1
Linking life- and suicide-related goal directed processes: a qualitative study.(RESEARCH)
Source: Journal of Mental Health Counseling
Publication Date: 10/01/2006
Author: Valach, Ladislav ; Michel, Konrad ; Dey, Pascal ; Young, Richard
COPYRIGHT 2006 American Mental Health Counselors Association
Valach, L., Michel, K., Young, R. A., & Dey, P. (2006). Linking life and suicide related goal
directed systems. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 28, 4, 353-372.
Note: A shorter version of this article was presented at the Conference of the European Society
for Suicide Prevention, Copenhagen, 2004.
Previous analyses of the narratives of 40 persons hospitalized in a general hospital after suicide
attempt found that they described their suicide attempts as goal-directed processes, sometimes
planned in advance, sometimes executed spontaneously. They also described short-term actions,
mid-term projects, and long-term careers reflecting goal-directed processes related to maintaining
and developing their lives. In this qualitative study that reports on a re-analysis of these data, the
research participants' narratives were examined for links between life-related and suicide-related
goal-directed processes. The analysis followed a distinction between a goal-directed view of
suicide processes and a dynamic systems view. The findings indicated that some links were goaldirected and consistent with the reasoning of life-maintaining projects. These "top-down" links
between life-related and the suicide-related goal-directed processes reflected the goal-directed
view of suicide processes. Other links indicated a substantially limited capacity for goaldirectedness, reflecting the "bottom-up" dynamic systems view. Finally, it third group of links
reflected a mix of "top-down" and "bottom-up" processes. Implications are offered for mental
health counselors working with suicidal clients.
**********
Suicide acts, despite their high rates and repeated occurrence, are extraordinary events. Some
authors (e.g., Maltsberger, 2004) indicate that suicide involves a complete break down of any
order in the person's life. Others (e.g., Maris, 1981) suggest that suicide is part of long-term,
orderly processes. We need to unpack this apparent dichotomy in terms of both breakdown and
orderly processes as they are experienced in clients who have attempted suicide.
The present study uses an action theoretical view of suicide acts and the processes that precede
suicide. This view is premised on an understanding of suicide as goal-directed action (Michel &
Valach, 1997). Like other everyday human actions, suicide attempts and other self-destructive
behaviors involve the intentional and goal-directed actions of the person both in the short-term,
self-destructive act itself, and in longer-term projects and careers of which the self-destructive
acts are a part. Michel and Valach (2001) showed that self-destructive and suicidal processes
could be seen in terms of joint goal-directed action. The "processes" we refer to in this article
include actions, projects, and careers and their associated verbal and non-verbal behaviors,
2
cognitions, feelings, and social interactions. This term applies equally to processes that are lifeaffirming or life-destroying. In as much as these processes are related to each other, we can also
refer to them as a goal-directed system.
In the reports of research participants after a non-lethal suicidal act, we were able to identify
short-term suicide actions, mid-term suicide projects, and a long-term suicide career, all of which
the participants constructed as goal-directed processes (Valach, Michel, Young, & Dey, 2002).
These narratives referred to both suicide-related and life-related processes (that is actions,
projects, and careers related to life instead of to suicide).
The specific question addressed in this article is how persons, after a suicide attempt, link
suicide-related and life-related processes, that is, how they switch between their life goal-directed
system and their suicide goal-directed system. The linking of these processes is a segment in the
stream of actions that allows a quick switch from being engaged in one system to pursuing
another. This phenomenon has not been investigated extensively. Specifically, in terms of the
narrative of a suicide attempt, we identify this link in the case when an action of an everyday
project is suddenly followed by an action related to an existing suicide project.
It is hypothesized that the links between suicide-related and life-related processes may occur in
one of two ways. First, the link may be organized as "top-down" steering and control in which
both sets of processes are linked within the explicit goal-directed system (see Figure 1). Here, the
person may have decided at some previous time to attempt suicide. The link is clear to the person
at the time of the attempt and seemingly rational. This type of link is anchored conceptually in
the goal-directed perspective described above.
[FIGURE 1]
Second, suicidal processes can also reflect bottom-up steering or control of a suicide action by
the person who is attempting suicide. In this case, the suicide action is described as starting with
smaller component parts and gradually building up to the larger units and is contrasted to topdown processes that begin with larger units and then proceed to smaller units. In bottom-up
processes, the links are suggested to occur in processes of a lower order in goal-directed action,
that is, words, phrases, movements, images, perception of environmental features, or sudden
feelings rather than goals or long-term plans. This perspective captures the unconscious processes
or responds to a momentary pull or impulse that is critical in the suicide attempt. However, this
bottom-up process still occurs in the context of life-maintaining processes, which are expected to
contextualize the person's narrative. Thus, the term "bottom-up" steering indicates a process in
which the specific behavioral elements appear first and the higher order processes such as goals
are developed later. Bottom-up steering is reflected in the terms "affective," "emotional" or
"impulsive" suicide (Simon, Swarm, Powell, Potter, Kresnow, & O'Carroll, 2001; Williams,
Davidson, & Montgomery, 1980) as distinct from the term "rational" suicide that is often used in
everyday language and in law for a suicide action that was not planned in advance and was
executed under strong emotion in a specific stressful situation. This perspective is captured in
dynamic systems theory (Vallacher & Nowak, 1997).
The purpose of this research is to unpack the link between life processes and suicide processes
using the notions of "top-down" and "bottom-up" steering we have described. The bottom-up
3
launching of a suicide-related process may be one particular way of getting involved in a suicidal
attempt, but not the general rule. Even more, we suggest that suicide- and life-related processes
are both goal-directed processes. The link between suicide- and life-related processes reflects
either the goal-directed system or the dynamic system, described above. If suicide-related and life
related processes co-exist, it would represent a substantial challenge to the conception of suicide
as disengagement (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1998). These explications will be illustrated with the
patients' narratives following a non-fatal suicide act.
METHOD
In this study we were interested in eliciting a narrative of the suicide attempt and related actions
in the context of a psychotherapeutic interview. This narrative gave us access to the construction
of the suicide act and related projects and careers and to their associated goal-directed processes.
Participants
A consecutive sample of 40 persons, hospitalized because of the health consequences of their
suicide attempts comprised the participants in this study. As they were hospitalized in a general
hospital in a Swiss city, they represented a non-random sample of the general population of
suicide attempters in the area. Their education spanned from compulsory schooling to university
degree, and their occupations from university student, home keeper to unemployed labourer.
Citizens of foreign countries were underrepresented (instead of the expected value N=8 (20%
inhabitants in Switzerland are of foreign nationality), there were only 4 foreigners in the sample).
A few days after their attempt, a psychotherapist invited them to participate in this study. Their
participation was explained as consisting of talking to a psychotherapist about their suicide
attempt. The participation rate for the study was reasonable (40 recruited clients out of 162
treated in the hospital after a suicide act in that year), although we did not attempt to obtain a
representative sample. The only systematic selection bias occurred when some of the persons
who had been admitted to the inpatient general hospital where they would have been recruited for
the study were relocated to a psychiatric hospital prior our request, and thus not available to be
interviewed. Consequently, we did not have any persons diagnosed with an acute psychotic
disorder. The sample consisted of 25 women, 15 men (mean age = 42 years, range 17 to 80
years). The interviews were conducted by psychotherapists, eight of whom were medical doctors
and one a psychologist. The narratives of two research participants could not be analyzed in
regard to the target issues for various reasons and thus were not included in this report, leaving a
final sample size of 38.
Setting
The interviews were conducted in a psychiatric outpatient clinic, a building about 100 meters
away from the hospital emergency ward where the research participants were in care. In the
interview room, the research participants were exposed to two video cameras, two video
recorders, and two large TV monitors. Nevertheless, the setting was private and calm.
Procedure
4
The interview, in which the clients were asked to describe how it came about that they had
attempted suicide, lasted 30 to 60 minutes and was video recorded. The research participants
were encouraged to talk about everything they considered relevant to their suicide attempt. The
interviewers prompted them for details, asked them to clarify unclear points, and were emphatic,
non-evaluative, and emotionally supportive.
Ethical Considerations
This project was supported by the Swiss National Research Foundation. It was approved by the
relevant hospital departments, and by the ethics committee of the Swiss National Research
Foundation. Following recruitment, the research participants were again informed about the
procedure in every detail and asked to sign a permission to videotape the interviews. They were
informed that these videotapes would be seen by project collaborators, transcribed and analysed
for research purposes. They were also asked for permission to allow us to quote substantial parts
of these interviews in an anonymous form in scientific professional publications.
Assumptions
In this study, we asked for the participants' narratives in an ecologically valid way. In effect, this
interview was part of the participant's risk assessment for suicide. We believed that these
participants could provide competent stories of their suicide attempt given the time and
opportunity to do so. We also believed that these stories would contain the information the
participants found important. We considered the present sample satisfactory for this method and
for the evaluation procedure (i.e., risk assessment).
Analysis
Forty interviews were translated from Swiss-German to German, transcribed, and 38 were
submitted to a content analysis. The quotations used in this report were then translated into
English and subsequently "smoothed" to capture colloquial English expression. In this analysis,
we identified descriptions of processes related to the suicide behaviour of the research
participants and distinguished between long-term, medium-term and short-term processes which
we labelled respectively as careers, projects and actions (Valach, Young, & Lynam, 2002).
Actions were identified as short-term processes, mostly several minutes long during which the
actual poisoning, jumping, cutting or shooting occurred. Projects covered a middle ground
between these short-term actions and the nearly lifelong processes we refer to as careers. For
example, when a research participant described processes in which she was trying to determine,
over a period of time, whether an existing relationship was really beneficial for her, it was
labelled as a "project." The time span in which all the relationships to a boyfriend occurred or,
rather, the social-action systems of relationships would be understood as a relationship career.
Within each of these goal-directed systems (career, project, action), we identified goals,
functional steps, and elements of these processes. Goals could be long-term, midterm, or shortterm. The analysis then identified the series of steps that the research participant used to
accomplish these goals. Finally, we identified a number of features of these action steps, that is,
action elements, which could be defined in physical terms, for example, time, and physical
objects involved. As we identified the person's suicide related actions, projects and career as well
5
as those related to his or her everyday life, we also identified the links between these life-related
and suicide-related processes by searching for statements describing how the person switched
between their ongoing life-related processes to their suicide-related processes. If this switch was
informed by a goal consideration, we labelled it "top-down," if the suicide action was launched
by more momentary processes, for example, by the availability of the means of attempt in a
moment of despair, we registered these as "bottom-up" processes.
FINDINGS
In the narratives generated in response to the question of how it came about that the person had
attempted suicide, the research participants described their non-fatal suicide attempts as actions
and related these attempts to mid-term projects and long-term careers. They also described a
series of actions that were part of life-maintaining projects and careers, but which suddenly
became part of both a suicide-related project and career. Most of the research participants
described mid-term suicide-related projects that they constructed as part of a suicide-related
career in their lives. However, we also found that there was a link between, or switch from, lifemaintaining actions and projects to suicide actions and projects (see the clients' narratives below).
We observed these links in the narratives of suicide processes where an action describing an
everyday project was suddenly followed by an action related to an existing suicide project. For
example, a middle-aged woman and mother of two school-aged children, while feeling more and
more disoriented that day, ate yoghurt at one moment and then jumped from her balcony at
another. A young woman, after being deserted by her boyfriend, was visited by her worried
mother. She expressed hostile feelings toward her mother's protective attitude and after her
mother left, she cut her wrists. Another young woman came home from work she hated, observed
her female friend having a good time with her boyfriend, went to her bedroom and swallowed a
large number of pills. A middle-aged woman took a taxi to a cliff behind her village, sat there,
and after seeing her husband's car, "let go" and fell into the gorge. A young woman after a
number of arguments with her boyfriend, who repeatedly left her only to come back a few days
later to tell her that their relationship was over, saw pain pills on the table and took a large
quantity of them.
In this analysis, the research participants' narratives addressed the actions prior to the suicide
behavior, the link to a suicide action and project, and finally, the suicide action itself. Analyzing
the descriptions of the links, that is, how the clients changed the course of their actions from lifeoriented to death oriented, we were able to distinguish three types of links following the "topdown" and "bottom-up" typology described earlier. First, some links reflected a "top-down"
steering and control, characteristic for goal directed processes (n = 17 of 38 cases). These
research participants reported a conscious decision to attempt a suicide, including having the goal
to attempt suicide and the choice of means. Second, bottom-up links occurred in 9 of 38 cases.
These narratives reflected unspecified goals that the research participants were not fully aware of.
For example, one research participant described cutting herself without wanting to kill herself
and then, in the course of cutting, she suddenly cut herself in a manner that endangered her life.
In these cases, the persons often launched the suicide action by utilizing behavior that is exhibited
by persons with disordered executive functions. For example, persons with disordered executive
functions may take and use a toothbrush if it were placed in front of them, even if there is no need
to brush one's teeth. However, the bottom up launching of suicide action could be triggered either
6
by contextual external or by behavioral and internal features. Finally, in some cases the suicide
action seemed to be launched by a mixture of "top-down" and "bottom-up" processes (n = 12 of
38 cases). In these cases, the person used goal considerations that led to an action step (as in top
down processes) as well as engaged in the suicide process without a clear goal, including what
the end state of the action was (as in bottom-up processes). In our sample we found that more
women (7 of 23; 30%) than men (2 of 15; 14%) reported bottom-up links and more men (8 of 15;
53%) than women (9 of 23; 39%) reported top-down links. The distribution of mixed links across
gender was about equal (7 of 15 women or 31%; 5 of 15 men or 33%). The following examples
illustrate this typology of links.
Top-Down Links Between Ongoing Life Processes and Suicide Actions
Demonstrating the top-down link between ongoing life processes and suicide actions is this case
of a middle-aged women.
Suicide preceding life-related actions. On Friday, [my husband] said he has to go to the office
and he would be late.... I waited till 11 o'clock and then went to bed. On Saturday morning he
was not there. I was worried. The roads" were icy and I thought that he had an accident. I went to
have a look whether the car was there. It was not there. I opened the letterbox and found a
message that he was going to come tomorrow when he would tell me more.... I left a message on
his answering machine that he should call me. He rang and said that he needed this day [to
himself]. He also wrote a letter. He spent the night in a hotel and promised that he was going to
come Sunday night to have a look. I did the housework as [I do] every Saturday. I went to do the
shopping.
The description of the husband's disengagement can be seen as a part of the ongoing partnership
and, therefore, as a life-related action even though the research participant's further narrative
shows that, for her, it was a prelude to a transition to her suicide related processes.
The link. I [had] made the decision to commit suicide a long time ago. Only the last straw was
missing that broke the camel's back. I thought about it very often but I never had the courage.
Because of my illness (eating disorder) my husband was not able to go on like this any more. It
was an unsolvable dilemma, a vicious circle, and he wanted to move out. That was decisive. I
couldn't have imagined life without him.
His informing me that he couldn't stand it any longer and that is why he was leaving was the last
straw. I was expecting this.
Suicide action. By this time everything was prepared. I went to have a look to see whether the
material (the medication she overdosed with) that I prepared long time ago, perhaps half a year
ago, was there or whether I would have to go to the pharmacist. I saw that everything was there. I
was just checking whether there was enough.
It was in the morning before shopping. Then I did the shopping, cleaned the parrot's cage, and
then I became frightened that he might not come [home] on Sunday night. I had to make sure that
he is going to come; otherwise the parrot would die without food the whole week long. So I told
7
him on the answering machine that I was starting therapy on Sunday morning. He should come in
the afternoon to feed the parrot. So I was sure that he would come.
Then I covered the cage so that he would be quiet in the morning ... Then I switched the light off.
Next morning he would see the food ... I wanted to lie down. I put two hot water bottles into the
bed. He phoned to ask whether I really was going to therapy. He promised to come the next
afternoon.
I went downstairs, took a glass and put the rolls of pills in the water. It was shortly after 11
because he phoned at 11.... I was ready ... I was sure that he was not going to come [home] before
lunchtime.
The moment [of] getting into a warm bed was always the best part of a day. But I was not able to
fall asleep. Normally, I could go to bed and fall asleep like a stone. I realized that my heart was
beating fast.
... I started with the sleeping pills. There was a whole box. There were also some painkillers,
some allergy pills. I simply took everything.
I had the feeling that there were about 20 sleeping pills and 30 painkillers. I know that I had a
sudden thought after taking the sleeping pills, asking myself about the meaning of what I was
doing.... I thought that I would have to get up and call the emergency physician to empty my
stomach. But it was only a passing thought and it was quickly over.
I went on until everything was gone. I know that with the last box I was asking myself whether I
had to take that as well.... I had the feeling that I needed it as well because it might not be
enough.... I did not have any other thought. I just had had enough of this life, in this vicious
circle.
It was clear to me that he was leaving me. There was so much damage done in the course of my
illness and I was sure it was going to get worse. I felt that he was at the end of his power. He
could not go on like this any longer.
I was not prepared to go to the hospital, which was the reason why my husband gave up.
He had arranged a hospital stay for me several times and I cancelled them, so he was
disappointed.
The vicious circle meant that the course of the day was exactly planned and I had to vomit
(because of an eating disorder) after every meal. It was three times a day following a special
routine. Both of us could not stand it any more.... I wanted to end my existence.... That is what I
wished and was waiting for. I understood that death was a possible consequence of me not eating
for the last two years.
Once I had told my husband that I want to be buried at home.
8
I woke up during the night and realized that I had thrown up. Terrible, I thought, you are
vomiting on the blanket. Then I couldn't remember anything. Then I remember that they were
trying to insert a stomach probe.
Summary. The action that preceded the suicide attempt was the woman's husband leaving her.
The link was that she indicated that she had decided a long time ago to die by suicide. She also
prepared the necessary means for her suicide act and was only waiting for a situation to arise,
which would give her the "last push." This situation occurred when her husband wanted to leave
her. Then she proceeded in a rational way, took precautions, made sure that the details would not
get out of hand after her death, and took pills. In her narrative, the link between her lifemaintaining processes and the suicide goal-directed system was a goal-directed, top-down
process. This woman had on-going processes in her life that were related to suicide, including
thoughts, feelings, expectations, and plans. She was able to differentiate these processes as a
distinct part of her life and ultimately they facilitated the suicide attempt. When these processes
were consciously implemented, the suicide action followed. The processes involved were
cognitively controlled and goal directed. The control and steering were top-down.
Bottom-Up Link Between Ongoing Life Processes and Suicide Actions Example 1
Demonstrating the bottom-up link between ongoing life processes and suicide actions is this case
of a middle-aged woman.
Life-related and suicide preceding actions. There was an internal restlessness.... I asked my sister
if I could come [to visit her] because I was not able to go to work because I was frightened that I
might faint in the garden. I was afraid of fainting and never waking up again. My thoughts were
too much for me. I was challenged by my thoughts. Even if I was playing with the children I was
drifting away, following my thoughts; thoughts about reincarnation again and again. I dealt with
death and asked myself what it means to die, where would I go ill died or one of my children
[died]?
In the morning when I went to my sister's, I was afraid of going into water. It was pulling me. I
am also attracted by [the] water at T. (the town the client lives in which lies on a large lake).
Water calls me.
There was a ray [of sunshine] on the water that lulled me into the water. God was occupying my
mind and [the thought] that coming out of water [was that] one could be created anew, perhaps as
an animal.... My son had to go to school and so we went together He was on his bike and I went
by car.
He took a short cut but I did not see it. It was very bad for me because I did not know when he
would come back, whether I was going to see him and that I had not said good-bye. I don't know
why. I wanted to see him again. I was afraid of parting.
I had difficulties parting in the morning with my daughter.... Children mean a lot to me. And if I
don't know whether I am going to see them again after they have gone somewhere it is very
difficult for me. I have this terrible feeling even if they only leave for school.
9
I drove slowly because I could not see my son any more. And this preoccupied me a lot.
I was also thinking about the end of the world. Then I thought that I could be seeing my son for
the last time. It was very intensive. Then I went to my sister's and did not realize what was
happening.
I was frightened that I would do something to myself without being able to control it.
My daughter said she did not understand something and I said I couldn't help her either She was
upset, complaining that she couldn't manage because she had gym over lunch and the school was
too much for her and she had to go to her piano lesson. Then I realized that it was too much for
her. Perhaps it was too much for me as well.
I realized that it was too much for all of us, getting from appointment to appointment.
There were very many thoughts that put me under pressure so that I felt I was going to get
muddled.
Then I said that I don't want my children to have a disordered mother whom they have to visit in
psychiatry. Rather no mother at all. That was strong. Then there was the thought that I wanted to
tell my sister that if I was not in control of my senses and health, she should switch off the life
supporting machines. I don't want anybody to suffer because I was mentally and physically gone.
I felt that strongly.
I was not courageous enough to tell her that I was thinking about what should happen to me. I
know that I talked to my stepmother and she said something about suicide and I said I promised
her that I was not going to do anything, but somehow it did not get through. I wasn't able to
concentrate. My daughter said something as well that I didn't understand and I thought that I
would be glad if no one said anything, as I wouldn't understand it.
I was restless and disorganized mentally. I knew that I would have to occupy myself otherwise I
would be restless. So I was sorting bills and thought, I know that all, why am I doing it. And as it
was finished and I was still restless I didn't know what to do. My sister asked me whether I
wanted to eat something, whether I wanted yoghurt. I was eating yoghurt and then I can't
remember anything.
The yoghurt was bilberry yoghurt and I ate it with distaste, as I don't like it. I did it just to keep
occupied.
[For the first time] in the evening I realized that I was very restless. My sister had a terrace and
the door was opened. I had the feeling that it should be closed because ...
Link. I felt being driven outside, that someone wanted me to jump or [I] myself [wanted to jump].
Suicide action. And then I didn't know anything. I was told that I got up after the yoghurt and
walked to the terrace and jumped ..., as if this was the clearest thing. She [my sister] saw it at the
last moment, asked what I was doing, but I had already jumped. It is about 10 to 15 meters down.
10
Summary. The actions that preceded the attempt involved the research participant trying to gain
control over her thoughts. In the link, she was challenged by thoughts, feeling muddled and
disorganized, and losing control. She felt she was being driven outside and that someone wanted
her to jump. Her actions were automatic. The internal process that directed her actions was the
experience of mental disorganization. Both of these represent bottom-up processes.
The research participant described a series of thoughts and situations in which she felt under
pressure. To deal with these disturbing thoughts, she introduced a sequence of measures--such as
visiting her sister, trying to distract herself by keeping occupied, asking others to keep quiet,
eating to achieve distraction, even wanting to close the door to the terrace. Then she felt like she
was being driven to the terrace and subsequently she jumped. The link between her life-related
actions and her suicide action was a bottom-up process she was not even aware of. However, this
suicide process was integrated into a suicide project, which she described in her narrative, for
example, dealing with the issue that "better no mother than a mother in a psychiatric clinic."
Example 2
Demonstrating the bottom-up link between ongoing life processes and suicide actions is this case
of a 22 year-old woman.
Life-related and suicide preceding actions. I came home from work, was frustrated as I felt
terrorized there. My boyfriend, who decided three weeks ago that he wanted to leave me and then
came back in order to leave me again the next day was at home and made me feel even worse.
We argued, shouted at each other, threw chairs and cushions. We argued about his not being able
to make up his mind. We argued and then a moment came when I had [had] enough. I went to my
room to cry.
Link. There were pills in my room that I simply took.
Suicide action. I could not go on any further. I did not want to live. I wanted to be dead. I wanted
my peace and quiet. I had not had [any] peace for quite long time. We were constantly arguing.
But peace is something important. I was unable to find it. It was probably a solution for my
problem. I wanted to have peace so that I would not be here, that I didn't want to be here. I
wanted to find my peace.
I simply took the pills. I was not frightened.... Ten minutes later my girl friend came, she knew
that I hadn't been feeling well recently, realized the situation, and they both took me to the
hospital.
Summary. The situation that preceded the suicide attempt involved the research participant's
argument with her boyfriend, followed by her going to her room to cry. The link was that there
were pills available, so she took them. The processes involved losing cognitive control through
an affective outburst and facilitated by the availability of pills. The control and steering were
bottom-up that catered to her strong affect.
11
Later in her narrative, this woman reported that although she had not seriously attempted suicide
before, she thought about it often. She already had gone to catch a tram to take her to a high
terrace in front of a church from which people with suicidal ideation occasionally leaped.
However, she did not entertain the idea of suicide as a definitive part of her agenda. While crying
and feeling unhappy, the availability of pills facilitated her suicide action. The link between her
life-related action and her suicide career was achieved in a bottom-up manner, while the suicide
itself proceeded in a goal-directed way.
Mixed (Top-Down and Bottom-Up) Links Between Ongoing Life Processes and Suicide Actions
Demonstrating the mixed top-down and bottom-up link between ongoing life processes and
suicide actions is this case of a middle-aged woman.
Preceding, life-related action. My husband was very tense because of his work and I could not
speak to him very often. I had the feeling that I was creating problems for him, because I was
always telling him that I was in pain. I was very sad. I was afraid of the future because of my
pain. I knew that I had to accept it but I did not always manage to. One morning I felt really not
well and had pain and there were tensions in the family with my son.
I wrote a note to my husband. I had had enough. My husband suspected that I had gone to a place
where a suicide recently occurred. I decided that it was the moment to do something. I was aware
of the fact that suicides [were] committed there.
I was afraid to die but nevertheless I went there ... I took a taxi and went there, high up near my
flat.... I did not intend to die. When I left [my flat] I was not clear about my intentions.
I thought that I might jump and that would be the best for him. I had the feeling that I was boring
him with my problems, pains and being depressed.
Once I got out of the taxi I did not know what to do. I did not feel well.... I walked a bit. There is
a high bridge next to the motorway over the gorge. It really is high. I was frightened because of
its height. There were no trees.
Links. The bridge was on the other side and I did not go there. It is a couple of minutes away.... I
saw the bridge from the stone I went to sit on.... The bridge frightened me. The bridge was out of
the question for me (to use for suicide, because it was too frightening) ... I was afraid of dying
even though I was ill.... I was thinking continually about it but i hoped not to have to die.... I did
not understand ... I don't know what was going on in me. I sat down on a big stone and saw that
there was a steep drop. I lay down because I was in pain. I did not feel well. Then I saw my
husband driving by.
There was a gorge near the stone. There were some small trees.... Then I saw my husband and
became frightened.... It was like watching a film. I was frightened that he might come and be mad
with me. I would have understood [his being mad with me] but I would prefer if he said a few
nice words to me.
12
Suicide action. He saw me but was not able to turn around on the motorway. He turned later and
came but I could not see him anymore because I was already down the gorge. I had fallen. I made
a movement in order to try to stop and I was not able to, it was too late.... I was very afraid and it
happened very quickly. Just for a moment when I was able to hold onto a bush and then it was
over. I did not have any time, it was too late.
I moved. I wanted to hold on and then was not able to do anything. I slid and let go. I was afraid
before I wanted to fall. But I could not do anything. I was not able to hold on. It was all over.
I was frightened when I came to a stop, but beforehand while sliding I was not frightened. When
I stopped with all the small injuries, for the first time I realized what happened. It was hard. I
could not understand.
I'd fallen down 40 to 50 meters and then stopped. I was sitting on several small trees. I was full of
cuts and bruises and I was frightened. I started to scream--I called because my husband was very
near. Then he called the police.
Summary. The situation preceding the suicide attempt involved the research participant not
feeling well and experiencing tension at home. The link occurs when she went to a place where
suicides were often attempted and accomplished. She wrote a note that indicated she couldn't go
on, but she was not clear about her intentions. She sat on the edge of a gorge, saw her husband,
was frightened and let go. The processes involved purposefully getting herself into the situation
where a suicide attempt was possible, but then let it happen (top-down, but last step is bottomup). The steering and control is mixed: bottom-up and top-down.
The research participant reported an intentional link to a situation suitable for suicide and an
automatic response resulting in "letting go." This could be described as a mixed way of linking
her life related processes and her goal directed system of suicide processes.
DISCUSSION
In this study, we attempted to demonstrate how "break downs" often described in suicide
processes are well-organized processes of goal-directed actions. Specifically, persons who have
attempted suicide construct their life-endangering actions (suicide actions) as goal-directed. We
suggested that there are links between life-related and suicide-related processes. In our view the
links between life-related and suicide related processes could be seen either as goal-directed (that
is "top-down") or as "bottom-up," which are often called stimulus-driven processes. Our findings
indicate that that while suicide processes are goal directed (are steered in a "top-down" manner),
the links between life- and suicide-related processes can also be seen as "bottom-up" processes.
These two classes of control in action and attention have already been suggested and supported
by neurological findings (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002).
We also illustrated the link between life-promoting and suicide-related goal-directed processes at
the lower level of action organization, which accommodates movements and processes we are not
highly conscious of. Under "action organization" we understand the systemic order of an action
process at three basic levels. The highest level contains conscious goal, the middle level consists
of action steps and sub-goals and the lowest level accommodates action elements. This link can
13
be understood in terms of dynamic systems theory (Vallacher & Novak, 1994, 1997). In general,
dynamic systems theory attempts to show how even in the nonhuman world many processes are
organized in a goal-directed-like manner (natural processes enfold as if they were goal directed).
This conceptualization is adequate when bottom-up launching of a suicide action is concerned.
Counseling Implications for Assessment of Suicidality
These findings have substantial implications for the practice of mental health counseling. We can
assume that conventional suicidality assessment is mostly suitable for those who link their life
processes to suicidal processes by a top-down link. Those who use bottom-up steering links
might be incorrectly assessed because their eminent suicidality cannot be identified in this way.
They are not aware of the urgency of their long-term suicidal intentions and their suicide attempt
arises in a way, which used to be called "spontaneous."
Mental health counselors often question whether a client's denial of suicidal intention is
trustworthy. However, asking about denial of suicidal intentions may not be very helpful in
clients who will apply bottom-up steering in their launching of suicide actions. Consequently,
assessing suicidality after a suicide attempt would imply assessing the type of link used, and, if a
bottom-up link were found, then it would be important to clarify in which processes it is
embedded. For example, if it were a part of an impulsive action organization, it could be a part of
a flashback based on traumatic experience; or it could be an emotionally anchored link; or it
could be accompanied by reduced executive faculties as the quotations above indicate. These
aspects would obviously have to be considered and addressed when suicide prevention is
considered. Suicide prevention, informed by this suggestion, would have to relate to more than to
the question of suicide intention. It would have to address suicide processes in their long, middle
and short-term organization. We would have to ask about a suicide career, suicide projects and
suicide actions. We would have to make sure that neither the goals of the suicide career nor the
goals of the suicide project or of the suicide action are eminent. Further, in all three types of
processes we would have to ask not only about the suicide goals (the level of socially meaningful
processes), but also about their functional order. Questions like "is there anything in this liferelated project that could be functionally served by a suicide action?" or "is there anything in this
life-related action which could be facilitated by a suicide action step?" would have to be
answered. Further, we would have to deal with the issue of whether some of the physically
defined features of the life-related career, life-related projects and life-related actions which are
described at the lowest level of the action organization could be used for suicide actions or
suicide actions steps. At the same time, we would always have to keep in mind that individual as
well as joint processes are considered as actions and other processes could be individual or joint
processes. Equally, the life-oriented system processes, individual and joint, would have to be
targeted intensively in the above-mentioned forms and at each level with the aim of assessing
their strength. In addition, as presented in this paper, the links would have to be identified and
addressed. While attending to the top-down steered links would follow the steps suitable for
monitoring goal-directed processes, the bottom-up steered links would have to be identified as
described above and dealt with according to their roots which could consist of traumatic
experiences.
Further Research
14
We have shown in this study that suicide-related processes and the links between them and lifedirected processes can be distinct and separate systems. Future research can clarify the nature of
these links. The conception of top-down links and the goal-directed organization of suicide might
call for a different research paradigm. The identification of bottom-up links can help to radically
improve the strength of many correlation or regression studies designed to analyze some specific
conditions of suicide, because such designs are particularly suited for identifying the conditions
of bottom-up links.
Limitations
This qualitative study is far from providing an authoritative and conclusive statement on suicide
processes. It deals with one aspect of these processes. We have addressed other aspects of suicide
processes in a series of publications (Valach, Scheidegger, Michel, Young, & Dey, in press,
Valach, Young, & Michel, 2004, Michel, Dey, Stadler, & Valach, 2004, Valach, Michel, Dey, &
Young, 2002, Valach, Michel, Young, & Dey, 2002, Young & Valach, 2002, Michel,
Maltsberger, Jobes, Leenaars, Orbach, Stadler, Dey, Young, & Valach, 2002, Michel & Valach,
2001, Michel & Valach, 1997) in order to develop a more comprehensive picture. The findings in
this study are based on a limited number of research participants and the sample may not be
representative. For example, one would expect more women and immigrants to be represented in
this group based on the demographic characteristics of the area where the research was
conducted. Our group reflected the population treated in a hospital setting for the medical
consequences of their suicide attempt. In addition, although we did ask in detail about the suicide
process as part of the clients' narratives, we did not attempt to clarify in the interview whether the
indicated link was bottom-up or top-down. Rather, we relied on the research participants' reports.
Thus, it is possible to argue that a different way of questioning may have led to slightly different
distribution of the results.
CONCLUSION
We described suicide processes as goal directed processes comparable to the organization of life
related processes in short-terms actions, midterm projects and long-term career. Further, we
argued that the links between life-related processes and suicide processes can often be understood
as goal-directed processes. They sometimes are organized in the way as it is described by
dynamic system theory which addresses the bottom-up steering in contrast to top-down steering
outlined in the theory of goal directed action. There also is a mixed form containing both forms
of steering.
The way we conceptualized the links between suicide related and life related systems is fairly
simplified to suit the format of this brief report. The merging of bottom-up and top-down
processes in any human action and behavior is more complex than could be specified in this
empirical study. Finally making sense of one's own and others' behavior in terms of goal
processes is a highly culturally specific feature anchored to some degree in social representations.
Thus, the questions examined here should be re-examined in other socially and culturally defined
groups.
REFERENCES
15
Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the Self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in
the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3, 201-216.
Maltsberger, J. T. (2004). The descent into suicide. International Journal of Psychoanalysis 85, 3,
653-67.
Maris, R. W. (1981). Pathways to suicide: A survey of self-destructive behaviors. Baltimore:
Johns Hopkins University Press.
Michel, K., Dey, P., Stadler, K., & Valach, L. (2004). Therapist sensitivity towards emotional
life-career issues and the working alliance with suicide attempters. Archives of Suicide Research,
8, 203-213.
Michel, K., Maltsberger, J. T., Jobes, D. A., Leenaars, A. A., Orbach, I., Stadler, K., Dey, P.,
Young, R. A., Valach, L. (2002). Discovering the truth in attempted suicide. American Journal of
Psychotherapy, 56, 3, 424-437.
Michel, K. & Valach, L. (1997). Suicide as goal-directed action. Archives of Suicide Research 3,
213-221.
Michel, K. & Valach, L. (2001). Suicide as goal-directed action. In Ed. K. van Heeringen (Ed.),
Understanding Suicidal Behaviour: the Suicidal Process Approach to Research and Treatment
(pp. 230-254). Chichester: Wiley & Sons.
Simon, T. R., Swann, A. C., Powell, K. E., Potter, L. B., Kresnow, M.-j., & O'Carroll, P. W.
(2001). Characteristics of impulsive suicide attempts and attempters. Suicide and LifeThreatening Behavior, 32, (supplement), 49-59.
Valach, L. Scheidegger, G., Michel, K., Young, R. A., & Dey, P. (2006). Suicidal ideation,
theory of mind and action. In J. R. Rogers (Ed.), (2006). Suicide 2005: Proceeding of the 38th
Annual Conference of the American Association of Suicidology Washington, D.C.: American
Association of Suicidology.
Valach, L., Young, R. A., & Michel, K. (2004). Counselling for vocational career and life
projects: Action conceptualization. In Swiss Association for Educational and Vocational
Guidance SVB-ASOSP (Ed.), Quality development ill vocational counseling and training.
International conference 2003: Final report (CD). Berne: Editor.
Valach, L., Michel, K., Dey, P., & Young, R. A. (2002). Self-confrontation interview with
suicide attempters. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 15, 1, 1-22.
Valach, L., Michel, K., Young, R. A., & Dey, P. (2002). Attempted suicide stories: suicide
career, suicide project, and suicide action. In L. Valach, R. A. Young, & M. J. Lynam (2002).
16
Action theory. A primer for applied research in the social sciences (pp. 153-171). Westport, CT:
Praeger.
Valach, L., Young, R. A., & Lynam, M. J. (2002). Action theory. A primer for applied research
in the social sciences. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Vallacher, R. R. & Nowak, A. (Eds.), (1994). Dynamical systems in social psychology. San
Diego: Academic Press.
Vallacher, R. R. & Nowak, A. (1997). The emergence of dynamical social psychology.
Psychological Inquiry, 8, 73-99.
Williams, C., Davidson, J., & Montgomery, I. (1980). Impulsive suicide behavior. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 36, 90-94.
Young, R. A. & Valach, L. (2002). The self-confrontation interview in suicide research,
intervention, and prevention. Lifenotes, 7, 1, 12, 14.
Correspondence may be addressed to: Valach, Ladislav, PhD, Psychopathology, Faculty of
Philosophy, University of Zurich, Clinic Schloessli, 8618 Oetwil am See, Switzerland, Tel.: ++41
1 929 85 06, Fax: ++4 11 929 85 20. E-mail
[email protected]
COPYRIGHT 2006 American Mental Health Counselors Association
Figure 1 Top-down steering in a goal directed action and bottom-up steering in dynamic systems
action GOAL (to die)
ATTRACTOR (to die)
steers
is drawn into
action ELEMENTS of the suicide
action
BEHAVIOR
time
Top-down steering
Bottom-up steering
time