A Meta-Analysis of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia
A Meta-Analysis of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia
A Meta-Analysis of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia
ognitive impairment is a core feature of schizophrenia, with converging evidence showing that it is strongly related to functioning in areas such as work, social relationships, and independent living (1, 2). Furthermore, cognitive functioning is a robust predictor of response to psychiatric rehabilitation (i.e., systematic efforts to improve the psychosocial functioning of persons with severe mental illness) (3), including outcomes such as work, social skills, and self-care (1, 4, 5). Because of the importance of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia, it has been identified as an appropriate target for interventions (6). Currently available pharmacological treatments have limited effects on cognition in schizophrenia (7, 8) and even less impact on community functioning (9). To address the problem of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia, a range of cognitive remediation programs has been developed and evaluated over the past 40 years. These programs employ a variety of methods, such as drill and practice exercises, teaching strategies to improve cognitive functioning, compensatory strategies to reduce the effects of persistent cognitive impairments, and group discussions. Several reviews of research on cognitive rehabilitation in schizophrenia have been published (1013). The general conclusions from these reviews have been that cognitive remediation leads to modest improvements in performance on neuropsychological tests but has no impact on functional outcomes. However, these reviews were limited by the relatively small number of studies that actually measured psychosocial functioning, precluding any definitive conclusions about the effects of cognitive remediation on psychosocial adjustment or the identification of program characteristics that may contribute to such effects. The rationale for cognitive remediation is chiefly predicated on
its presumed effects on psychosocial functioning and improved response to rehabilitation. Therefore, a critical examination of the effects of cognitive remediation on functional outcomes is necessary in order to determine its potential role in the treatment of schizophrenia. In recent years, the number of studies that examined psychosocial functioning has grown sufficiently to permit a closer look at the impact of cognitive remediation. We conducted a meta-analysis of controlled studies to evaluate the effects of cognitive remediation on cognitive functioning, symptoms, and functional outcomes. We also examined whether characteristics of cognitive remediation programs (e.g., hours of cognitive training), the provision of adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation, treatment settings, patient demographics, or type of control group was related to improved outcomes. We hypothesized that cognitive remediation would improve both cognitive functioning and psychosocial adjustment. We also hypothesized that programs that provided more hours of cognitive training would have stronger effects on cognitive functioning and that adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation would be associated with greater improvements in functional outcomes.
Method
Studies for the meta-analysis were identified by conducting MEDLINE and PsycINFO searches for English language articles published in peer-reviewed journals. The following search terms were used: cognitive training, cognitive remediation, cognitive rehabilitation, and schizophrenia. Studies meeting the following criteria were included: 1) a randomized, controlled trial of a psychosocial intervention designed to improve cognitive functioning; 2) an assessment of performance with at least one neuropsychological measure that had the potential to reflect generalization of efajp.psychiatryonline.org
1791
COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA TABLE 1. Neuropsychological Assessments Included in Each Cognitive Domain Domain Attention/vigilance Assessment Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) information and mental control subtests Search-a-Word Cancellation tasks Continuous Performance Tests Span of apprehension Labyrinth Test Sustained Attention Test Span: hits, time, and overall Preattentional processing Cross-over reaction time Cross-modal reaction time Embedded Figures Test COGLAB apprehension/masking Dichotic listening tasks Trail Making Test, Parts A and B WAIS, WAIS-R, or WAIS-III digit symbol subtest Stroop Test, color and word conditions Reaction time tests Letters and category fluency WAIS, WAIS-R, WAIS-III, or WMS digit span WAIS-III letter-number sequencing and arithmetic subtests Digit Span Distractibility Test Other digit span tasks Trained Word Recall Task Other arithmetic tasks Sentence span Dual span Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test Wechsler Memory ScaleRevised (WMS-R) visual span Dual span WMS, WMS-R, or WMS-III logical memory and verbal paired associates subtests California Verbal Learning Test Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Word List Recall Task Verbal learning paradigm Denman Neuropsychological Memory Test Span-Completeness Verbal Learning Test WMS, WMS-R, or WMS-III visual recall, visual reproduction, faces, and figural memory subtests Memory for Designs Test Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test Kimura recurring figures Denman Neuropsychological Memory Test WAIS, WAIS-R, or WAIS-III similarities and picture arrangement subtests Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children mazes subtest Stroop Test interference condition Independent Living Scaleproblem solving Gorhams Proverbs Test and other proverb interpretation tasks Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Trail Making Test (B A) Hinting Task Labyrinth Test Tower of Hanoi Tower of London Response inhibition Six elements Categories COGLAB card sorting test Social perception (Emotion Matching Test and Emotion Labeling Test) Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Test Social cognition Global cognitive scores Mini-Mental State Examination Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Shipley Institute of Living Scale IQ estimate WAIS-R comprehension subtest Verbal IQ Cognitive style Hayling Sentence Completion Task (continued) Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007
Speed of processing
Social cognition Other cognitive Cognitive measures of multiple domains Cognitive measures not considered sensitive to change
1792
ajp.psychiatryonline.org
McGURK, TWAMLEY, SITZER, ET AL. TABLE 1. Neuropsychological Assessments Included in Each Cognitive Domain (continued) Purdue Pegboard Tactile performance WMS orientation subtest Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale Holtzman Inkblot Test Paranoid Depression Scale Present State Exam Thought, language, and communication Bay Area Functional Performance Evaluation Percent sick talk/incoherence during the interview Life skills profile Global Assessment Scale Nurses Observation Scale for Inpatient Evaluation Disability Assessment Schedule Employment Social Behaviour Schedule Micro-Module Learning Test Assessment of Interpersonal Problem-Solving Skills Social adjustment evaluated by computing the Q statistic (18). Then the significance level of the mean effect sizes was computed by conducting fixedeffects linear models except when the Q statistic indicated significant within-group heterogeneity, in which case we used random effects models. Moderator analyses were then conducted on those domains with significant heterogeneity, based on the Q statistic, to determine whether any participant, setting, or program variables explained variations between studies in effect sizes. These analyses were performed by clustering studies into two contrasting groups based on the moderator variable and computing the Q between and Q within statistics (18).
Symptoms
Functioning
fects rather than assessments on trained tasks only; 3) data available on either group means and standard deviations for baseline and postintervention cognitive tests or statistics from which effect sizes could be calculated; 4) a minimum of 75% of the sample reported to have schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, or schizophreniform disorder.
Moderator Variables
Several variables were considered as potential moderators of cognitive remediation. Each moderator variable was divided into two levels based on a median split. The moderator variables and levels were 1) participant characteristics: age (years) (1537/38 50), 2) the setting (inpatient/outpatient), 3) the type of control group (active control [e.g., another intervention, such as cognitive behavior therapy or motivational interviewing]/passive control [e.g., viewing educational videos or treatment as usual]), 4) program characteristics: type of intervention (drill and practice/ drill and practice plus strategy coaching or strategy coaching alone), hours of practice (determined for the overall program as well as individual cognitive domains), and the provision of adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation (no/yes). Some programs that provided training in social cognition employed a combination of cognitive remediation and other rehabilitation approaches, such as social skills training (19, 20), whereas others employed strictly cognitive remediation methods, such as computer-based training tasks (21). The number of hours of social cognition training was included in the total number of cognitive remediation hours only for the programs that did not combine the training with another rehabilitation approach. A variety of psychiatric rehabilitation approaches were provided in conjunction with cognitive remediation, including social skills training (20, 22), social skills/social perception training (19, 23), supported employment (24), vocational rehabilitation (25), and vocational rehabilitation and social information processing groups (26).
Meta-Analytic Procedure
Meta-analyses were conducted with BioStat software (17). In order to control for study differences in sample size when mean effect sizes were computed, studies were weighted according to their inverse variance estimates. To determine whether mean effect sizes were statistically significant, the confidence interval (CI) and z transformation of the effect size were used. The homogeneity of the effect sizes across studies for each outcome domain was Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007
Results
Data from 26 studies (1,151 subjects) were included. The studies, characteristics of participants and programs,
ajp.psychiatryonline.org
1793
COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA TABLE 2. Description of Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia Sample Characteristics Inpatient/Outpatient; Mean Age (years); Mean Education (years); % Male Inpatients; 44.8; not reported; 100% Inpatients; 44.8; not reported; 100% Inpatients; 44.8; not reported; 100% Drill and Practice/ Drill and Strategy Coaching Drill and practice Cognitive Remediation Program Included Psychiatric Rehabilitation No
Author Wagner (1968) (45), treatment 1 Wagner (1968) (45), treatment 2 Wagner (1968) (45), treatment 3
Treatment group (N) Noncomputerized attention training (N=8) Noncomputerized abstraction training (N=8) Noncomputerized attention and abstraction training (N=16) Noncomputerized training using self-talk (N=5) Computerized training with advancement criteria (N=10)
No
No
Meichenbaum & Cameron Inpatients; 36.0; not reported; (1973), includes 3-week 100% follow-up (16) Benedict & Harris (1989) (46) Inpatients; 30.3; not reported; not reported
No
No
Olbrich & Mussgay (1990) (43) Hermanutz & Gestrich (1991) (42)
No
No
Computerized attention training (N=16) Computerized training using Captains Log (N=40)
No No
No
Computerized attention training using orientation remedial module (N=27) Noncomputerized training using integrated psychological therapy (N=49)
Wykes et al. (1999) (27); Wykes et al. (2003) (30), 6-month follow-up
No
Medalia et al. (2000) (51), treatment 1 Medalia et al. (2000) (51), treatment 2
No No
1794
ajp.psychiatryonline.org
Control Group (active/ passive control, N) Viewing treatment group stimuli without responding (active control, N=8) Viewing treatment group 3 hours/1 week stimuli without responding (active control, N=8) Viewing treatment group 3 hours/1 week stimuli without responding (active control, N=8) Task practice without self- 4.1 hours/3 weeks talk (active control, N=5) 1. Computerized training without advancement criteria (active control, N=10); 2. Treatment as usual (passive control, N=10) Arts and crafts groups (active control, N=15) 1. Integrated psychological therapy focusing on cognitive, communication, and social training (active control, N=10); 2. Treatment as usual (passive control, N=10) Treatment as usual (passive control, N=17) Treatment as usual (passive control, N=29) 12.5 hours/814 weeks
Hours/Weeks of Cognitive Remediation Cognitive Effect Size (excluding other (follow-up effect size in treatment) parentheses) 3 hours/1 week Visual learning memory= 0.80; reasoning and problem solving=1.40 Visual learning memory= 1.21; reasoning and problem solving=1.12 Visual learning memory= 0.09; reasoning and problem solving=0.55 Verbal working memory= 0.77 (1.31); reasoning and problem solving= 1.26 (1.99) Speed of processing=1.57
0.04
0.32
1.02 (1.65)
1.89 (2.08)
3.50 (3.99)
1.57
12 hours/3 weeks
Attention/vigilance=0.52; 0.40 Verbal working memory=0.43; reasoning and problem solving=0.26 Treatment versus active Treatment versus ac- Treatment versus control: attention/vigitive control=0.18; active control= lance=0.18; treatment treatment versus 0.43; treatment versus passive control: passive control= versus passive attention/vigilance= 0.09 control=0.24 0.09 Attention/vigilance=0.41; verbal learning and memory=0.13 Speed of processing=0.58; attention/vigilance= 0.65; verbal working memory=0.89; verbal learning and memory= 0.69; visual learning memory=0.26 Speed of processing=0.74; attention/vigilance= 1.08; reasoning and problem solving=0.54; other=0.00 Attention/vigilance=0.19 Speed of processing=0.02; attention/vigilance= 0.38; verbal learning and memory=0.06; visual learning memory= 0.06; reasoning and problem solving=0.14 Speed of processing=0.26; verbal working memory=0.27 (0.07); nonverbal working memory= 0.06; reasoning and problem solving=0.20 (0.33) Verbal learning and memory=0.43 Verbal learning and memory=0.39 0.27 0.51
0.00
6 hours/3 weeks
0.79
Watching National Geo6 hours/6 weeks graphic documentaries (passive control, N=27) Group supportive therapy 68.3 hours/26 weeks emphasizing social skills (active control, N=42)
0.19 0.08
0.20 (0.20)
0.59
0.05
Treatment as usual (passive control, N=18) Treatment as usual (passive control, N=18)
1795
COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA TABLE 2. Description of Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia (continued) Sample Characteristics Inpatient/Outpatient; Mean Age (years); Mean Education (years); % Male Outpatients in work therapy; 43.6; 13.2; 78% Cognitive RemediaDrill and Practice/ tion Program InDrill and Strategy cluded Psychiatric Coaching Rehabilitation Drill and practice Vocational rehabilitation and social information processing groups
Treatment group (N) Computerized training using CogReHab plus weekly social information processing group (N=31) Same as above (N=47) Same as above (N=45) Same as above (N=57)
Bell et al. (2003) (52), includes follow-up Fiszdon et al. (2004) (53), includes follow-up Fiszdon et al. (2005) (54) Van der Gaag (2002) (23) Inpatients; 31.1; not reported; 64%
No
Noncomputerized training using Drill and practice attention process training (N=13)
No
Computerized training using Drill and strategy Social skills/social orientation remedial module and coaching perception training CogReHab plus group social groups cognition exercises (N=67) Computerized and noncomputerized training (N=14) Drill and strategy coaching No
Ueland & Rund (2005) (21); Inpatients; 15.3; not reported; Ueland & Rund (2004) 50% (44), 12-month follow-up
Computerized training using Cogpack (N=21) Noncomputerized training using attention process training and shaping (N=18) Computerized training using Cogpack and noncomputerized training, plus cognitive adaptation therapy (N=47)
No
Drill and practice Social skills training groups Drill and strategy Vocational coaching rehabilitation
1796
ajp.psychiatryonline.org
Hours/Weeks of Cognitive Remediation Cognitive Effect Size Average Effect Size Control Group (active/ (excluding other (follow-up effect size in for Cognitive passive control, N) treatment) parentheses) Measures Treatment as usual 36 hours/26 weeks Speed of processing=0.31; 0.50 (passive control, N=34) verbal working memory=0.30; reasoning and problem solving=0.50; social=0.90 Passive control, N=55 Verbal working memory= 0.40 (0.48) 0.40 (0.48) Passive control, N=49 Verbal working memory= 0.53 (0.66) 0.53 (0.66) Passive control, N=68 Verbal learning and mem0.36 ory=0.36 Leisure/games group 4 hours/11 weeks Speed of processing= 0.26 (active control, N=21) 0.02; attention/vigilance=0.09; verbal learning and memory= 0.41; visual learning memory=0.47; reasoning and problem solving=0.09; social=0.51 Treatment as usual 8 hours/8 weeks Speed of processing=0.56; 0.52 (passive control, N=17) verbal working memory=0.52; verbal learning and memory=0.49 Treatment as usual 24 hours/43 weeks Speed of processing=0.32; 0.53 (passive control, N=11) attention vigilance= 0.54; verbal working memory=0.48; verbal learning and memory= 0.57; reasoning and problem solving=0.45 Enriched supportive ther- 75 hours/104 weeks Speed of processing=0.83 0.60 (0.67) apy including psychoed(0.86); social=0.36 (0.66) ucation, illness selfmanagement, and stress management (active control, N=54) Treatment as usual 30 hours/12 weeks Attention/vigilance=0.31 0.37 (0.33) (passive control, N=12) (0.28); verbal working memory=0.54 (0.60); verbal learning and memory=0.33 (0.29); visual learning memory=0.11 (0.17); reasoning and problem solving=0.57 (0.31) Treatment as usual 24 hours/12 weeks Speed of processing=0.27; 0.33 (passive control, N=21) verbal working memory=0.42; verbal learning and memory=0.45; reasoning and problem solving=0.18 Treatment as usual 15 hours/3 weeks Speed of processing=0.69; 0.78 (passive control, N=21) verbal learning and memory=0.88 Treatment as usual 18 hours/6 weeks Attention/vigilance=0.25; 0.37 (passive control, N=13) verbal working memory=0.38; verbal learning and memory=0.48 1. Vocational rehabilita24 hours/8 weeks Treatment versus active Treatment versus tion and self-managecontrol; attention/vigiactive control= ment training for negalance=0.46; verbal 0.54; treatment tive symptoms (active learning and memory= versus passive control, N=45); 2. Treat0.61; treatment versus control=0.54 ment as usual (passive passive control; attencontrol, N=46) tion/vigilance=0.46; verbal learning and memory=0.55; reasoning and problem solving=0.60
0.32
0.07 (0.09)
0.37 (0.51)
0.25 (0.51)
0.31 (0.16)
0.45
1.76
0.41
0.68
(continued)
ajp.psychiatryonline.org
1797
COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA TABLE 2. Description of Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia (continued) Sample Characteristics Inpatient/Outpatient; Mean Age (years); Mean Education (years); % Male Outpatients; 35.1; 10.2; 57% Drill and Practice/ Drill and Strategy Coaching Drill and practice Cognitive Remediation Program Included Psychiatric Rehabilitation No
Treatment group (N) Noncomputerized training using frontal/executive program with errorless learning (N=20)
and effect sizes are displayed in Table 2. The mean sample size was 50 (SD=36, range=10138). The mean age of the participants was 36.3 years (SD=6.0, range of means=15 47), the mean years of education was 11.8 (SD=1.0, range of means=1013), 69% of the participants were men, and 60% were inpatients. The mean duration of cognitive remediation programs was 12.8 weeks (SD=20.9, range=1 104). Programs targeted for training an average of 2.9 cognitive domains (SD=1.6, range=16), whereas changes in cognitive functioning were assessed on an average of 3.1 cognitive domains (SD=1.6, range=16). Sixty-nine percent of the programs used a drill and practice intervention; 23% provided adjunctive psychosocial rehabilitation.
hours of cognitive remediation were unrelated to program type (2=0.4, df=1, n.s.).
Discussion
The results provide support for the effects of cognitive remediation on improving cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, with effect sizes in the medium range for overall cognitive functioning (0.41) and six of the seven cognitive domains (0.390.54). The effects of cognitive remediation on cognitive performance were remarkably similar across the 26 studies included in the analysis despite differences in length and training methods between cognitive remediation programs, inpatient/outpatient setting, patient age, and provision of adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation. The results indicate that cognitive remediation produced robust improvements in cognitive functioning across a variety of program and patient conditions. The effect sizes of cognitive remediation were homogeneously distributed across studies for overall cognitive functioning and six of the seven cognitive domains, precluding the examination of moderators of treatment effects for most cognitive outcomes. Thus, contrary to our
Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007
1798
ajp.psychiatryonline.org
Hours/Weeks of Cognitive Remediation Cognitive Effect Size Control Group (active/ (excluding other (follow-up effect size in passive control, N) treatment) parentheses) Cognitive behavioral ther- 40 hours/16 weeks Speed of processing=0.56 apy for psychosis (active (1.02); verbal working control, N=20) memory=0.80 (0.76); verbal learning and memory=1.19 (2.07); visual learning memory=0.75 (1.22); reasoning and problem solving=1.76 (2.05)
hypothesis, the number of hours programs devoted to cognitive remediation was not related to the amount of improvement in overall cognitive functioning. However, hours of training, as well as use of drill and practice rather than combined drill and practice with strategy coaching, were related to improvements in verbal learning and memory, suggesting that this domain may be more sensitive to the method and extent of cognitive remediation. It is possible that a relatively limited amount of cognitive remediation (e.g., 515 hours) is sufficient to produce improved cognitive functioning and that all studies provided an adequate amount of treatment. Alternatively, the amount of cognitive remediation may not be related to immediate gains in cognitive functioning but could contribute to the retention of improvements following the termination of treatment. The impact of amount of cognitive remediation on the maintenance of treatment effects could not be evaluated in this meta-analysis because only six studies conducted follow-up assessments an average of 8 months after completion of the program. However, the mean effect size for overall cognitive performance for these studies was in the medium range (0.66), comparable in magnitude to the immediate effects of cognitive remediation. These findings provide preliminary support for the longer-term benefits of cognitive remediation on cognitive performance and point to the need for more research on the maintenance of treatment effects. The overall effect size of cognitive remediation on improving symptoms was significant but in the small range (0.28). Previous reviews of the effects of cognitive remediation either have not examined symptoms (10, 11) or were inconclusive because of the small number of studies (12, 13). The apparently limited impact of cognitive remediation on symptoms is consistent with numerous studies showing that cognitive impairment is relatively independent of other symptoms of schizophrenia (3133). Cognitive remediation may have some beneficial effects on symptoms by providing positive learning experiences that serve to bolster self-esteem and self-efficacy for achieving personal goals, thereby improving depression. Several studies have reported that cognitive remediation improved mood (24, 27, 34).
Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007
Cognitive remediation also had a significant effect on improving psychosocial functioning, with an average effect size of 0.35, just slightly lower than the average effect size of 0.41 for improved cognitive performance. For example, patients who participated in cognitive remediation showed greater improvements in obtaining and working competitive jobs (24, 25), the quality of and satisfaction with interpersonal relationships (19), and the ability to solve interpersonal problems (20). These findings are unique because until recently a sufficient number of studies had not measured functional outcomes from which to draw firm conclusions. The impact of cognitive remediation on improved functioning is important because the primary rationale for cognitive remediation in schizophrenia is to improve psychosocial functioning (35). In contrast to the uniform effects across studies of cognitive remediation on overall cognitive performance and symptoms, there was significant variability in its effects on psychosocial functioning. Furthermore, as hypothesized, cognitive remediation programs that provided adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation had significantly stronger effects on improving functional outcomes (0.47) than programs that did not (0.05). This effect is consistent with previous research showing that cognitive impairment attenuates response to psychiatric rehabilitation (1, 36, 37) and suggests that improved cognitive performance may enable some patients to benefit more from rehabilitation. The findings are also consistent with the results of a meta-analysis of integrated psychological therapy (38) in which the strongest effects on functioning were found in programs that integrated cognitive remediation and social skills training rather than programs that provided either intervention alone (39). Cognitive remediation programs that included strategy coaching had stronger effects on functioning than programs that focused only on drill and practice. Strategy coaching typically targets memory and executive functions by teaching methods such as chunking information to facilitate recall and problem-solving skills. It is unclear whether strategy coaching is more effective because people are better able to transfer skills from the training setting into their daily lives (35) or because teaching such strategies helps patients compensate for the effects of perajp.psychiatryonline.org
1799
COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA TABLE 3. Results of Meta-Analysis of Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophreniaa Subjects (N) 1,214 659 655 428 858 424 564 228 709 615 Cognitive Remediation Hours Median 12.5 6.6 0.0 0.4 0.0 0.0 3.0 26.0 Range 375 014 03 08 08 03 032 284 Analysis Hedgess Q 35.3 9.8 20.7 3.9 26.6* 14.5* 21.8 2.8 12.2 25.7** df 28 14 13 10 15 7 14 2 14 10
Outcome Domain Global cognition Attention/vigilance Speed of processing Verbal working memory Verbal learning and memory Visual learning and memory Reasoning/problem solving Social cognition Symptoms Functioning
a
Effect Size 0.41 0.41 0.48 0.52 0.39 0.09 0.47 0.54 0.28 0.35
95% CI 0.29 to 0.52 0.25 to 0.57 0.28 to 0.69 0.33 to 0.72 0.20 to 0.58 0.26 to 0.43 0.30 to 0.64 0.22 to 0.88 0.13 to 0.43 0.07 to 0.62
T Score 6.9*** 5.1*** 5.9*** 5.2*** 5.5*** 0.6 5.4*** 3.9*** 3.6*** 1.9*
After consideration of the consistency of effect sizes across six of the seven cognitive domains and overall cognitive functioning, the clinical and theoretical significance of these moderator effects on verbal learning and memory is unclear. *p<0.05. ** p<0.01. ***p<0.001.
FIGURE 1. Effect Sizes for Overall Cognition in Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia
Benedict & Harris (46), treatment as usual Benedict & Harris (46), active control Wagner (45), treatment 1 Meichenbaum & Cameron (16) Penads et al. (29) Field et al. (49) Sartory et al. (56) Hogarty et al. (19) Vauth et al. (25), active control Vauth et al. (25), treatment as usual Lpez-Luengo & Vzquez (55) Bellucci et al. (34) Burda et al. (48) Bell et al. (26, 52); Fiszdon et al. (53, 54) Olbrich & Mussgay (43) Silverstein et al. (22) Ueland & Rund (44) McGurk et al. (24) Wagner (45), treatment 3 Benedict et al. (47) van der Gaag et al. (23) Wykes et al. (27) Medalia et al. (50) Hermanutz & Gestrich (42), active control Spaulding et al. (20) Wagner (45), treatment 2 Hermanutz & Gestrich (42), treatment as usual Medalia et al. (51), treatment 2 Medalia et al. (51), treatment 1 2.0 1.0 0.0 1.0 2.0 3.0 Effect Size
sistent cognitive impairments on functioning (24) or both. Further research is needed to address this question. The effects of cognitive remediation were not influenced by the nature of the control condition. Thus, simply actively or passively engaging patients in treatments designed to control for the amount of clinician contact did not appear to confer any benefit in cognitive functioning beyond the provision of usual services. These findings are consistent with the meta-analysis of the cognitive remediation-based integrated psychological therapy program (39) but differ from the psychotherapy literature, where there is ample evidence for nonspecific effects related to therapist attention (40). The mechanisms underlying the effects of cognitive remediation on improved cognitive performance, functioning, and symptoms appear to differ from those involved in psychotherapy. The results raise questions about the need to control for the amount of clinician attention given to treatment control groups in research on cognitive remediation. So what has been learned after almost 40 years of research on cognitive remediation for schizophrenia? Although a great deal more is known about schizophrenia and its neurocognitive underpinnings and the technology for assessing and remediating cognitive impairments has evolved (e.g., most programs now employ at least some computer-based training), the effect sizes on cognitive functioning do not appear to have increased appreciably in recent years. The failure to develop more potent programs could be due to limitations imposed by the illness itself and not the fault of treatment developers. It may be argued that a similar phenomenon has occurred in the pharmacological treatment of schizophrenia, where despite the enormous investment of resources into the development of new drugs, the clinical gains in treating symptoms over the past 50 years are debatable (41). Alternatively, the ability to improve the effectiveness of cognitive remediation may depend on attention to critical issues in research design. Two such issues deserve special consideration: the evaluation of the persistence of remeAm J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007
1800
ajp.psychiatryonline.org
diation effects on cognitive functioning and the assessment of the impact of remediation on functional outcomes. Despite the number of controlled studies of cognitive remediation, only six studies (16, 19, 21, 2830) examined whether improvements in cognitive functioning were maintained at a posttreatment follow-up, precluding the exploration of moderators of treatment effects. The relative lack of data addressing this question may be important because different program, patient, or setting factors could influence the long-term maintenance of cognitive effects compared to short-term effects. Similarly, only 11 studies evaluated functional outcomes (16, 19, 20, 22, 24, 25, 27, 29, 4244), and this was the first meta-analysis to quantitatively demonstrate that cognitive remediation improved psychosocial functioning. Furthermore, the impact of cognitive remediation on functioning was moderated by several factors, including the provision of adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation, cognitive training method, and patient age, suggesting potentially important factors for improving the impact of treatment programs. Thus, the ability to make cognitive remediation programs more effective may have been constrained by the neglect of most studies to measure the long-term effects of remediation and its impact on functional outcomes, resulting in the inability to identify moderators of treatment that could be the focus of efforts to hone and refine the intervention. Future research on cognitive remediation should routinely evaluate psychosocial functioning and the long-term effects of treatment on all outcomes of interest. In addition, research that systematically examines the interactions between cognitive remediation and psychiatric rehabilitation is warranted. In summary, cognitive remediation was found to have consistent effects on improving cognitive performance, functioning, and symptoms. In addition, the impact of cognitive remediation on functional outcomes was significantly greater in studies that also provided psychiatric rehabilitation, suggesting that these two treatment approaches may work together in a synergistic fashion. These findings challenge the assumption that simply improving cognitive functioning in schizophrenia will spontaneously lead to better psychosocial outcomes. The results do suggest, however, that cognitive remediation may improve the response of some patients to psychiatric rehabilitation. Overall, this meta-analysis indicates that cognitive remediation may have an important role to play in improving both cognitive performance and functional outcomes in schizophrenia.
Received June 8, 2007; revision received Aug. 27, 2007; accepted Aug. 28, 2007 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07060906). From the Dartmouth Psychiatric Research Center, the Department of Community and Family Medicine, and the Department of Psychiatry, Dartmouth Medical School; the Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego; and the School of Psychology, Argosy University, Santa Monica, Calif. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. McGurk, Dartmouth Psychiatric Research Center, Main
Building, 105 Pleasant St., Concord, NH 03301; susan.r.mcgurk@ dartmouth.edu (e-mail). All authors report no competing interests. Supported by NIMH grant MH77210 and National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research grant H133G050230.
References
1. McGurk SR, Mueser KT: Cognitive functioning, symptoms, and work in supported employment: a review and heuristic model. Schizophr Res 2004; 70:147174 2. Mueser KT: Cognitive functioning, social adjustment and longterm outcome in schizophrenia, in Cognition in Schizophrenia: Impairments, Importance, and Treatment Strategies. Edited by Sharma T, Harvey P. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 2000, pp 157177 3. Corrigan PW, Mueser KT, Bond GR, Drake RE, Solomon P: The Principles and Practice of Psychiatric Rehabilitation: An Empirical Approach. New York, Guilford, 2007 4. Smith TE, Hull JW, Romanelli S, Fertuck E, Weiss KA: Symptoms and neurocognition as rate limiters in skills training for psychotic patients. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:18171818 5. Wykes T, Dunn G: Cognitive deficit and the prediction of rehabilitation success in a chronic psychiatric group. Psychol Med 1992; 22:389398 6. Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF, Heaton RK: Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2004; 72:2939 7. Marder SR: Initiatives to promote the discovery of drugs to improve cognitive function in severe mental illness. J Clin Psychiatry 2006; 67(suppl 9):3135 8. Rund BR, Borg NE: Cognitive deficits and cognitive training in schizophrenic patients: a review. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1999; 100:8595 9. Harvey PD, Green MF, Keefe RS, Velligan DI: Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia: a consensus statement on its role in the definition and evaluation of effective treatments for the illness. J Clin Psychiatry 2004; 65:361372 10. Krabbendam L, Aleman A: Cognitive rehabilitation in schizophrenia: a quantitative analysis of controlled studies. Psychopharmacology 2003; 169:376382 11. Kurtz MM, Moberg PJ, Gur RC, Gur RE: Approaches to cognitive remediation of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Rev 2001; 11: 197210 12. Pilling S, Bebbington P, Kuipers E, Garety P, Geddes JR, Martindale B, Orbach G, Morgan C: Psychological treatments in schizophrenia, II: meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of social skills training and cognitive remediation. Psychol Med 2002; 32:783791 13. Twamley EW, Jeste DV, Bellack AS: A review of cognitive training in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:359382 14. Thalheimer W, Cook S: How to calculate effect sizes from published research: a simplified methodology. www.work-learning.com/effect_sizes.htm 15. Cohen J: Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988 16. Meichenbaum D, Cameron R: Training schizophrenics to talk to themselves: a means of developing attentional controls. Behav Ther 1973; 4:515534 17. Borenstein M, Rothstein H: Comprehensive Meta-Analysis: A Computer Program for Research Synthesis. Englewood, NJ, Biostat, 1999 18. Hedges LV, Olkin I: Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. New York, Academic Press, 1985 19. Hogarty GE, Flesher S, Ulrich RF, Carter M, Greenwald D, PogueGeile MF, Kechavan M, Cooley S, Di Barry AL, Garrett A, Pare-
ajp.psychiatryonline.org
1801
20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
28.
29.
30.
31.
32.
33.
34.
35. 36.
37.
1802
ajp.psychiatryonline.org