This article was downloaded by: [University of Helsinki]
On: 06 April 2014, At: 23:47
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Early Years: An International Research
Journal
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ceye20
Children’s physical activity in day care
and preschool
a
a
b
a
Jyrki Reunamo , Liisa Hakala , Leila Saros , Satu Lehto , Annaa
Liisa Kyhälä & Juha Valtonen
a
a
Department of Teacher Education, University of Helsinki,
Helsinki, Finland
b
School of Education, University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland
Published online: 11 Oct 2013.
To cite this article: Jyrki Reunamo, Liisa Hakala, Leila Saros, Satu Lehto, Anna-Liisa Kyhälä & Juha
Valtonen (2014) Children’s physical activity in day care and preschool, Early Years: An International
Research Journal, 34:1, 32-48, DOI: 10.1080/09575146.2013.843507
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2013.843507
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/termsand-conditions
Early Years, 2014
Vol. 34, No. 1, 32–48, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2013.843507
Children’s physical activity in day care and preschool
Jyrki Reunamoa*, Liisa Hakalaa, Leila Sarosb, Satu Lehtoa, Anna-Liisa Kyhäläa and
Juha Valtonena
a
Department of Teacher Education, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland; bSchool of
Education, University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
(Received 8 April 2013; final version received 9 September 2013)
The purpose of the study was to investigate the dynamics of physical activity
(PA) in day care and preschool. The participants were 823 Finnish 1–7-year-old
children from 50 day care centres and preschools. The research methods were
systematic observation, evaluation of children’s skills and interviews with children. Altogether 18,366 observations were collected. Vygotsky’s concept of the
‘zone of proximal development’ is redefined in relation to PA. The results of the
study show that environmental factors, and specifically peers, do indeed play a
significant role in enhancing PA among children. There are cogent reasons for
paying more attention to the dynamics of children’s PA. Furthermore, there is
reason to ponder extensively how to provide opportunities for PA and, more
precisely, the development of motor competence in the day care and preschool
context.
Keywords: early childhood; high physical activity; Vygotsky; peer relations;
physical education; learning environment
Introduction
Physical activity (PA) is commonly defined as ‘any body movements produced by
the skeletal muscles and that results in a substantial increase over the resting energy
expenditure’ (Malina, Bouchard, and Bar-Or 2004, 6). From the muscular perspective, these movements can be classified as gross motor skills (running, jumping,
throwing etc.) or fine motor skills (writing, knitting etc.). In contrast, when defined
by their functional aspects, it is a question of fundamental motor skills (FMS). FMS
are movements intended to gain or maintain balance, transport the body from one
point to another and impart force to or receive force from an object (Gallahue,
Ozmun, and Goodway 2012, 16–17). In order to achieve basic motor competence, a
highly important factor for choices concerning a physically active lifestyle, a child
needs to have plenty of opportunities for diverse physical activities and fundamental
skill learning. Children with motor learning difficulties tend to be less physically
active than their coordinated peers (Hands and Larkin 2002). In this study, children’s
PA is categorized into three levels: low, intermediate or high. Additionally, PA is
seen as culturally mediated. The general model employed in the study is influenced
by Vygotsky (cf. Reunamo 2009). This model is presented in Figure 1.
*Corresponding author. Email: jyrki.reunamo@helsinki.fi
© 2013 TACTYC
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
Early Years
33
Figure 1. The feedback loop of culturally mediated PA.
Zone of proximal PA
The zone of proximal development (ZPD) is a concept formulated by Vygotsky
(1978). In ZPD, development is understood as the distance between a child’s personal skills and the activities possible with more competent guidance. According to
Vygotsky, developmental factors are interpsychological, and skills are seen as something that can be learned with the assistance of others. In early childhood education
and care (ECEC), learning environments are versatile. They consist both of material
elements, such as facilities, the immediate environment and equipment, and immaterial elements, such as social and psychological settings (Ministry of Social Affairs
and Health 2005). Studies have indicated that the ECEC environment has a significant impact on children’s PA (Bower et al. 2008; Hodges et al. 2012). In particular,
this is the case with directed PA, outdoor play and time allotted for both. Less fixed
playground equipment and a large playground seem to increase the amount of high
PA in children (Bower et al. 2008; Dowda et al. 2009). Outdoors is where free play
and gross motor activity are most likely to occur (Burdette, Whitaker, and Daniels
2004). However, there are also findings that emphasize the importance of social factors on children’s PA, especially interrelationship between children (Jago et al. 2011;
Wilkin 2011).This suggests that there is a need to widen the concept of ZPD. Consequently, a more accurate concept is suggested for the purpose: the zone of proximal
PA. By this, we refer to the distance between the children’s own PA and the PA that
is possible to create with peers.
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
34
J. Reunamo et al.
Personal PA and skills
Quoting Finnish national recommendations, a child needs at least two hours of brisk
PA every day (Ministry of Social Affairs and Health 2005). The urge to give guidelines suggests that the benefits of PA are obvious. This is especially the case from
the vantage point of health and motor development (see Malina, Bouchard, and
Bar-Or 2004). The urge for recommendations also suggests that there are trends in
contemporary society that constrain children’s PA. Sociocultural trends like increasing concerns over safety, the increasing divide between the fit and unfit or lean and
overweight, which are partly the result of the socioeconomic status of the family
and, more generally, the spread of sedentary lifestyles (see Dollman, Norton, and
Norton 2005; Wedderkopp et al. 2004; Lehto et al. 2009) form the rationale for manifold actions to enhance PA in western countries, even among very young children.
A physically active lifestyle in early childhood forms the basis for a healthy
and physically active lifestyle in adulthood (Yang et al. 1999; Reilly et al.
2004; Sääkslahti 2005). It also forms the basis for the development of perceptual and fundamental skills (Sääkslahti 2005; Gallahue, Ozmun, and Goodway
2012). The development of FMS is highly important during the early years of a
child’s life because, particularly from middle childhood onward, a child with
limited motor competence or proficiency chooses sedentary activities over PA
(Wrotniak et al. 2006; Gallahue, Ozmun, and Goodway 2012). Access to PA is
the route for learning gross motor skills and FMS, and personal PA is the route
to motor competence. Similarly, motor competence seems to be a trigger for
increased levels of personal PA.
Physical orientation
Children’s physical orientation refers to their personal choices and the production of
physical activities. According to Vygotsky (2004), children combine the skills
acquired and use them to construct a new reality, one that conforms to their own
needs and desires. Children’s personal choices matter. Children learn through play,
and they display and build their inner selves: who they are, what they know and
how they feel (Butcher and Eaton 1989). According to Reunamo, Saros, and
Ruismäki (2012), children’s PA is low during activities arranged by teachers.
Furthermore, in day care, children have a large degree of freedom to decide on and
invent their own physical activities.
Children’s orientation changes as they develop. Gubbels et al. (2011) found
that social factors were associated with PA intensity in 2–3-year-old children.
Peer group size was negatively associated with the level of activity during
indoor activity. In line with this study, social environmental factors such as
smaller group size among 2–3-year-old children (Cardon, Van Cauwenberghe
and De Bourdeaudhuij 2011) and 3–5-year-old children (Brown et al. 2009)
were shown to be associated with PA intensity. A major transition takes place
from the ability to engage in one-to-one relationships to the ability to relate to
peers in groups (Hay, Payne, and Chadwic 2004; for age groups cf. Gallahue,
Ozmun, and Goodway 2012). Children’s lack of ability to follow the rules of
organized physical play, or personality traits like shyness and anxiety, have been
found to reduce PA (Dwyer et al. 2008).
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
Early Years
35
Shared creation of PA
According to Vygotsky (2004), children use their skills as tools for creating new
forms of culture. Children’s personal PA orientation becomes a shared and creative
process. It was found in a study of preschool policies and practices that children
spent significantly less time in sedentary activities when the interaction was better
between the children and teachers and among the children (Dowda et al. 2004).
Livesey et al. (2010) studied 9–12-year-old students and Vannatta et al. (2009) studied students from second grade elementary to high school. They found that children
with poor motor performance had lower levels of PA and were less liked by their
peers. It may be that these tendencies have their roots in early childhood. Brown
et al. (2009) found that high PA was most likely in situations where children were
without adults; child-initiated activities resulted in higher PA than adult-initiated
activities. Cardon et al. (2008) noticed that PA decreased when there were more
teachers supervising. The conventional ‘isolated’ view of the physical environment
should thus be modified to include the social environment as child-initiated rather
than staff-initiated play (cf. Brown et al. 2009).
According to Kyhälä, Reunamo, and Ruismäki (2012), a high degree of PA and
learning takes place in highly variable and nonstructured learning environments,
often created by children. When evaluating children’s PA, it is worth looking at both
direct education and children’s activities in their personal contexts, which they have
themselves helped to create. Children’s PA and skills should be considered as a
social whole in which children’s mutual relations play an important role. Social
interaction forms the basis for structured PA (Lehto, Reunamo, and Ruismäki 2012).
Methods
The purpose of the study was to investigate the dynamics of culturally mediated PA
in day care and preschool. The research questions were as follows:
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
What are the environmental conditions for PA?
How is children’s PA related to their other skills and qualities?
What kinds of choices are related to children’s physical orientation?
What kinds of factors are related to the creation of a physically active
learning environment?
Participants
In Finland, an average of 46% of all under school-aged children attended day care
organized by their municipality in 2005 (Ministry of Social Affairs and Health
2006. 823 children participated in the study, of whom 143 were 1–3-year-olds, 380
were 4–5-year-olds and 280 were 6–7-year-olds (age was not reported for 20
children). The study included 78 children with special needs. There were 419 boys
and 382 girls in the sample (gender information from the children’s skill evaluation
was not reported for 37 children). The participants represented 50 day care centres
or preschools attached to primary schools, with varying practices and pedagogy, in
eight municipalities in southern Finland. There were units using Steiner and
Montessori pedagogies, 24-hour day care centres, private and municipal units, and
half-day preschools. In Finland, day care centre children are usually arranged into
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
36
J. Reunamo et al.
groups, although the group composition and number of children can vary: In one
unit, there were only nine children (including children with special needs), and in
another, all 54 children belonged to the same group. Stratified sampling was used to
select one group of children from each day care unit for the study. The exact number
of parents who did not permit their child to participate in the research is not known,
but the percentage of refusals was certainly under 10%. All children with parental
permission in the selected groups participated in the research.
The participation of half of the day care centres was based on the choice of the
staff, and for the other half, the choice was made by the municipality. This may have
influenced the sampling; for example, in the former, less pedagogically interested
day care centre staff may have chosen not to participate. However, in a comparison
between the two types of choices, no meaningful differences could be detected. A
couple of units cancelled their participation because of transfers or changes. The
units represented the general population structure.
Measures and procedure
Observations and child evaluations were performed independently from each other.
The observer and the child evaluators had no access to each other’s data. The observations were made during the children’s everyday activities. A great number of
observations were needed in order to trace the connections between children’s PA
and their strategies and skills.
Observation
According to Kohl, Fulton, and Caspersen (2000), several techniques have been
used to assess PA among children, including self-report, direct observation, mechanical or electronic monitoring and direct or indirect calorimetry. Observation is an
applicable method in the actual context and environment of the day care.
Seventy kindergarten teachers were trained to observe children. Because the
observation had several categories and day care activities are difficult to define the
training lasted the whole autumn semester of 2009. The training included four sessions with a one-month period for practising between the sessions. In the training,
the teachers observed videos of everyday situations, and the observation categories
were discussed one by one. There were 18,366 observations made between January
and May 2010. The observers did not collect data in their own day care centres or
groups (with one exception). The observations give a random sample of the children’s actions from 8:00 am to 12:00 noon in day care and preschool. The children
were observed at four-minute interval according to systematic sampling methods.
The actual observation lasted only one minute, and the rest of the time was used for
coding and for preparing the next observation. Approximately, 60 observations were
made in one day. Because the group sizes varied and children were sometimes
absent, the number of observations per child also varied (M = 22, SD = 13).
The following observed items were general actions occurring between 8:00 am
and12:00 noon at the day care centre: the children’s actions; the children’s objects
of attention; the children’s nearest peer contacts; the children’s PA; and the nearest
educator’s actions. The children’s PA level was categorized into one of the three
following categories:
Early Years
37
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
(1) Low (sitting, using pen, eating etc.)
(2) Intermediate (walking, whole body movements)
(3) High (includes at least some running, romping, physical exertion etc.)
The reliability for the observed PA was evaluated during the last training session using seven videos: the observers were unanimous on the categories of 88%
of the observations using the above three-category scale. The evaluation was difficult when the PA of the observed child varied during observation. For example,
in one video where children were listening to the teacher’s instructions, the
observed child was mostly sitting still but also made some whole body movements and one more intensive movement. Seventy-two per cent of the observers
categorized the activity as intermediate, 23% as low and 5% as high. The
children’s creative action was measured with the children’s involvement scale
(Laevers 1994). The observation instrument was originally prepared for
Reunamo’s (2007) preliminary research. This instrument can be retrieved from
http://www.helsinki.fi/~reunamo/apu/observation_instrument.pdf.
Interview on children’s orientations
Reunamo’s interview tool (cf. Reunamo 2007) was utilized to study the children’s
orientations in day care situations. The interviews were conducted by children’s own
teachers who had been trained to use the interview tool. The interview consisted of
sixteen situations. The children were asked to describe how they saw themselves
acting in different situations, for example, Another child has the toy you want, what
do you do? or You fail, what do you do then? A picture was used in each question
to help the children to orientate to the situation at hand. Ten situations included
other children, five included an adult and one situation a personal failure with no
other participants. Most of the 1–2-year-old children were omitted from the interview, because the interview was too difficult for them. The interviewers wrote down
the children’s answers. All the interviews were conducted in separate quiet rooms.
After the interview, the answers were categorized into five groups of orientations:
accommodative, participative, dominant, withdrawn and uncertain. If a child’s
answer could not be categorized in any of the four first-mentioned categories, it was
categorized as uncertain, which most often meant that the child did not know what
to do in the situation. The classification was performed by two researchers, who categorized all the children’s views question by question (not child by child) to ensure
a unified classification. The classifiers did not use any information about the children’s identity, neither did they use any information about the children’s qualities
(e.g. age or gender), background, group or the learning environment of the day care
they attended. The classification was made solely on the basis of the children’s
descriptions. The reliability of the categories was not measured for the two raters,
rather, the answers that were difficult to categorize were discussed and a shared
understanding of the category was sought. The percentage of answers in each category was calculated. When the data were analysed, it was found that children often
had different orientations for children than for adults. Different summary variables
were added for situations with children and situations with adults. The interview
instrument can be retrieved from http://www.helsinki.fi/~reunamo/apu/interview_
instrument_with_pictures.pdf.
38
J. Reunamo et al.
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
The child evaluations
The teachers of the groups evaluated the children’s skills, and personal qualities
were rated on a five-point scale, including their fine motor skills, ability to adapt to
new situations, confidence in the day care centre, metacognitive skills, ability to
cope with their feelings, independence, sensitivity in interaction, language communication skills, social skills, coping strategies, concentration skills and use of willpower. The children’s gross-motor skills were evaluated with the statement Needs a
lot of support in gross motor development. The child evaluation instrument can be
retrieved from http://www.helsinki.fi/~reunamo/apu/child_eval.pdf.
Statistical analysis
The observed data were merged with the interview data and the child evaluations. In
the comparison of observation counts, Chi-square tests were used. In the comparison
of group differences, Mann–Whitney tests were used instead of t-tests to eliminate
possible violations in normal distributions. All the reported group differences in the
article are highly statistically significant (p < 0.0005) unless stated otherwise.
Ethical considerations
The study was part of a research and development project designed to empower both
educators and children. Educators were considered research colleagues in the dissemination of the research results. Educators were given feedback on tools for developing their work based on the research findings. Teams of educators received tools
for pedagogical evaluation, and each team also received feedback on their group’s
activities and interaction based on the preliminary results; a large-scale dissemination project is already under way. To ensure that the children expressed their own
point of view their own teachers interviewed them. Valuing the children’s perspective was emphasized in the interview training. An assistant, e.g. a translator for
immigrant children, was provided where necessary. Permission to participate in the
research was gathered from all the families of the children. It is not possible to identify individual children in the data or results. The English webpages of the project
are at http://blogs.helsinki.fi/orientate/. The development project is cooperating with
the Finnish national organization for early childhood PA Nuori Suomi, see http://
www.nuorisuomi.fi/in_english.
Results
Altogether 10.0% (n = 1829) of children’s activities were observed to be physically
highly active, including at least some running, romping and physical exertion. Intermediate PA (for example, walking and whole body movements) was observed to be
33.6% (n = 6166) of their activities, and low PA accounted for 56.4% (n = 10,340)
of the observations. The observed duration of high PA before noon (8:00–12:00) in
day care and preschool was 24 min (10% of the time observed).
When focusing solely on high PA, boys were highly physically active in 11.5%
of activities and girls in 8.3% of activities. Children’s PA increased with age: 1–3year-old children were observed to be highly physically active 7.4% of the time and
the percentage increased for 6–7-year-old children to 11.2%. Because boys and older
children were more physically active, their impact is studied in the following
Early Years
39
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
analysis to check their role as intermediate variables. However, all the intermediate
variables may not occur in the analysis, because all the differences in physical activities cannot be checked which means that care is required when interpreting age and
gender.
PA in the learning environment
The learning environment is where children come into contact with different
physical activities. The distribution of children’s PA in daily activities can be seen in
Figure 2.
More than half (57.1%) of high PA occurred during free play outdoors. Free play
outdoors often refers to activities taking place in the playground or play park. Generally, children choose their activities themselves from several options. The second
largest amount of high PA was observed during free play indoors (14.2%), although
high PA during indoor play accounted for only 6.3% of all indoor play. In total,
71.9% of high PA occurred during free play activities, although only 40.4% of the
time children spent in free play was high PA.
Direct education (teaching) included all kinds of educator-directed activities,
including physical education, and involved such things as movement activities, gymnastics, dance, athletics, games and tasks. Even with physical education included in
direct education, it accounted for just 13.5% of children’s total high PA, which is
6.8% of all teaching sessions. Teacher scaffolding was defined in the observation
instructions as teacher participation that enriches children’s own processes and activities. Outdoor activity with a teacher scaffold consisted of 9.7% of children’s entire
high PA, although children spent only 2.8% of their time in these activities. This
means that teacher-scaffolded outdoor activities have the greatest density of high
physical activities. Altogether, educator-directed activities (teaching, scaffolding,
basic care and eating situations) accounted for 59.5% of children’s total time
between 8:00 am and12:00 noon but only 28.7% of their high physical activities.
Boys engaged in more outdoor high PA activities during teacher scaffolding
(38.8%) than did girls (30.1%). The largest age difference was in outdoor free play,
All activity
High PA
60 %
50 %
40 %
30 %
20 %
10 %
0%
Outdoor Indoor
Direct Outdoor Basic care Scaffolded Eating
play
free play free play Education activity
indoors
inside
with
teacher
scaffold
Figure 2. The percentage of high PA in daily activities in comparison to all daily activity
between 8:00 am and 12:00 noon in day care and preschool.
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
40
J. Reunamo et al.
in which 6–7-year-old children were highly physically active 36.5% of the time and
1–3-year-old children only 22.3% of the time.
ZPD is not a synonym for children’s learning environment. ZPD is the distance
between children’s own activities, and the activities that children can engage in a
supportive environment. ZPD describes how deeply children are involved in their
activities within the environment. The relationship between children’s PA and
involvement can be seen in Figure 3.
In simple, stereotypic repetitive and passive activities with no energy or cognitive demand (Figure 2), children were very seldom (in 2.3% of cases) highly physically active. The more children’s involvement is sustained, intense, concentrated,
creative, persistent and includes mental engagement, the higher the percentage of
high PA becomes, reaching 19.7% in the most sustained activity. High PA and high
involvement seem to attract each other. However, which is the cause and which is
the effect? It may be that during intense physical exertion children also become
mentally involved in their activities. It is also possible that higher mental
involvement evokes PA.
Lowto medium PA
High PA
120%
100%
80 %
60 %
40 %
20 %
0%
Simple,
stereotypic,
repetitive,
passive, no
energy, no
cognitive
demand
Frequently
Mainly
Continuous
interrupted
continuous
activitywith
activityand
activity,easily
intense
engagement
distracted, moments,child
mental
not easily
engagement is distracted
lacking
Sustained
intense activity,
concentration,
creativity,
mental
engagement,
andpersistence
Figure 3. Children’s PA in different levels of involvement.
Table 1. Children’s mean involvement in low and medium PA and high PA in different
activities.
Day care activity
Outdoor activity with teacher scaffold
Outdoor free play
Direct education inside
Basic care
Indoor free play
Scaffolded play indoors
Eating
*
Mean involvement during
low and medium PA
Mean involvement
during high PA
2.958
3.051
3.272
2.46
3.245
3.414
2.433
3.695
3.678
3.789
2.8
3.494
3.545
2.385*
The difference is not statistically significant at the .0005 level.
Early Years
41
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
A closer look at mental involvement in different activities (Table 1) shows that
the interplay between PA and involvement is the strongest in outdoor activity. When
children attended outdoor activities with a teacher scaffold, children’s mean involvement in PA was higher in comparison with lesser PA. Outdoor activity with a teacher
scaffold seems to be a promising environment for intense learning. Furthermore, outdoor free play in general has a lot of involved high PA. It is worth noting that in
those few cases (n = 247) where children were highly physically active during direct
education, their involvement was the highest. This means that direct education can
be both physically and mentally intense at the same time, and they seem to reinforce
each other. Unfortunately, these activities are very rare, accounting for only 3 min a
day on average.
Gross motor skills and personal qualities
The educators were asked to evaluate children’s gross motor development in the following item: Needs a lot of support in gross motor development. About 47.8% of
the children were evaluated as needing at least some support in gross motor development. The evaluated need for support in gross motor development decreased steeply with age: 76.3% of 1–3-year-old children were evaluated as needing at least
some support in gross motor development, whereas only 35.3% of 6–7-year-old
children were considered to need support in gross motor development. Boys were
seen to be more often in need of at least some support in gross motor development
(57%) than were girls (38.4%).
Children in need of at least some support in gross motor development were
observed to be involved in high PA 9.5% of the time, whereas other children were
highly physically active 10.4% of the time, χ2 (1, N = 17,969) = 3.593 p = 0.058.
The children who needed the most support in gross motor development (5 on a scale
of 1 to 5) were observed to be highly physically active in only 5.3% of cases. Thus,
children who need gross motor exercise the most get it the least. This tendency was
observed in all age-groups, although the differences were not statistically significant,
due to the small number of observations in high PA for children needing support
(n = 33).
Children’s gross motor difficulties are tightly interwoven with their other skills,
as can be seen in Table 2.
The differences in Table 2 are the largest in children’s skills. Children with gross
motor difficulties also have difficulties in adapting, both socially and to new contexts in general. These children also seem to have difficulties in their sensitivity.
Gross motor difficulties seem to be very general symptoms connected to all aspects
of a child’s personal life.
Physical orientation
Children’s PA was associated with their level of attention in their learning environment as can be seen in Figure 4.
Altogether, 62.6% of children’s high PA occurred when they attended to either a
group of children (36.2% of all high PA) or to another child (26.4% of all high PA),
which means that child-related activities were high in PA. A total of 33.4% of
children’s high PA occurred when they attended to other things: 18.1% to the whole
situation and 15.3% to non-social objects. Only 4% of high PA occurred when
42
J. Reunamo et al.
Table 2. Differences in the mean values of evaluated personal qualities (1–5) in groups of
children with no and at least some gross motor difficulties.
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
Evaluated skill
Needs a lot of support in fine motor skills
Needs a lot of support in learning and metacognitive
skills
Needs support in language communication skills
Withdraws easily, contacts with others are often weak
Recognizes the feelings of others and interacts
sensitively
Recognizes own feelings and copes with them
Has willpower and uses it with other children
Is trusting and confident in the day care center
Has good social skills in a group of children
Adapts easily to new situations and other children
Is creative in pretend play
Can concentrate easily
Participates in and eagerly initiates activities
Copes appropriately in new, challenging situations
Is independent and self-directive
Attention during low or medium PA
No gross
motor
difficulties M
At least some
gross
motor difficulties
M
1.47
1.77
2.8
2.9
1.52
1.68
3.57
2.62
2.39
3.27
3.48
3.31
4.02
3.89
3.75
4.3
4.05
3.92
3.67
4.05
3.05
2.83
3.48
3.33
3.17
3.7
3.42
3.26
2.97
3.3
Attention during high PA
40 %
35 %
30 %
25 %
20 %
15 %
10 %
5%
0%
A group of
children
A child
The whole
situation
Non social
object
Adult
Figure 4. The percentages of children’s different objects of attention during low/medium
PA and high PA activities.
children attended to an adult, even though these cases accounted for 13.2% of all
observations. The result is clear: Other children are connected with high PA, adults
with low PA.
The highest gender differences in PA were in attending to a group of children.
Boys were highly physically active in 26.6% of cases where they attended to a
group of children, but for girls attending to a group of children only accounted for
13.8% of high PA.
Early Years
43
3
2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
01m
1.1 2 m
2.1 3 m 3.1 5 meters 5.110 m
Over 10 m
Figure 5. Children’ mean PA (1–3) in different distances to the nearest educator during
observation.
Age differences were also observed for children’s PA in relation to other children. 1–3-year-old children were highly physically active while attending to another
child 31.1% of the time, while 6–7-year-old children’s PA was high 22.2% of the
time, χ2 (1, N = 964) = 8.320, p < 0.004. Attending to one child seemed to be more
physically stimulating for young children. On the other hand, when 6–7-year-old
children attended to a group of children, their PA was high in 45.5% of cases,
whereas with 1–3-year-olds, it was high in 25.6% of cases. This result highlights the
developmental aspects of children’s peer contacts. The role of teachers is highlighted
in Figure 5.
In the vicinity of the educator (0–1 m), children are rarely (only 3.0% of the
time) highly physically active. However, when the distance to the nearest educator
is greater than 10 meters, the children’s PA is high 29.5% of the time. This tendency
is similar in all age groups. It is also similar for boys and girls, although for girls
greater distance from the educator does not result in as high PA as for boys. We
need more in depth study of the teachers’ activities to draw conclusions on the
meaning of these results.
Shared creation of PA
Children who were observed to be highly physically active had a lower mean for
evaluated withdrawnness (M = 1.89, SD = 0.98) than children with lesser PA
(M = 2.03, SD = 1.09). This difference was observed for all three age groups,
although the differences were not always statistically significant. The phenomenon
was also similar for boys and girls.
There was also a significant connection between the independent measures of
interview and observation. Children who were observed to be high in PA displayed
fewer uncertain strategies with other children (12.3% of total strategies) than children with less high PA (14.2% of total strategies). This means that children’s uncertainty in their strategies with other children was reflected in lower PA. It is probable
that when other children are the main instigators of high PA, withdrawn and uncertain children are excluded from these activities.
44
J. Reunamo et al.
All free play outdoors
High PA during free play outdoors
90 %
80 %
70 %
60 %
50 %
40 %
30 %
20 %
10 %
0%
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
Hanging about Role play or Orientation
Playor
exploring with together with imaginary play
toys and
others
physical
setting
Rule play
Figure 6. Children’s main activities and the proportion of high PA in each activity during
free play outdoors.
In free play outdoors, children have many opportunities to create their PA environment together with their peers, and they are usually free to choose their activity.
Thus, it is important which activities children choose (cf. Figure 6).
Playing with toys or physically exploring the environment (material play)
accounted for 43.1% of children’s time in free play outdoors, and 28.4% of play
was highly physically active. Hanging about together accounted for 21.6% of children’s time, and it accounted for about a third of high PA. Roleplay was engaged in
14.7% of the time, and 38% of role play involved high PA. However, the highest
density of high PA was in rule play: It accounted for a mere 4.6% of children’s time,
but children were highly physically active for 76.7% of the time that they engaged
in this activity. On the other hand, children were engaged in orientation (seeking for
focus) for 9.9% of the time outdoors, but only 6.7% of the time spent in orientation
was physically highly active. In free play indoors, children’s PA was very different,
as can be seen in Figure 7.
All free play indoors
High PA during free play indoors
40 %
35 %
30 %
25 %
20 %
15 %
10 %
5%
0%
Play or
Role play or Hanging about
exploring with imaginary play together with
toys and
others
physical setting
Rule play
Action not
allowed
Figure 7. Children’s main activities and the proportion of high PA in each activity during
free play indoors.
Early Years
45
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
The main activity in free play indoors was play or exploring with toys and the
physical setting (material play), which accounted for 36.4% of their time. However,
only 5.2% of this activity involved high PA. Children spent 24.5% of their playtime
indoors in role play and imaginary play, but they accounted for just 8.3% of high PA.
Hanging about with others accounted for 7.8% of children’s time, and 9.9% of that
activity involved high PA. Indoor rule play (6.7% of the time) is very different from
rule play outdoors, as it consists of just 6.9% high PA. ‘Action not allowed’ consisted
of just 1.7% of children’s activities during free play indoors, but 29.4% of it involved
high PA. This means that action not allowed is often associated with high PA.
Discussion
The results of this study are based on systematic and comprehensive sampling. The
interview and child evaluation were independent measures from observation.
The observers did not see the evaluations and interviews, and they did not discuss
the observed children with the evaluators. A wide variety of day care centres and
preschools were included in the study. However, a wider perspective of cultural
aspects is not discussed here.
There are several findings from this study that should be considered in the development of PA for young children. The first finding is that there is too little PA. Only
24 min of high PA in the most active part of day between 8:00 am12:00 noon does
not meet the recommendations for PA (Ministry of Social Affairs and Health 2005).
The low level of PA observed here has been found elsewhere (cf. Pate et al. 2008).
Another finding that cannot be ignored is that 71.9% of children’s high PA occurred
during free play. Direct physical education and scaffolded activities accounted for
just 2.5% of children’s high PA. It is worrisome that the tradition of physical education focuses on this 2.5% exercise sessions. Thus, the vast majority of children’s
everyday PA may be neglected.
The results highlight the importance of children’s own production of PA. At the
same time, it is essential to pay attention to the different needs of children, in order
to promote motor competence. Let us recall that motor competence is a highly
important factor for choices concerning a physically active lifestyle. Withdrawn and
uncertain children need encouragement and experiences of success. The high level
of PA during forbidden activities needs more attention. The high level of involvement that children display in high PA indicates that high PA can be a valuable tool
for learning.
Children’s gross motor skills seem to be highly intertwined with their personality
and other skills. Children who need physical support the most may receive it the
least. PA is often the best remedy for cognitive, social and emotional well-being.
Teachers need to rethink their role as educators. They should not consider
themselves the only providers of PA, even though they play a significant role in
evaluating and enhancing motor development. The dynamics of children’s PA is
central. Teachers need to empower children to take responsibility for their own wellbeing and help them build a sustainable and healthy culture that is shared by everybody.
One finding was that children are more physically active the further away they
are from their teachers. However, this does not mean that educators should leave
children on their own. Children who happily and actively play football or tag can
keep on playing. However, timid, clumsy, uncertain children with weak peer
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
46
J. Reunamo et al.
contacts need teachers to help them become involved in the shared production of
PA. Younger children’s interest in PA is best ignited by one friend; the interest of
older children, especially boys, is more easily ignited by a group of children.
The important changes needed to create more comprehensive PA are possible.
Children need more free play outdoors. Outdoor play scaffolded by a teacher is a
great way to enhance PA. Moreover, special attention should be given to girls’ PA
when scaffolding play. Children spend a lot of time in material play. By giving children real, heavy tools to work with, the amount of PA can be increased. Rule play
outdoors was the best physical activator. Readily available equipment and a teacher
who helps to get play started are methods to increase the amount of PA.
When examining the results in the light of the feedback loop of culturally mediated PA, the following picture emerges: free play outdoors and, to a lesser degree,
free play indoors are potential environments for PA. Culturally mediated gross motor
skills are combined in many ways in children’s personal, emotional and social skills.
Children’s gross motor skills impact on every aspect of their well-being. Children’s
physical orientation becomes concrete with one or more peers, especially in their
choices of friends away from the teachers. In the shared creation of PA, children create their own physical environment with their peers in material play, role-play and
hanging about with others. Withdrawn and uncertain children are easily left out of
shared activities. Eventually, at the end of the feedback loop, the zone of proximal
PA looks different. In this new environment, teachers not only adapt teaching to the
children; rather, they help children to control and enhance the flow of their own PA.
According to McWilliams et al (2009), policy makers and childcare providers
would benefit from more specific PA recommendations and standards. Establishing
comprehensive guidelines for PA in childcare could result in higher activity levels
and healthier children. However, these recommendations should not obscure the
potential of children’s own role in PA creation presented in this article.
References
Bower, Julie K., Derek. P. Hales, Deborah. F. Tate, Daniela. A. Rubin, Sara. E. Benjamin,
and Dianne. S. Ward. 2008. “The Childcare Environment and Children’s Physical
Activity.” American Journal of Preventive Medicine 34 (1): 23–30.
Brown, William, Karin. A. Pfeiffer, Kerry. L. McIver, Marsha Dowda, L. Addy, and Russell.
R. Pate. 2009. “Social and Environmental Factors Associated with Preschoolers Nonsedentary Physical Activity.” Child Development 80 (1): 45–58.
Burdette, Hillary L., Robert C. Whitaker, and Stephen R. Daniels. 2004. “Parental Report of
Outdoor Playtime as a Measure of Physical Activity in Preschool-aged Children.”
Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 158 (4): 353–357.
Butcher, J., and W. Eaton. 1989. “Gross and Fine Motor Proficiency in Preschoolers: Relationships with Free Play Behaviour and Activity Level.” Journal of Human Movement
Studies 16: 27–36.
Cardon, Greet, Eveline Van Cauwenberghe, and Ilse De Bourdeaudhuij. 2011. “What Do We
Know About Physical Activity in Infants and Toddlers: A Review of the Literature and
Future Research Direction.” Science & Sport 26 (3): 127–130.
Cardon, Greet, Eveline Van Cauwenberghe, Valery Labarque, Leen Haerens, and Ilse De
Bourdeaudhuij. 2008. “The Contribution of Preschool Playground Factors in Explaining
Children’s Physical Activity During Recess.” International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 11 (5): 1–8.
Dollman, J., K. Norton, and L. Norton. 2005. “Evidence for Secular Trends in Children’s
Physical Activity Behavior.” British Journal of Sports Medicine 39 (12): 892–897.
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
Early Years
47
Dowda, M., W. H. Brown, K. L. McIver, K. A. Pfeiffer, J. R. O’Neill, and C. L. Addy.
2009. “Policies and Characteristics of the Preschool Environment and Physical Activity
of Young Children.” Pediatrics 123 (2): 261–266.
Dowda, Marsha, Russell R. Pate, Stewart G. Trost, M. Joao, C. A. Almeida, and John.
R. Sirard. 2004. “Influences of Preschool Policies and Practices on Children’s Physical
Activity.” Journal of Community Health 29 (3): 183–196.
Dwyer, John, Lisa Needham, Janis Randall Simpson, and Elizabeth Shaver Heeney. 2008.
“Parents Report Intrapersonal, Interpersonal, and Environmental Barriers to Supporting
Healthy Eating and Physical Activity among Their Preschoolers.” Applied Physiology,
Nutrition & Metabolism 33 (2): 338–346.
Gallahue, David L., John C. Ozmun, and Jacqueline D. Goodway. 2012. Understanding
Motor Development: Infants, Children, Adolescents, Adults. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Gubbels, J. S., S. P. Kremers, D. H. van Kann, A. Stafleu, M. J. Candel, P. C. Dagnelie,
C. Thijs, and N. K. de Vries. 2011. “Interaction between Physical Environment, Social
Environment, and Child Characteristics in Determining Physical Activity at Childcare.”
Health Psychology 30 (1): 84–90.
Hands, Beth. P., and Dawne Larkin. 2002. “Physical Fitness and Developmental Coordination Disorder.” In Developmental Co-Ordination Disorder, edited by C. A. Cermak and
D. Larkin, 172–184. Albany: Thomson Learning.
Hay, D. F., A. Payne, and A. Chadwic. 2004. “Peer Relations in Childhood.” Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45 (1): 84–108.
Hodges, E. A., C. Smith, S. Tidwell, and D. Berry. 2012. “Promoting Physical Activity in
Preschoolers to Prevent Obesity: A Review of the Literature.” Journal of Pediatric Nursing 28 (1): 3–19.
Jago, R., K. Macdonald-Wallis, J. L. Thompson, A. S. Page, R. Brockman, and K. R. Fox.
2011. “Better with a Buddy: Influence of Best Friends on Children’s Physical Activity.”
Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise 43 (2): 259–265.
Kohl, H. W., J. E. Fulton, and C. J. Caspersen. 2000. “Assessment of Physical Activity
among Children and Adolescents: A Review and Synthesis.” Preventive Medicine 31 (2):
54–76.
Kyhälä, A., J. Reunamo, and H. Ruismäki. 2012. “Physical Activity and Learning Environment Qualities in Finnish Day Care.” Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences 45:
247–256.
Laevers, F. 1994. “The Innovative Project Experimental Education and the Definition of
Quality in Education.” In Defining and assessing quality in early childhood education,
edited by F. Laevers, 159–172. Leuven: Leuven University Press.
Lehto, Reetta, Clary Corander, Carola Ray, and Eva Roos. 2009. “Perheen sosioekonomisen
aseman ja perherakenteen yhteydet alakouluikäisten lasten terveellisiin elintapoihin
[Family’s Socio-economic and Family Structure in Relation to Elementary School
Students’ Healthy Lifestyle, in Finnish].” Sosiaalilääketieteellinen aikakausilehti 46 (4):
258–271.
Lehto, S., J. Reunamo, and H. Ruismäki. 2012. “Children’s Peer Relations and Children’s
Physical Activity.” Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences 45: 277–283.
Livesey, D., M. Mow, T. Toschack, and Y. Zheng. 2010. “The Relationship between Motor
Performance and Peer Relations in 9- to 12-year-old Children.” Child: Care, Health &
Development 37 (4): 581–588.
Malina, Robert M., Claude Bouchard, and Oded Bar-Or. 2004. Growth, Maturation, and
Physical Activity. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
McWilliams, C., S. C. Ball, S. E. Benjamin, D. Hales, A. Vaughn, and D. S. Ward. 2009.
“Best-practice Guidelines for Physical Activity at Child Care.” Pediatrics 124 (6):
1650–1659.
Ministry of Social Affairs and Health. 2005. Recommendations for Physical Activity in Early
Childhood Education. Helsinki: Handbooks of the Ministry of Social Affairs and Health.
2005: 17.
Ministry of Social Affairs and Health. 2006. Lasten päivähoidon tilannekatsaus [Report on
the Situation of the Children’s Day Care, in Finnish]. Reports of the Ministry of Social
Affairs and Health. 2006: 16.
Downloaded by [University of Helsinki] at 23:47 06 April 2014
48
J. Reunamo et al.
Pate, R. R., K. Mc Iver, M. Dowda, W. H. Brown, and C. Addy. 2008. “Directly Observed
Physical Activity Levels in Preschool Children.” Journal of School Health 78 (8):
438–444.
Reilly, J. J., D. M. Jackson, C. Montgomery, L. A. Kelly, C. Slater, S. Grant, and J. Y. Paton.
2004. “Total Energy Expenditure and Physical Activity in Young Scottish Children:
Mixed Longitudinal Study.” Lancet 363 (9404): 211–212.
Reunamo, J. 2007. “Adaptation and Agency in Early Childhood Education.” European Early
Childhood Education Research Journal 15 (3): 365–377.
Reunamo, J. 2009. “Vygotsky: A Systemic Approach.” In Educational Environment in Early
Childhood: Education in Estonia and Finland IV, edited by J. Hytönen, 29–48. Helsinki:
University of Helsinki, Department of Applied Sciences of Education. Research reports;
311.
Reunamo, J., L. Saros, and H. Ruismäki. 2012. “The Amount of Physical Activity in Finnish
Day Care.” Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences 45: 501–506.
Sääkslahti, A. 2005. “Effects of Physical Activity Intervention on Physical Activity and Motor
Skills and Relationships between Physical Activity and Coronary Heart Disease Risk
Factors in 3-7-Year-Old Children.” PhD diss., University of Jyväskylä, Finland https://
jyx.jyu.fi/dspace/bitstream/handle/123456789/13496/9513921050.pdf?sequence=1.
Vannatta, Kathryn, Maria A. Garstein, Meg Zeller, and Robert B. Noll. 2009. “Peer Acceptance and Social Behavior during Childhood and Adolescence: How Important are
Appearance, Athleticism and Academic Competence?” International Journal of Behavioral Development 33 (4): 303–311.
Vygotsky, L. S. 1978. Mind in Society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. 2004. “Imagination and Creativity in Childhood.” Journal of Russian and
East European Psychology 42 (1): 7–97.
Wedderkopp, N., K. Froberg, H. S. Hansen, and L. B. Andersen. 2004. “Secular Trends in
Physical Fitness and Obesity in Danish 9-year-old Girls and Boys: Odense School Child
Study and Danish Substudy of the European Youth Heart Study.” Scandinavian Journal
of Medicine & Science in Sports 14 (3): 150–155.
Wilkin, T. J. 2011. “Pediatric Debate: Can We Modulate Physical Activity in Children? No.”
International Journal of Obesity 35 (10): 1270–1276.
Wrotniak, B., L. H. Epstein, J. M. Dorn, K. E. Jones, and V. A. Kondilis. 2006. “The Relationship between Motor Proficiency and Physical Activity in Children.” Pediatrics 118
(6): 1758–1765.
Yang, X., R. Telama, M. Leino, and J. Viikari. 1999. “Factors Explaining the Physical
Activity of Young Adults: the Importance of Early Socialization.” Scandinavian Journal
of Medicine & Science in Sports 9 (2): 120–127.