Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
2022
…
10 pages
1 file
How do people process numerosity? Do they rely on general magnitude processing (e.g. area, density, etc.) 1,2,3 ? Alternatively, do they depend on a designated module underlying numerosity judgements 4,5 ? In a 2016 paper, Cicchini and colleagues 6 show results that strongly support the latter. They demonstrated that humans automatically perceive and spontaneously use numerosity rather than other physical magnitudes (i.e. area of convex hull or density) when asked to make comparison judgments. Here we present an alternative account for their findings. We suggest that saliency of the different attributes of
2019
Numerosity perception is foundational to mathematical learning, but its computational bases are strongly debated. Some investigators argue that humans are endowed with a specialized system supporting numerical representation; others argue that visual numerosity is estimated using continuous magnitudes, such as density or area, which usually co-vary with number. Here we reconcile these contrasting perspectives by testing deep networks on the same numerosity comparison task that was administered to humans, using a stimulus space that allows to measure the contribution of non-numerical features. Our model accurately simulated the psychophysics of numerosity perception and the associated developmental changes: discrimination was driven by numerosity information, but non-numerical features had a significant impact, especially early during development. Representational similarity analysis further highlighted that both numerosity and continuous magnitudes were spontaneously encoded even wh...
Scientific Reports
Numerosity perception is thought to be foundational to mathematical learning, but its computational bases are strongly debated. Some investigators argue that humans are endowed with a specialized system supporting numerical representations; others argue that visual numerosity is estimated using continuous magnitudes, such as density or area, which usually co-vary with number. Here we reconcile these contrasting perspectives by testing deep neural networks on the same numerosity comparison task that was administered to human participants, using a stimulus space that allows the precise measurement of the contribution of non-numerical features. Our model accurately simulates the psychophysics of numerosity perception and the associated developmental changes: discrimination is driven by numerosity, but non-numerical features also have a significant impact, especially early during development. Representational similarity analysis further highlights that both numerosity and continuous mag...
Human observers can rapidly judge the number of items in a scene. This ability is underpinned by specific mechanisms encoding number or density. We investigated whether judgments of number and density are biased by a change in volume, as they are by a change in area. Stimuli were constructed using nonoverlapping black and white luminance-defined dots. An eight-mirror Wheatstone stereoscope was used to present the dots as though in a volume. Using a temporal two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) task and the Method of Constant Stimuli (MOCS), we measured the precision and bias (PSE shift) of numerosity and density judgments, separately, for stimuli differing in area or volume. For two-dimensional (2-D) stimuli, consistent with previous literature, perceived density was biased as area increased. However, perceived number was not. For three-dimensional (3-D) stimuli, despite a vivid impression of the dots filling a cylindrical volume, there was no bias in perceived density or number as volume increased. A control experiment showed that all of our observers could easily perceive disparity in our stimuli. Our findings reveal that number and density judgments that are biased by area are not similarly biased by volume changes.
The existence of perceptually distinct numerosity ranges has been proposed for small (i.e., subitizing range) and larger numbers based on differences in precision, Weber fractions, and reaction times. This raises the question of whether such dissociations reflect distinct mechanisms operating across the two numerosity ranges. In the present work, we explore the predictions of a single-layer recurrent on-center, off-surround network model of attentional priority that has been applied to object individuation and enumeration. Activity from the network can be used to model various phenomena in the domain of visual number perception based on a single parameter: the strength of inhibition between nodes. Specifically, higher inhibition allows for precise representation of small numerosities, while low inhibition is preferred for high numerosities. The model makes novel predictions, including that enumeration of small numerosities following large numerosities should result in longer reaction times than when a small numerosity trial following small numerosities. Moreover, the model predicts underestimation of number when a display containing a large number of items follows a trial with small numerosities. We behaviorally confirmed these predictions in a series of experiments. This pattern of results is consistent with a single, flexible object individuation system, which can be modeled successfully by dynamic on-center, off-surround network model of the attentional priority (saliency) map.
Psychological Science, 2014
Despite the existence of much evidence for a number sense in humans, several researchers have questioned whether number is sensed directly or derived indirectly from texture density. Here, we provide clear evidence that numerosity and density judgments are subserved by distinct mechanisms with different psychophysical characteristics. We measured sensitivity for numerosity discrimination over a wide range of numerosities: For low densities (less than 0.25 dots/ deg 2 ), thresholds increased directly with numerosity, following Weber's law; for higher densities, thresholds increased with the square root of texture density, a steady decrease in the Weber fraction. The existence of two different psychophysical systems is inconsistent with a model in which number is derived indirectly from noisy estimates of density and area; rather, it points to the existence of separate mechanisms for estimating density and number. These results provide strong confirmation for the existence of neural mechanisms that sense number directly, rather than indirectly from texture density.
Psychological Science, 2013
When are judgments sensitive to the magnitude of numerical information presented in unfamiliar units, such as a price in a foreign currency or an unfamiliar product attribute? Prior work has demonstrated both magnitude sensitivity and insensitivity. We propose that people exhibit "deliberational blindness", failing to consider the meaning of even unfamiliar units. When an unfamiliar unit is not salient, people fail to take their lack of knowledge into account, and their judgments reflect magnitude sensitivity. However, subtly manipulating the visual salience of the unit (e.g., enlarging font size of the unit relative to the number) prompts recognition of the unit's (un)familiarity and reduces magnitude sensitivity. In five experiments, we demonstrate this unit salience effect, provide evidence for the deliberational blindness account and rule out alternative explanations such as non-perception and fluency. Lastly, we propose an integrative model of numerical judgment.
Experimental Brain Research
It is debated whether the representation of numbers is endowed with a directional-spatial component so that perceiving small-magnitude numbers triggers leftward shifts of attention and perceiving large-magnitude numbers rightward shifts. Contrary to initial findings, recent investigations have demonstrated that centrally presented small-magnitude and large-magnitude Arabic numbers do not cause leftward and rightward shifts of attention, respectively. Here we verified whether perceiving small or large non-symbolic numerosities (i.e., clouds of dots) drives attention to the left or the right side of space, respectively. In experiment 1, participants were presented with central small (1, 2) vs large-numerosity (8, 9) clouds of dots followed by an imperative target in the left or right side of space. In experiment 2, a central cloud of dots (i.e., five dots) was followed by the simultaneous presentation of two identical dot-clouds, one on the left and one on the right side of space. Lat...
Revista de Antropologia, 2017
arXiv:2306.13664v1 [physics.pop-ph] 11 Jun 2023, 2023
ADALÉKOK BORBÉLY MACZKY EMIL ÉLETPÁLYÁJÁHOZ, 2024
Historia Mexicana El Colegio de México, 2003
Postcolonial Directions in Education , 2015
International Conference on Transformation in Communication (ICOTIC), 2016
Ahmet Köroğlu, 2012
Christian-Muslim Relations. A Bibliographical History. Volume 10 Ottoman and Safavid Empires (1600-1700), 2017
SBV Journal of Basic, Clinical and Applied Health Science, 2020
IOP Conference Series: Earth and Environmental Science
Descubrir El Arte Issn 1578 9047 2010 Vol N 142, 2010
Estudios de la Gestión revista internacional de administración, 2023