Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online from Scribd
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 17
Running head: TEST ANXIETY 1
The Affect of Test Anxiety on Student Performance on Tests
Alaa K. Abd-El-Hafez Long Island University
April 2014 TEST ANXIETY 2 The Affect of Test Anxiety on Student Performance on Tests Over the past decade, there has been increased testing in schools and pressures associated with it (Lee, Lowe, & Sena, 2007; McDonald, 2012). Legislation, such as No Child Left Behind and Race to the Top, have created test based accountability systems, increasing the importance placed on student test outcomes. Since school and student performance are determined through the use of high-stakes tests, it is critical to examine any variable which may interfere with the valid measurement of student achievement and school effectiveness (Chamberlian, Daly, & Spalding, 2011; Embse & Hasson, 2012; Lee et al., 2007). One such variable is test anxiety. Test anxiety has been shown to impair test performance by many studies (DiBattista & Gosse, 2006; Embse & Hasson, 2012; Mcdonald, 2001; Putwain, 2008). However, the relationship is not simply linear and often moderated by other factors and interactions (Chamberlian et al., 2011; McDonald, 2001). This paper will explore the impact of test anxiety on student performance on exams using the literature from quantitative research studies. Test Anxiety Test anxiety is the set of cognitive, physiological, and behavioral responses related to concerns about possible failure or poor performance on an exam or a similar evaluative situation (Bodas, Ollendick, & Sovani, 2008, p. 387). Test anxiety, however, is not a diagnosable disorder; yet many students from elementary school to college have been reported to experience test anxiety. One study has reported that 25% to 40% of school children experience test anxiety (Bodas et al., 2008). Another study, Barterian, Carlson, Goforth, Embse, and Segool (2013), have reported lesser percentage of school-aged children experiencing test anxiety, 10% to 30%. The study also reported that about 10% of children are highly test anxious and TEST ANXIETY 3 experience impairment in test performance as a result. DiBattista and Gosse (2006) stated that the percent of college students who experience test anxiety vary from 10% to 15%. Test anxiety was found to be more prevalent among minority ethnic groups (Putwain, 2008). Specifically, higher levels of test anxiety were found among African American elementary school-aged children with a reported percentage of 41%. Female students report higher level of test anxiety than male students (Barterian, Carlson, Goforth, Embse, and Segool, 2013). Students with learning disabilities were more likely to be test anxious. They were also more likely to experience the debilitating effects of test anxiety. Test anxiety was found to be a significant predictor of their reading and mathematics achievement scores (Lee et al., 2007). Research has also shown that test anxiety is prevalent in many cultures across the globe with higher levels of test anxiety reported from Islamic culture than from Western European cultures (Bodas et. al., 2008; Putwain, 2008). In educational testing, test anxiety may obstruct the true potential of students. With an increase in testing and accountability of students, teachers, and schools, a variable such as test anxiety must be explored to ensure the genuine measurement of performance (Chamberlian, Daly, & Spalding, 2011; Embse & Hasson, 2012; Lee et al., 2007). Research on test anxiety and its impact has flourished. In fact, research on test anxiety is more widespread than it appears because test anxiety is sometimes hidden under broader constructs (Pekrun & Stober, 2004). Of the multiple dimensions of test anxiety, it is the worry component that is related to lower examination performance (Eum and Rice 2010; Putwain, 2008). Some studies have shown that worry and sensitivity to distraction independently predict performance on examination (Lee et al., 2007; Pekrun & Stober, 2004). Historical Overview of Test Anxiety TEST ANXIETY 4 Studies related to test anxiety were conducted as early as 1914 but first discussed in depth in 1932 by Luria (Burns, 2004; Pekrun and Stober, 2004). It had received considerable attention since then. The relation between test anxiety and test performance was first examined in 1952 and a negative relationship was reported (Pekrun and Stober, 2004). Higher test scores were found to be correlated with lower test performance (Burns, 2004). Highly test-anxious individuals were more self-critical and more likely to experience performance-interfering worry during examinations than were individuals who were observed to be low in test anxiety (Burns, 2004, p. 119). Furthermore, highly test-anxious students were three times more likely to drop out of college (Burns, 2004). The Test Anxiety Questionnaire was developed to assess individual differences in adults and the Test Anxiety Scale for Children (Pekrun and Stober, 2004). In the 1960s and early 1970s, several contributions were made to test anxiety research. The first contribution was a distinction between test anxiety as a transitory state and as a stable personality trait. A second contribution is a distinction between the four dimensions of test anxiety: (a) worry; (b) emotionality; (c) interference; and (d) lack of confidence (DiBattista & Gosse, 2006; Pekrun & Stober, 2004; Lee et al., 2007). Advances in test anxiety and its effect on cognitive performance model construction occurred in the 1970s and 1980s. Since 1952, the number of publications on test anxiety exceeded 1000. It has been shown by educational psychologists that test anxiety is an important factor in primary, secondary, and tertiary education (Pekrun & Stober, 2004). Test Anxiety and Working Memory Test anxiety is a complex multi-dimensional, and dynamic construct (Bodas et. al, 2008, p. 388). After reviewing the literature, Mowbray (2012) found that individuals experience TEST ANXIETY 5 test anxiety in two dimensions of emotionality and cognitions (worry). The cognitive aspect of test anxiety was found to have the most impact/connection with test performance. According to attentional control theory, anxiety impacts the ability to (a) suppress task-irrelevant stimuli (inhibition); (b) shift attention between multiple tasks; and (c) the phonological loop. Anxiety, in general, is believed to promote bottom-up attentional processes. That is, anxiety results in an involuntary attention shift toward relevant sensory events such as perceived threat related stimuli. Unable to disengage from such stimuli, ones attention is diverted and loses focus on a task. This is referred to in the literature as retrieval failure test anxiety. Students who know the material fail to recall it on a test due to cognitive interference (Mowbrary, 2012). Attentional control theory states that anxiety reduces inhibition, overwhelming the working memory with task-irrelevant thoughts. Also, highly anxious students can only work at one task at a time. Participants with high levels of test anxiety were found to take longer to complete tasks including examination tasks, especially if that task required a great deal of attention. Students with the same level of test anxiety are not affected similarly, those with greater working memory capacities are better buffered against performance deficits of test anxiety. Highly anxious students also showed deficits in encoding study material. This is due to interference with the phonological loop. The relationship between test anxiety and exam performance can only be explained by both failure to obtain, encode, and organize exam material and failure to retrieve previously learned information during a testing situation (Broader & Musch, 1999; Mowbrary, 2012). There are several limitations to Mowbrarys (2012) study. First, attentional control theory addresses anxiety in general rather than specifically test anxiety. Anxiety is a diagnosable disorder but test anxiety is not (Bodas et al., 2008). Research has shown that instruments TEST ANXIETY 6 measuring anxiety in general are not necessarily applicable to test anxiety (Burns, 2004; Mowbrary, 2012). Second, working memory has been shown to be unrelated to test anxiety. Third, the interventions (cognitive therapies) offered in the study do not have a lasting effect, further reduce efficiency, and can actually be detrimental to exam performance (Mowbrary, 2012). Test Anxiety at the College Level Burns (2004) found no significant relationships between anxiety at the time of an exam and performance on that exam. There was also no significant relationship between anxiety at the time of a test and the final grade for a course. However, a positive relationship was found between performance expectations at a test and levels of anxiety at that time, higher expectations resulted in higher anxiety levels. Positive relationships were also found between the scores on previous exams and the level of anxiety on the subsequent exam. That is, the higher the scores on previous exams, the higher the anxiety level on the next exam. The study also did not find a significant relationship between poor preparedness and number of absences and anxiety at the time of an exam (Burns, 2004). In contrast to the findings found in Burns (2004), DiBattista and Gosse (2006) found a negative relationship between the level of test anxiety, as measured by Revised Test Anxiety Scale (RTAS) scores, and various measures of test performance. However, DiBattista and Gosse (2006) found that immediate feedback, through using the Immediate Feedback Assessment Technique (IFAT) answer form, on an exam did not increase the levels of test anxiety in students but actually reduced it for the majority of them. Furthermore, students preference for the IFAT was not related to test anxiety or any measure of test performance (DiBattista and Gosse, 2006). TEST ANXIETY 7 Broder and Musch (1999) compared how two rival models of test anxiety (the interference model and the deficit model) explained academic performance on a statistics test. In the interference model, test anxiety is an interfering agent. Highly test anxious students know the content but unable to recall or retrieve it during the test. In the deficient model, anxiety is merely an emotional reaction that accompanies the awareness of not being prepared for a test. Highly test anxious students have difficulty acquiring content to begin with due to poor study habits. The study found that the academic performance of highly test-anxious students is affected directly by their lack of knowledge (deficit model) and indirectly by distractive thinking during the test situation itself. Thus, the study found that a mixture of those two models best describe the academic performance of highly test anxious students (Broader & Musch, 1999). Eum and Rice (2010) explored the relationship between test anxiety (cognitive), perfectionism, goal orientation, and academic performance, as assessed by GPA and word recall task. Most test anxious students performed poorer on the recall task. This is supported by the interference model (Broder and Musch, 1999). There was a negative relationship between test anxiety and academic performance. Those findings are similar to those found by DiBattista and Gosse (2006). Eum and Rice (2010) also found a positive relationship between cognitive test anxiety and maladaptive perfectionism (high expectations and extreme self-blame for failing to meet those expectations) and avoidance goal orientations. Furthermore, most test anxious students were found to be female. These findings were supported by Barterian (2013). It is important to note however, that the majority of participants were females (62%). The study was also limited in that it only assessed one dimension of test anxiety (cognitive). Similar to DiBattista and Gosse (2006), Broader and Musch (1999) found a statistically significant negative relationship between test anxiety and exam performance. However, Broader TEST ANXIETY 8 and Musch (1999) found that test anxiety explained only about 5% of the variance. Furthermore, math skills not test anxiety was the most important factor, explaining 9% of the variance, in exam performance. The authors concluded that in a statistics performance exam, academic skills (math skills) are more important than task-irrelevant thoughts during a test. Ability, not negative thoughts during the test, was the most important factor in predicting math test performance. The results of Broader and Musch (1999) are actually more significant than they appear to be. As the authors have pointed out, most test anxiety take place on a statistics exam. Even then, test anxiety only explained 5% of the variance of exam performance. These results imply that test anxiety would be even lower for other disciplines (Broader & Musch, 1999). It is difficult to generalize the results of Broader and Musch (1999) to other disciplines. The authors picked the discipline where college students have reported the most test anxiety on. Second, the three variables examined in the study (test anxiety, math skills, and study habits) explained only 25% of the variance in exam performance. The authors could have explained more of the variance had they used more variables as previous studies have done. Third, as previous research has shown, using evaluative measures such as the Test Anxiety instrument and the Study Habits instruments put more pressure on highly-test anxious student. They induce more anxiety and so may not be true reflections of the students. Furthermore, relaying on self- reports such as the Study Habits instruments put the validity of this instrument into question (Broader & Musch, 1999). Test Anxiety at the K-12 Level Barterian et al. (2013) was the first study to examine the differences in elementary students reported test anxiety between a high-stakes test (NCLB achievement testing) and classroom testing. Three hundred thirty-five students from Grades 3 to 5 participated in the TEST ANXIETY 9 study. The study found that students reported a higher level of test anxiety on the high-stakes test than on the classroom tests. More cognitive and psychological symptoms of test anxiety were reported about the NCLB assessment (Barterian et al., 2013). Contrary to these findings, Bodas et al. (2008) found no statistically significant relationship between high stakes testing and test anxiety in Indian elementary school children. The difference in findings may be due to cultural differences (western versus eastern). Bodas et al. (2008) reported that parents and available resources play a big role in reducing any test anxiety associated with high stakes testing. Higher expectations of Indian children could have led to desensitization to test anxiety (Bodas et al., 2008). Bodas et al. (2008) was also restricted in that the instrument used after translation was weakly reliable. The reliability coefficients of the instruments were in the range of moderate to lower than those reported for the original instruments (Bodas et al., 2008). Barterian et al. (2013) also found that students who experienced higher levels of test anxiety also experienced associated impairments in test performance. Specifically, the cognitive symptoms of test anxiety more than the psychological symptoms were associated with the impairments in test performance. The findings of this study also validated teachers perceptions of their students test anxiety levels on the NCLB exam. Although 25% of the students reported high levels of test anxiety on the NCLB testing versus classroom testing, 15% of the students reported less test anxiety about the NCLB assessment (Barterian et al., 2013). While Barterian et al. (2013) focused on elementary school children, Chamberlian, Daly, and Spalding (2011) focused on high school students, ages 16 to 19. The two studies also differed in their instruments. While Chamerlian et al. (2011) did not use a high stakes test (NCTM) as Barterian et al. (2013) did, the study did use a highly stressful oral exam, a mock TEST ANXIETY 10 French language speaking test. Chamberlian et al. (2011) examined the impact of test anxiety on academic performance, a mock exam, and on heart rate. The study found a negative relationship between test anxiety and academic performance, as measured by UK General Certificate of Secondary Education (GCSE) scores. However, no statistically significant relationship was found between test anxiety and performance on the mock exam. This is inconsistent with the findings from Barterian et al. (2013). Chamberlian et al. (2011) was limited in that the exam used in the study was a mock exam rather than an actual exam. Had the actual exam been used, the impact of test anxiety on test performance could have been statistically significant. Furthermore, the sample size was relatively small compared to Barterian et al. (2013). However, the instruments used to measure test anxiety in both studies were restricted in that they only measured two dimensions (worry and emotionality) of test anxiety (Chamberlian et al., 2011). Putwain (2008) examined the impact of test anxiety on test performance among high school students. The study found a negative relationship between test anxiety and test performance, as measured by GCSE examination scores. Higher levels of test anxiety were associated with lower GCSE examination performance. As in Chamberlian et al. (2011), the instrument used in Putwain (2008) to assess test anxiety only measured two dimensions: cognitive and emotionality. The cognitive component of test anxiety accounted for 7% of variance in examination performance. These findings are similar to those found in Chamberlian et al. (2011) in terms of the relationship between test anxiety and GCSE scores. Putwain (2008) also found that the impact of test anxiety on performance varied with socioeconomic background. Students from lower socio-economic background had lower GCSE scores partially due to test anxiety. As discussed in Putwain (2008), these findings cannot be TEST ANXIETY 11 generalized to other nationalities. The study was also limited in that prior ability was not controlled for (Putwain, 2008). In comparison to other previously mentioned studies (Barterian et al., 2013; Bodas et al., 2008; Chamerlian et al., 2011; Putwain, 2008), the participants in Lee, Lowe, and Sena (2007) were both elementary and secondary school students. Lee et al., (2007) had the largest number of participants (774 students) from among the previously mentioned studies. The study examined test anxiety-assessment performance relationship among students with and without learning disabilities (LD). The study differentiated between the four different dimensions of test anxiety, an aspect that Chamberlian et al. (2011) and Putwain (2008) both lacked. Lee et al. (2007) found that students with LD had higher scores on the worry and cognitive obstructions/inattention parts of the test anxiety assessment. Students with LD scored lower on performance enhancement/facilitation. That is, students with LD are less likely to perform well on exams. Besides LD status, the study found that age and gender were significant predictors of test anxiety. This is contrary to the findings of Putwain (2008) which found no significant relationship between gender and test anxiety. Overall, older age predicted higher cognitive obstruction/inattention scores and lower physiological hyperarousal scores. Female students with LD predicted lower lie scale scores. That is, they have a more accurate prediction of their scores (Lee et al., 2007). However, as in Putwain (2008), Lee et al. (2007) stated that the findings of this study cannot be generalized to other nationalities. Criticism of Research on Test Anxiety Research on test anxiety has been criticized. Measurement of test anxiety has not been an easy task. Studies found that the physiological measures of test anxiety are unrelated and there is no preferred measure of anxiety. Self-report inventories of anxiety are unreliable due to the TEST ANXIETY 12 assumption of the awareness of test anxiety as a conscious process (Burns, 2004). Similarly, Chamberlian et al. (2011) and Eum and Rice (2010) have mentioned that self-report inventories often use mood induction procedures that have induced an anxious mood in participants. Also, self-report measures of test anxiety often use negative wording for the majority of items. Ideally, measures should use both negative and positive wording to assess both negative and positive thoughts. Test anxiety scales are often structured and specific eliciting particular responses. Furthermore, administering test anxiety measures before an actual exam can affect students test performance (Chamberlian et al., 2011). There have been questions on whether general trait anxiety measures rather than test anxiety measures should be used. It has been found that test anxiety is related to but different from trait anxiety (Burns, 2004; DiBattista & Gosse, 2006). DiBattista and Gosse (2006) found an inverse relationship between test anxiety and performance on exams but no relationship between trait anxiety and test performance. The linear relationship often shown between anxiety and performance has been criticized (Burns, 2004; Chamberlian et al., 2011; Mcdonald, 2001). The relationship includes some interactions such as time pressures, difficulty of the tasks, exam format, and low performance on previous exams. All these interactions and more should be taken into consideration when measuring test anxiety (Burns, 2004; Putwain, 2008). Addressing Test Anxiety Armstrong, Boxer, Ford, and Ford (2012) found that college students who were exposed to humorous material (cartoons) prior to taking a difficult math test exhibited lower levels of test anxiety associated with the anticipated test and their performance was enhanced. The authors concluded that humorous materials are effective in inhabiting the amount of test anxiety associated with a test, and thus enhance performance. This finding was supported by other TEST ANXIETY 13 research which found that humor lessens test anxiety if it is presented prior to the anxiety- evoking event such as test while the event is being anticipated (Armstrong et al., 2012). There were several limitations of Armstrong et al. (2012). First, the study assumed that there is one type of test anxious students, those that know the material but unable to retrieve it during the test. The authors assumed that test anxiety acted as an interfering agent rather than a mere emotional reaction that accompanies the awareness of not being prepared for a test. The study simply adopted the interference model with no reference to the deficit model (Broader & Musch, 1999). Second, the study did not take into consideration other psychological mechanisms besides test anxiety in explaining the enhancement of math performance. Perhaps an increase in judgment rather than a decrease in test anxiety contributed to the enhancement of math performance. Third, the study took place in a laboratory rather than in a realistic environment (Armstrong et al., 2012). Unlike Armstrong et al.(2012), Mowbrary (2012) focused on interventions that were effective for the two types of test anxious students. One cognitive intervention given was the attentional Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM-A), an attention training procedure. CBM-A is designed to reduce negative attentional bias by training the participants to manage or remove negative feelings or thoughts associated with an anxiety-related stimuli and direct their attention toward an anxiety-free stimuli. With enough training, this intervention allows attentional resources to be applied to relevant on task stimuli. The second cognitive intervention offered is the acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT). The purpose of ACT is to help an individual accept negative feeling and thoughts, to allow those feelings and thoughts to pass by rather than be occupied with them (Mowbrary, 2012). TEST ANXIETY 14 The above cognitive therapies do not have lasting effects. The literature shows thoughts and behaviors return to their pre-restructured form, particularly in times of stress and test-related situations are perceived as very stressful events (Mowbrary, 2012, p. 150). As was discussed in the article, cognitive therapies may actually use more working memory resources and further reduce efficiency. Also, CBM-A has been shown to negatively impact exam performance by accidently removing important exam information. Studies using ACT have been limited by small sample size making results tentive (Mowbrary, 2012). Mowbrary (2012) focused on one dimension of test anxiety, cognitive (worry). However, Stober (2004) focused on all four dimensions of test anxiety. Mowbrary (2012) was a review of the literature while Stober (2004) was a study examining the relationship between different dimensions of test anxiety and coping strategies. The study found significant gender differences in test anxiety and in ways of coping with test anxiety. Overall, female students showed higher levels of test anxiety (higher total scores on the TAI-G assessment). This finding is similar to those found in Barterian et al. (2013). Also, overall, test anxiety was related only to coping by social support (Stober, 2004). Stober (2004) found that females scored higher on two dimensions of test anxiety (as measured by TAI-G assessment): worry and emotionality. Both males and females used social support to cope with those two dimensions. Females only coped with pre-exam anxiety related to those two dimensions using task-orientation and preparation. With interference, males significantly related to avoidance coping while females related to social support. Lack of confidence was found to be statistically significant only for females and avoidance coping was found to be related to it. Overall, females used task-orientation and preparation and social support and less avoidance more than males (Stober, 2004). TEST ANXIETY 15 Stober (2004) had few limitations. Although this study offered more coping strategies than both Armstrong et al.(2012) and Mowbrary (2012), the study pertained to only three points. As stober (2004) discussed, the study focused on pre-examination phase and so does not accurately replicate test anxiety and coping strategies during the actual exam. Similar to Armstrong et al.(2012) and Mowbrary (2012), the study focuses on university students. It is thus difficult to generalize the results considering that test anxiety is found at a very early age (Stober, 2004). Summary/Conclusion Research on the impact of test anxiety on test performance yielded mixed results. Studies (Barterian et al., 2013; Broader and Musch, 1999; DiBattista & Gosse; 2006; Embse & Hasson, 2012; Eum & Rice, 2010; Lee et al., 2007; Mcdonald, 2001; Putwain, 2008) have found that there is a negative relationship between test anxiety and exam performance. Other studies (Bodas et al., 2008; Burns, 2004; Chamberlian et al., 2011) found no statistically significant affect of test anxiety on exam performance. One thing for certain is that the relationship between performance and test anxiety is nonlinear (Chamberlian et al., 2011; McDonald, 2001). Instruments often used in the studies did not assess all four dimensions of test anxiety. Culture played an important role in examining test anxiety and so findings often could not be generalized to other nationalities. Perhaps future research can examine the impact of test anxiety on test performance while using gender, age, nationality, learning disabilities, and socioeconomic background as moderators. It is also important for studies to control for prior ability to yield accurate results.
TEST ANXIETY 16 References Armstrong, J. M., Boxer, C. B., Ford, B. F., & Ford, T. F. (2012). Effect of humor on state anxiety and math performance. International Journal of Humor Research, 25(1), 59-74. doi: 10.1515/humor-2012-0004 Bodas, J. B., Ollendick, T. O., & Sovani, A. S. (2008). Test anxiety in indian children: A cross- cultural perspective. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 21(4), 387-404. doi: 10.1080/10615800701849902 Broder, A. B., & Musch, J. M. (1999). Test anxiety versus academic skills: A comparison of two alternative models for predicting test performance in a statistics exam. British Journal of Educational Psychology,69, 105-116. doi: 10.1348/000709999157608 Burns, D. B. (2004). Anxiety at the time of the final exam: Relationships with expectations and performance.Journal of Education for Business, 80(2), 119-124. doi: http://tlc.apa.uoit.ca/wp-content/uploads/2011/12/Anxiety-at-the-time-of-the-exam.pdf Carlson, J. C., Barterian, J. B., Embse, N. E., Goforth, A. G., & Segool, N. S. (2013). Heightened test anxiety among young children: Elementary school students anxious responses to high-stakes testing.Psychology in the Schools,, 50(5), 489-499. doi: 10.1002/pits Chamberlain, S. C., Daly, A. D., & Spalding, V. S. (2011). Test anxiety, heart rate and performance in a-level french speaking mock exams: an exploratory study.Educational Research, 53(3), 321330. doi: 10.1080/00131881.2011.598660 DiBattista, D. D., & Gosse, L. G. (2006). Test anxiety and the immediate feedback assessment technique. The Journal of Experimental Education, 74(4), 311-327. Retrieved from http://www.epsteineducation.com/home/articles/file/research/testanxietyandtheif-at.pdf TEST ANXIETY 17 Embse, N. E., & Hasson, R. H. (2012). Test anxiety and high-stakes test performance between school settings: Implications for educators. Preventing School Failure, 56(3), 180187. doi: 10.1080/1045988X.2011.633285 Eum, K. E., & Rice, K. R. (2010). Test anxiety, perfectionism, goal orientation, and academic performance. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 24(2), 167-178. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2010.488723 Lee, S. W., Lowe, P. A., & Whitaker Sena, J. D. (2007). Significant predictors of test anxiety among students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 360-376. doi:10.1177/00222194070400040601 Mcdonald, A. M. (2001). The prevalence and effects of test anxiety in school children. Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 21(1), 89-101. doi: 10.1080/01443410020019867 Mowbray, T. M. (2012). Working memory, test anxiety and effective interventions: A review. The Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 29(2), 141156. doi: 10.1017/edp.2012.16 Pekrun, R. P., & Stober, J. S. (2004). Advances in test anxiety research. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping,,17(3), 205-211. doi: 10.1080/1061580412331303225 Putwain, D. P. (2008). Test anxiety and gcse performance: the effect of gender and socio- economic background. Educational Psychology in Practice,24(4), 319334. doi: 10.1080/02667360802488765 Stober, J. S. (2004). Dimensions of test anxiety: Relations to ways of coping with pre-exam anxiety and uncertainty. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 17(3), 213-226. doi: 10.1080/10615800412331292615
The Relationship between Calm Concentration Training Model and Reduced Test-Anxiety and Improved Academic Test Scores in Students: A Quasi-Experimental Design
Emotional and behavioral problems and academic achievement impact of demographic and intellectual ability among early adolescent students of government and private schools