Gordons Music Learning Theory.
Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 1427646)
Lancaster, E. F. (1984-85). The American Beginning Piano Method. The Yamaha Music
Education system. The Piano Quarterly, 33(128), 19-33.
Landers, R. (1984). The Talent Education School of Shinichi SuzukiAn Analysis. Smithtown,
New York: Exposition Press.
Lee, A. J. (2007). Two non-traditional cello methods for young beginning cello students: A
mixed methods study. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 3291782)
Lee, S.-Y. (1992). The Suzuki beginner: A teacher's guide to the Suzuki principles of violin.
Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 9230395)
Lee, S.-Y. (2001). Left-hand technique in the Suzuki Cello Method: An analytical overview and
comparison with contemporary cello pedagogy. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital
Dissertations. (AAT 0802806)
Lo, S. Y.-D. (1993). A reading course for Suzuki piano students. Retrieved from ProQuest
Digital Dissertations. (AAT 9416618)
Lowe, M. & Gordon, E. E. (2004). Music Moves for Piano. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications.
26
Luce, J. R. (1958). Sight-reading and ear-playing abilities related to the training and
background of instrumental music students. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital
Dissertations. (AAT 5805901)
Luce, J. R. (1965). Sight-reading and ear-playing abilities as related to instrumental music
students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 13(2), 101-109.
Mainwaring, J. (1941). The meaning of musicianship: A problem in the teaching of music.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 11(3), 205-214.
Mainwaring, J. (1947). The assessment of musical ability. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 17(1), 83-96.
Mainwaring, J. (1951). Psychological factors in the teaching of music. Part II: Applied
musicianship. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 21(3), 199-213.
McPherson, G. E. (1993). Factors and abilities influencing the development of visual, aural and
creative performance skills in music and their educational implications. Retrieved from
ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 9317278)
McPherson, G. E. (2005). From child to musician; Skill development during the beginning
stages of learning an instrument. Psychology of music, 33, 5-35.
McPherson, G. E. and Gabrielsson, A. (2002). From sound to sign. In R. Parncutt and G. E.
McPherson (Eds.), The Science and Psychology of Music Performance, (pp. 99-115).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Medford, C. D. (2003). Teaching musicality from the beginning of a child's instruction: The
GBYSO Intensive Community Program and how it incorporates the philosophies of
Kodaly, Orff, and Suzuki. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 3084848)
27
Menczel, V. (1997). The application of the Suzuki method in Israel and the United States: A
comparative case study. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 9737473)
Moorhead, M. N. (2005). The Suzuki Method: A comparative analysis of the perceptual/cognitive
listening development in third grade students trained in the Suzuki, traditional, and
modified Suzuki music methods. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT
3167512)
Musco, A. M. (2001). Survey: Rote learning in instrumental music. Unpublished survey reported
in Musco (2010).
Musco, A. M. (2006). The effects of learning songs by ear in multiple keys on pitch accuracy and
attitudes of band students. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT
3251862)
Musco, A. M. (2010). Playing by Ear: Is Expert Opinion Supported by Research? Bulletin of the
Council for Research in Music Education, 184, 49-63.
Music Educators Journal. (2011). Retrieved February 2011. http://mej.sagepub.com/
ONeill, A. A. M. (2003). Parent as home teacher of Suzuki cello, violin, and piano students:
Observation and analysis of Suzuki method practice sessions. Retrieved from ProQuest
Digital Dissertations. (AAT 3119251)
Perkins, M. M. (1993). A comparative study of the violin playing techniques developed by Kato
Havas, Paul Rolland, and Shinichi Suzuki. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital
Dissertations. (AAT 9320157)
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J. and Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: An information processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83 (2), 157171.
28
29
Sperti, J. (1970). Adaptation of certain aspects of the Suzuki method to the teaching of the
clarinet: An experimental investigation testing the comparative effectiveness of two
different pedagogical methodologies. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations.
(AAT 7124833)
Stamou, L. (1998). The effect of Suzuki instruction and early childhood music experiences on
developmental music aptitude and performance achievement of beginning Suzuki string
students. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 9909369)
Starr, C. and Starr, W. (1983). To Learn With Love. Secaucus, NJ: Warner Bros.
Suzuki Association of the Americas, (2010). Retrieved September 26, 2010, from
http://suzukiassociation.org/
Suzuki, S. (1969). Nurtured by Love. A New Approach to Education (Waltraud Suzuki, Trans.).
New York: Exposition Press.
Suzuki, S. (1981). Ability Development From Age Zero. Athens, OH: Senzay. (Original work
published in Japanese in 1969).
Suzuki S. (1986). Talent Education for Young Children. New Albany, IN: World-Wide Press.
(Original work published in Japanese in 1969).
Suzuki, S. (1989). Shinichi Suzuki: His speeches and essays. Secaucus, NJ: Warner Bros.
Suzuki, S. (1993). How to Teach Suzuki Piano. Secaucus, NJ: Warner Bros.. Suzuki Association
of the Americas (2010). Retrieved October 2010. http://suzukiassociation.org/
Tommis, Y., and Fazey, D.M.A. (1999). The acquisition of the pitch elements of music literacy
skills by 34 year-old pre-school children: A comparison of two methods. Psychology of
Music, 27(2), 230-244.
Velleman, B. L. (1978). Speaking of jazz. Music Educators Journal, 65 (2), 2831.
30
Wagner, J. (1985). Ear before Eye: The Yamaha Method. Clavier, 24 (1), 10-13.
Whiteside, A. (1997). Abby Whiteside on Piano Playing. Portland OR: Amadeus Press. (Original
work published 1938).
Williams, M. K. (2000). An alternative class piano approach based on selected Suzuki
principles. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. (AAT 9976972)
Yamaha Music Foundation (2003). Yamaha Music Education System. Yamaha Corp.
31