ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE
published: 04 September 2013
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00555
HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE
Memory for hand-use depends on consistency of
handedness
James M. Edlin*, Emily K. Carris and Keith B. Lyle
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, USA
Edited by:
Ruth E. Propper, Montclair State
University, USA
Reviewed by:
Stephen D. Christman, University of
Toledo, USA
Andrew Parker, Manchester
Metropolitan University, UK
*Correspondence:
James M. Edlin, Department of
Psychological and Brain Sciences,
University of Louisville, 2301 S 3rd
Street, Louisville, KY 40292, USA
e-mail:
[email protected]
Individuals who do not consistently use the same hand to perform unimanual tasks
(inconsistent-handed) outperform consistent right- and left-handed individuals on tests of
episodic memory. We explored whether the inconsistent-hander (ICH) memory advantage
extends to memory for unimanual hand use itself. Are ICHs better able to remember which
hand they used to perform actions? Opposing predictions are possible, stemming from the
finding that some regions of the corpus callosum are larger in ICHs, especially those that
connect motor areas. One hypothesis is that greater callosally mediated interhemispheric
interaction produces ICHs’ superior retrieval of episodic memories, and this may extend
to episodic memories for hand use. Alternatively, we also hypothesized that greater interhemispheric interaction could produce more bilateral activation in motor areas during the
performance and retrieval of unimanual actions. This could interfere with ICHs’ ability to
remember which hand they used. To test these competing predictions in the current study,
consistent- and inconsistent-handers performed unimanual actions, half of which required
manipulating objects and half of which did not. Each action was performed four times in one
of five conditions that differed in the ratio of left to right hand use: always left (4:0), usually
left (3:1), equal (2:2), usually right (1:3), or always right (0:4). We compared consistent- and
inconsistent-handers on recall of the left:right ratio for each action. ICHs remembered how
they performed actions better than consistent-handers, regardless of ratio. These findings
provide another example of superior episodic retrieval in ICHs. We discuss how greater
interaction might benefit memory for hand use.
Keywords: handedness, interhemispheric interaction, self-performed tasks, enactment, episodic memory, action
memory, individual differences
INTRODUCTION
Individuals differ in the consistency with which they use a single
preferred hand to perform unimanual tasks. Some individuals are
highly consistent while others are relatively inconsistent, making
greater use of both hands. We refer to this interindividual variable as handedness consistency, although it might also be called
manual lateralization. Of interest to memory researchers, degree
of handedness consistency, as measured by self-report, predicts
performance on tests of episodic memory (for review, see Prichard
et al., 2013). On average, inconsistent individuals remember events
more accurately and with greater subjective vividness and detail
than do consistent individuals.
Why is handedness consistency related to memory? The dominant theory in the literature is based on two assumptions: (1)
inconsistent-handers (ICHs) have greater interhemispheric interaction than consistent-handers (CHs), and (2) greater interhemispheric interaction enhances some types of memory retrieval
(Christman and Propper, 2001). The first assumption is tenuously supported by consistency-based anatomical differences in
the corpus callosum. The corpus callosum is the major pathway for
communication between the left and right cerebral hemispheres
and has sometimes been found to be larger in ICHs than CHs
(Witelson, 1985; Habib et al., 1991; Cowell et al., 1993; Luders
et al., 2010), but not always (Jäncke and Steinmetz, 2003; Welcome
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
et al., 2009). Differences in measurement techniques and measurement of different subregions of the corpus callosum may have led
to these discrepancies. Because the corpus callosum is a bundle
of fibers that branch to different cortical regions, consistencybased differences may exist in some callosal regions, but not others
(Nowicka and Tacikowski, 2011). For example, Luders et al., found
differences only in the anterior and posterior midbody of the corpus callosum, which primarily connect the sensory-motor cortices
(Hofer and Frahm, 2006).
Behavioral studies provide more definitive evidence that ICHs
have greater interhemispheric interaction than CHs. Interhemispheric transfer time, measured as the difference in response
times for information processed in the hemisphere opposite the
response hand and information processed in the same hemisphere
as the response hand, is shorter in ICHs than CHs (Cherbuin and
Brinkman, 2006; Bernard et al., 2011). Furthermore, ICHs exhibit
greater interhemispheric transfer of skill learning (Chase and Seidler, 2008). Finally, Lyle and Martin (2010) found that ICHs were
more accurate than CHs at detecting matches between letters (e.g.,
A and a) that were briefly flashed to separate visual fields.
The second assumption of the interhemispheric interaction
hypothesis has sometimes (see Christman and Propper, 2001)
been grounded in the hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry (HERA) model (Tulving et al., 1994), according to which
www.frontiersin.org
September 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 555 | 1
Edlin et al.
Memory for hand use
episodic memory is left lateralized in frontal regions at encoding and right lateralized at retrieval. If this is the case, then
increased interhemispheric interaction could improve episodic
retrieval by enhancing the transfer of information from the left
to the right hemisphere. It appears, however, that the HERA
model may only hold for relatively simple episodic retrieval
tasks that require primarily familiarity-based judgments. Extensive neuroimaging evidence shows that more complex tasks that
require recall of specific episodic details produce frontal activation bilaterally during retrieval (see Nolde et al., 1998; Miller et al.,
2002). From this perspective, greater interhemispheric interaction may improve retrieval by enhancing coordination of retrieval
areas across the hemispheres (Lyle et al., 2008b), but this should
apply only to more complex tasks. Findings have so far supported this prediction with consistency-based differences limited
to tasks that require recall. These tasks have included free recall
(e.g., Propper et al., 2005; Lyle et al., 2008a; Christman and Butler, 2011), cued recall (Parker and Dagnall, 2010), associative
recognition (Lyle et al., 2008b), and recall of source information (Christman et al., 2004; Lyle et al., 2008b). Differences have
not been found on tasks with a small or non-existent recall
component, including old/new recognition (Propper and Christman, 2004; Lyle et al., 2008a), short-term digit span (Lyle et al.,
2008b), and implicit word-fragment completion (Propper et al.,
2005).
Despite abundant evidence for consistency-based differences
in episodic memory, memory for hand use itself has never been
examined. If individuals perform unimanual actions multiple
times with different combinations of their left and right hands,
does their ability to recall how often they used a particular hand
for each task depend on their consistency in everyday life? The
question is of interest for three reasons. First, it is currently unclear
to what extent consistency-based differences in memory occur for
non-verbal information, including motoric and frequency information. Most studies that have documented a memory advantage
for ICHs have employed word stimuli. We count among these
Lyle et al.’s (2008b) tests of source memory. Although the tests
revealed that ICHs were more likely than CHs to remember nonverbal source information, subjects were remembering the source
of words. The only example of an inconsistently handed memory
advantage for strictly non-verbal stimuli is Lyle and Jacobs’s (2010)
finding that ICHs were less likely than CHs to falsely remember visual details from a slideshow depicting a complex event.
Although suggestive, an inconsistently handed advantage occurred
in only one of two studies Lyle and Jacobs conducted. The studies
had somewhat different procedures and Lyle and Jacobs reasoned
that the advantage occurred in the procedure that necessitated
greater recall of episodic details. Nonetheless, we consider this single result only preliminary evidence for an inconsistently handed
memory advantage for non-verbal stimuli. Note also that, when
Lyle et al., tested ICHs and CHs on memory for faces in an old/new
recognition procedure, they did not find an advantage for ICHs.
The authors attributed this null effect to the fact that interhemispheric interaction plays little role in face recognition (Gazzaniga
and Smylie, 1983), but we cannot rule out that it was due to the
non-verbal nature of the stimuli. If ICHs’ memory advantage does
extend to non-verbal information, then we would expect it to
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
occur on a test of memory for the ratio of left to right-hand usage
because such a test clearly requires specific recall.
The second reason for our interest in memory for hand use is
that the hemispheric basis of unimanual action has been found
to differ between ICHs and CHs and, from this, one could predict an inconsistently handed memory disadvantage. When using
the dominant hand, both CHs and ICHs activate the contralateral
motor cortex, but ICHs show greater activation of the ipsilateral
motor cortex than CHs (Dassonville et al., 1997; Bernard et al.,
2011). Greater ipsilateral activation may be related to the increased
thickness of the corpus callosum that connects motor regions in
ICHs. Callosal connections appear to be involved in spreading activation from one hemisphere to the other (e.g., Kinsbourne, 2003;
Bloom and Hynd, 2005). When ICHs execute unimanual actions,
greater callosal connectivity may cause activity in the controlling
motor cortex (contralateral to the hand in use) to spread to the
ipsilateral cortex. It is possible that this would have negative consequences for memory for hand use. Retrieval of action memories
is associated with reactivation of primary motor areas that were
active while performing the actions (Nyberg et al., 2001). This
reactivation during retrieval is thought to aid memory for actions
themselves (Masumoto et al., 2006), producing superior memory for self-performed actions than for verbally encoded action
phrases (for review, see Cohen, 1989) or observed actions (e.g.,
Hornstein and Mulligan, 2001). Presumably, reactivation may also
help individuals remember which hand they used to perform
actions, with activity in a given hemisphere providing evidence
that the action was performed with the contralateral hand. However, CHs and ICHs may not benefit equally from reactivation.
Conceivably, ICHs’ greater bilateral activation during unimanual
action could be mirrored in greater bilateral activation during
retrieval, causing confusion about which hand was used. More
strictly unilateral activation/reactivation among CHs could lead
to greater precision in memory.
The third and final reason for our interest in memory for hand
use is that the study of consistency-based differences in memory
has rested on the critical assumption that people can accurately
remember how they use their hands in everyday life. In this area of
research, consistency has invariably been measured by self-report
on hand preference inventories. On these inventories, subjects
report the frequency with which they use one hand or the other
to perform everyday unimanual tasks (e.g., brushing one’s teeth).
These are reports of past behavior and therefore constitute a type
of memory judgment. These judgments are generally assumed to
be accurate. In other words, individuals who report consistency (or
inconsistency) are assumed to actually perform everyday tasks in
a consistent (or inconsistent) manner. However, memory is fallible in many respects and remembering contextual information, as
opposed to item information, can be especially challenging (Johnson et al., 1993). It may be that people can remember that they
performed certain actions (item information) without remembering how they performed them (contextual information). Therefore, we sought to conduct an initial investigation of memory for
hand use. Although hand preference inventories probe memory
for actions performed outside the laboratory, we felt a reasonable
first step was to test memory for actions performed in a controlled
laboratory setting.
www.frontiersin.org
September 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 555 | 2
Edlin et al.
Memory for hand use
In sum, our goal was to determine the relationship between
handedness consistency and memory for hand use. ICHs’ putatively greater interhemispheric interaction could give them an
advantage over CHs if it facilitates recall of episodic detail. Alternatively, greater interaction between motor areas, and possible
bilateral reactivation of these regions during retrieval, could reduce
ICHs’ ability to determine which hand or hands they used1 . Given
the centrality of handedness inventories in this research area, we
modeled our memory test after the structure of those inventories.
Namely, we measured memory for left:right hand-use ratios for
actions. In addition, we examined memory for actions performed
with and without objects. Hand preference inventories primarily
consist of questions about actions performed with objects, so these
types of actions were of greatest interest to us. However, we were
also interested for exploratory purposes in whether the presence
of an object would affect memory for hand-use or moderate any
consistency-based memory differences.
Table 1 | Actions performed during the hand-use task.
Without objects
Blow a kiss
Bounce the ball (small rubber ball)
Count to five
Drop a coin in the box (small box with slot and four
pennies)
Cover your eye
Give a thumbs up
Flip over the card (playing card)
Move the mouse in a circle (mouse with cord
removed)
Knock on the desk
Open the tupperware (small rubbermaid container)
Pat your head
Pull out your chair
Point to the monitor
Roll the die (six-sided die)
Snap your fingers
Take a piece of tape (roll of scotch tape)
Squeeze your hand
Take off the marker cap (dry erase marker)
Wave
Use the hole puncher (single hole punch and index
card)
a
MATERIALS AND METHODS
With objectsa
Subjects received the objects included in parenthesis.
SUBJECTS
Subjects were undergraduates aged 18–30 who received credit in
psychology courses for participating and provided informed consent under protocols approved by the University of Louisville IRB.
Using the handedness inventory described below, and following
the method from previous studies (e.g., Lyle et al., 2008b, 2012;
Edlin and Lyle, 2013), we classified subjects according to their
inventory scores. Subjects were classified as CHs if the absolute
value of their inventory score was 80 or greater (n = 50, M absolute
score = 92.8, eight males) or as ICHs if the absolute value of their
inventory score was <80 (n = 35, M absolute score = 57.6, nine
males, one unknown). Although we categorized subjects by consistency instead of direction of handedness, our sample included
seven subjects who could be classified as left-handed due to negative inventory scores. Of these, two were CHs (M score = −92.5,
one male) and five were ICHs (M score = −59, two males, one
unknown).
MATERIALS
We assessed degree of handedness consistency using a modified
version of Oldfield’s (1971) Edinburgh Handedness Inventory.
The inventory queries hand preferences for 10 activities (writing,
drawing, using a spoon, opening jars, using a toothbrush, throwing, combing hair, using scissors, using a knife without a fork, and
striking a match). For each activity, the response options (and corresponding point values) are Always Right (+10), Usually Right
(+5), No Preference (0), Usually Left (−5), and Always Left (−10).
Inventory scores range from −100 (consistently left-handed) to
+100 (consistently right-handed) in 5-point increments.
For the hand-use task, we selected 20 actions (see Table 1),
half of which required manipulating objects (e.g., roll the dice)
and half of which did not (e.g., snap your fingers). The necessary objects were provided to subjects in a container at the
1 These two possibilities are of
primary interest to the authors, but other predictions
are certainly possible. For example, one might have expected CHs to have superior
memory relative to ICHs for actions performed with the left hand due to the greater
distinctiveness of left-hand usage for the former group than the latter (remembering
that the vast majority of our subjects were right-hand dominant).
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
beginning of the experiment. Actions were performed at one of
five possible left:right hand-use ratios: always left (4:0), usually
left (3:1), equal (2:2), usually right (1:3), or always right (0:4).
Assignment of action to ratio was counterbalanced. For the performance/encoding phase of the procedure, we created four blocks
of action commands. Each command instructed subjects to perform 1 of the 20 actions with a particular hand (e.g., “roll the
dice with your left hand”). Thus, there were 20 commands per
block. The commands were presented in pseudo-random order
such that object actions and no-object actions were evenly distributed throughout each block. Commands were presented in a
different order in each block. For actions assigned to the always-left
and always-right ratios, the command was to use the same hand
in every block. For actions assigned to the other three ratios, the
commands varied in accordance with the particular ratio. Among
actions assigned to the same ratio, the sequence of left/right commands was different for each action. For the retrieval phase of the
procedure, a new random ordering of the 20 actions (without a
performance command) was created for each subject.
PROCEDURE
Subjects first completed the handedness inventory and were
given the container of objects. Subjects then began the performance/encoding phase. Action commands appeared on a computer screen one at a time. For actions that required an object,
subjects took the object out of the container, performed the action
with the specified hand a single time, and replaced the object in the
container. For no-object actions, subjects simply performed the
action one time. Subjects were instructed to perform each action
four times. Left:right hand-use ratio varied between actions such
that each subject performed some actions in all five of the ratios.
After each action, subjects rated the action on how difficult it was to
perform the action with the specified hand and how natural it felt
to do so. Ratings were made on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (difficult /unnatural) to 9 (easy/natural). Performance and ratings were
self-paced. Subjects performed all 20 actions in each of 4 blocks.
After the fourth and final block, there was a surprise memory
www.frontiersin.org
September 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 555 | 3
Edlin et al.
Memory for hand use
test. Each action was presented on the screen and subjects were
instructed to select the hand-use ratio they remembered using to
perform the task by pressing a number one through five that corresponded to always left, usually left, equal, usually right, or always
right. Subjects were required to choose a ratio for each action.
RESULTS
RECALL
We submitted proportion of correct responses on the hand-use
memory test to a 2 (consistency: CH or ICH) × 5 (ratio: always
left, usually left, equal, usually right, or always right) × 2 (action
type: object or no object) mixed-factorial ANOVA with consistency as a between-subjects factor and ratio and action type as
within-subjects factors.
Addressing our primary research question, there was a main
effect of consistency with ICHs (M = 0.57) producing more correct responses than CHs (M = 0.47), F (1, 83) = 4.38, p = 0.039,
η2p = 0.05. ICHs remembered all ratios numerically better than did
CHs, as shown in Figure 1 and as reflected in the non-significant
consistency X ratio interaction, F (4, 80) = 0.232, p = 0.920, η2p =
0.011. The consistency X action type interaction was also nonsignificant, F (1, 83) = 1.41, p = 0.239, η2p = 0.017, indicating
that the inconsistently handed advantage occurred regardless of
whether actions were performed with or without objects.
There was also a main effect of ratio, F (4, 80) = 12.72,
p < 0.001, η2p = 0.389, indicating that some hand-use behaviors
were better remembered than others (see Figure 2). Most strikingly, proportion correct for the always-right ratio (M = 0.68) was
significantly higher than for all other ratios, smallest t (84) = 2.97,
p = 0.004. Also, proportion correct was higher for the equal ratio
(M = 0.56) than for the usually left (M = 0.39) or usually right
(M = 0.47) ratios, smallest t (84) = 2.34, p = 0.022. In addition,
proportion correct for usually right was higher than for usually
left, t (84) = 2.16, p = 0.033, and always left (M = 0.49) was higher
than usually left, t (84) = 2.20, p = 0.030. We examined the distribution of incorrect responses, which is shown in Figure 3. Three
FIGURE 1 | Mean proportion correct as a function of hand-use ratio and consistency. Error bars indicate ±1 SEM.
FIGURE 2 | Mean proportion correct as a function of hand-use ratio. Error bars indicate ±1 SEM.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
www.frontiersin.org
September 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 555 | 4
Edlin et al.
Memory for hand use
them (7.9). However, for right-hand actions, the fluency difference
between actions performed without an object (M = 8.8) versus
with one (M = 8.7) was very small, albeit significant, t (84) = 4.10,
p < 0.001. For left-hand actions, the difference between actions
performed without an object (M = 7.9) versus with one (M = 7.2)
was markedly larger, t (84) = 10.85, p < 0.001.
FIGURE 3 | Distribution of incorrect responses as a function of
hand-use ratio.
patterns are evident. First, when the correct ratio was unequal,
incorrect responses were usually shifted to a less extreme (versus
more extreme) ratio than the actual one. Second, when the correct
ratio was equal, incorrect responses tended to be shifted to the
usually left or usually right ratio (rather than to one of the always
ratios). Third, there was a slight tendency to respond with ratios
in which there was more right-hand usage (rather than with ratios
in which there was more left-hand usage); this can be seen most
clearly in the equal ratio condition. None of these patterns suggest
a strong response bias that could account for any of the significant
between-ratio accuracy differences.
The ratio X action type interaction approached significance,
F (4, 80) = 2.32, p = 0.058, η2p = 0.027, but the trend was not
readily interpretable. Briefly, proportion correct with and without
objects was very similar and statistically indistinguishable for all
ratios except usually right, for which object actions (M = 0.55)
were remembered better than no-object actions (M = 0.40),
t (84) = 2.63, p = 0.01.
RATINGS
Because difficulty and naturalness ratings were highly correlated,
r(85) = 0.53, p < 0.001, we combined the two ratings into a composite fluency rating. We submitted ratings to a 2 (hand: left
or right) × 2 (action type: object or no object) × 2 (consistency:
CH or ICH) mixed-factorial ANOVA with hand and action type
as within-subjects factors and consistency as a between-subjects
factor.
The main effect of consistency was not significant, F (1,
83) = 1.08, p = 0.302, η2p = 0.013, and neither were the interactions between consistency and hand or action type, F s < 1, suggesting that the inconsistently handed memory advantage described
above was not due to greater fluency when performing the actions.
There were significant main effects of hand, F (1, 83) = 121.24,
p < 0.001, η2p = 0.594, and action type, F (1, 83) = 129.028,
p < 0.001, η2p = 0.609, but these were qualified by a significant
interaction between the two factors, F (1, 83) = 68.479, p < 0.001,
η2 = 0.452. Overall, subjects felt more fluent when performing
right-hand actions (M = 8.7) than left-hand actions (M = 7.5),
and when performing actions without objects (8.4) than with
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
DISCUSSION
The primary goal of this study was to compare the ability of ICHs
and CHs to remember how they used their hands. Recent studies
have found that ICHs have superior episodic memory but have not
revealed whether this advantage extends to non-verbal stimuli or
to memory for hand use, in particular. Empirically, many studies
have shown an inconsistently handed advantage for verbal stimuli
(e.g., Propper et al., 2005; Lyle et al., 2008a; Christman and Butler,
2011), but, to our knowledge, only one has done the same for nonverbal stimuli (Lyle and Jacobs, 2010). Given that ICHs differ from
CHs in language lateralization (Knecht et al., 2000) and that ICHs
have more diffuse semantic networks (Sontam and Christman,
2012), it was conceivable that these factors resulted in a memory advantage specifically for verbal stimuli. Theoretically, ICHs’
putatively greater interhemispheric interaction (Christman and
Propper, 2001; Lyle et al., 2008b) fostered opposing predictions
about memory for hand use. Greater interaction could benefit
memory by increasing recall of episodic details or harm it by
producing bilateral reactivation in motor cortex during retrieval
of unimanual actions. Our results resolved these empirical and
theoretical uncertainties by clearly showing that ICHs performed
better than CHs on our test of memory for hand-use ratio. This
finding supports the conclusion that the inconsistently handed
memory advantage does extend to non-verbal events, including
self-performed actions.
Although we found a consistency-based difference in memory for hand use, elucidating the cause of this difference requires
additional research. The results of our ratings data argue against
the possibility that the memory difference was due to differences
between ICHs and CHs in hand-use fluency, because the groups
provided similar fluency ratings. Following, we consider three
other possible explanations for ICHs’ superior ability to remember which hand they used to perform actions. One is that, while
reactivation of motor cortex at retrieval can potentially be used by
either CHs or ICHs to determine which hand or hands were used
at performance/encoding, ICHs are more skilled at interpreting
this reactivation. If ICHs routinely experience bilateral activation
during performance (Dassonville et al., 1997) and corresponding bilateral reactivation during retrieval (Nyberg et al., 2001),
they may have adapted some means of differentiating between
contralateral activation due to hand-use and ipsilateral activation
from callosal “overflow.” This could make ICHs more sophisticated decision makers than CHs when recalling which hand or
hands they used to perform an action during instances of bilateral
motor cortex reactivation.
Alternatively, individuals may use motor cortex reactivation
only to determine that an action was performed versus not, and not
to determine exactly which hand or hands were used to perform
the action. Prior studies showing a memory advantage for performed actions compared to actions encoded using other methods
www.frontiersin.org
September 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 555 | 5
Edlin et al.
Memory for hand use
(e.g., reading action phrases; see Cohen, 1989) have focused on
remembering the presence or absence of an action and not specific details of the action. If specific hand-use information is not
gleaned from motor cortex reactivation, then correctly remembering hand use would presumably rely on recollecting specific
episodic details encoded during the act of performance (e.g., visual
details, cognitive operations). If this explanation is correct, we
could attribute the superior performance of ICHs on this task to
the already established fact that ICHs are superior to CHs when
recalling episodic details.
A third possible explanation for ICHs’ superior memory for
hand-use assumes, like our first explanation, that motor cortex
reactivation at retrieval can serve as a useful source of information about which hand or hands were used, but that ICHs are
less reliant on it than CHs. ICHs’ hand-use memories may be
based on both motor cortex reactivation and, as proposed in our
second explanation, recall of additional episodic details. In contrast, CHs may rely largely on motor cortex reactivation, which
may be insufficient to remember specific hand-use ratios. Propper
and Christman (2004) found that ICHs’ memories are more likely
than CHs’ to be accompanied by a rich recollective experience
(indexed by “Remember” responses), whereas CHs’ memories are
more likely than ICHs’ to be accompanied only by a non-specific
sense of familiarity (indexed by “Know” responses). In the context
of remembering hand use, CHs’ reliance on motor cortex reactivation may give rise to a similar sense of familiarity while ICHs’ use
of additional episodic details may produce a sense of recollection.
Consideration of these possibilities raises the important lingering question of exactly how ICHs’ putatively greater interhemispheric interaction might cause them to remember the episodic
past more accurately and in greater detail. One idea not yet put
forth in the literature is that ICHs’ threshold for recruiting both
hemispheres, versus only one, during episodic memory tests may
be lower than CHs’. As mentioned in the introduction, complex
memory tasks produce bilateral frontal activation (see Nolde et al.,
1998; Miller et al., 2002) and greater connectivity between the
hemispheres via the corpus callosum has been proposed as an
explanation for ICHs’ superior memory performance on these
tasks. However, recruitment of both hemispheres to perform a
task comes at a cost, which is dependent on interhemispheric
transfer times. Banich (1998) proposed that simple tasks with low
attentional demands are more efficiently processed by a single
hemisphere specialized for that task. As task complexity increases,
processing load overcomes the cost of transferring information
across the corpus callosum. The threshold at which it is more
efficient to recruit both hemispheres than rely on a single specialized hemisphere would theoretically be lower for individuals
who have faster interhemispheric transfer times, including ICHs
(Cherbuin and Brinkman, 2006; Bernard et al., 2011). Therefore,
in addition to ICHs having an advantage on memory tasks that
typically induce bilateral processing in all individuals (ICHs and
CHs alike), ICHs may also be more likely to recruit additional
hemispheric processing capability on tasks are not typically associated with bilateral activation (that is, not associated with bilateral
activation in CHs, who constitute the majority of the population).
Our finding that ICHs were better at recalling hand-use ratios
than CHs might be considered worrisome given that, in research
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
examining the relationship between consistency and cognition (as
well as consistency and personality; e.g., Christman et al., 2008;
Lyle and Grillo, 2013) classification as ICHs or CHs is based on
self-reported hand use. If ICHs are superior to CHs at recalling
instances in which they used a combination of their left and right
hands to perform tasks in everyday life, as they were better at
remembering these instances in the laboratory, then a troubling
possibility presents itself. ICHs and CHs could conceivably have
similar real-world hand-use behaviors for the actions queried on
hand preference inventories, including a similar rate of inconsistent behavior, but ICHs may be more likely to remember that
they behaved inconsistently. If this were the case, it would undercut the idea that people who use their hands inconsistently have
better episodic memory, and instead mean that people who have
better episodic memory are more likely to remember instances
of inconsistency when completing handedness questionnaires. Of
course, recalling the exact left:right ratios for laboratory tasks performed only four times and after a retention interval of only a few
minutes is different in many respects than recalling one’s pattern
of behavior for real-world actions performed innumerable times
outside of the laboratory. Nonetheless, ICHs have been found to
have more accurate and more detailed memories for events outside of the lab (Christman et al., 2003; Propper et al., 2005; Parker
and Dagnall, 2010). Of course, it is also possible that people rely
more on semantic memory instead of retrieving specific events
when recalling patterns of behavior for real-world actions. There is
some evidence that consistency-based differences do not extend to
semantic memory (Propper et al., 2005), therefore ICHs and CHs
may be equally accurate when reporting hand-use on handedness
inventories. Future research is needed to explore the accuracy of
responses on handedness questionnaires.
Finally, we consider our finding that, regardless of consistency,
some hand-use ratios were remembered better than others. In particular, the always-right ratio was remembered significantly better
than all others. In interpreting this finding, recall that our sample included only seven left-handed subjects (i.e., subjects with
negative scores on the handedness inventory). The strong representation of right-handers was evident in the subjective fluency
data where subjects rated left-hand actions as being more difficult
and less natural than right-hand actions. Hence, most subjects
in our sample, regardless of consistency, were right-hand dominant and we found that memory was best for actions that were
always performed with the dominant hand. This may be due
to the strength of motor cortex reactivation during recall. Prior
studies have reported greater activation in the contralateral hemisphere when performing tasks with the dominant hand than with
the non-dominant hand (Dassonville et al., 1997). If this pattern
is mirrored in reactivation at retrieval, and individuals rely on
reactivation to determine the hand used to perform an action,
then stronger reactivation following right- than left-hand actions
should have resulted in superior memory for always-right actions
than always-left actions. This is the result we obtained. For actions
performed with both hands, both motor cortices may have been
reactivated, but there may have been stronger reactivation in the
dominant (left) hemisphere than in the non-dominant (right)
hemisphere, and the former may have masked the latter. This could
lead to failures to remember that these actions were performed
www.frontiersin.org
September 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 555 | 6
Edlin et al.
Memory for hand use
with both hands, reducing accuracy for the usually left, equal, and
usually right ratios.
Also, right-handers have been found to exhibit bilateral activation in motor cortex when performing tasks with their left
hand (e.g., Kim et al., 1993; Cramer et al., 1999). The unexpected ipsilateral (left hemisphere) activation may be related to
the increased complexity of using the non-dominant hand (Haaland et al., 2004). If bilateral activation during performance is
mirrored at retrieval, and individuals use patterns of reactivation
to determine which hand was used, this could have contributed to
poorer memory for always-left actions than always-right actions.
In summary, we compared the ability of ICHs and CHs to
remember left:right hand-use ratios for actions and found that
ICHs outperformed CHs. Both groups were significantly better at
remembering the actions performed in the always-right ratio than
all other ratios.
REFERENCES
sex on the morphology of the corpus callosum – a study with brain
magnetic resonance imaging. Brain
Cogn. 16, 41–61. doi:10.1016/02782626(91)90084-L
Hofer, S., and Frahm, J. (2006). Topography of the human corpus callosum revisited – comprehensive fiber
tractography using diffusion tensor
magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 32, 989–994. doi:10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2006.05.044
Hornstein, S. L., and Mulligan, N. W.
(2001). Memory of action events: the
role of objects in memory of selfand other-performed tasks. Am. J.
Psychol. 114, 199–217. doi:10.2307/
1423515
Jäncke, L., and Steinmetz, H. (2003).
“Brain size: a possible source of
interindividual variability in corpus
callosum morphology,” in The Parallel Brain: The Cognitive Neuroscience
of the Corpus Callosum, eds E. Zaidel,
and M. Iacoboni (Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press), 51–63.
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., and
Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychol. Bull. 114, 3–28. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3
Kim, S. G., Ashe, J., Hendrich, K., Ellermann, J. M., Merkle, H., Ugurbil, K.,
et al. (1993). Functional magnetic
resonance imaging of motor cortex – hemispheric asymmetry and
handedness. Science 261, 615–617.
doi:10.1126/science.8342027
Kinsbourne, M. (2003). “The corpus
callosum equilibrates the cerebral
hemispheres,” in The Parallel Brain:
The Cognitive Neuroscience of the
Corpus Callosum, eds E. Zaidel, and
M. Iacoboni (Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press), 271–281.
Knecht, S., Drager, B., Deppe, M.,
Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Floel, A.,
et al. (2000). Handedness and
hemispheric language dominance
in healthy humans. Brain 123,
2512–2518. doi:10.1093/brain/123.
12.2512
Luders, E., Cherbuin, N., Thompson,
P. M., Gutman, B., Anstey, K. J.,
Sachdev, P., et al. (2010). When more
is less: associations between corpus
callosum size and handedness
Banich, M. T. (1998). The missing
link: the role of interhemispheric
interaction in attentional processing. Brain Cogn. 36, 128–157. doi:10.
1006/brcg.1997.0950
Bernard, J. A., Taylor, S. F., and Seidler, R. D. (2011). Handedness, dexterity, and motor cortical representations. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 88–99.
doi:10.1152/jn.00512.2010
Bloom, J. S., and Hynd, G. W. (2005).
The role of the corpus callosum in
interhemispheric transfer of information: excitation or inhibition?
Neuropsychol. Rev. 15, 59–71. doi:10.
1007/s11065-005-6252-y
Chase, C., and Seidler, R. (2008).
Degree of handedness affects
intermanual transfer of skill
learning. Exp. Brain Res. 190,
317–328. doi:10.1007/s00221-0081472-z
Cherbuin, N., and Brinkman, C. (2006).
Hemispheric interactions are different in left-handed individuals. Neuropsychology 20, 700–707. doi:10.
1037/0894-4105.20.6.700
Christman, S. D., and Butler, M.
(2011). Mixed-handedness advantages in episodic memory obtained
under conditions of intentional
learning extend to incidental learning. Brain Cogn. 77, 17–22. doi:10.
1016/j.bandc.2011.07.003
Christman, S. D., Garvey, K. J., Propper, R. E., and Phaneuf, K. A.
(2003). Bilateral eye movements
enhance the retrieval of episodic
memories. Neuropsychology 17,
221–229. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.
17.2.221
Christman, S. D., Henning, B., Geers,
A. L., Propper, R. E., and Niebauer,
C. L. (2008). Mixed-handed persons are more easily persuaded and
are more gullible: interhemispheric
interaction and belief updating. Laterality 13, 403–426. doi:10.1080/
13576500802079646
Christman, S. D., and Propper, R.
E. (2001). Superior episodic memory is associated with interhemispheric processing. Neuropsychology 15, 607–616. doi:10.1037/08944105.15.4.607
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
Christman, S. D., Propper, R. E., and
Dion, A. (2004). Increased interhemispheric interaction is associated with decreased false memories in a verbal converging semantic
associates paradigm. Brain Cogn. 56,
313–319. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.
08.005
Cohen, R. L. (1989). Memory for action
events: the power of enactment.
Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1, 57–80. doi:10.
1007/BF01326550
Cowell, P. E., Kertesz, A., and
Denenberg, V. H. (1993). Multiple dimensions of handedness
and the human corpus callosum. Neurology 43, 2353–2357.
doi:10.1212/WNL.43.11.2353
Cramer, S. C., Finklestein, S. P.,
Schaechter, J. D., Bush, G., and
Rosen, B. R. (1999). Activation of
distinct motor cortex regions during ipsilateral and contralateral finger movements. J. Neurophysiol. 81,
383–387.
Dassonville, P., Zhu, X. H., Ugurbil,
K., Kim, S. G., and Ashe, J. (1997).
Functional activation in motor cortex reflects the direction and the
degree of handedness. Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 14015–14018.
doi:10.1073/pnas.94.25.14015
Edlin, J. M., and Lyle, K. B. (2013).
The effect of repetitive saccade execution on the attention network test:
enhancing executive function with
a flick of the eyes. Brain Cogn. 81,
345–351. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2012.
12.006
Gazzaniga, M. S., and Smylie, C.
S. (1983). Facial recognition and
brain asymmetries – clues to
underlying mechanisms. Ann. Neurol. 13, 536–540. doi:10.1002/ana.
410130511
Haaland, K. Y., Elsinger, C. L., Mayer,
A. R., Durgerian, S., and Rao, S.
M. (2004). Motor sequence complexity and performing hand produce differential patterns of hemispheric lateralization. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 621–636. doi:10.1162/
089892904323057344
Habib, M., Gayraud, D., Oliva, A.,
Regis, J., Salamon, G., and Khalil, R.
(1991). Effects of handedness and
www.frontiersin.org
lateralization. Neuroimage
52,
43–49. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2010.04.016
Lyle, K. B., and Grillo, M. C.
(2013). Consistent-handed individuals are more authoritarian. Laterality. Available at: http://pubget.com/
paper/23586369
Lyle, K. B., Hanaver-Torrez, S. D., Häcklander, R. P., and Edlin, J. M. (2012).
Consistency of handedness, regardless of direction, predicts baseline
memory accuracy and potential for
memory enhancement. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 38, 187–193.
Lyle, K. B., and Jacobs, N. E. (2010). Is
saccade-induced retrieval enhancement a potential means of improving eyewitness evidence? Memory
18, 581–594. doi:10.1080/09658211.
2010.493891
Lyle, K. B., Logan, J. M., and Roediger, H.
L. (2008a). Eye movements enhance
memory for individuals who are
strongly right-handed and harm it
for individuals who are not. Psychon.
Bull. Rev. 15, 515–520. doi:10.3758/
PBR.15.3.515
Lyle, K. B., McCabe, D. P., and Roediger,
H. L. (2008b). Handedness is related
to memory via hemispheric interaction: evidence from paired associate recall and source memory tasks.
Neuropsychology 22, 523–530. doi:
10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.523
Lyle, K. B., and Martin, J. M.
(2010). Bilateral saccades increase
intrahemispheric processing but
not interhemispheric interaction:
implications for saccade-induced
retrieval enhancement. Brain Cogn.
73, 128–134. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.
2010.04.004
Masumoto, K., Yamaguchi, M., Sutani,
K., Tsuneto, S., Fujita, A., and
Tonoike, M. (2006). Reactivation
of physical motor information in
the memory of action events. Brain
Res. 1101, 102–109. doi:10.1016/j.
brainres.2006.05.033
Miller, M. B., Kingstone, A., and
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2002). Hemispheric encoding asymmetry is more
apparent than real. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 702–708. doi:10.1162/
08989290260138609
September 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 555 | 7
Edlin et al.
Nolde, S. F., Johnson, M. K., and Raye,
C. L. (1998). The role of prefrontal
cortex during tests of episodic
memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2,
399–406.
doi:10.1016/S13646613(98)01233-9
Nowicka, A., and Tacikowski, P. (2011).
Transcallosal transfer of information
and functional asymmetry of the
human brain. Laterality 16, 35–74.
doi:10.1080/13576500903154231
Nyberg, L., Petersson, K. M., Nilsson, L. G., Sandblom, J., Aberg, C.,
and Ingvar, M. (2001). Reactivation of motor brain areas during
explicit memory for actions. Neuroimage 14, 521–528. doi:10.1006/
nimg.2001.0801
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness:
the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi:10.1016/
0028-3932(71)90067-4
Parker, A., and Dagnall, N. (2010).
Effects of handedness and saccadic bilateral eye movements on
components of autobiographical
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
Memory for hand use
recollection. Brain Cogn. 73,
93–101. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.
03.005
Prichard, E., Propper, R. E., and Christman, S. D. (2013). Degree of handedness, but not direction, is a systematic predictor of cognitive performance. Front. Psychol. 4:9. doi:10.
3389/fpsyg.2013.00009
Propper, R. E., and Christman, S. D.
(2004). Mixed-versus strong righthandedness is associated with biases
towards “remember” versus “know”
judgements in recognition memory:
role of interhemispheric interaction.
Memory 12, 707–714. doi:10.1080/
09658210344000503
Propper, R. E., Christman, S. D., and
Phaneuf, K. A. (2005). A mixedhanded advantage in episodic memory: a possible role of interhemispheric interaction. Mem. Cognit. 33,
751–757.
Sontam,V., and Christman, S. D. (2012).
Semantic organisation and handedness: mixed-handedness is associated with more diffuse activation of
ambiguous word associates. Laterality 17, 38–50.
Tulving, E., Kapur, S., Craik, F. I.
M., Moscovitch, M., and Houle,
S. (1994). Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic
memory – positron emission
tomography findings. Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 2016–2020.
doi:10.1073/pnas.91.6.2016
Welcome, S. E., Chiarello, C., Towler,
S., Halderman, L. K., Otto, R., and
Leonard, C. M. (2009). Behavioral
correlates of corpus callosum size:
anatomical/behavioral relationships
vary across sex/handedness groups.
Neuropsychologia 47, 2427–2435.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
2009.04.008
Witelson, S. F. (1985). The brain
connection – the corpus callosum
is larger in left-handers. Science
229, 665–668. doi:10.1126/science.
4023705
Conflict of Interest Statement: The
authors declare that the research was
www.frontiersin.org
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that
could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Received: 15 June 2013; accepted: 21
August 2013; published online: 04 September 2013.
Citation: Edlin JM, Carris EK and
Lyle KB (2013) Memory for handuse depends on consistency of handedness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:555. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00555
This article was submitted to the journal
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.
Copyright © 2013 Edlin, Carris and Lyle.
This is an open-access article distributed
under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The
use, distribution or reproduction in other
forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and
that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted
academic practice. No use, distribution or
reproduction is permitted which does not
comply with these terms.
September 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 555 | 8