Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
2014
…
4 pages
1 file
Reading is perhaps one of the most important skills taught in schools. Its use is not confined to the curricular area under which it is taught, namely language(s), but it is used right across the school curriculum. Consequently, the success in reading has serious implications for other subjects as well. Despite the obvious significance of reading, it continues to be an area of concern especially in the primary grades. Largely, children are taught reading skills which are often not lasting, and do not result in meaningful reading. Contemporary research in reading conceptualizes it as essentially a meaning-making process
IRA E-ssentials, 2014
is the William Moran Distinguished Professor of Literacy and Reading at the University of North Carolina at Greensboro. Dr. Duffy spent 25 years teaching teachers how to teach reading and conducting research on classroom reading instruction at Michigan State University, where he was a Senior Researcher in the Institute for Research on Teaching and where he holds the rank of Professor Emeritus. He is also a former elementary and middle school teacher. A past President of the National Reading Conference and a member of the Reading Hall of Fame, Dr. Duffy has worked with teachers and children across the United States and overseas, has written and edited several books on reading instruction, and has published over 150 articles and research studies, with an emphasis on explicit teaching and teacher development.
Literacy, 1985
A central concern of researchers and teachers of reading has been the process by which the child forms an association between written and spoken language. It has however been recognised that a wide variety of motivational, linguistic, cognitive and perceptual factors may contribute to a child's success in mastering the art of reading. The Aston Index supplies the following fist of factors which may influence learning: general health, birth history, family Iaterality, social background, emotional climate at home and level of spoken language. This article is concerned with one such area which has been relatively unexplored: the development by the child of what may be called 'reading-associated concepts'. The following areas of knowledge can usefully be combined under this heading: (a) Knowledge that general terms are used by the teacher in a narrow and specific sense when talking about reading. For example, 'above', as in 'look at the words above the picture'. The words are not hanging in the air above the picture! (b) Knowledge of a new terminology associated with reading (e.g. number, word, sentence). (c) Knowledge about what reading actually is. The child must be aware that print can be articulated, that the continuous Bow of spoken language breaks down into words which are represented by discrete groups of markings on the page. (d) Knowledge of the arbitrary conventions of printed English. For example, print is read from left to right along each line and pages are turned from right to IeA.
The diversity of learners is one among the needs that every classroom teacher should address to ensure that appropriate learning goals are attained by everyone. One of the most prevalent concerns of educators these days is the varied learning styles of learners-that learners have different convenient ways of learning that influence their abilities to understand their lessons.
American Cancer Society eBooks, 2012
2013
Research in cognitive science and neuroscience has made enormous progress toward understanding skilled reading, the acquisition of reading skill, the brain bases of reading, the causes of developmental reading impairments and how such impairments can be treated. My question is: if the science is so good, why do so many people read so poorly? I mainly focus on the United States, which fares poorly on cross-national comparisons of literacy, with about 25-30% of the population exhibiting literacy skills that are low by standard metrics. I consider three possible contributing factors, all of which turn on issues concerning the relationships between written and spoken language: the fact that English has a deep alphabetic orthography; how reading is taught; and the impact of linguistic variability as manifested in the Black-White "achievement gap." I conclude that there are opportunities to increase literacy levels by making better use of what we have learned about reading and language but also institutional obstacles and understudied issues for which more evidence is badly needed. 332 SEIDENBERG of such impairments. We have computational models that specify the mechanisms that underlie basic reading skills, how children acquire them, and how differences in experience (with spoken language and reading) and individual differences (in learning and memory capacities, motivation and other factors) result in varied reading outcomes. This vast research base has led to the development of intervention and remediation methods that can reliably help many children who need it. Researchers disagree about many details-it's science, not the Ten Commandments-but there is remarkable consensus about the basic theory of how reading works and the causes of reading successes and failures (for reviews, see Rayner et al.
Reading Research Quarterly
Reading: An Orchestration of Cues
Sonika Kaushik
The Language Cueing System Goodman (1996) emphasizes on the importance of the harmonious functioning of the four language cueing systems-semantics, syntax, graphophonics and pragmatics. Successful or meaningful reading happens when all four systems function in tandem with each other. Semantics refers to the meaning aspect of a language system; syntax refers to the sentence structure or word-order in a particular language. For instance, in Hindi, the verb always follows the object, unlike English where the verb precedes the object. Graphophonics refers to the specific symbol-sound association in a language. For example, the sound /k/ in Hindi can be associated with only one symbol or letter. Pragmatics is about the use of language in specific ways in certain contexts. An overreliance on just one or two cueing systems or a complete neglect of one or more systems by the reader can seriously affect reading and the meaning. The centrality of meaning in reading cannot be denied or compromised.
Miscue analysis, a technique for analysing reading, allows one to 'peep' into the ways in which a reader uses the four cueing systems. This technique was developed by Kenneth Goodman (Rhodes and Shanklin, 1993). Goodman (1996) coined the term miscue to refer to what are commonly perceived as errors while reading. This was to avoid the negative connotations of the word error. The technique looks at the 'errors' made by children during reading very favourably, and examines why and how the reader has 'deviated' from the text. Interestingly, this analytical process reveals that all kinds of readers make miscues while reading.
The analysis in the following section will reveal the complex ways in which the four systems function simultaneously to generate meaning. This multiple cueing system of language has been used as the framework for examining the reading of two children. 'Good' and 'poor' readers are terms routinely used in research literature. However, the intention is not to 'label' children but only to highlight the sharp qualitative differences in their reading.
Miscues: Nature of Reading
Two Grade III children-one a struggling reader and the other a good reader-were given an unfamiliar story, Dehati ki gaay (Shankar, 1999), to read aloud. Their reading was recorded, and later analysed to study the nature of miscues, or deviations they were making from the original text. The analysis revealed the qualitative differences in their miscues and consequently reading. A section of the text read by the children will be discussed here.
Consider Text A-the reading sample of the poor reader.The reader makes three miscues in the first sentence (S1). The first miscue is the splitting of the word gaon (village) into ga and v; the nasal sound is ignored completely. He does not bother to blend the parts of the split word and moves ahead. Soon, in the same sentence (S1), he misreads the word gaay (cow) as gaya (went/the name of a place) and thi (auxiliary verb 'was', in this context referring to a female) as tha (changing the referent to male). Locally, tha seems in consonance with gaya. Gaya and tha are words in Hindi, and may look visually similar to the words they have replaced. So, at the level of individual words, one may choose to overlook these 'slips'. But on examining the syntax of the whole sentence as read by the child, the resulting meaning does not seem very satisfactory. The second sentence (S2) is riddled with miscues but they are of a self-correcting nature. The reader skips the second word, pratidin (everyday), splits the word paanch (five) into paan and ch and blends it to sound out paanch, this time not missing the nasal sound-the anusvar. A similar splitting and blending takes place with the word kilo. After reading doodh (milk), the reader reads diya (gave) in place of deti (gives), corrects it, and reads hai (is) in place of thi (was). Considering the deletion of the word pratidin and the substitution with the word hai, the resulting sentence makes complete meaning even though the tense of the second sentence (S2) has been changed and is not in keeping with the first one. It is difficult to explain this miscue hai because visually, the text does not lead to it. In the third sentence (S3), baich (to sell) is read as pahun and cha which, if blended together, would make pahuncha (to bring it, here). This makes sense until one reaches the word use(that) which is read as usse, and the sentence structure begins to crumble at the word paise (money). Again at the end of the sentence (S3), the use of the plural form of the word rehna (to live) as rehete thay (changes the referent from singular to plural) brings incongruence between the singular subject, the dehati (the villager), and the substituted word usse. In the fourth sentence (S4), the word gaon is repeated, and the child reads it differently from the way he read it in the first sentence. This time, the anusvar stays and the long vowel \a\ is ignored. He progresses, and when he encounters the word vivah (wedding), his reading starts to falter. He makes two attempts at reading the word, the first time by splitting it, and then by blending it or trying to read the whole word. In both cases, he creates non-words. Again, he changes the tense when he reads hai in place of ho, and makes unsuccessful attempts at sounding out and blending the word raha, and finally ends with th for tha. In the fifth sentence (S5), he reads in a similar manner. Interestingly, he stumbles over words he had earlier read with ease in the first few sentences. dehati and doodh are two such words. In S5 of Text A he makes three attempts at reading the word vivah, but does not meet with success.
An analysis of his reading shows clearly that the most commonly used reading strategy displayed in his reading is that of sounding out the constituent sounds in a word, and then trying to blend them together. More often than not, the use of this strategy does not lead him to the word in the text. Undeterred, he either leaves it at the non-word that has been created, or reads a form of the word which is acceptable in Hindi but syntactically inappropriate, and moves on. Sakeg and uhe in S5 and veeh in S4 of Text A are a few examples of such non-words. The child knows the letters and matras and he is using this knowledge in isolation to read. It seems strange that he is able to use this knowledge effectively in some places, and in other places he does not seem to be able to use it. One can speculate that the sole use of graphophonics cannot go a long way in supporting reading. In this case, the child is not actively bringing his knowledge of the sentence structure in Hindi to support his knowledge of the Hindi alphabet. More importantly, he does not seem to know that one reads for meaning, and so makes no attempt to preserve the global meaning of the text. Evidently, he is creating pockets of coherent phrases in some places, and in other places is completely abandoning the meaning and indiscriminately sounding out words. He is not worried about carrying forward or building on the meaning that has been created in the preceding sentences. Moreover, narratives tend to be in the past tense, and active use of the knowledge of this often used genre could have guided him to not switch tense from one sentence to the other.
The reading of the same section of the text by the good reader resulted in three miscues. In the second sentence (S2) of Text B-the reading sample of the good reader, the child inserts the word ek after doodh, but goes back and repeats the stretch from doodh, this time without the earlier inserted word, ek. Like the poor reader, he also stumbles over the word vivah in the fourth sentence (S4), perhaps because it is not used as commonly as its synonym, shaadi. He makes two attempts, the first one being vaan which does not occur in the word at all, and then viha which gives him a clue about the word, and finally he reads it correctly. Lastly, there seems to be tentativeness in reading the word sakega in the fifth sentence (S5). This child is evidently monitoring his reading, and is alert to a changed and inappropriate syntax when he makes the insertion with ek and instantly goes back and rectifies it. He is simultaneously attentive to the sentence structure of the story and the meaning that is being created, and makes selective use of the strategy of focusing on the letters in a word.
The word vivah proves to be interesting in analysing the reading of the two children. The poor reader makes more attempts to read it, and is phonologically closer to the word in his attempts. He comes as close as vivh but does not use his knowledge of the world and steadfastly focuses only on the splitting and blending of the word. The reading of the preceding sentences does not support him because a coherent meaning does not emerge out of his reading. Therefore, his reading increasingly collapses. The other child consistently monitors his reading for meaning, and makes deviations or miscues which keep the meaning intact.
Conclusion
Miscue analysis urges us to listen to what children are telling us about their reading capabilities and the kind of help they need. Conceptualization of reading as an exercise in decoding or simply sounding out words, limits our ability as teachers to support children's reading. More often than not, a child struggling with reading is asked to learn his letters and matras well (Kaushik, 2004). Such a suggestion overlooks the real complexity of the reading process. It also fails to communicate to the child that she has to actively integrate all the knowledge she has about the language-its sentence structure, the meaning of words, the letter-sound associations-towards one central objective of reading to construct meaning. The role that we have chosen for ourselves, one of correcting children and eliminating errors while reading needs to be redefined (Owocki and Y. Goodman, 2002).
Original Text -
Војноисторијски гласник 2, 2023
Osmanli Istanbulu. IX. Uluslararası Osmanlı İstanbulu Sempozyumu, Bildiriler. Feridun M. Emecen, Ali Akyıldız (ed.), 2024
Chemistry of Materials, 1992
Prometeica - Revista de Filosofía y Ciencias
Revista de Estudos Institucionais, 2021
Dissertationes Archaeologicae, 2021
International Journal of Social Sciences Perspectives, 2020
مدل سازی در مهندسی, 2018
De colonias a estados nacionales: independencias y descolonización en América y el mundo en los siglos XIX y XX, 2019
Renaissance Studies, 2019
Tecnologías digitales y transformaciones sociales Desigualdades y los desafíos en el contexto latinoamericano actual, 2020
American Journal of Social and Management Sciences, 2012
Documenting the AUC Response to COVID-19 Collection, 2020
NEELIMA SONI, 2025
Journal of educational research and practice, 2022
MedPulse International Journal of Anesthesology, 2019
Engineering review
AIChE Journal, 2014
Academia Mental Health and Well-Being, 2024