© 2011 AIPR, Inc.
ISSN: 1445-2308
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
Volume 11, Number 1, pp. 61-71
Thinking Styles of Psychic Claimants
BY ALEJANDRO PARRA
Abstract. A number of papers have investigated the idea of rational
versus intuitive thinking and how this might relate to paranormal
beliefs. Those who possess both intuitive and rational thinking styles are
more likely to report paranormal experiences and subjective
paranormal ability than those who express either intuitive or rational
thinking. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the
differences between psychic claimants (N = 49) and non-psychic
claimants (N = 45) on such personality factors as Global Constructive
Thinking, Emotional Coping, Behavioural Coping, and Esoteric
Thinking. The sample consisted of 94 participants, all of whom believed
in psi. Participants completed the Constructive Thinking Inventory and
the Anomalous Experiences Inventory. The psychic claimants group had
significantly higher scores on 12 out of 23 factors/facets which could not
all be explained by chance. Compared to non-psychic claimants, the
psychic claimants tend to have more positive attitudes; their thinking is
action-oriented; they are good behavioural copers; they think in ways
that promote effective action; and they are more accepting of others. At
the same time, they are more rigid in their thinking than non-psychic
claimants.
Keywords: Anomalous Experiences Inventory, behavioural coping,
categorical thinking, Constructive Thinking Inventory, emotional coping,
paranormal belief
INTRODUCTION
Thinking styles have been the subject of hundreds of research projects
studying the links between personality type and different aspects of life.
According to Sternberg (1997), a thinking style is not an aptitude, but rather
the way one chooses to use one’s aptitudes. Thinking style refers to what
people prefer to do, and how they like to do it. Observing that IQ tests tend
to be poor predictors of people’s capacity to solve everyday practical
problems or successfully negotiate major life events, Epstein and Meier
(1989) began to explore a nonintellectual form of intelligence unrelated to
61
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
IQ (Epstein & Meier, 1989). They identified a nonintellectual cognitive
ability which they termed constructive thinking (CT). CT is defined as a
person’s “ability to think in a manner that solves everyday life problems at a
minimal cost in stress” (Katz & Epstein, 1991, p. 789). More specifically,
CT is a form of experiential intelligence—that is, common sense information
that is acquired through experience and reflects the ability to deal with
problems effectively through the use of different thinking styles and
behavioural and emotional coping strategies (Epstein & Meier, 1989).
Epstein and his colleagues view constructive thinking as an aspect of
practical intelligence and general coping ability (Epstein, 1992; Epstein &
Meier, 1989) which, while distinct from measures of neuroticism (Katz &
Epstein, 1991), is predictive of how productive persons become while
managing stress in their lives (Epstein & Katz, 1992). CT has accurately
predicted success in a variety of life domains (e.g., school, work, and
interpersonal relationships) and could help to explain the relationship
between personality and subjective well-being (Epstein & Meier, 1989).
A number of papers have investigated the idea of rational versus
intuitive thinking and how this might relate to paranormal beliefs (e.g., Irwin
& Young, 2001). In support of a relationship between intuitive thinking and
paranormal beliefs, Aarnio and Lindeman (2005) found that higher intuition
and lower analytical thinking contributed to higher belief, more so in women
than in men. They also found that superstitious individuals accepted more
violations of core ontological distinctions than skeptics did, and that
ontological confusions discriminated believers from skeptics better than
intuitive thinking, analytical thinking, or emotional instability (Lindeman &
Aarnio, 2007). The paranormal beliefs of teachers were found to be
correlated with cognitive perceptual and disorganized schizotypal thinking
and intuitive thinking styles. The overall pattern of the correlations suggests
that intuitive thinking style and schizotypal thinking contribute
independently to paranormal belief, confirming the findings of Wolfradt,
Oubaid, Straube, Bischoff, and Mischo (1999) that a statistically significant
difference in paranormal belief exists between groups characterized by
distinct thinking styles, although the size of the effect was small.
Wolfradt et al. (1999) also found that those who possessed both
intuitive and rational thinking styles were more likely to report paranormal
beliefs, paranormal experiences, and subjective paranormal ability than were
those who expressed either intuitive thinking only, or rational thinking only.
Correlational analyses also showed that anomalous experiences were closely
related to schizotypal traits and thinking styles. Participants with a
complementary thinking style tended to have higher scores on anomalous
experiences and cognitive-perceptual aspects of schizotypy and self-efficacy.
Intuitive thinkers scored highest on interpersonal aspects of schizotypy and
interpersonal intolerance of ambiguity (Wolfradt et al., 1999).
62
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
However, Irwin and Young (2001) did not find the same patterns in
their study. They also noted that Wolfradt et al. failed to replicate their 1999
results in a follow-up study. In fact, Irwin and Young found that paranormal
beliefs were more related to an intuitive thinking style on its own—that is,
not in concert with a rational thinking style, as Wolfradt et al., 1999, found.
Nevertheless, there was a trend toward a relationship between a combination
of rational and intuitive thinking and New Age beliefs.1 These later studies
did not address paranormal experiences, which should be investigated with
regard to the combination pattern. Lindeman (1998) proposed that
paranormal and other “pseudoscientific” beliefs are associated with a
tendency to prefer the intuitive experiential style of reasoning. Some
evidence bearing on this speculation has been reiterated by Wolfradt and
colleagues.
Unfortunately, there are too few studies on constructive thinking in
psychics. We decided to perform exploratory analyses of the relationship
between claims in our dataset of extrasensory experiences and measures of
constructive thinking. According to some studies (Aarnio & Lindeman,
2005; Genovese, 2005; Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007; Wolfradt, et al., 1999),
high intuitive thinking and low analytical thinking predict paranormal belief.
Of these, reliance on intuitive thinking played a much more important role
than did low analytical thinking (Epstein, 1994; Evans, 2003; Sloman,
1996). It appeared, moreover, that people who rely on intuitive thinking are
more superstitious than non-intuitive thinkers (Epstein et al., 1996; Wolfradt
et al., 1999). The notion that paranormal belief, magical thinking, and
superstitions belong to the realm of intuition,2 whereas logical and scientific
thinking belong to the realm of analytical reasoning, helps explain why
rational knowledge or scientific education does not necessarily diminish
these beliefs and why the relationship between paranormal belief and
analytical thinking found in earlier studies (Blackmore, 1997; Wolfradt et
al., 1999) is weak or non-existent. Therefore, our study will focus more on
individual differences in profiles of CT styles in relation to paranormal
experiences and abilities rather than on beliefs as such.
Using the Constructive Thinking Inventory (CTI; Epstein & Meier,
1989), we report the test results on personality variables and thinking styles
1 The variable “New Age Philosophy” relates to a two-factor model of paranormal belief
derived from Rasch-scaling analyses of Tobacyk’s (1988) Revised Paranormal Belief Scale.
It is a set of beliefs that seem too consistently different in content and psychological purpose
than traditional PB (see Lange & Houran, 2010; Lange, Irwin, & Houran, 2000).
2 Note, however, that intuition can be understood as a sudden unconscious knowing, and there
are several studies that document a very “logical” or systematic causal process for paranormal
belief formation involving tolerance of ambiguity (see Lange & Houran, 1998, 1999b, 2000).
63
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
of ‘psychic claimants’ previously recruited for psychometry testing at the
Instituto de Psicología Paranormal (Institute of Paranormal Psychology; IPP)
in Buenos Aires, Argentina, and ‘non-psychic claimants’ who believe in
paranormal events, but seldom if ever report psi experiences (see Parra &
Argibay, 2008).3 We originally tested psychic ability to see if ‘psychics’
could distinguish between photos of dead people and photos of living
people. Results showed that psi-hitting was higher in the ‘psychic’ group
than in the ‘non-psychic’ group which scored at the level of mean chance
expectation. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the
differences between groups of psychic claimants and non-psychics claimants
(using the AEI’s factors ‘Experience’ and ‘Ability’) on different thinking
styles, such as Global Constructive Thinking (total score), Emotional
Coping, Behavioural Coping, and Esoteric Thinking. We make it clear in the
present study that no test of psychic ability per se was conducted.
METHOD
Participants
One hundred fifty-six participants were recruited by media
advertisements and a mailing list. An advertisement was also published on
the internet (www.alipsi.com.ar). The advertisements contained a brief
explanation of the psi test procedure and encouraged presumptive
participants to contact us for an interview in order to obtain more
information. Ninety-four remained (60%) for the categorization procedure
(see below).
Psychic Claimants. The sample consisted of 49 participants (72% female),
well-educated and believed in psi. The age range was 19 to 76 years (mean =
45 years; SD = 11 years). Seventy-eight percent of the participants did have
some training in meditation or other techniques based on practicing an
internal focus of attention.
3 This paper relies on self-reports on psychic abilities and experiences to distinguish psychics
claimants from non-psychic claimants. Therefore, self-reported psychics believe they have
psychic ability; we do not state that they actually have psychic ability. Thus, the results from
this study do not necessarily reveal the thinking styles that characterize people who actually
have psychic ability. The results might only reveal thinking styles that characterize people who
believe they have had psychic experiences and/or believe they have psychic ability.
64
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
Non-psychic Claimants. The sample consisted of 45 participants (82%
female), all of whom were also well-educated and believed in psi. The age
range was 22 to 76 years (mean = 49 years; SD = 14 years).
Questionnaires
Constructive Thinking Inventory (CTI; Epstein, 1998). The CTI is a 108item self-report inventory that assesses constructive and destructive beliefs
and thinking patterns. We used the Spanish version, which includes the 5point Likert-rating scale (Epstein, 2001). It is appropriate for ages 18-80 in
individual or group format, and takes 15-30 minutes to administer. The CTI
is based on Epstein’s Cognitive-Experiential Self-Theory, according to
which people have two fundamental adaptive systems—that is, an
“experiential system” that automatically learns from lived experience and a
“rational/intellectual system” that operates by conscious reasoning (see
Epstein with Brodsky, 1993). The items were grouped into the following six
main categories: (1) Emotional coping; (2) Behavioural coping; (3)
Categorical thinking; (4) Esoteric thinking; (5) Personal superstitious
thinking; and (6) Naïve optimism. Since the CTI cannot be hand-scored, a
computer scoring program is included with the CTI Introductory Kit. The
individual’s responses are entered into the software, and the program scores
the protocol, automatically generating a report with raw scores and gender
with a profile of the results.
Anomalous Experiences Inventory (AEI; Gallagher, Kumar, & Pekala,
1994). We used the American version, which we translated into Spanish. It is
a 70-item self-report inventory that maps five major dimensions (or
subscales) of subjective experience. The inventory (AEI) contains items
concerning anomalous/paranormal experiences and beliefs, use of drugs and
alcohol, and fear of the paranormal/anomalous. The AEI subscales showed
some convergent validity when correlated with selected personality
measures. The AEI’s experiences, belief, and abilities subscales correlate
significantly with traits related to experience seeking and fantasy proneness.
It may be particularly helpful in identifying different types of people for
research on psi-related abilities in the laboratory.
Procedure
The participants met during two-hour workshops, free of charge,
organized at the IPP. Experimenters A.P. and J.C.A. aimed to create an
informal social atmosphere, engaging in friendly conversation with the
65
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
participants before the test. Participants completed the Constructive
Thinking Inventory (CTI) and Anomalous Experiences Inventory (AEI).
After completing the questionnaires the participants took part in a formal
psychometry session (see Parra & Argibay, 2008, for results of the
psychometry test).
Categorization Procedure
We used the following criteria to split the sample into Psychics
Claimants/Non-Psychics Claimants: Participants who scored 75 (median
score) on the factor “Abilities” on the Anomalous Experiences Inventory
(AEI) were categorised as the Psychic Claimant group (N = 45; 28%) (i.e., “I
can influence or change an event by concentrating on that event” or “I am
able to see auras surrounding peoples’ bodies”). Participants who scored
25 (median score) on the factor “Experiences” (AEI) were categorised as the
Non-Psychic Claimants group (N = 49; 31%)—that is, people who had fewer
spontaneous psi experiences (and, of course, no ability or control over
them—that is, “I often seem to become aware of events before they happen,”
or “I often know what others are feeling or thinking without them telling
me”). The “Belief” scale was not used because 100% of the sample were
paranormal believers. Sixty-two participants were excluded.
RESULTS
Descriptive statistics are given in Table 1. The two groups were about
equal in size, and the actual score ranges were close to the corresponding
theoretical ranges. Cronbach Alpha values were mostly high, although the
Fear and Use of Drugs and Alcohol values ranged from only .61 to .64.
Mean scores were significantly different between groups on Experiences,
Abilities, and Beliefs.
Table 2 shows the differences between the psychic claimants and nonpsychic claimants on CTI factors and facets. Twelve out of 23 (52%)
statistical comparisons were significant. Applying the 5% rule, we would
expect no more than 1 of 23 tests to be significant by chance. These 12 are
highlighted in bold in Table 2. For these 12 significant findings, effects size
differences, given as Cohen’s d, are all moderate to high, ranging from .33 to
1.26.
Seven of these 12 (30%) were significant at p .01, which is still very
high if we apply the 5% rule. These seven are: (1) Nonsensitivity facet of the
Emotional Coping factor; (2) Behavioural Coping factor; (3)
Conscientiousness facet of the Behavioural Coping factor; (4) Categorical
Thinking; (5) Distrust of Others facet of the Categorical Thinking factor, (6)
66
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
Esoteric Thinking, and (7) Formal Superstitious Thinking facet of the
Esoteric Thinking Factor.
Table 1
AEI Factors: Means, SDs, Range, and Cronbach’s Alpha ( ) Levels (N = 94)
Theorical
Range
Actual
Range
Mean
SD
Alpha
Experiences
.00 to .79
.17 to .79
.52
.13
.71
Abilities
.00 to .88
.43 to .88
.54
.11
.82
Beliefs
.42 to .99
.66 to 1.00
.86
.08
.96
Fear
.00 to .83
.00 to .83
.13
.18
.64
Use of drugs and alcohol
.00 to .43
.00 to .43
.04
.10
.61
Experiences
.00 to .79
.00 to .55
.17
.10
.74
Abilities
.00 to .88
.00 to .31
.12
.10
.80
Beliefs
.42 to .99
.42 to .92
.70
.13
.94
Fear
.00 to .83
.00 to .67
.13
.15
.64
Use of drugs and alcohol
.00 to .43
.00 to .43
.06
.10
.62
Group
AEI
Psychic
Claimants
(N = 45)
NonPsychic
Claimants
(N = 49)
DISCUSSION
Although these results are encouraging, it is important to keep in
mind the low magnitude d values as effect sizes of the scores. People
claiming ability or control over psychic experiences somewhat encompass
the ability to deal effectively with the inner world of feelings and the outer
world of events. According to Epstein (2001):
Good emotional copers are particularly effective in dealing with
negative feelings. . . . They are characterized more by peace of mind
and low levels of stress than by peaks of joy. Copers are calm and
centered, and they experience less stress in living than others. (p. 10)
Psychic claimants seem clearly to be open to inner feelings and emotions.
Many psychic claimants seem to act more empathically than telepathically.
Perhaps empathy can function alongside psi, thereby mutually enhancing the
strength of these abilities. Healers and other paranormal/anomalous
experients (such as psychics and mediums) seem to use emotional empathy
and become absorbed in the process, often to the point of feeling that they
are “merging” with the clients and sitters.
67
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
68
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
One interesting outcome was that the psychic claimants also had
significantly higher mean scores than did non-psychic claimants on the
Behavioural coping factor. According to Epstein (2001):
[Behavioral coping] has a more positive emphasis; it is actionoriented thinking. People who are good behavioral copers think in
ways that promote effective action. It allows them to take on
challenges and risks, as they have the confidence that things will
work out well. Instead of worrying about deadlines, they get right to
work; instead of punishing themselves for a mistake, they figure out
how to correct it” (pp. 11-12)
Two of three facets, Positive Thinking and Conscientiousness facets, also
had significantly higher mean scores than did non-psychic claimants.
Optimism is a characteristic of the Positive Thinking facet. Epstein also
indicates that conscientious people who are good behavioural copers are
more accepting of others, more optimistic, and more action-oriented than
those who are good emotional copers. Good emotional copers, on the other
hand, are more self-accepting, take things less personally, and are less
distressed when things do not go their way. Emotional coping and
behavioural coping contribute directly to constructive thinking.
We found that the psychic claimants also had significantly higher
mean scores than did non-psychic claimants on the Categorical thinking
factor. Psychic claimants are more rigid thinkers than non-psychic claimants.
They tend to classify people as good or bad, “for” or “against” them,
“winners” or “losers”. On two of the three facets, Polarized Thinking and
Distrust of Others, the psychic claimants also had significantly higher mean
scores than did non-psychic claimants, which implies that psychic claimants
tend to be more opinionated in their thinking and also characterized by a
distrust of others, and a constant suspicion that people around them think
about their psychic abilities.
Finally, on Esoteric thinking, including the facets of Belief in the
Unusual and Formal Superstitious Thinking, psychic claimants also had
significantly higher mean scores than did non-psychic claimants. According
to Epstein (2001):
Superstitious thinking refers to beliefs about unusual and paranormal
phenomena and standard superstitions . . . and includes believing in
traditional superstitions (breaking a mirror, walking under a ladder,
having a black cat cross your path), good-luck charms, astrology,
ghosts, extrasensory perception, and mind control. (p. 11)
69
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
However, personal superstitious thinking, as used in the CTI, does not refer
to traditional superstitions, but to personal superstitions; thus, psychic
claimants are not likely to believe that talking about the prospect of success
will prevent them from succeeding. Epstein also indicates that Esoteric
thinkers believe in ghosts, flying saucers, thought control, and astrology, and
that these phenomena can allow them to bypass their rational mind and
contact their experiential mind, which in certain circumstances can be
advantageous. We stress that our findings at this stage are exploratory, not
confirmatory, and it remains to be seen which if these findings are valid, and
which are the products of chance.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
We are grateful to the Bial Foundation for its financial support of this
research project. We thank Lance Storm for his advice.
REFERENCES
Aarnio, K., and Lindeman, M. (2005). Paranormal beliefs, education, and
thinking styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1227-1236.
Gallagher, C., Kumar, V. K., and Pekala, R. J. (1994). The Anomalous
Experiences Inventory: Reliability and validity. Journal of
Parapsychology, 58, 402-428.
Katz, L., and Epstein, S. (1991). Constructive thinking and coping with
laboratory-induced stress. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 61, 789-800.
Epstein, S., and Meier, P. (1989). Constructive thinking. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 332-339.
Epstein, S. (1992). Constructive thinking and mental and physical wellbeing. In L. Montada, S. H. Filipp, and M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Life crises
and experiences of loss in adulthood (pp. 385-409). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Epstein, S. (1998). Constructive thinking: The key to emotional intelligence.
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Epstein, S. (2001). CTI, Inventario de Pensamiento Constructivo: Una
medida de inteligencia emocional. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
Epstein, S., and Katz, L. (1992). Coping ability, stress, productive load and
symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 813-825.
Genovese, J. E. C. (2005). Paranormal beliefs, schizotypy, and thinking
styles among teachers and future teachers. Personality and Individual
Differences, 39, pp. 93-102.
70
Australian Journal of Parapsychology
Irwin, H. J, and Young, J. M. (2001). Intuitive versus reflective processes in
the formation of paranormal beliefs. European Journal of
Parapsychology, 17, 45-53.
Lange, R., and Houran, J. (1998). Delusions of the paranormal: A haunting
question of perception. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 186,
637-645.
Lange, R., and Houran, J. (1999b). The role of fear in delusions of the
paranormal. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 159-166.
Lange, R., and Houran, J. (2000). Modeling Maher’s attribution theory of
delusions as a cusp catastrophe. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and
Life Sciences, 4, 235-254.
Lange, R., Irwin, H. J., and Houran, J. (2000). Top-down purification of
Tobacyk’s Revised Paranormal Belief Scale. Personality and Individual
Differences, 29, 131-156.
Lange, R., and Houran, J. (2010). A transliminal view of intuitions in the
workplace. North American Journal of Psychology, 12, 501-516.
Lindeman, M. (1998). Motivation, cognition and pseudoscience.
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 257-265.
Lindeman, M., and Aarnio, K. (2007). Superstitious, magical, and
paranormal beliefs: An integrative model. Journal of Research in
Personality, 41, 731-744.
Parra, A. and Argibay, J.C. (2008). Reading faces: An experimental
exploration of psychometry using photographs and names. Australian
Journal of Parapsychology, 8, 47-57.
Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Tobacyk, J. J. (1988) A revised Paranormal Belief Scale.
(Unpublished manuscript, Louisiana Tech University, Ruston,
LA)
Wolfradt, U., Oubaid, V., Straube, E. R., Bischoff, N., and Mischo, J.
(1999). Thinking styles, schizotypal traits and anomalous experiences.
Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 821-830.
Salta 2015 (C1137ACQ)
Buenos Aires
ARGENTINA
Email:
[email protected]
71