Journal of Abnormal Psychology
2005, Vol. 114, No. 4, 505–521
Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association
0021-843X/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.505
Temperament as a Unifying Basis for Personality and Psychopathology
Lee Anna Clark
University of Iowa
Personality and psychopathology long have been viewed as related domains, but the precise nature of
their relations remains unclear. Through most of the 20th century, they were studied as separate fields;
within psychopathology, clinical syndromes were separated from personality disorders in 1980. This
division led to the revelation of substantial overlap among disorders both within and across axes and to
the joint study of normal and abnormal personality. The author reviews these literatures and proposes an
integrative framework to explain personality–psychopathology relations: Three broad, innate temperament dimensions—negative affectivity, positive affectivity, and disinhibition— differentiate through both
biologically and environmentally based developmental processes into a hierarchical personality trait
structure and, at their extremes, are risk factors (diatheses) for psychopathology, especially given adverse
life experiences (stress).
Keywords: temperament, psychopathology, personality, taxonomy, DSM–V
The first model of personality–psychopathology relations may
have been the doctrine of the four humors, attributed to Hippocrates and Galen (Maher & Maher, 1994). The balance of these
four humors— blood (sanguis), phlegm, bile (choler), and black
bile (melancholer)— determined one’s temperament: sanguine,
phlegmatic, choleric, or melancholic, respectively. Temperament,
in turn, determined vulnerability to illness, and a humoral imbalance led to physical or mental illness. Balance—and thus health—
was restored by such techniques as bleeding and purging.
With the advent of modern science, which in psychology began
in the late 19th century, theories of personality–psychopathology
relations were based on Darwinian concepts (Maher & Maher,
1994). One basic theme was that all types of mental illness
reflected a general character deficiency that was genetically based,
and the specific forms of which reflected biological and/or personal development. Freud’s well-known theory of the stages of
character development, each with its associated psychopathology,
is the most elaborate and influential of these views.
In the early 20th century, Kraepelin posited that personality
disorders were formes frustes of the major psychoses. Kretschmer
went a step further, hypothesizing a single continuum from personality (schizothyme) through personality disorder (schizoid) to
clinical syndrome (schizophrenia; Livesley, Schroeder, Jackson, &
Jang, 1994). Interestingly, Pavlovian conditioning theorists rejuvenated the ancient four-humoral theory, substituting variation in
neuronal responses for the strength and balance of humors (Maher
& Maher, 1994), and Eysenck explicitly related his three-factor
personality model to Pavlovian concepts. Whereas Eysenck’s dimensions still play a major role in the field, the link to conditioning
theories, for the most part, does not.
This precis suggests that personality and psychopathology have
been studied in relation to each other since antiquity, but that is
only half the story. The other half is their division, both between
psychiatry (with its major focus on abnormal processes) and psychology (which generally emphasizes the study of normal functioning) and also within each field. One sign of the split within
psychology was the separation in 1965 of the Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology into the Journal of Abnormal Psychology
and the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
The Advent of the DSM: An Atheoretical Model
In mid-20th century, psychiatry systematized its accumulated
knowledge of psychopathology in the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–I, DSM–II; American Psychiatric Association, 1952, 1968). In these DSMs, personality pathology was considered alongside other disorders with little attention
to their potential interrelations. However, with the advent of DSM–
III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), personality disorders were given their own “Axis II,” which reopened the door for
the systematic study of the two domains in relation to each other.
The DSMs were intended as descriptive documents to aid both
research and treatment planning. Whereas prevailing theories no
doubt influenced each of the DSMs, theory per se was not a
guiding principle in their development, and DSM–III explicitly
declared its atheoretical intent. However, the creation of separate
axes clearly indicated that personality disorders were considered a
distinct type of psychopathology, which also invited study of their
interrelation.
A notable phenomenon of the successive DSMs was the increase
in diagnoses, as well as in subtypes and specifiers to address
within-diagnosis heterogeneity (Watson, 2003). Some of the diagnostic increase was due to new disorders (e.g., cyclothymia), but a
substantial proportion resulted from subdivison (e.g., bipolar disorder into Bipolar I and Bipolar II disorders). These changes also
were based largely on descriptive studies rather than theory-driven
research.
Virtually without offering either a theoretical rationale or empirical justification, DSM–III included hierarchical exclusion rules
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lee Anna
Clark, Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, 111 Jessup Hall,
Iowa City, IA 52242-1316. E-mail:
[email protected]
505
CLARK
506
affecting 60% of all disorders (Boyd et al., 1984), prohibiting
Diagnosis A if it was “due to” Diagnosis B. Both clinicians and
researchers chafed at these strictures because (a) they prevented
full diagnostic representation of patients’ symptom sets, (b) they
made it difficult to study disorders lower in the hierarchy, and (c)
the “due to” rule was difficult to apply in that it required a
subjective and inferential judgment regarding causal primacy of
the relevant disorders in the absence of empirical data or clear
guidance from the DSM.1 Consequently, some adopted the more
straightforward rule of simply not diagnosing A if B was present,
whereas others, researchers in particular, ignored the rules in order
to study their effects. In response to these twin pressures, the
exclusion rules were largely removed from DSM–III–R (American
Psychiatric Association, 1987).
Diagnostic Comorbidity
The most obvious and immediate effect of removing the exclusion rules was the well-known revelation of a high degree of
diagnostic co-occurrence both within and across axes, which presented a challenge for a taxonomy in which, ideally, diagnoses are
distinct entities. The usual term in the literature for this phenomenon is comorbidity, but as Lilienfeld, Waldman, and Israel (1994)
discussed, there are problems with that term, including that it does
not distinguish between mere overlap and statistical covariation.
Nonetheless, I will use the term comorbidity because of its near
universal usage in the literature, begging the readers’ understanding that in some places it may be imprecise.2
Clark, Watson, and Reynolds (1995) reviewed the emerging
comorbidity literature and discussed various factors that affect
comorbidity rates, such as time frame (i.e., concurrent or lifetime
diagnoses), range of inclusion (i.e., only clear cases or also subclinical varieties), diagnostic method (i.e., paper-and-pencil selfreport or interview), and sample type (e.g., clinic or community
sample). Further, Mineka, Watson, and Clark (1998) noted that
comorbidity is increased by excessive diagnostic splitting and
when disorders share criteria. Others (e.g., Caron & Rutter, 1991;
Klein & Riso, 1993; Lilienfeld, 2003; Maser & Cloninger, 1990;
Meehl, 2001) also have examined a wide range of issues important
in comorbidity, including additional methodological factors that
affect the estimation of comorbidity rates.
Although all of these issues pose challenges to diagnostic validity (and a full discussion of them falls beyond the scope of this
article), nonetheless, importantly, comorbidity cannot be fully
accounted for methodologically (e.g., see Angold, Costello, &
Erkanli, 1999). That is, even after taking various methodological
reasons into account, much—perhaps most— comorbidity remains
notably higher than would be expected by chance based on diagnostic prevalence rates.
Widiger and Clark (2000) reviewed more recent literature that
further explores the bases of comorbidity and challenges the assumption of a distinct boundary between normal and pathological
behavior. Focusing primarily on the anxiety and depressive disorders, they found evidence supporting both genetic and environmental influences on comorbidity but noted that these phenomena
were not limited to those diagnoses, pointing, for example, also to
literatures exploring “the high degree of overlap among personality disorders and between the personality disorders and Axis I
disorders” (p. 954). Further, they noted the emergence of studies
indicating the presence of broad latent factors underlying certain
sets of comorbid disorders.
Insofar as the Widiger and Clark (2000) review indicates that
these various literatures are not entirely distinct, but have overlapping elements, it points to the possibility of a more comprehensive
integration. Suggesting the form that this broader integration might
take is the primary purpose of this article. To that end, I first
examine briefly the literatures on comorbidity among Axis I disorders, among Axis II disorders, and across the axes. I then
examine the literatures concerning overlap of normal-range personality with first Axis II and then Axis I disorders. In both cases,
I focus on recent trends and important issues that these literatures
raise. I then propose an overarching framework to integrate the
broad domains of personality and psychopathology and the various
literatures they subsume. I present an initial view of the form and
structure of this framework and consider the various challenges
that such a framework needs to address.
Etiological Models
In considering the broad literature on diagnostic comorbidity, it
is useful to keep in mind the four major etiological models of
comorbidity that have been proposed. These models fall into two
major types based on whether they attribute causal significance to
temporal order (for discussions, see Maser & Cloninger, 1990;
Lilienfeld et al., 1994; Mineka et al., 1998). The predisposition or
vulnerability model postulates that a prior disorder increases the
likelihood that a person will experience a later onset disorder,
whereas the pathoplasty model posits that a prior disorder influences the severity, course, or treatment response of a later onset
disorder. The other two models posit that comorbid disorders
reflect the same underlying process or etiological factor, so that
neither can be viewed as clearly causing the other. The common/
shared cause, liability, or factor (hereafter, I shall use the term
shared factor) model hypothesizes a shared genetic diathesis (e.g.,
Carey & DiLalla, 1994), whereas the spectrum model proposes
underlying continua that extend from normality to mild, moderate,
and severe psychopathology (e.g., social phobia and avoidant PD
overlap because the latter is a more severe form of the former).
It is important to note that these models are not mutually
exclusive and each may be partially correct or simply incomplete.
For example, the predisposition and pathoplasty models together
would predict that an earlier onset disorder provides an initial
vulnerability to a later onset disorder and, moreover, the presence
of the earlier onset disorder affects the individual’s response to the
later onset disorder, thereby influencing severity, course, or treatment response. I return to a fuller consideration of these models
subsequently.
Comorbidity Within Axes
Comorbidity among Axis I disorders. To examine within-Axis
I comorbidity, I ran a PsycINFO search for the term comorbidity,
limiting the focus to depression, anxiety, and substance abuse or
substance dependence, and specifically excluding articles with
1
2
I thank an anonymous reviewer for contributing to this last point.
I thank an anonymous reviewer for raising this point.
SPECIAL SECTION: TEMPERAMENT AS THE UNIFYING BASIS
keywords personality or Axis II; this yielded over 1,600 articles in
English on human subjects from 2000 to early 2004. A scan of the
articles revealed that although not all of them directly addressed
within-Axis I comorbidity, a substantial proportion did. Clearly,
the issue is still of major interest, and I identified three trends in
this recent literature: investigations of diagnostic sequencing (bidirectionality appears to be the norm; e.g., Hettema, Prescott, &
Kendler, 2003; Stice, Burton, & Shaw, 2004), of less prevalent
disorders (e.g., body dysmorphic disorder; Gustad & Phillips,
2003), and, more generally, of a wider variety of pairs of disorders
(e.g., alcohol abuse and bipolar disorder, obsessive– compulsive
disorder and schizophrenia). It is noteworthy that the latter two
topics emerged despite the limitation of the literature search.
Comorbidity among Axis II disorders. The high degree of
overlap among Axis II personality disorders was well documented
almost by the time DSM–III–R was published in 1987 and certainly before DSM–IV in 1994. Widiger and Rogers’ (1989) literature review reported an average 85% of multiple Axis II disorders
in patient samples, and subsequent research has both confirmed
and elaborated this finding. Examples include (a) the examination
of different populations (e.g., community-dwelling adults; Zimmerman & Coryell, 1989), (b) the use of different types of assessment instruments (e.g., Oldham et al., 1992, used two different
structured interviews), and (c) the study of the phenomenon in
different cultures (Marinangeli et al., 2000, studied 156 Italian
inpatients). Thus, Axis II comorbidity rates are high (a) in both
patient and nonpatient samples, (b) when diagnosed with different
assessment instruments, and (c) across different Western cultures.
It is interesting to note that whereas the general literature on
personality disorders is increasing at about the same rate as the
broader scientific literature, few specific personality disorders
(with borderline PD the clearest exception, perhaps because of a
high-prevalence rate and social cost) have active literatures (Blashfield & Intoccia, 2000).3 The high within-Axis II comorbidity rate
may contribute to this slow growth because it casts doubt on the
validity of the diagnostic system. Unless the validity of specific
personality disorders can be established, research into them seems
fruitless. Thus, understanding within-Axis II comorbidity could
have a profound influence on the future of personality disorder
research.
Cross-Axis Comorbidity
As mentioned earlier, the separation of Axis II personality
disorders from Axis I clinical syndromes in DSM–III invited
research into relations between these broad types of psychopathology. In a review of issues that had emerged from the ensuing
research on Axis I–Axis II relations, Widiger and Shea (2001)
noted three different types of relations between pairs of comorbid
disorders: (1) spectrum (e.g., schizotypal PD with schizophrenia;
avoidant PD with social phobia), (2) cross-over (e.g., borderline
PD has features of both a mood disorder and a personality disorder), and (3) overlapping (e.g., substance abuse disorders share
criteria with antisocial PD). They also discussed potential solutions
to the problems these issues pose (e.g., converting to a dimensional
format) but noted difficulties with each proposed solution. They
recommended continued investigation into whether psychopathology is fundamentally categorical or dimensional. That is, they
recognized that understanding the basic nature and structure of
507
psychopathology is a key element in resolving the problems posed
by comorbidity.
It is interesting to note that perhaps because of the fundamental
questions it poses, cross-axis comorbidity appears to have captured
more research attention than has within-axis comorbidity. Initially,
most studies were little more than descriptions of comorbidity
rates between two diagnoses. Whereas this type of research was
important to establish the phenomenon, it has reached the end of its
usefulness (Tyrer, Gunderson, Lyons, & Tohen, 1997), and there
should be a moratorium on purely and simply descriptive studies
of comorbidity between two disorders.
What are beginning to emerge instead, and what we now need
more of, are investigations that (a) compare comorbidity rates
across related disorders (e.g., differential comorbidity of PDs with
various anxiety or substance use disorders); (b) directly test one or
more causal models of comorbidity; (c) examine patterns of comorbidity through meta-analyses of multiple studies, inclusion of
variables other than diagnoses, or large-scale studies of phenotypic
or genotypic structure; and (d) examine the effect of measurement
method, instrument, or criterion set on comorbidity rates. Many
individual studies exist, but an integrative review of findings is
lacking and would greatly benefit the field. I illustrate each of
these themes briefly.
Brieger, Ehrt, and Marneros (2003) provide a meta-analysis of
personality disorders in unipolar and bipolar mood disorder patients. Approximately half of the patients with either type of mood
disorder had a comorbid personality disorder, with highly similar
patterns of relations with specific PDs. These data suggest that if
Axis I–Axis II comorbidity is due to a shared liability factor, then,
in the case of mood disorders, this factor likely is also shared
between unipolar and bipolar disorders. By contrast, meta-analysis
of eating disorders and personality disorder found a nearly twofold
difference in personality disorder comorbidity with bulimia (61%)
versus mixed samples of eating disorder patients (34%). Thus,
although one or more general vulnerability factors for psychopathology may cross Axis I and II, general factors alone cannot
explain all the findings.
Klein and Schwartz (2002) tested four models of relations
between dysthymia and borderline PD over a 5-year period and
found that a shared-factor model best fit the data. As part of the
same study, Klein (2003) found that self and informant reports of
personality disorder at baseline— despite their modest correlation
3
An anonymous reviewer questioned the validity of the Blashfield and
Intoccia (2000) analysis and cited several other personality disorders
whose literatures he or she thought were more developed, so I ran a series
of PsycINFO searches on these disorders, including borderline, antisocial,
narcissistic, dependent, and obsessive– compulsive personality disorder
(PD) in both keyword and title for the periods 1985–1991, 1992–1997, and
1998 –2003 and found general support for Blashfield and Intoccia’s (2000)
conclusions. In the keyword searches, all six disorders showed roughly
linear increases across the three periods (if anything, growth was slower
between the latter two), but only borderline and antisocial PD reached over
50 articles a year in any period. In the title search, the curves were flat and
indicated 5 or fewer articles per year for narcissistic, dependent, and
obsessive– compulsive PD (which suggests that Blashfield & Intoccia,
2000 did a title search), whereas the other three showed increases that
ranged from slight (schizotypal PD from 4 to 9 articles per year) to mild
(antisocial PD from 5 to 17) and marked (borderline PD from 40 to 74).
508
CLARK
( ⫽ .24)— equally predicted depressive symptoms 7.5 years later
( ⫽ .24 to .29, controlling for the other). It is interesting to note
that they also predicted future social functioning to the same
degree (see Kool, Dekker, Duijsens, de Jonghe, de Jong, &
Schouws, 2000, for a related finding). Thus, a shared factor may
underlie not only personality disorder and depressive symptoms
but also social functioning.
Comorbidity between substance abuse and personality disorder
has attracted a great deal of interest. Comorbidity with antisocial
PD increases about twofold (approximately 30% to 60%) from
mild to severe drug abuse/dependence (Flynn, Craddock, Luckey,
Hubbard, & Dunteman, 1996). Other personality disorders also are
prevalent, although DSM–III–R and DSM–IV criteria yield quite
different comorbidity rates for a few disorders even in the same
sample (e.g., Poling et al., 1999), raising further concerns about the
validity of specific Axis II disorders.
A special section on borderline PD in a 2002 issue of Biological
Psychiatry illustrates the attention that borderline PD has garnered
in recent years. Skodol, Gunderson, et al. (2002) reviewed 15
studies representing over 1,500 individuals and reported that the
Axis I–Axis II overlap for “any PD” averaged 46% and for
borderline PD 22%, with borderline PD accounting for almost half
(47%) of those with any PD. Again, not only comorbidity but the
variability in comorbidity rates are explanatory challenges.
Several studies have examined patterns of comorbidity through
large-scale studies of phenotypic and/or genetic structure (Kendler,
Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003; Krueger, 1999; Krueger, Caspi,
Moffitt, & Silva, 1998; Slade, cited in Watson, 2005; Vollebergh
et al., 2001), revealing a remarkably consistent structure: A hierarchical model with two broad factors (externalizing and internalizing), with the externalizing factor consisting of substance dependence and either conduct disorder or antisocial PD, and with the
internalizing factor having two subfactors, “misery” (comprising
generalized anxiety disorder [GAD] and depressive disorders) and
“fear” (comprising phobias and panic disorder). These studies
largely focused on Axis I disorders, but three of them included
antisocial PD or antisocial behavior, thus providing a link to Axis
II disorders.
In summary, a large number of studies document and continue
to explore the existence and nature of comorbidity across a wide
range of disparate disorders. The pervasiveness and extent of the
phenomenon is such that explanations of the overlap between pairs
of disorders in isolation of the “big picture” are unlikely to move
our understanding forward significantly. Before turning to studies
that begin to suggest what the big picture might look like, however, there are several aspects of the recent literature that deserve
comment.
The Twin Challenges of Severity and Heterogeneity
In their review of major issues in the diagnosis and classification
of psychopathology, Clark et al., (1995) noted that comorbidity
rates were higher in clinical versus community samples due, in
part, to the fact that those with two disorders could seek help for
either condition (Berkson, 1946). Berkson’s bias cannot account
fully for the association of comorbidity with severity of diagnosis,
however, and therefore Clark et al., (1995) suggested that severity
was “a crucial factor in course, treatment, and outcome” (p. 132).
The subsequent literature provides some support for this view:
Explanatory models of comorbidity often involve the concept of
severity, and empirical data reveal a parallelism between diagnostic comorbidity and level of social functioning. It seems likely that
if shared factors underlie not only different types of psychopathology but also social functioning, then they must represent broad,
general concepts such as severity or, more specifically, type and
level of functioning.
However, little has been done to elucidate the nature of severity,
most likely because its extreme nonspecificity renders it elusive.
That is, whereas severity may be invoked and have utility as an
abstract, theoretical, explanatory concept, its ultimate viability
depends on the extent to which it can be articulated in relation to
measurable variables (cf. Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). Moreover,
there are exceptions that will test the rule; specifically, there is
some evidence that conduct disorder is milder in children with
versus children without anxiety disorder (Walker et al., 1991).
In contrast to the broad nature of severity, which generally
increases overlap nonspecifically, heterogeneity within classes of
disorders is associated with differential comorbidity rates. Thus, at
the same time that models may need a very broad concept such as
severity to explain the pervasiveness and extent of comorbidity (as
well as relations with social functioning), the considerable heterogeneity not only within broad diagnostic categories but within
specific disorders also needs explanation.
Hierarchical Models
To capture the empirical reality of psychopathology, therefore,
explanatory models must account for both broad shared factors and
extensive heterogeneity within both diagnostic categories and single diagnoses. Such models have begun to emerge. An early
example is the tripartite model of anxiety and depressive disorders
(Clark & Watson, 1991), which was elaborated into an integrative
hierarchical model by Mineka et al. (1998). More recently, Klein
and Schwartz (2002) noted that whereas a shared-factor model
clearly provided the best fit to explain observed relations between
dysthymia and borderline PD over time, “the presence of significant unique determinants of both depressive symptoms and BPD
features is also noteworthy” (p. 532). However, their data did not
permit investigation into the nature of these determinants.
Kendler et al. (2003) reported not only a shared genetic risk for
internalizing and externalizing disorders but also specific genetic
risk factors for alcohol and drug dependence, a sharedenvironment factor for conduct disorder, two unique environment
factors (one for major depressive disorder, GAD, and alcohol
dependence, and the other for conduct disorder and antisocial PD)
and, finally, unique, disorder-specific environment factors for all
disorders. This rich model documents well the existence of both
shared and unique factors that have been observed in a range of
Axis I and Axis II disorders. Research must turn now to replicating
and, if replicable, explaining the nature of these factors.
Reemergence of Interest in Personality–Psychopathology
Relations
As stated earlier, through much of the 20th century psychologists and psychopathologists pursued their research independently
of each other. However, just as the creation of separate axes in
DSM–III invited study of Axis I–Axis II relations, the DSM–III
SPECIAL SECTION: TEMPERAMENT AS THE UNIFYING BASIS
definitions of personality disorders in terms of traits (American
Psychiatric Association, 1980, p. 305) did the same for relations
between personality and PD, and much research has been carried
out in this arena from a variety of perspectives.
Some researchers have sought primarily to clarify the maladaptive trait dimensions comprising personality disorder (e.g., Clark,
McEwen, Collard, & Hickok, 1993; Livesley, Jackson, & Schroeder, 1992), whereas others have focused specifically on relations
between normal personality traits and PD diagnoses (e.g., Costa &
Widiger, 2002). Whether abnormal personality dimensions are
continuous with those of normal personality or occupy their own
distinctive space also has garnered attention. Finally, and not
surprisingly given increasing awareness of cross-axis comorbidity,
whether and how normal and abnormal personality traits relate to
Axis I disorders also became a research target. I discuss each of
these subtopics in turn.
Etiological Models
Just as I introduced the review of diagnostic comorbidity with a
consideration of the major etiological models proposed to account
for the phenomena, it is instructive to do the same regarding
personality–psychopathology relations. Tellingly, the models proposed to explain diagnostic comorbidity overlap considerably with
those offered to explain personality–psychopathology relations
(e.g., Klein, Wonderlich, & Shea, 1993; Watson & Clark, 1995),
so I recap them here briefly, pointing out the parallels.
As mentioned earlier, the predisposition or vulnerability and the
pathoplasty model both postulate a temporal relation between two
conditions. In the former, the first condition is a risk factor for the
second, whereas in the latter, the first condition influences severity, course, or response to treatment. What is important to note here
is that the prior condition may be a temperament/personality factor
(i.e., a trait) as well as a disorder. Thus, for example, high trait
neuroticism, as well as anxiety disorders, may be a risk factor for
depression.
The pathoplasty model has two variants that are specific to
personality–psychopathology relations (scar and complication),
each of which posits that experience of psychopathology leads to
change in personality traits (Lewinsohn, Steinmetz, Larson, &
Franklin, 1981). The difference between the models is the degree
and scope of the effect. The scar model posits fundamental and
lasting trait-level change, so that following a major depressive
episode, for example, the persons’ trait neuroticism does not return
to its premorbid baseline. By contrast, the complication model
posits a “state” effect of disorder, with temperament/personality
reverting to baseline with remission.
Finally, recall that both the shared-factor and the spectrum
models posit that two conditions reflect the same underlying
process or etiological factor, so that neither can be viewed as
clearly causing the other. Again, the important point here is that
these may be overlapping disorder–trait pairs as well as comorbid
disorders.
Personality—Axis II Personality Disorder Relations
Maladaptive Personality Traits and Personality Disorder
Some proponents of dimensional approaches to personality disorder have developed instruments to assess maladaptive personal-
509
ity traits underlying the domain (e.g., Dimensional Assessment of
Personality Pathology [DAPP], Livesley & Jackson, in press;
Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality [SNAP],
Clark, 1993). The scales of these two inventories not only show
strong, systematic correlations with each other (Clark, Livesley,
Schroeder, & Irish, 1996) and with measures of the five-factor
model (FFM) of personality (Larstone, Jang, Livesley, Vernon, &
Wolf, 2002; Reynolds & Clark, 2001) but also with the DSM
personality disorders (Bagge & Trull, 2003; Hurt & Oltmanns,
2002; Morey et al., 2003; Pukrop, 2002; Reynolds & Clark, 2001).
Some overlap among these domains is due, no doubt, to shared
content—after all, the DSM personality disorders are defined as
“inflexible and maladaptive” personality traits (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 630), and development of both instruments began with a systematic study of DSM personality disorder
characteristics. Thereafter, however, each measure was created
independently, so the high degree of observed correlation was by
no means predetermined. Moreover, Livesley and colleagues’ data
have indicated that normal-range personality traits and those comprising personality disorder have a parallel structure, with genetic
and environmental components that are “remarkably similar to the
phenotypic factors” (Livesley, Jang, & Vernon, 1998, p. 941; see
also McCrae, Jang, Livesley, Riemann, & Angleitner, 2001), again
suggesting that the overlap is not simply explained by methodological factors.
Normal Personality Traits and Personality Disorder
The literature is replete with correlational reports of personality
trait measure X with personality disorder measure Y, and a comprehensive review of this literature is beyond the scope of this
article. However, the three topics that emerge most frequently—
relations between personality disorder and both the FFM of personality and Cloninger’s temperament– character model, and research on the construct of impulsivity—merit brief discussion.
Five-factor model. Saulsman and Page (2004) reviewed 15
samples from 12 studies published between 1990 and 1998 representing almost 2,900 individuals assessed with measures of the
FFM and the DSM–IV personality disorders. Weighted mean correlations were consistently patterned with the respective sets of
criteria comprising the 10 DSM Axis II disorders. Some significant
differences in correlational pattern were observed across sample
types (e.g., patient vs. nonpatient) or measurement method (e.g.,
interview vs. self-report), but the overall pattern was robust, and
subsequent studies have confirmed the existence of reliable, systematic relations between the FFM and DSM personality disorders
(e.g., Brieger, Sommer, Bloeink, & Marneros, 2000; Huprich,
2003; Morey et al., 2002; Reynolds & Clark, 2001).
Morey et al. (2002) cautioned that whereas FFM scores clearly
distinguish personality-disordered groups from community norms,
they largely fail to distinguish specific personality disorders from
each other. However, the lower order facets of the FFM domains
may provide more specific assessments of personality pathology,
similarly to the way lower order trait measures do, such as the
DAPP and SNAP (Reynolds & Clark, 2001).
Cloninger’s model. Personality–PD relations also have been
examined in the framework of Cloninger’s model, using the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ; Cloninger, 1987) or
its successor, the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI;
510
CLARK
Cloninger, Svrakic, & Pryzbeck, 1993). Support for Cloninger’s
hypotheses regarding specific dimension– disorder mappings is
mixed, with some studies finding some support (e.g., Griego,
Stewart, & Coolidge, 1999; Maggini, Ampollini, Marchesi, Gariboldi, & Cloninger, 2000; Mulder, Joyce, Sullivan, Bulik, &
Carter, 1999; Pukrop, 2002; Svrakic, Whitehead, Przybeck, &
Cloninger, 1993) and others not so (e.g., Ball, Tennen, Poling,
Kranzler, & Rounsaville, 1997; Guiterrez, Sangorrin, MartinSantos, Torres, & Torrens, 2002; Nagoshi, Walter, Muntaner, &
Haertzen, 1992).
A further cautionary note is also in order. O’Connor (2002)
investigated whether the structure of a broad array of widely used
personality and psychopathology measures differed across clinical
and nonclinical samples. The results overwhelmingly revealed
structural invariance across sample type for most measures,
whereas the TPQ and TCI were almost unique in having very
unstable factor structures both across and within sample types.
That is, the instruments have an unreliable factor structure even
when administered to samples drawn from the same broad population, raising concerns about the meaning of the instruments’
scores. Nevertheless, whereas specific TPQ and TCI findings may
not be reliable, the data clearly document the general point that
normal personality and personality disorder are interrelated.
Impulsivity. Impulsivity has long been regarded a key component of psychopathy and, later, antisocial PD. Interest in trait
impulsivity, including its biological basis, has a venerable history, stemming from early work by Eysenck (1967) and Gray
(1972). More recently, impulsivity has garnered research attention in part because it is considered a core trait of borderline PD
which, as noted earlier, has the largest current literature among
the PDs. Looper and Paris (2000) hypothesized that impulsivity
was the shared dimension underlying DSM Cluster B PDs
(antisocial, borderline, histrionic, and narcissistic) and that
these disorders were differentiated on the basis of severity of
impulsivity (antisocial and borderline are more severe than
histrionic and narcissistic), presence of other traits, gender, and
sociocultural influences.
Similarly, Krueger et al., (2002) provided a brief review of the
broader literature linking “disinhibitory personality traits” (p. 413),
such as impulsivity and novelty seeking, with antisocial PD, criminal behavior, and substance (ab)use disorders. They noted that
longitudinal studies indicated that impulsivity and related traits
precede and predict subsequent behaviors related to antisocial PD
(e.g., delinquency, antisocial and criminal behavior), as well as
substance (ab)use. Using data from a large twin sample, they
presented a hierarchical model with both impulsivity and conduct
disorder–antisocial behavior and substance dependence as manifestations of an underlying externalizing construct with substantial
heritability. In addition, impulsivity had a specific genetic component and all variables had significant nonshared environmental
components.
In summary, extensive research has established that there are
considerable relations between normal- and abnormal-range personality, at broad higher order levels, at more specific lower order
levels, and even at the level of subtrait facets. The challenge now
is to clarify both the most frequent patterns and the particularities
of these relations.
Personality–Axis I Disorder Relations
As noted earlier, researchers turned away from the study of
personality–psychopathology relations for some time during the
20th century. Thus, the vigor with which this research has reemerged and the swiftness with which researchers have embraced
the basic notion that these domains are interrelated is all the more
striking. The first thrust appears to have come from depression
researchers who noticed its interrelation with neuroticism (N) or
negative affectivity (e.g., Akiskal, Hirschfeld, & Yerevanian,
1983; see Klein et al.’s 1993 review). Much research into comorbidity also invoked personality as an explanatory factor (e.g., Clark
& Watson’s, 1991, tripartite model and Mineka et al.’s, 1998,
integrative hierarchical model of the depression–anxiety disorder
overlap; see also reviews by Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994;
Clark et al., 1995).
In addition, many studies report on the relation between personality trait X with Axis I disorder Y. A comprehensive and integrative literature review would be beneficial to our understanding
of this broad domain but again is beyond this article’s scope.
However, the FFM and Cloninger’s measures also have been used
in a number of studies in this domain, so a brief look at these
subliteratures provides an overview of their breadth.
Five-Factor Model
By the mid-1990s, that high N is associated with psychopathology was well established (Clark et al., 1995) and subsequent
research has continued to confirm and broaden this finding. Also
of interest is that (a) domain or facet scores for Extraversion (E),
Agreeableness (A), and Openness (O) differentiated among
schizophrenia, unipolar and bipolar depression patients (Bagby et
al., 1997), (b) community dwellers who later developed an eating
disorder had low A and high O scores premorbidly (Ghaderi &
Scott, 2000); (c) symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder correlated negatively and differentially with Conscientiousness (C) (inattention– disorganization symptoms) and A (hyperactivity–impulsivity and oppositional behaviors (Nigg et al.,
2002). A key point here is that FFM personality variables correlate
with Axis I and Axis II disorders at a similar magnitude.
Cloninger’s Model
Similarly, studies that have used the TPQ or TCI to examine
relations between personality and Axis I disorders in patient samples also have found relations of the same magnitude as those with
Axis II for a wide range of Axis I disorders, including depression
(e.g., Farmer et al., 2003), bipolar disorder (e.g., Osher, Cloninger,
& Belmaker, 1996), eating disorder (e.g., Vervaet, van Heeringen,
& Audenaert, 2004), social phobia (e.g., Pelissolo et al., 2002),
substance abuse (e.g., Sher, Bartholow, & Wood, 2000), schizophrenia (e.g., Szoeke et al., 2002), and depersonalization disorder
(e.g., Simeon, Guralnik, Knutelska, & Schmeidler, 2002).
Again, the challenge in these data is the similarity in the magnitude of correlations between personality and disorders on the two
DSM axes of disorders, indicating that personality traits do not
have a “privileged” relation with Axis II personality disorders.
Thus, it seems reasonable to pursue the possibility of a general
model to explain personality–psychopathology relations rather
SPECIAL SECTION: TEMPERAMENT AS THE UNIFYING BASIS
than separate models to explain relations between personality and
each of the two axes.
Reintegration of Personality and Psychopathology
Insofar as there is now considerable evidence of interrelations
between and among Axis I disorders, personality disorders, and
personality traits, it is time to turn to the question of how these
domains are related to one another. Taken as a whole, a broad
conclusion that can be drawn is that a more comprehensive model
of personality–psychopathology relations is warranted. To date,
explanatory models of the overlap between and among diagnoses
and personality traits typically have been developed to account for
specific empirical observations or have been discussed as a set of
theoretical models without serious consideration as to how they
might be integrated into a broad, overarching theoretical framework. Given various problems with the current DSM–IV structure,
having such a framework would, at least, have utility as a guide to
research to inform the DSM revision process and, at most, would
serve, ultimately, to replace the current DSM structure. In light of
the above literature review, what features must such a model have,
and what other features would it have ideally?
At a minimum, a comprehensive model of personality–
psychopathology relations would be both a structural and a causal
model that accounts for (a) extensive comorbidity both between
and among Axis I and II disorders; (b) relations between a basic set
of personality traits and a broad range of both Axis I and II
pathology; (c) variation in comorbidity and personality–
psychopathology relations due to specificity within classes of
disorder and heterogeneity within disorders; and (d) temporal
sequencing of traits with disorders and comorbid disorders. In
addition, such a model would ideally (e) clarify the nature of
personality and psychopathology, that is, how these concepts
themselves are to be understood and, in doing so, address such
relevant issues as states versus traits, temperament (nature) versus
character (nurture), and dimensions versus categories; and, finally,
(f) be consistent with the growing literatures for both personality
and psychopathology regarding genetic and environmental factors,
specific biological factors, and development.
As every college sophomore knows, variables A and B can be
correlated because (a) A causes B, (b) B causes A, (c) A and B
form a feedback loop of mutual causation, (d) a third variable
causes both A and B, or (e) some combination of the above.
Equally well known is that the most parsimonious explanation for
a given set of data should be considered first. Given the multiple
findings reviewed above, it seems most parsimonious to posit the
existence of a small set of “third variables” as underlying and
accounting for the many interrelations between and among personality and psychopathology.
As mentioned earlier, the various etiological models proposed to
explain diagnostic comorbidity and personality–psychopathology
relations are overlapping and not mutually exclusive. I argue here
that they can be integrated into a single hierarchical model. I
propose the following framework as an initial oversimplification
and then develop various aspects of the framework through the rest
of this article.
511
Temperament Dimensions Are Shared Factors Underlying
Personality and Psychopathology
Adult personality traits emerge through differentiation from
three innate biobehavioral dimensions, two of which are affective
systems—positive and negative affectivity—and the third of which
([dis]inhibition) is a regulatory system,4 and they share these
genetic diatheses with later developing disorders (shared factors).
At least some disorders are phenomenologically more extreme
manifestations of personality dimensions (spectra) and most, if not
all, disorders are more likely to develop in individuals who are
more extreme on relevant temperament–personality dimensions
(predisposition–vulnerability), particularly given adverse life experiences and/or the experience of another disorder (diathesis–
stress). Comorbid disorders each may affect the presentation of the
other, and the personality traits of individuals who develop psychopathology also may be affected by that experience (pathoplasty), both in the short term (complication) and long term (scar).
Key points regarding this framework including some elaborations, are the following. First, a distinguishing feature is that it
does not posit, as many current models do, that adult personality
traits per se underlie psychopathology, but rather that personality
and psychopathology are linked primarily through their shared
underlying genetic diatheses of broad temperament dimensions. In
this regard, the framework is consistent with, but more explicitly
developmental than, the formulations of Clark and Watson (1999)
and Watson, Wiese, Vaidya, and Tellegen (1999) and represents a
modification of Rothbart and Ahadi’s (1994) model, which proposed infant temperament as the basis for personality from which,
in turn, psychopathology develops through interaction with
environmental–learning factors. To be sure, the Rothbart and
Ahadi (1994) model describes perhaps the most common developmental pathway, but positing the underlying temperaments,
rather than personality per se, as the etiological agent of psychopathology, provides greater conceptual flexibility and more easily
incorporates cases in which personality is, conversely, affected by
psychopathology.
Second, the framework explicitly embraces a two-affect systems
model of positive and negative affectivity– emotionality–
activation, which are manifested as general biobehavioral systems
of approach–withdrawal, respectively, and which are regulated, in
large part, by a third system dimension that itself is nonaffective
but that plays a fundamental “gatekeeping” role in the degree to
which incoming stimuli are subjected to its inhibitory influence
(see Clark & Watson, 1999; Watson et al., 1999; and see also
Depue’s work [e.g., Depue, 1996; Depue & Collins, 1999; Depue
& Lenzenweger, 2001]).
4
Insofar as affect or emotion is a core and defining feature of temperament, one might argue that only two of these system dimensions may
properly be called temperaments. However, because the third dimension
affects how these emotional systems are experienced and manifested, I
shall use the term temperament to characterize all three dimensions. This
is consistent with Allport’s (1937) definition of temperament as “the
characteristic phenomenon of an individual’s emotional nature, including
his [sic] susceptibility to emotional stimulation, his customary strength and
speed of response, the quality of his prevailing mood, and all peculiarities
of fluctuation and intensity of mood” (p. 54) in that the third (dis)inhibitory, regulatory system clearly plays a role in these qualities.
512
CLARK
Whereas negative temperament is associated with a broad range
of psychopathology (e.g., Krueger, Caspi, Moffitt, Silva, & McGee, 1996; Watson & Clark, 1984), positive temperament plays an
important but more limited role in psychopathology, being linked
primarily with depression (Clark & Watson, 1991; Durbin et al.,
2005) and, perhaps to a lesser extent, schizophrenia and social
phobia (see Mineka et al., 1998). Indeed, important variance in
positive temperament may distinguish “ordinary normality” from
the high level of adaptation that characterizes those who are
particularly psychologically healthy; that is, high levels of positive
affectivity may distinguish those who are average versus particularly adept at dealing with life’s slings and arrows (Shiner, 2000;
Shiner, Masten, & Tellegen, 2002). The third dimension, disinhibition (vs. constraint) is associated with the range of psychopathology broadly known as externalizing disorders (e.g., substance
abuse, antisocial PD– conduct disorder, borderline PD; Kendler et
al., 2003; Krueger, 1999; Krueger et al., 1998, 2002; Lynam,
Leukefeld, & Clayton, 2003; Slade, cited in Watson, 2005; Vollebergh et al., 2001).
Third, positing that personality emerges through differentiation
has two implications. One is that personality should become more
stable over time and, indeed, Roberts and DelVecchio (2000)
reviewed 152 longitudinal studies of personality and found increasing trait stability with age. A second is that the framework
embraces the view that personality is hierarchically structured:
Traits and subtraits that emerge from the same temperament dimension are more closely related than those that emerge from
different underlying dimensional systems and also may be expected to show parallel patterns in relation to psychopathology.
This is the case from the top of the hierarchy downward. For
example, FFM A and C may be considered components of the Big
Three factor, Disinhibition (Markon, Krueger, & Watson, 2005).
As such, A and C should—and do—relate similarly to broad types
of psychopathology (e.g., substance abuse, antisocial PD– conduct
disorder; Lynam et al., 2003), and also show differential relations
when disorder is more finely specified (e.g., recall that subsets of
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms related differentially to A and C; Nigg et al., 2002).
Moreover, progress has been made on further specification of
the middle level of the hierarchy: The three basic dimensions
differentiate through development, both biological and experiential, into approximately 15 midlevel traits5 (e.g., aggression, affiliativeness, impulsivity) that, accordingly, have both genetic and
environmental components. These traits, singly or in combination,
are linked more specifically to different types of psychopathology.
For example, comparing substance use among college students,
Clark and Watson (1999) found that Carefree Orientation and
Antisocial Behavior (subtraits of Disinhibition) were linked differentially to lower GPA and alcohol and cigarette usage versus
other drug use, promiscuous sex, reckless driving, and (negatively)
religious behavior, respectively. Preliminary data suggest that below the midlevel traits there is at least one more finely differentiated level (i.e., that of personality trait facets and specific criteria
of disorders; e.g., Livesley et al., 1998; Nitschke, Heller, Imig,
McDonald, & Miller, 2001; Watson, Wu, & Cutshall, 2004), but
clarification of this level to any degree of specificity remains a
future prospect.
At the higher order level, existing research suggests that the
negative affectivity and disinhibition systems may be more tightly
linked or, at least, that the interplay between these systems is more
important for psychopathology than that between the positive
affectivity and disinhibition systems. Specifically, Markon et al.’s
(2005) meta-analysis of the structure of personality trait measures
supports Digman’s (1997) finding that negative affectivity and
disinhibition together form a still higher order “alpha” factor
(whereas E and O combine to form a “beta” factor), and other
research indicates that the combination of negative affectivity and
disinhibition represents a particularly maladaptive form of psychopathy (e.g., Hicks, Markon, Patrick, Krueger, & Newman,
2004; see also Clark & Watson, 1999, for a discussion of interplay
between biological systems linked with negative affectivity and
disinhibition, respectively).
Fourth, because the proposed framework encompasses both
static, structural issues and dynamic, developmental issues, in
methodological terms it hypothesizes both between-subjects and
within-subjects effects. Specifically, for example, it would predict
that related personality traits and disorders would (a) load on the
same factor in nomothetic cross-sectional structural analyses
(shared factors, spectra); (b) show shared genetic and, in some
cases, environmental factors in, for example, twin, family, and
adoption designs (shared factors); and (c) be related systematically
within individuals in longitudinal path analyses (shared factors,
predisposition–vulnerability). Each of these predictions has support in existing data, discussed subsequently. It is important to
stress that it is unlikely that a shared genetic diathesis is the only
causal link between personality and psychopathology; that is, they
also may share one or more environmental components but, as I
also discuss subsequently, much less progress has been made on
this front.
Fifth, the proposed framework offers a new perspective on
various difficulties with the current Axis I–Axis II distinction. For
example, it is difficult to explain why certain pairs of disorders that
are linked for different reasons (e.g., avoidant PD appears to be a
severe form of social phobia, Widiger, 1992b; schizophrenia and
schizotypal PD most likely share one or more etiological factors;
Grove et al., 1991) are placed on separate axes. Similarly, it is not
clear why GAD, which typically is a chronic disorder that is very
strongly associated with negative affectivity is placed on Axis I
rather than on Axis II. Further, the evidence is equivocal regarding
5
This number is, admittedly, semiarbitrary but, importantly, only semiarbitrary. That is, in between the well-known two to five broad factors
(Eysenck’s [1967] or Digman’s [1997] Big Two; Eysenck’s [Eysenck &
Eysenck, 1985] or Tellegen’s [in press] Big Three; the FFM) and what
Wiggins and Pincus (1992) called fine-grained groupings, such as the 75 of
Norman (1967), the 133 of Goldberg (1990), or the 160 of Cattell (cited in
Wiggins, 1984), efforts have been made to delineate sets of lower order
personality traits at the middle level (called optimal by Briggs, 1989, p.
246) and have converged on trait sets ranging from lows of 8 (Comfrey
Personality Scales; Comfrey, 1970), 10 (Guilford–Zimmerman Temperament Survey; Guilford & Zimmerman, 1956) and 11 (Multidimensional
Personality Questionnaire; Tellegen, in press), to mid-range numbers of 15
(Edwards Personality Preference Schedule; Edwards, 1959) and 16 (16Personality Factors; Cattell, Cattell, & Cattell, 1993), to highs of 20
(California Psychological Inventory; Gough, 1987) and 22 (Personality
Research Form; Jackson, 1989). The aforementioned SNAP (Clark, 1993;
15 scales) and DAPP (Livesley & Jackson, in press; 18 scales) also fall on
or close to this number.
SPECIAL SECTION: TEMPERAMENT AS THE UNIFYING BASIS
whether dysthymia is a separate disorder from the depressive PD
currently identified as in need of further study (e.g., Ryder, Bagby,
& Schuller, 2002). Livesley (e.g., Livesley et al., 1994) was among
the first to argue that there is neither a theoretical nor an empirical
basis for separating personality disorder from other types of psychopathology (see also Widiger & Shea, 2001). I find their arguments compelling, and thus the proposed framework focuses instead on clarifying personality–psychopathology relations without
regard for the current Axis I–Axis II distinction.
Supportive Data
Clearly, this framework is built upon a number of existing
pieces and supportive evidence that already have begun to accrue.
I have previously mentioned the structural models that both account for anxiety and depressive disorder comorbidity through
shared dimensions of positive and negative affectivity and also
explain the distinctiveness of specific anxiety disorders through
more specific components, such as anxious arousal in panic disorder (Clark & Watson, 1991; Mineka et al., 1998). It is not yet
clear whether these specific components are unique to psychopathology or also have personality trait counterparts. However, numerous studies have shown that various symptom dimensions are
not unique to psychopathology but are continuously distributed
and found in normal samples (e.g., Watson, 2005), which suggests
common underlying personality-subtrait–psychopathology-symptom dimensions.
Similarly, a number of studies have shown Axis I–Axis II
relations can be accounted for in large part by shared personality
traits. For example, disinhibition and related personality traits,
such as FFM A and C, have been shown to account for the overlap
between substance abuse and Cluster B personality disorders
(Casillas & Clark, 2002), antisocial personality disorder (Sher &
Trull, 1994), and antisocial behavior (Iacono, Carlson, Taylor,
Elkins, & McGue, 1999; Lynam et al., 2003). In a related vein,
Nestor (2002) presented evidence showing that personality variables (specifically, impulse dyscontrol and affect dysregulation)
mediated psychopathology–violence relations. These three-way
interrelations (personality with both Axis I–Axis II psychopathology and Axis I–Axis II comorbidity) are most parsimoniously
explained by positing that they all are based in a shared underlying
system. The fact that disorder comorbidity is found to be due
largely to shared relations with personality traits (and not vice
versa) suggests that personality is a developmentally simpler manifestation of the underlying temperament dimensions than is
psychopathology
Consistent with this suggestion, Carey and DiLalla’s (1994)
exploration of genetic causal models for personality–Axis I relations (specifically focused on depression and anxiety disorders)
indicated that personality was causally prior to disorder and explained much of the comorbidity. Subsequent longitudinal data
indicate that elevations in relevant personality traits frequently, if
not always, precede emergence of the related Axis I and Axis II
disorders (e.g., Mufson, Nomura, & Warner, 2002; see Krueger et
al., 2002). As mentioned earlier, that elevation in personality traits
typically precedes psychopathology is not inconsistent with a
framework in which basic temperament dimensions underlie both.
The emerging hierarchical, spectrum model for more prevalent
mental disorders discussed earlier (e.g., Kendler et al., 2003;
513
Krueger, 1999; Krueger et al., 1998, 2002; Vollebergh et al., 2001)
also fits well within the proposed framework. The consensus
model proposes a stable internalizing factor to account for certain
Axis I comorbidity through the common dimension of negative
affectivity, and an externalizing factor that encompasses antisocial
PD– conduct disorder and the personality dimension of low constraint (i.e., disinhibition–impulsivity). Various data also indicate
that negative affectivity contributes to externalizing factors (e.g.,
Krueger et al., 1996), which may account for the modest relation
found between the internalizing and externalizing factors (Kendler
et al., 2003; Krueger, 1999; Vollebergh et al., 2001).
The data incorporated to date by these particular studies stop
short of a full integration of Axis I syndromes, Axis II PDs, and
personality dimensions. As noted earlier, however, Klein and
Schwartz’s (2002) model also crossed the Axis I–Axis II boundary, linking depressive disorders and borderline PD via a shared
underlying factor, which lays the groundwork for the addition of
borderline and other personality disorders to the emerging consensus model.
Development
Abundant evidence of continuity from infancy through to adulthood has accumulated in recent years. In a special issue of the
Journal of Personality on personality development, 7 of 10 substantive articles presented longitudinal data (Graziano, 2003), and,
importantly, several long-term projects that began in childhood
now have “come of age.” I illustrate the observed continuity with
an example from each of these projects. First, a small but intensive
study of prosocial behavior, in which interview and other-report
data were collected every 2 years from age 4 –5 to 23–24, reported
stable prosocial dispositions (Eisenberg et al., 1999). The largescale, epidemiological sample Australian “Dunedin” study classified children at age 3 into three temperament groups, now in their
20s. Those in the “undercontrolled” group are more impulsive,
unreliable, and antisocial adults, whereas those in the “inhibited”
group are more likely to be depressed adults and to have fewer
social supports (Caspi, 2000). A 10-year, University of Minnesota
study reports clear relations between child (age 8 –12) and young
adult (age 17–23) measures of Positive and Negative Emotionality
and Disinhibition (Shiner et al., 2002). Moreover, both childhood
and adult antisocial conduct are related to adult Disinhibition,
whereas maladaptivity is related to Negative Emotionality. Behavioral inhibition and, to a lesser extent, quality of peer relations also
were stable over time (Gest, 1997).
Data are emerging also from three Scandinavian studies, two of
which began in the mid to late 1960s and the other began in 1980.
One study now reports, again by way of illustration, that compliance, self-control, and low aggression in 8-year-olds related to
socialization and low aggression and impulsivity 25 years later
when the participants had reached their mid-30s (Laursen, Pulkkinen, & Adams, 2002). Another found continuity over 17 years in
the construct of difficult temperament, manifested more strongly as
low social adjustment in childhood and as anger in adulthood
(Pesonen, Raikkonen, Keskivaara, & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, 2003).
The third study examined relations between temperament factors
that emerged from ratings of age 4 behavioral data with teacher
ratings at age 10, a psychologist’s ratings at age 25, and selfratings at age 36. Whereas all five factors showed some consis-
514
CLARK
tency, two of the five factors—Aggression (a marker of Digman’s
alpha) and Outgoingness (a beta marker)—were the most robust.
Moreover, several of the childhood temperament factors were
related to adult alcohol and drug-related problems (e.g., childhood
aggression and low persistence predicted, respectively, intoxication frequency at age 25 and drug-related criminality at age 30).
Specific Challenges
State versus trait variance. A major challenge in modeling
personality–psychopathology relations is that trait scores change
with diagnostic state (Clark et al., 1994), which has led some
researchers (e.g., Goel, Terman, & Terman, 2003; Reich, Noyes,
Hirschfeld, Coryell, & O’Gorman, 1987) to conclude that personality scales are state rather than trait measures. However, using a
5-week pharmacotherapy trial, Santor, Bagby, and Joffe (1997)
demonstrated that personality trait changes (specifically, N and
E—the two major affective dimensions) were not— or only modestly—accounted for by changes in depression scores and, therefore, concluded that the meaning of the state-dependent nature of
personality scores needed reexamination.
Building on this finding, Clark, Vittengl, Kraft, and Jarrett
(2003) demonstrated that personality scores (including prominently SNAP Negative and Positive Temperament) could be separated into variable (state) and stable (trait) components. It is
important to note that they found that, whereas both components
correlated concurrently with depression severity level, only the
trait portion predicted future levels. This is consistent with the
findings of many studies that treatment-responders’ scores on
certain personality traits decrease but still remain higher than
controls’ (e.g., Agosti & McGrath, 2002; Du, Bakish, Ravindran,
& Hrdina, 2002), whereas treatment nonresponders’ profiles are
virtually unchanged from pretreatment scores.
Thus, personality measures—and more specifically, those related to the higher order dimensions of negative and positive
temperament— combine state and trait variance. Currently, most
models of personality–psychopathology relations ignore state variance or consider it a nuisance factor, part of the error term, but a
comprehensive model ultimately must account for this variance as
well. Notably, state negative–positive affect and trait negative–
positive affectivity (neuroticism– extraversion), respectively, are
themselves related (Watson & Clark, 1984, 1997), so inclusion of
both sets of variables will provide a more complete picture of
psychopathology both structurally and developmentally.
Heritability and environmental variance: Temperament and
character? Adult personality is acknowledged widely to result
from environmental influences acting on innate tendencies. From
our current vantage point, without greater specification of the
various components, this appears to be little more than a banal
truism. However, this was not always the case. Only relatively
recently have multiple studies confirmed that genes account for
roughly half of the observed variance in personality traits (e.g.,
Tellegen et al., 1988; Turkheimer, 2000). Moreover, whereas the
developmental studies discussed earlier have established clear
links between child and adult temperament–personality and thus
are consistent with studies showing the strong heritability of personality, a great deal of change occurs over this time span as well,
and the more difficult tasks of clarifying the role of experience in
turning temperament into personality–psychopathology, as well as
understanding the processes by which temperament develops into
personality–psychopathology, lie ahead of us.
Until behavior genetics studies in the late 20th century indicated
otherwise, shared familial environment was believed to be the
major environmental factor shaping personality. Beginning with
the seminal study of Tellegen et al. (1988), however, research has
shown consistently that the effect of shared environment is negligible for most traits, that the predominant etiological factors in
personality are genetic and unshared environment. This finding has
been replicated with a variety of personality measures including
the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (Krueger, 2000),
California Psychological Inventory (Bouchard, McGue, Hur, &
Horn, 1998), the TCI (Gillespie, Cloninger, Heath, & Martin,
2003), and the DAPP (Jang, Livesley, & Vernon, 1998), as well as
scales from the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory and
Zuckerman’s Sensation-Seeking Scales (Mustanski, Viken,
Kaprio, & Rose, 2003).
Turkheimer and Waldron’s (2000) review of the nonshared
environment research is particularly sobering with regard to understanding the role of environment in the development of
personality–psychopathology. They conclude that studies of measurable differences in “objective” nonshared family environment
(e.g., differential parental behavior) contribute little to personality.
It is interesting to note that the differential effects of peers and
teachers are somewhat stronger (see also Rose et al., 2003) but, for
the most part, “the environment is all interaction and little main
effect” (Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000, p. 92; see also Wachtel,
1994, for a longitudinal, developmental approach, a cyclical dynamic process relating personality and psychopathology).
More important, the nonshared environmental components of
the 15 or so proposed midlevel traits may reflect primarily what
Turkheimer and Waldron (2000) have termed the “effective environment” (p. 78). Moreover, measuring this effective environment
may prove to be more tractable than would appear at first blush
because it is, at least partly and perhaps largely, dependent on
individuals’ perceptions and interpretations of situations which, in
turn, have been shown—at least for some perceptions (e.g.,
stress)—to be strongly related to personality (Monroe & Depue,
1986). Therefore, if this proves to be a more general phenomenon,
a start will have been made toward specifying one set of elements
in interactions involving the nonshared environment.
It is important to note that Turkheimer (2000) predicts that a
similar conclusion will be drawn regarding the behavioral effects
of single genes, specifically, that additive gene effects provide the
predictive power largely observed to date (e.g., with regard to
personality stability), whereas developmental changes occur
through gene– gene (as well as gene– environment, and
phenotype– environment) interactions and thus will prove more
elusive (but see Dick & Rose, 2002, for a more optimistic view of
the future contribution of behavior genetics research).
Furthermore, although the (nonshared) environmentally influenced components of personality may stem primarily from individuals’ specific experiences (or their perceptions of those experiences), a given set of experiences (perceptions) within
individuals may contribute jointly to the development of psychopathology as well as personality. Recall, for example, the Kendler
et al. (2003) finding that conduct disorder and antisocial PD shared
a unique-environment factor. Assuming this finding can be extended to include personality traits associated with antisocial PD
SPECIAL SECTION: TEMPERAMENT AS THE UNIFYING BASIS
(e.g., disinhibition), research explicating environmental factors in
the development of disinhibition may elucidate environmental
factors in the development of conduct disorder and antisocial PD
as well.
It is noteworthy that studies have not found greater heritability
for traits identified as temperament as compared with supposedly
more environmentally shaped character traits (Gillespie et al.,
2003; Reimann & De Raad, 1998). At first glance, this finding
would appear to refute the hypothesis that there are certain core
traits or temperaments that are developmentally primary, but further consideration suggests otherwise. That is, personality trait
structure is rather diffuse and undifferentiated in infancy and is
elaborated increasingly through childhood, adolescence, and finally adulthood (Rothbart & Ahadi, 1994). Thus, all personality
traits measured in adulthood may be roughly equally heritable
because they all share their temperamental origin, yet all are
increasingly differentiated through development via influences
that include both within-person (e.g., cognitive and self-regulatory
changes) and person– environment (e.g., social–interpersonal experiences, life events) factors. Consequently, whereas temperament and character may be distinguished conceptually, with only
the former present at birth, attempts to identify certain adult
personality traits as temperaments and others as character is most
likely a fruitless venture.
In summary, whereas it clearly is premature to attempt to
delineate the specific forms that these various types of developmentally critical interactions will take, for the purposes of constructing a comprehensive model linking personality and psychopathology, the important point to glean from these considerations
and existing data is that adult personality variables represent
complex combinations of variance due to states as well as traits,
and variance due not only to basic temperament but also to the
elaboration of temperament through individuals’ experiences and
their reactions to those experiences. The most basic tenet of the
proposed model—that three temperament dimensions are the
shared factors underlying observed personality–psychopathology
relations—is consistent with the findings of studies that have
examined genetic and environmental contributions to psychopathology, personality, and their overlap.
To cite a specific example, Mineka et al.’s (1998) integrative
hierarchical model, which was developed to explain the shared and
specific factors in depression and anxiety disorders, was based, in
part, on genetic studies indicating that anxiety and depressive
symptoms correlated because of a genetic factor they shared with
neuroticism (Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, 1993).
Similarly, the externalizing factor linking personality (disinhibition) and psychopathology (substance dependence, antisocial behavior) identified by Krueger et al. (2002) may be interpreted as
representing one or more underlying temperament dimensions.
Thus, key factors in the proposed framework for understanding
relations among different forms of psychopathology are not themselves specifically psychopathological; rather, they are identified
as major dimensions of normal temperament structure that have a
substantial genetic component.
This view is consistent with the additional findings of Kendler
et al. (1993) that (a) the genetic component of the overarching
externalizing factor (i.e., the underlying temperament(s) in the
proposed framework) was notably larger than the genetic component of any other element in the model and (b) the unique (non-
515
shared environment) component of every element (i.e., both personality and psychopathology elements) in the model was
substantial and larger than any other component, except for the
externalizing factor itself, in which it represented a nontrivial but
modest component. This is precisely what would be expected if the
externalizing factor represented an underlying, largely genetically
based, temperament factor or, more likely, the combined effects of
two factors—Negative Temperament and Disinhibition—which,
as stated earlier, accounts for the moderate correlation with internalizing which is associated strongly with Negative Temperament.
Biological findings. Insofar as temperament is conceived as a
dimension that, being present at birth, has a large genetic component, data on biological factors that link personality and psychopathology also may provide support for the proposed framework.
This literature is growing, diverse, and not yet well integrated,
although there are promising beginnings (e.g., Depue & Collins,
1999; Siever & Davis, 1991; see also Clark & Watson, 1999).
Clearly, a review of this literature is outside this article’s focus, but
a few examples give a sense of the wide scope of such research and
provide a glimpse into a future in which biological variables also
are integrated into the understanding of personality and
psychopathology.
Among the domains being researched are (a) sleep disturbances
(e.g., Asaad, Okasha, & Okasha, 2002, found similarities between
borderline PD and major depressive disorder); (b) mood and social
affiliation (e.g., Knutson et al., 1998, reported that a selective
serotonin reuptake inhibitor, most often used to treat depression,
decreased negative affect and increased social affiliation in nonpatient volunteers); (c) startle reactions (e.g., abnormal potentiation of startle indexed psychopathic traits in both a community and
an incarcerated sample [Vanman, Mejia, Dawson, Schell, & Raine,
2003, and Patrick, 1994, respectively]); and (d) autonomic nervous
system regulation (e.g., Thayer & Lane, 2000, found restricted
cardiac variability to be associated with both normal and pathological worry [GAD]).
Earlier, I noted the research interest in the construct of impulsivity, and a number of studies have addressed the biology of
impulsivity or impulsive aggression. This dimension has been
identified as a key component of borderline PD (and, more
broadly, the Cluster B personality disorders) and generally is
regarded as a personality–temperamental dimension, not a pathological dimension per se (e.g., Siever, Torgersen, Gunderson,
Livesley, & Kendler, 2002; Skodol, Siever, et al., 2002; Soloff,
Kelly, Strotmeyer, Malone, & Mann, 2003). In a related vein,
Iacono et al. (1999) presented evidence that trait disinhibition—
which they describe as the inability or unwillingness to inhibit
behavioral impulses, and which has been linked to a range of
externalizing psychopathology—is a manifestation of underlying
central nervous system processes associated with various psychophysiological anomalies, including electrodermal responses and
event-related potentials. Fowles (2000) also noted that electrodermal hyporeactivity was a reliable correlate of psychopathy–
undersocialized aggressive conduct disorder and that it appears to
relate to an impulsivity dimension of psychopathy.
The links between manifest behaviors and specific physiological
and neurological reactions undoubtedly are complex; yet, with
continued research, gradually the biological underpinnings of
temperament–personality no doubt will be elucidated. Such research will be facilitated to the extent that the dimensions and
516
CLARK
facets of observed personality traits can be clarified. That is, the
more reliably and validly target behaviors and behavioral sets, as
well as the personality dimensions they represent, are defined and
assessed, the more likely it will be that meaningful associations
with biological variables are discovered. Conversely, it seems
unlikely that specific biological correlates of broad, heterogeneous
targets such as borderline PD will be identified.
Dimensions versus categories. Throughout this article,
temperament–personality variables have been described as dimensions, whereas disorders— both Axis I and Axis II— generally
have designated conventional, DSM-based categories. Yet, evidence indicates that for many disorders, the reliability and validity
with which they can be assessed increases to the extent that they
are treated as dimensions rather than categories (Asendorpf, 2003;
Flett, Vredenberg, & Krames, 1997; Widiger, 1992a). In the face
of such data, how are we to justify use of the “industry standard”
categories? Researchers have begun to grapple with this problem.
Recently Waldman and Lilienfeld (2001) and Haslam (2002)
have confronted the widely held notion that there is a single
categorical type and have distinguished a number of different types
of categories ranging from practical categories that represent simply the dichotomization of a dimension either by using “most
valid” cutpoints (e.g., high blood pressure) or by focusing only on
the dimensions’ extremities (e.g., introverts vs. extraverts), to
configural categories that emerge from the conjunction of extremity on several underlying dimensions, to natural categories that
have a specific causal basis and appear to be quite rare biologically
with regard to mental disorder (e.g., Huntington’s disorder).
Haslam (2002) argues further that the “dimensional– categorical
debate” in psychology–psychiatry actually is a debate over two
kinds of categories. That is, dimensionalists are not arguing that
there is no useful distinction between, for example, persons low
and high on depression severity or maladaptive personality traits,
but rather that the distinction between normality and psychopathology is based on extrinsic, pragmatic grounds, such as increased
risk for self-harm, not on an underlying, natural discontinuity.
Conversely, according to Haslam (2002), categoricalists are arguing that the distinction is intrinsic, even if imprecise (e.g., Santor
& Coyne, 2001). Thus, understanding of the etiology of psychopathology is needed to resolve this debate. Should etiological
explanations be consistent with those that produce continuous
dimensions, the former will have “won the day,” whereas the latter
will hold sway should the underlying causes prove to be such that
they lead to discontinuity.
Further, categoricalists who argue that the DSM disorders are
categories on the basis of underlying discontinuities bear the
burden of proof to identify the category boundaries in a relatively
precise manner. To date, however, the empirical data have been
supportive of the existence of such boundaries for only a few
disorders (see reviews by Haslam, 2003; Haslam & Kim, 2002).
Toward a Comprehensive Hierarchical Model for DSM–V
(or VI?)
A great deal of ground has been covered in this review, so I
recap here the key points of the proposed comprehensive, hierarchical framework of relations among temperament, personality and
psychopathology to accompany or even substitute in DSM–V for
the current set of disorders. First, the framework is both structural
and developmental: It is intended to explain comorbidity, encompassing both more and less prevalent disorders, in virtually all
combinations, as well as the overlap of disorders with personality
traits by positing that both personality and psychopathology develop from three underlying, genetically based temperament dimensions: negative affectivity, positive affectivity, and disinhibition. This dimensional framework incorporates the related
concepts of severity of psychopathology and strength or extremeness of personality traits, and existing data suggest its applicability
across populations—patient and nonpatient, in the United States
and elsewhere—and across measurement modalities.
This framework can account for the fact that comorbidity is
rampant but is neither random nor uniform. That is, general vulnerability factors are necessary, but far from sufficient, to explain
the relevant phenomena. The differential comorbidity rates found
even between pairs of similar disorders, indicate that some pairs of
disorders share more (and/or larger) underlying factors than do
others. Thus, the framework proposes that initially broad temperament dimensions are differentiated through development, and
more specific personality– disorder associations are due to sharing
one or more differentiated traits and, most likely, one or more
etiologically relevant environmental factors as well. Moreover, the
model, being hierarchical, accounts for heterogeneity within disorders as currently defined. Heterogeneous disorders can be parsed
into their components, permitting delineation of more specific
factors that distinguish among psychopathological variants.
Watson (2005) has made strides toward identifying the facets of
several disorders that are continuous into the normal range, suggesting they may have personality-subtrait counterparts.
This comprehensive framework also may be extended beyond
personality and psychopathology, bringing such variables as social
functioning and violent behavior within its domain. It is a causal
model that provides for development and diagnostic sequencing.
Finally, it is important to point out that it is a scientific model—
simultaneously theoretically and empirically based. With the current knowledge of psychopathology, researchers can no longer
afford the “luxury” of an atheoretical taxonomy if studies are to
continue to advance the field. To those who argue that the DSM
must serve many masters and thus ultimately must yield to important practical considerations, I respond with James Clerk Maxwell’s famous dictum, “There is nothing more practical than a
good theory.”
Clearly, a fully specified model of personality–psychopathology
relations is a long way off, but the current proposal provides a
framework within which to develop specific working models.
Whereas it might be premature to adopt such a framework for all
of DSM–V, at a late 2004 conference on refining the research
agenda for DSM–V, cosponsored by the American Psychiatric
Association and three NIH agencies, there was a remarkable consensus that a dimensional model of personality disorder would be
preferable to the current system. Moreover, we are not so far away
from developing a general dimensional model that the basic direction in which we need to move is unclear either conceptually or
empirically.
Conceptually, the consideration that whereas the Axis I–Axis II
distinction had great utility when introduced in DSM–III, it may
have become a barrier to progress and now should be set aside, is
a major step forward, as is the general acknowledgment that
dimensions are more powerful, and likely more valid, explanatory
SPECIAL SECTION: TEMPERAMENT AS THE UNIFYING BASIS
constructs. Empirically, the demonstration that broad personality
dimensions explain much of the overlap among different types of
psychopathology is a giant step forward from the earlier held
position that personality and psychopathology are independent
domains.
Finally, the importance of the gradual breaking down of traditional disciplinary boundaries and the increase in interdisciplinary
research for developing a comprehensive model cannot be overestimated. As described by Plomin and Caspi (1998), for example,
“genetic approaches to complex traits such as personality have
moved from single-gene models and methods to approaches that
can identify genes of small effect size in multiple-gene systems
called quantitative trait loci” (p. 387). The success of these approaches, however, depends heavily not only on advances in
molecular biology but equally on the availability of psychometrically reliable and valid measures of personality and psychopathology. Moreover, if innate temperament dimensions underlie
personality–psychopathology links, developmental psychologists
will be relied on to direct the longitudinal research needed to
bridge from infant temperament to adult personality.
The greatest barrier to adoption of the proposed framework for
DSM-V may be that it presents a challenge to categorical diagnoses, some of which have strong advocacy groups that likely will
oppose any perceived attempts to undermine their efforts on behalf
of those diagnosed with the disorder. Thus, it is important to
emphasize that dimensions and categories are not fundamentally
incompatible. Indeed, dimensions can underlie even natural categories. For example, Huntington’s disorder is caused by an abnormally high number of CAG nucleotide repeats. A greater number
of repeats is associated with earlier age of onset, and persons
having a certain intermediate range of repeats show symptoms of
the disorder but may or may not develop the full-blown syndrome
(Meehl, 2001). This fact in no way detracts from its status as a
serious disorder, and the same may be true for the full range of
psychopathology.
If the framework proposed leads, as hoped, to more sophisticated research that accounts well for increasing amounts of data in
multiple domains, ranging from molecular biology to personality
trait structure to developmental psychopathology, researchers will
gain the knowledge needed to clarify dimension– category relations and to determine the nature of the categories formed from
underlying dimensions in various domains. It is possible, for
example, that some personality disorders are indeed best diagnosed
by using the prototypic categorical model that was adopted explicitly for all Axis II disorders beginning with DSM–III–R. In the
meantime, if it can be agreed that the data do not support classical
categorical models for many, if not most, types of psychopathology, but that— even so—there is great practical utility in having
categories for certain decision-making purposes, then a rapprochement may be possible, perhaps even for DSM–V.
References
Agosti, V., & McGrath, P. J. (2002). Comparison of the effects of fluoxetine, imipramine, and placebo on personality in atypical depression.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 71, 113–120.
Akiskal, H. S., Hirschfeld, R. M. A., & Yerevanian, B. I. (1983). The
relationship of personality to affective disorders: A critical review.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 801– 810.
517
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New
York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Erkanli, A. (1999). Comorbidity. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 57– 87.
Asaad, T., Okasha, T., & Okasha, A. (2002). Sleep EEG findings in
ICD–10 borderline personality disorder in Egypt. Journal of Affective
Disorders, 71, 11–18.
Asendorpf, J. B. (2003). Head-to-head comparison of the predictive validity of personality types and dimensions. European Journal of Personality, 17, 327–346.
Bagby, R. M., Bindseil, K. D., Schuller, D. R., Rector, N. A., Young, L. T.,
Cooke, R., et al. (1997). Relationship between the five-factor model of
personality and unipolar, bipolar, and schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry
Research, 70, 83–94.
Bagge, C. L., & Trull, T. J. (2003). DAPP–BQ: Factor structure and
relations to personality disorder symptoms in a non-clinical sample.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 19 –32.
Ball, S. A., Tennen, H., Poling, J. C., Kranzler, H. R., & Rounsaville, B. J.
(1997). Personality, temperament, and character dimensions and the
DSM–IV personality disorders in substance abusers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 545–553.
Berkson, J. (1946). Limitations of the application of the fourfold table
analysis to hospital data. Biometrics Bulletin, 1, 47–53.
Blashfield, R. K., & Intoccia, V. (2000). Growth of the literature on the
topic of personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157,
472– 473.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., McGue, M., Hur, Y.-M., & Horn, J. M. (1998). A
genetic and environmental analysis of the California Psychological
Inventory using adult twins reared apart and together. European Journal
of Personality, 12, 307–320.
Boyd, J. H., Burke, J. D., Jr., Gruenberg, E., Holzer, C. E. III, Rae, D. S.,
George, L., et al. (1984). Exclusion criteria of DSM–III: A study of
co-occurrence of hierarchy-free syndromes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 983–989.
Brieger, P., Ehrt, U., & Marneros, A. (2003). Frequency of comorbid
personality disorders in bipolar and unipolar affective disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 28 –34.
Brieger, P., Sommer, S., Bloeink, R., & Marneros, A. (2000). The relationship between five-factor personality measurements and ICD–10 personality disorder dimensions: Results from a sample of 229 subjects.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 14, 282–290.
Briggs, S. R. (1989). The optimal level of measurement for personality
constructs. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.). Personality psychology:
Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 246 –260). New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Carey, G., & DiLalla, D. L. (1994). Personality and psychopathology:
Genetic perspectives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 32– 43.
Caron, C., & Rutter, M. (1991). Comorbidity in child psychopathology:
Concepts, issues and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 32, 1063–1080.
Casillas, A., & Clark, L. A. (2002). Dependency, impulsivity, and selfharm: Traits hypothesized to underlie the association between Cluster B
personality and substance abuse disorders. Journal of Personality Disorder, 16, 424 – 436.
518
CLARK
Caspi, A. (2000). The child is the father of the man: Personality continuities from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78, 158 –172.
Cattell, R. B., Cattell, A. K., & Cattell, H. E. (1993). Sixteen Personality
Factor Questionnaire (5th ed.). Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality
and Ability Testing.
Clark, L. A. (1993). Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality
(SNAP). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Clark, L. A., Livesley, W. J., Schroeder, M. L., & Irish, S. (1996). The
structure of maladaptive personality traits: Convergent validity between
two systems. Psychological Assessment, 8, 294 –303.
Clark, L. A., McEwen, J. L., Collard, L. M., & Hickok, L. G. (1993).
Symptoms and traits of personality disorder: Two new methods for their
assessment. Psychological Assessment, 5, 81–91.
Clark, L. A., Vittengl, J. R., Kraft, D., & Jarrett, R. B. (2003). Personality
and psychosocial functioning predict depression severity after acutephase cognitive therapy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 406 – 430.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and
depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316 –336.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1999). Temperament. In L. Pervin & O. John
(Eds.). Handbook of personality (2nd ed., pp. 399 – 423). New York:
Guilford Press.
Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality,
and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
103, 103–116.
Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Reynolds, A. (1995). Diagnosis and classification in psychopathology: Challenges to the current system and future
directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 121–153.
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and
classification of personality variants: A proposal. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 44, 573–588.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.
Comfrey, A. L. (1970). Manual for the Comfrey Personality Scales. San
Diego, CA: EdITS.
Costa, P. T. Jr., & Widiger, T. A. (Eds.) (2002). Personality disorders and
the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological
tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281–302.
Depue, R. (1996). A neurobiological framework for the structure of personality and emotion: Implications for personality disorders. In J. E.
Clarkin & M. F. Lenzenweger (Eds.), Major theories of personality
disorder (pp. 347–390). New York: Guilford Press.
Depue, R., & Collins, P. E. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of
personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569.
Depue, R., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (2001). A neurobehavioral dimensional
model. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Handbook of personality disorders (pp.
136 –176). New York: Guilford Press.
Dick, D. M., & Rose, R. J. (2002). Behavior genetics: What’s new? What’s
next? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 70 –74.
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher order factors of the Big Five. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1246 –1256.
Du, L., Bakish, D., Ravindran, A. V., & Hrdina, P. D. (2002). Does
fluoxetine influence major depression by modifying five-factor personality traits? Journal of Affective Disorders, 71, 235–241.
Durbin, E., Klein, D. N., Hayden, E. P., Buckley, M. E., & Moerk, K. C.
(2005). Temperamental emotionality in preschoolers and parental mood
disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 28 –37.
Edwards, A. L. (1959). The Edwards Personal Preference Schedule (rev.
ed.) New York: Psychological Corporation.
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland,
A., & Carlo, G. (1999). Consistency and development of prosocial
dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 70, 1360 –1372.
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL:
Thomas.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1985). Personality and individual
differences. New York: Plenum Press.
Farmer, A., Mahmood, A., Redman, K., Harris, T., Sadler, S., & McGuffin,
P. (2003). A sib-pair study of the Temperament and Character Inventory
scales in major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 490 –
496.
Flett, G. L., Vredenberg, K., & Krames, L. (1997). The continuity of
depression in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychological Bulletin,
121, 395– 416.
Flynn, P. M., Craddock, S. G., Luckey, J. W., Hubbard, R. L., & Dunteman, G. H. (1996). Comorbidity of antisocial personality and mood
disorders among psychoactive substance-dependent treatment clients.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 10, 56 – 67.
Fowles, D. C. (2000). Electrodermal hyporeactivity and antisocial behavior: Does anxiety mediate the relationship? Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 177–189.
Gest, S. (1997). Behavioral inhibition: Stability and associations with
adaptation from childhood to early adulthood. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 72, 467– 475.
Ghaderi, A., & Scott. B. (2000). The Big Five and eating disorders: A
prospective study in the general population. European Journal of Personality, 14, 311–323.
Gillespie, N. A., Cloninger, C. R., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (2003).
The genetic and environmental relationship between Cloninger’s dimensions of temperament and character. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1931–1946.
Goel, N., Terman, M., & Terman, J. S. (2003). Dimensions of temperament
and bright light response in seasonal affective disorder. Psychiatry
Research, 119, 89 –97.
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The
Big Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
59, 1216 –1229.
Gough, H. (1987). California Psychological Inventory administrators
guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Gray, J. A. (1972). The psychophysiological basis of introversion– extraversion: A modification of Eysenck’s theory. In V. Nebylitsyn & J. A.
Gray (Eds.), Biological basis of individual behavior (pp. 182–205). New
York: Academic Press.
Graziano, W. G. (2003). Personality development: An introduction toward
process approaches to long-term stability and change in persons. Journal
of Personality, 71, 893–903.
Griego, J., Stewart, S. E., & Coolidge, F. L. (1999). A convergent validity
study of Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory with the
Coolidge Axis II Inventory. Journal of Personality Disorders, 13, 256 –
267.
Grove, W. M., Lebow, B. S., Clementz, B. A., Cerri, A., Medus, C., &
Iacono, W. G. (1991). Familial prevalence and coaggregation of schizotypy indicators: A multitrait family study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 173–176.
Guilford, J. P., & Zimmerman, W. S. (1956). Fourteen dimensions of
temperament: Psychological Monographs, 70 (10, Whole No. 417).
Guiterrez, F., Sangorrin, J., Martin-Santos, R., Torres, X., & Torrens, M.
(2002). Measuring the core features of personality disorders in substance
abusers using the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). Journal
of Personality Disorders, 16, 344 –359.
Gustad, J., & Phillips, K. A. (2003). Axis I comorbidity in body dysmorphic disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 44, 270 –276.
Haslam, N. (2002). Kinds of kinds: A conceptual taxonomy of psychiatric
categories. Philosophy, Psychiatry, Psychology, 9, 203–217.
SPECIAL SECTION: TEMPERAMENT AS THE UNIFYING BASIS
Haslam, N. (2003). The dimensional view of personality disorders: A
review of taxometric evidence. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 75–93.
Haslam, N., & Kim, H. C. (2002). Categories and continua: A review of
taxometric research. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 128, 271–320.
Hettema, J. M., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2003). The effects of
anxiety, substance use, and conduct disorders on risk of major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 33, 1423–1432.
Hicks, B. M., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Krueger, R. F., & Newman,
J. P. (2004). Identifying psychopathy subtypes on the basis of personality structure. Psychological Assessment, 16, 276 –288.
Huprich, S. K. (2003). Evaluating NEO Personality Inventory—Revised
profiles in veterans with personality disorders. Journal of Personality
Disorder, 17, 33– 44.
Hurt, S., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2002). Personality traits and pathology in
older and younger incarcerated women. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
58, 457– 464.
Iacono, W. G., Carlson, S. R., Taylor, J., Elkins, I. G., & McGue, M.
(1999). Behavioral disinhibition and the development of substance-use
disorders: Findings from the Minnesota Twin Family Study. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 869 –900.
Jackson, D. N. (1989). Personality research form manual (3rd ed.). Port
Huron, MI: Sigma Assessment Systems.
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vernon, P. A. (1998). A twin study of
genetic and environmental contributions to gender differences in traits
delineating personality disorders. European Journal of Personality, 12,
331–344.
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., & Heath, A. C. (1993). Major
depression and phobias: The genetic and environmental sources of
comorbidity. Psychological Medicine, 2, 361–371.
Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The
structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 60, 929 –937.
Klein, D. N. (2003). Patients’ versus informants’ reports of personality
disorders in predicting 7 1/2-year outcome in outpatients with depressive
disorders. Psychological Assessment, 15, 216 –222.
Klein, D. N., & Riso, L. P. (1993). Psychiatric disorders: Problems of
boundaries and comorbidity. In C. G. Costello (Ed.), Basic issues in
psychopathology (pp. 19 – 66). New York: Guilford Press.
Klein, D. N., & Schwartz, J. E. (2002). The relation between depressive
symptoms and borderline personality disorder features over time in
dysthymic disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16, 523–535.
Klein, M. H., Wonderlich, S., & Shea, M. T. (1993). Models of relationships between personality and depression: Toward a framework for
theory and research. In M. H. Klein, S. Wonderlich, & M. T. Shea
(Eds.), Personality and depression: A current view (pp. 1–54). New
York: Guilford Press.
Knutson, B., Wolkowitz, O. M., Cole, S. W., Chan, T., Moore, E. A.,
Johnson, R. C., et al. (1998). Selective alteration of personality and
social behavior by serotonergic intervention. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 373–379.
Kool, S., Dekker, J., Duijsens, I., de Jonghe, F., de Jong, P., & Schouws,
S. (2000). Personality disorders and social functioning in depressed
patients. Social Behavior & Personality, 28, 163–176.
Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives
of General Psychiatry, 56, 921–926.
Krueger, R. F. (2000). Phenotypic, genetic, and nonshared environmental
parallels in the structure of personality: A view from the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 179, 1057–1067.
Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1998). The
structure and stability of common mental disorders (DSM–III–R): A
519
longitudinal-epidemiological study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
107, 216 –227.
Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., & McGee, R. (1996).
Personality traits are differentially linked to mental disorders: A
multitrait–multidiagnosis study of an adolescent birth cohort. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 105, 299 –312.
Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., Patrick, C. J., Carlson, S. R., Iacono, W. G.,
& McGue, M. (2002). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: Modeling the externalizing
spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 411– 424.
Larstone, R. M., Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Vernon, P. A., & Wolf, H.
(2002). The relationship between Eysenck’s P-E-N model of personality,
the five-factor model of personality, and traits delineating personality
dysfunction. Personality and Individual Difference, 33, 25–37.
Laursen, B., Pulkkinen, L., & Adams, R. (2002). The antecedents and
correlates of agreeableness in adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 38,
591– 603.
Lewinsohn, P. M., Steinmetz, J., Larson, D., & Franklin, J. (1981).
Depression-related cognitions: Antecedents or consequences? Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 90, 213–219.
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2003). Comorbidity between and among childhood
externalizing and internalizing disorders: Reflections and directions.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 285–291.
Lilienfeld, S. O., Waldman, I. D., & Israel, A. C. (1994). A critical
examination of the use of the term and concept of comorbidity in
psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 1, 71–103.
Livesley, W. J. (1998). Suggestions for a framework for an empirically
based classification of personality disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 137–147.
Livesley, W. J., & Jackson D. N. (in press). Manual for the Dimensional
Assessment of Personality Pathology. Port Huron, MI: Sigma Press.
Livesley, W. J., Jackson, D. N., & Schroeder, M. L. (1992). Factorial
structure of personality disorders in clinical and general population
samples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 432– 440.
Livesley, W. J., Jang, K., & Vernon, P. A. (1998). Phenotypic and genetic
structure of traits delineating personality disorder. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 55, 941–948.
Livesley, W. J., Schroeder, M. L., Jackson, D. N., & Jang, K. L. (1994).
Categorical distinctions in the study of personality disorder: Implications
for classification. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 6 –17.
Looper, K. J., & Paris, J. (2000). What dimensions underlie Cluster B
personality disorders? Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41, 432– 437.
Lynam, D. R., Leukefeld, C., & Clayton, R. R. (2003). The contribution of
personality to the overlap between antisocial behavior and substance
use/misuse. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 316 –331.
Maggini, C., Ampollini, P., Marchesi, C., Gariboldi, S., & Cloninger, C. R.
(2000). Relationships between Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire dimensions and DSM–III–R personality traits in Italian adolescents. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41, 426 – 431.
Maher, B. A., & Maher, W. B. (1994). Personality and psychopathology:
A historical perspective. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 72–77.
Marinangeli, M. G., Butti, G., Scinto, A., Di Cicco, L., Petruzzi, C.,
Daneluzzo, E., & Rossi, A. (2000). Patterns of comorbidity among
DSM–III–R personality disorders. Psychopathology, 33, 69 –74.
Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the
structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integrative hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 139 –
157.
Maser, J. D., & Cloninger, C. R. (1990). Comorbidity of mood and anxiety
disorders: Introduction and overview. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger
(Eds.), Comorbidity of mood and anxiety disorders (pp. 3–12). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.
McCrae, R. R., Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Riemann, R., & Angleitner, A.
(2001). Sources of genetic structure: Genetic, environmental, and arti-
520
CLARK
factual influences on the covariation of personality traits. Journal of
Personality, 69, 511–535.
Meehl, P. E. (2001). Comorbidity and taxometrics. Clinical Psychology:
Science and Practice, 8, 507–519.
Mineka, S., Watson, D. W., & Clark, L. A. (1998). Psychopathology:
Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annual Review of
Psychology, 49, 377– 412.
Monroe, S. M., & Depue, R. A. (1986). Conceptualization and measurement of human disorder in life stress research: The problem of chronic
disturbance. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 36 –51.
Morey, L. C., Gunderson, J. G., Quigley, B. D., Shea, M. T., Skodol, A. E.,
McGlashan, T. H., et al. (2002). The representation of borderline,
avoidant, obsessive– compulsive, and schizotypal personality disorders
by the five-factor model. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16, 215–234.
Morey, L. C., Warner, M. B., Shea, M. T., Gunderson, J. G., Sanislow,
C. A., Grilo, C., et al. (2003). The representation of four personality
disorders by the Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality
dimensional model of personality. Psychological Assessment, 15, 326 –
332.
Mufson, L., Nomura, Y., & Warner, V. (2002). The relationship between
parental diagnosis, offspring temperament, and offspring psychopathology: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 71, 61– 69.
Mulder, T., Joyce, P. R., Sullivan, P. F., Bulik, C. M., & Carter, F. A.
(1999). The relationship among three models of personality psychopathology: DSM–III–R personality disorder, TCI scores, and DSQ defenses. Psychological Medicine, 29, 943–951.
Mustanski, B. S., Viken, R. J., Kaprio, J., & Rose, R. J. (2003). Genetic
influences on the association between personality risk factors and alcohol use and abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 282–289.
Nagoshi, C. T., Walter, D., Muntaner, C., & Haertzen, C. A. (1992).
Validation of the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire in a sample
of male drug users. Personality & Individual Differences, 13, 401– 409.
Nestor, P. G. (2002). Mental disorder and violence: Personality dimensions
and clinical features. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1973–1978.
Nigg, J. T., John, O. P., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C. L., Willcutt,
E. G., Hinshaw, S. P., & Pennington, B. (2002). Big Five dimensions
and ADHD symptoms: Links between personality traits and clinical
symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 451– 469.
Nitschke, J. B., Heller, W., Imig, J. C., McDonald, R. P., & Miller, G. A.
(2001). Distinguishing dimensions of anxiety and depression. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 25, 1–22.
Norman, W. T. (1967). 2,800 personality trait descriptors: Normative
operating characteristics for a university population. Ann Arbor: Department of Psychology, University of Michigan.
O’Connor, B. P. (2002). The search for dimensional structure differences
between normality and abnormality: A statistical review of published
data on personality and psychopathology. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 83, 962–982.
Oldham, J. M., Skodol, A. E., Kellman, H. D., Hyler, S. E., Rosnick, L., &
Davies, M. (1992). Diagnosis of DSM–III–R personality disorders by
two structured interviews: Patterns of comorbidity. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 149, 213–220.
Osher, Y., Cloninger, C. R., & Belmaker, R. H. (1996). TPQ in euthymic
manic-depressive patients. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30, 353–
357.
Patrick, C. J. (1994). Emotion and psychopathy: Startling new insights.
Psychophysiology, 31, 319 –330.
Pelissolo, A., Andre, C., Pujol, H., Yao, S. N., Servant, D., Braconnier, A.,
et al. (2002). Personality dimensions in social phobics with or without
depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 94 –103.
Pesonen, A.-K., Raikkonen, K., Keskivaara, P., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L.
(2003). Difficult temperament in childhood and adulthood: Continuity
from maternal perceptions to self-ratings over 17 years. Personality and
Individual Differences, 34, 19 –31.
Plomin, R., & Caspi, A. (1998). DNA and personality. European Journal
of Personality, 12, 387– 407.
Poling, J., Rounsaville, B. J., Ball, S., Tennen, H., Kranzler, H. R., &
Triffleman, E. (1999). Rates of personality disorders in substance abusers: A comparison between DSM–III–R and DSM–IV. Journal of Personality Disorders, 13, 375–384.
Pukrop, R. (2002). Dimensional personality profiles of borderline personality disorder in comparison with other personality disorders and healthy
controls. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16, 135–147.
Reich, J., Noyes, R., Hirschfeld, R., Coryell, W., & O’Gorman, T. W.
(1987). State and personality in depressed and panic patients. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 181–187.
Reimann, R., & de Raad, B. (1998). Editorial: Behavior genetics and
personality. European Journal of Personality, 12, 303–305.
Reynolds, S. K., & Clark, L. A. (2001). Predicting personality disorder
dimensions from domains and facets of the five-factor model. Journal of
Personality, 69, 199 –222.
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency
of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of
longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 3–25.
Rose, R. J., Viken, R. J., Dick, D. M., Bates, J. E., Pulkkinen, L., & Kaprio,
J. (2003). It does take a village: Nonfamiliar environments and children’s behavior. Psychological Science, 14, 273–277.
Rothbart, M. K., & Ahadi, S. A. (1994). Temperament and the development of personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 55– 66.
Ryder, A. G., Bagby, R. M., & Schuller, D. R. (2002). The overlap of
depressive personality disorder and dysthymia: A categorical problem
with a dimensional solution. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 10, 337–
352.
Santor, D. A., Bagby, R. M., & Joffe, R. T. (1997). Evaluating stability and
change in personality and depression. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 1354 –1362.
Santor, D. A., & Coyne, J. M. (2001). Evaluating the continuity of
symptomatology between depressed and nondepressed individuals.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 216 –225.
Saulsman, L. M., & Page, A. C. (2004). The five-factor model and
personality disorder empirical literature: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 1055–1085.
Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., & Wood, M. D. (2000). Personality and
substance use disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 68, 818 – 829.
Sher, K. J., & Trull, T. J. (1994). Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: Alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 103, 92–102.
Shiner, R. L. (2000). Linking childhood personality with adaptation: Evidence for continuity and change across time into late adolescence.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 310 –325.
Shiner, R. L., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (2002). A developmental
perspective on personality in emerging adulthood: Childhood antecedents and concurrent adaptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1165–1177.
Siever, L. J., & Davis, K. L. (1991). A psychobiological perspective on the
personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1647–1658.
Siever, L. J., Torgersen, S., Gunderson, J. G., Livesley, W. J., & Kendler,
K. S. (2002). The borderline diagnosis III: Identifying endophenotypes
for genetic studies. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 964 –968.
Simeon, D., Guralnik, O., Knutelska, M., & Schmeidler, J. (2002). Personality factors associated with dissociation: Temperament, defenses,
and cognitive schemata. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 489 – 491.
Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Pfohl, B., Widiger, T. A., Livesley, W. J.,
& Siever, L. J. (2002). The borderline diagnosis I: Psychopathology,
comorbidity, and personality structure. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 936 –
950.
Skodol, A. E., Siever, L. J., Livesley, W. J., Gunderson, J. G., Pfohl, B., &
SPECIAL SECTION: TEMPERAMENT AS THE UNIFYING BASIS
Widiger, T. A. (2002). The borderline diagnosis II: Biology, genetics,
and clinical course. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 951–963.
Soloff, P. H., Kelly, T. M., Strotmeyer, S. J., Malone, K. M., & Mann, J. J.
(2003). Impulsivity, gender, and response to fenfluramine challenge in
borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 119, 11–24.
Stice, E., Burton, E. M., & Shaw, H. (2004). Prospective relations between
bulimic pathology, depression, and substance abuse: Unpacking comorbidity in adolescent girls. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 62–71.
Svrakic, D. M., Whitehead, C., Przybeck, T. R., & Cloninger, C. R. (1993).
Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven-factor model
of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50,
991–999.
Szoeke, A., Schuerhoff, F., Ferhadian, N., Bellivier, F., Rouillon, F., &
Leboyer, M. (2002). Temperament in schizophrenia: A study of the
tridimensional personality questionnaire (TPQ). European Psychiatry,
17, 379 –383.
Tellegen, A. (in press). Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L.
& Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and
together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1031–1039.
Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration
in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders,
61, 201–216.
Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they
mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 160 –164.
Turkheimer, E., & Waldron, M. (2000). Nonshared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin,
126, 78 –108.
Tyrer, P., Gunderson, J., Lyons, M., & Tohen, M. (1997). Extent of
comorbidity between mental state and personality disorders. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 11, 242–259.
Vanman, E. J., Mejia, V. Y., Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Raine, A.
(2003). Modification of the startle reflex in a community sample: Do one
or two dimensions of psychopathy underlie emotional processing? Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 2007–2021.
Vervaet, M., van Heeringen, C., & Audenaert, K. (2004). Personalityrelated characteristics in restricting versus binging and purging eating
disordered patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45, 37– 43.
Vollebergh, W. A. M., Iedema, J., Bijl, R. V., de Graaf, R., Smit, F., &
Ormel, J. (2001). The structure and stability of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 597– 603.
Wachtel, R. (1994). Cyclical processes in personality and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 51–54.
Waldman, I. D., & S. O. (2001). Applications of taxometric methods to
problems of comorbidity: Perspectives and challenges. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 520 –527.
521
Walker, J. L., Lahey, B. B., Russo, M. E., Christ, M. A. G., McBurnett, K.,
Lober, R., et al. (1991). Anxiety, inbihition, and conduct disorder in
children: Relations to social impairment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 187–191.
Watson, D. (2003). Subtypes, specifiers, epicycles, and eccentrics: Toward
a more parsimonious taxonomy of psychopathology. Psychological Science, 10, 233–238.
Watson, D. (2005). Rethinking the mood and anxiety disorders: A
symptom-based hierarchical model for DSM–V. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 114, 522–536.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition to
experience unpleasant emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 465–
490.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1995). Depression and the melancholic
temperament. European Journal of Personality, 9, 351–366.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive emotional
core. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs, (Eds.), Handbook of
personality psychology (pp. 767–793). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general
activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76, 820 – 838.
Watson, D., Wu, K. D., & Cutshall, C. (2004). Symptom subtypes of
obsessive– compulsive disorder and their relation to dissociation. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18, 435– 458.
Widiger, T. A. (1992a). Categorical versus dimensional classification:
Implications from and for research. Journal of Personality Disorder, 6,
287–300.
Widiger, T. A. (1992b). Generalized social phobia versus avoidant personality disorder: A commentary on three studies. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 101, 340 –343.
Widiger, T. A., & Clark, L. A. (2000). Toward DSM–V and the classification of psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 946 –963.
Widiger, T. A., & Rogers, J. H. (1989). Prevalence and comorbidity of
personality disorders. Psychiatric Annals, 19, 132–136.
Widiger, T. A., & Shea, M. T. (2001). Differentiation of Axis I and Axis
II disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 399 – 406.
Wiggins, J. S. (1984). Cattell’s system from the perspective of mainstream
personality theory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 19, 176 –190.
Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1992). Personality: Structure and assessment. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 473–504.
Zimmerman, M., & Coryell, W. (1989). DSM–III personality disorder
diagnoses in a nonpatient sample: Demographic correlates and comorbidity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 682– 689.
Received May 8, 2004
Revision received December 15, 2005
Accepted December 21, 2005 䡲