Risk in Infancy
Origins and Implications
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Claire B. Kopp and Sandra R. Kaler
University of California, Los Angeles
ABSTRACT." This article focuses on biological risks that
can adversely influence development during infancy and
later. In the first part of the article, the origins of risks
and their potential consequences are discussed relative to
prepregnancy, prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal periods.
Epidemiological data are presented. The second part of
the article addresses issues pertaining to assessment of
infant development and interventions provided for infants
whose development may be in jeopardy. Finally, goals for
prevention are highlighted and the need for a nationally
derived data base on risk and outcomes is emphasized.
Infancy, the foundation period of our species, is no more
and no less important than other phases of life. Still, infancy marks the beginning of our contacts with the outside
world, and for this reason it has been accorded special
status by some philosophers, scientists, and lay people.
There are others, we among them, who prefer to think
of infancy as a way-point in childhood's journey. Important, developmentally related events occur before birth,
and many more will occur long after. When genetic heritage and prenatal life are favorable, the infant's roots are
securely anchored and sound development should occur.
If physical or psychological adversities arise later on, there
is potential for resiliency given that rearing conditions
are reasonably supportive. Alternatively, unfavorable genetic or prenatal factors set the stage for vulnerabilities.
Sometimes an adverse condition is so profound that development is irreversibly impaired. In other instances,
risk factors may disrupt processes of growth but not conelusively alter them. In this case development is less predictable and depends on both the nature of early risks
and the child's ensuing life experiences. Often supportive
caregiving can buffer the child and facilitate development,
whereas rearing that is neglectful, uncaring, or abusive
compounds earlier vulnerabilities. Exposed to multiple
and continuing liabilities, a few children manage to adapt,
many more marginally accommodate to educational and
social demands, and still others ultimately fail and deteriorate mentally, behaviorally, and socially.
Risks that compromise development are diverse and
complex. They include those that are (a) biological in
origin; (b) from the environment, primarily adverse rearing conditions; and (c) from a combination of the two.
Our focus is on biological risk, and where appropriate,
combined risks are mentioned as well. Topics discussed
224
include the origins of risks and their effects on development, epidemiology, assessment of status in infancy, and
interventions for infants whose development may be
problematic.
Origins and Implications of Risk
Risk Factors, Development, and Timing
The phrase biological risk encompasses different kinds of
adverse conditions; these are discussed relative to prenatal,
perinatal, and postnatal life along with implications for
development.
Biological risks stem from genetic conditions (e.g.,
mutant recessive genetic material) as well as from exposure to harmful nonsocial environmental factors. Viral
infections or use of drugs during pregnancy are examples
of the latter. Biological risks pose serious threats to the
developing organism and can result in death or outcomes
that include physical malformations, growth retardation,
neurological (e.g., cerebral palsy) and physiological problems, mental retardation, sensory disorders (e.g., blindness or deafness), learning disabilities and other educational difficulties, psychiatric disorders, and social deviancy. Some professionals also consider preterm birth
and low birth weight as negative outcomes.
Biological risks may show developmental effects early
in life or later, may lead to severe impairments or subtle
dysfunctions, and may influence all aspects of human
behavior or only one or two. Because development is a
lifelong process, problems observed in infancy are n o t
necessarily those manifested in subsequent years. On the
other hand, particularly during early life, development,
whether proceeding as expected or not, is associated with
increasing maturation of brain structures and functions.
The immature brain is adaptive to new experiences and,
equally important, often has potential for recovery should
certain kinds of brain insult occur.
Developmental risk, a term frequently used by
professionals, is reserved for immature organisms and
refers to a statistical probability that ongoing development
•will be compromised in some way. Statistical probabilities
are expectations about the percentage of children that
will have developmental problems.
Timing refers to the particular period in early development when a risk event occurs, that is, before pregnancy, at conception, during prenatal life, at the perinatal
period, or after birth (postnatal life). Consideration of
February 1989
•
American Psychologist
Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0003-066X/89/$00.75
Vet. 44, No. 2, 224-230
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
timing leads to inferences about the effects a risk event
can have when the developing organism is at age x as
opposed to one that is at age y (Freeman, 1985; Hagberg,
1978; Kopp, 1983).
In the remainder of this section, risks, developmental
outcomes, and timing are interrelated, and illustrative
examples are provided. We use chronological order beginning with risks that occur before pregnancy and end
with those that arise after birth.
Prepregnancy risks. Data unequivocally show that
deleterious factors occurring years before a pregnancy
may have developmental implications for the conceptus,
throughout infancy and beyond. Prepregnancy risks inelude maternal chronic illness, history of drug use, inadequate nutrition during childhood and adolescence,
genetic vulnerability within the family, previous numerous closely spaced pregnancies, and female age (extremes)
at time of conception.
Among the economically disadvantaged, prepregnancy risks may run through generations. Poor nutrition
and lack of health care during childhood are often compounded by adverse pregnancy conditions (e.g., lack of
prenatal care, drug use, or exposure to infections). Combinations such as this portend constitutionally vulnerable
infants who become vulnerable children and then highrisk childbearing adults. All too often, health and developmental disorders such as poor school achievement and
social deviancy arise. Unfortunately, the extent of these
problems is unknown.
Of course, conditions other than poverty give rise to
prepregnancy risks. In our culture, social and economic
forces have moved increasing numbers of women into the
job market. Concomitantly, these women are postponing
first pregnancies beyond the optimal childbearing period
(20 to 30 years). The largest growth in fertility rates has
occurred among women in their early 30s, with first births
among 30- to 34-year-olds almost doubling between 1970
and 1979 (Dorfman, 1986). A substantial number of these
births were to educated women who were not in economic
distress. This trend in later pregnancies will likely continue.
Concerns have heightened about child outcomes. In
addition to the well-established association between maternal age and chromosomal abnormalities (e.g., the risk
for Down syndrome for women over 40 years is manyfold
greater than for women in their 20s), there has been worry
about other forms of nonoptimal development. Earlier
studies suggested that maternal age was a risk factor in
and of itself; however, conditions that coexist with increasing age such as hypertension or diabetes may explain
previous findings. Recently completed, well-designed
studies are optimistic. In a comparison of 511 pregnancies
Support for writing this article was provided in part by National Science
Foundation Grant BNS 87-10028 and Department of Education Grant
G0086 35232 to Claire B. Kopp.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Claire
B. Kopp, Department of Psychology, Franz Hall, University of California,
Los Angeles, CA 90024.
February 1989 • American Psychologist
of women over 40 with 26,000+ pregnancies of women
between 20-30 years, controlling for maternal weight,
cigarette smoking, and parity, there were no higher risks
for older women (Spellacy, Miller, & Winegar, 1986). Undeniably, entering pregnancy with good overall health
status and having access to careful prenatal monitoring
are critically important.
Prenatal period. In the aggregate, risks that arise
around the time of conception or during the first weeks
of pregnancy (when structures are being formed) are associated with more serious developmental ramifications
than those that arise earlier or later. Impairments tend
to be both major and irreversible and include malformations of head, face, limbs, and organs; sensory and
neurological disorders; and severe mental retardation (IQ
of 50 or less). Brain disorders are often obvious at birth;
later, developmental delays and intellectual problems become apparent. The seriousness of early prenatal risks is
underscored by the fact that few affected individuals escape long-term difficulties, and many require lifelong assistance.
Adverse conditions and events related to prenatal
risks include mutant recessive genes and chromosomal
disorders, environmental agents (e.g., harmful agents such
as drugs, chemicals, and toxins), maternal viral infections
such as rubella, and chronic and severe maternal health
conditions that continue into pregnancy (e.g., major kidney disease and cardiac anomalies). Multiple biological
factors and adverse environmental events are also likely
to be involved.
The developmental picture for later pregnancy risks
is less clear. It is known that some children develop normally, a few will have major handicapping conditions,
and others will show various kinds of less severe intellectual and social impairments. For some indeterminate
number, there will be a variety of dysfunctions involving
physiological and psychological systems (Vorhees, 1986).
The relatively new research discipline called behavioral teratology focuses on behavioral effects subsequent
to stress or damage to the developing brain. Intellectual
and behavioral dysfunctions that occur in the absence of
malformations can be as important as those that occur
with them. Researchers are examining behavioral effects
of alcohol, chemicals, pollutants, and other agents using
a variety of animal models and human studies. There is
evidence that methylmercury, alcohol, lead, and high
doses of ionizing radiation are disruptive to children's
development, and there is suspicion that nicotine, narcotics, some pollutants, hormones, and lower doses of
ionizing radiation may affect development (Vorhees &
Mollinow, 1987). The challenge for behavioral teratology
is substantial because long-term effects as well as transient
ones must be demonstrated while controlling for variables
other than the risk agents, and it is important that the
reason underlying variability in outcome effects be ascertained. A case in point is alcohol: Not all children
born to alcoholic mothers show fetal alcohol syndrome
or even less severe symptomatology.
Other research approaches yield promising findings.
225
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Drawing on suggestive data linking pregnancy illnesses
and schizophrenia, Mednick, Machon, Huttunen, and
Bonett (1988) examined the incidence of schizophrenia
in a 1957 Finnish birth cohort in which pregnant women
were exposed to a widespread severe type A2 influenza
epidemic. Using a variety of procedures, the authors were
able to ascertain that second-trimester viral exposure was
significantly linked to a greater percentage of offspring
who were later admitted to psychiatric hospitals with a
diagnosis of Schizophrenia. Expressing caution about their
findings, the authors nonetheless argue for an association
between mid-pregnancy risk and psychiatric vulnerability.
Moreover, they suggest that second-trimester risk may
hold implications for brain functioning in that risks may
interfere with the development of late appearing cortical
neurons, destruction of existing cells, or disruption of cell
migration. The implications of this research are profound
for defining long-term effects, particularly those that pertain to emotional dysfunction rather than intellectual
impairment.
Perinatal period. This third time frame overlaps
prenatal and postnatal life and extends from the seventh
prenatal month to the end of the first postnatal month.
Illustrating the importance ascribed to this period, perinatal mortality statistics are used as one measure of the
health and well-being of nations.
Perinatal trends in the United States show a 1985
mortality rate of 10.6 deaths per 1,000 live births, contrasted with the 7 to 9 per 1,000 rates found in Scandinavian and some European countries (Miller, 1987; National Center for Health Statistics [NCHS], 1987). Part
of the United States's high rate is due to higher mortality
for certain minority groups (for Blacks the 1984 rate was
18.4, whereas for Whites it was 9.4 per 1,000 [NCHS,
1987]). Poverty, teenage pregnancies, and lack of prenatal
care are implicated (Hughes, Johnson, Rosenbaum, Butler, & Simons, 1988).
What are perinatal risks? Historically, they have been
defined as any symptom or condition that may stress the
fetus or newborn. Thus preterm birth, congenital malformations, undergrowth in the newborn period whether
observed in full-term or preterm babies, respiratory distress, newborn seizures, asphyxia, birth trauma, and
newborn infections have been labeled perinatal risks.
Although categorizing these risks as perinatal has
been effective for deriving general mortality statistics, the
category is less useful for estimating developmental outcomes. More recently, attempts are being made to separate conditions that had their origins in prenatal life from
those that originate solely within the perinatal period.
Profound growth retardation observed in either preterm
or full-term newborns is an example of a risk that began
in the prenatal period, whereas an infection acquired from
the mother during the birth process exemplifies a de novo
perinatal risk. These distinctions encourage more precise
diagnosis and treatment. Moreover, evidence also suggests
that early or late pregnancy risks predispose the fetus to
additional perinatal stresses (Freeman, 1985), and a
combination of prenatal-perinatal risks has more adverse
226
developmental consequences than de novo perinatal risks
(Rosen, 1985).
Even if definitional problems did not exist, perinatal
risks pose difficult challenges for understanding and determining outcomes. For one, nationally derived statistics
for nondeath outcomes are virtually nonexistent. Developmental estimations are generated by different research
and clinical teams. Moreover, documented outcomes for
infancy and childhood vary widely and are a function of
quality of medical care in the newborn period, the risk
condition that is studied, criteria selected as measures of
outcome, and social class and family rearing conditions.
Second, despite evidence that the environment in
which the infant lives is a continuing influence on development, the interaction of biological risk and rearing milieu is largely unspecified. It is clear that perinatal risks
combined with nonoptimal rearing conditions portend
poor developmental outcomes, whereas good rearing
ameliorates the risk (Sameroff& Chandler, 1975; Werner
& Smith, 1982). Because nonoptimal rearing is relatively
more common among socially and economically distressed individuals than among affluent individuals, social
economic status (SES) is a frequently used index of rearing
conditions. However, SES is not a pure measure. Nominally it is a descriptor of education and occupational status, but SES actually subsumes considerable heterogeneity
within middle and lower class categories in terms of caregiver knowledge and attributes, family atmosphere, and
home density. Furthermore, accumulating evidence suggests more extensive variation exists in today's society
among lower class families than in our recent past. This
appears to result from increased levels of joblessness,
poverty, homelessness, drug use, and caregiver emotional
disturbance. Until more systematic documentation is
made of child characteristics, specific rearing conditions,
and home atmosphere factors it will be difficult to further
specify the influences of combined biological and social
risks on development.I
No discussion ofperinatal risks is complete without
calling attention to low birth weight; this is one of the
most highly visible perinatal risks. Low birth weight is
the designation given to infants born weighing less than
5 lb; most often, low birth weight goes along with preterrn
birth, that is, birth at or before the 37th week of pregnancy. Low-birth-weight infants are further differentiated
by their size in relation to gestational age; if disproportional, prenatal stresses were encountered.
Multiple factors contribute to low birth weight, including social and economic conditions, maternal size,
maternal health, smoking during pregnancy, lack of prenatal care, geographic locale, ethnicity, toxic exposure
during pregnancy, poor pregnancy histories, and more
(e.g., Kramer, 1987). However, there is great discrepancy
among our population. Healthy, economically comfortable, White women between 20 and 30 years of age can
Werner and Smith (1982) provided an interesting picture of biological risk factors, child characteristics, and home conditions that distinguish children and adolescents who succeed versus those who fail.
February 1989 • American Psychologist
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
have a 3% low birth weight rate, whereas economically
digressed ethnic teenagers often have rates that are three
times higher (NCHS, 1987).
During the recent past, major improvements have
occurred in long-term outcomes for larger low-birthweight infants as a result of m o d e m medical technology
and skill found in newborn intensive-care nurseries. 2 Sophisticated care is also permitting increased rates o f survival for the tiniest low-birth-weight infants; however, developmental appraisals suggest a greater proportion of
developmental, neurological, and health problems among
this group than with their heavier preterm counterparts,
In terms of numbers, approximately 6% to 8% of the
larger low-birth-weight infants, and perhaps twice that
number for the smallest, will show major developmental
sequelae although there is considerable variability across
samples (e.g., Hoy, Bill, & Sykes, 1988).
Given this extraordinary complexity, what generalizations can be made about perinatal outcomes? As,
suming differentiation in diagnosis, with de novo perinatal
risks most children develop normally, some have mild to
moderate dysfunctions, and a few are seriously handicapped. It is considerably more difficult to repeat even
this most general statement with respect to outcomes for
children exposed to dual prenatal-perinatal risks. More
research with carefully defined groups is needed.
Postnatal risks. The postnatal period extends from
the end of the first month of life to the end o f the first
year. Postnatal risks include respiratory disorders, infections, accidents, exposure to environmental contaminants, and nutritional deficiencies. In some cases, such
as meningitis, a short, acute episode may be as devastating
as a chronic, debilitating condition such as anemia. Postnatal risk events contribute only a small amount to major
handicapping conditions among infants and children
(Hagberg, 1978). Their contribution to less severe developmental problems is unknown. (Discussion of postnatal
risks of nutritional factors and accidents is provided by
Lozoff(pp. 231-236) and Christopherson (pp. 237-241),
respectively, in this issue.
Concluding comment. There is little doubt that risk
events hold serious consequences for development. Steps
can be taken to prevent risks from occurring or, in some
cases, to ameliorate their effects. Preventions and interventions are discussed later in this article.
Epidemiology
In this section, we discuss the number of infants born
each year who have discernible problems and mention
the types and percentages of developmental disordersthat
are noted afterinfancy. With the exception of birth and
mortality statistics,all figuresrepresent approximations
as a resultof differencesin reporting methods, sampling
criteria,selectionof outcome variables,and definitionof
mental retardationand handicap.3
During 1985, more than 3,700,000 births were recorded in the United States.Of these,3% showed evidence
of major malformations (Kalter & Warkany, 1983). In
February 1989 * American Psychologist
real numbers, this means more than 100,000 infants were
born with problems that will interfere with their own
lives and those o f their families. A precise cause for malformations can be identified only 40% of the time (Vorbees, 1986). Examples include Fetal Alcohol Syndrome,
with an overall incidence of I to 2 cases per 1,000 births
(Abel, 1984) and Down syndrome, with a similar overall
incidence rate. 4
Six to seven percent of all births in this country are
of low birth weight (250,000 infants in 1985). Included
in this figure are the 1% of infants who are born with a
birth weight of 3.5 lb or less (Hughes et al., 1988). Excluding those with major malformations, from 1985
births 15,000 of the larger infants and 3,500 smaller ones
have major handicapping conditions.
Unfortunately, these numbers do not convey the
magnitude of the problem. Only the severest and most
obvious difficulties are diagnosed in the first two years of
life. After infancy, 3 individuals per 1,000 members of
our population show evidence of major developmental
disorders. It is thought that most of these stem from prenatal risks. Roughly 35% are due to chromosomal dis.
orders including Down syndrome, 20% to multiple
anomalies, 8% to inadequate fetal supply, 5% to adverse
drug and chemical effects, around 4% to single gene defects (e.g., biochemical disorders that result in Tay Sachs
disease, phenylketonuria, or other conditions associated
with mental retardation), and about 3% to 4% for early
prenatal infections; unknown causes, presumably prenatal in origin, are felt to be instrumental factors in 10%
or more. Perinatal risks account for approximately 10%
and postnatal risks less than 5% of severe developmental
problems (Hagberg, 1975, 1978; Susser, Hauser, Kiely,
Paneth, & Stein, 1985).
Less severe developmental problems (e.g., IQs in the
50-70 range or minor neurological and sensory disorders)
are found in approximately 10 individuals per 1,000
(Haskins, 1986). In contrast to the major developmental
disorders, it is believed that only about 20% are due to
biological causes; the majority are linked to deleterious
social and economic circumstances or to a combination
of biological and social vulnerabilities (Haskins, 1986).
Many of the less severe developmental difficulties are primarily intellectual and social and are not identified until
the child enters educational settings. Sometimes intelligence is in the normal range, but school achievement is
lower than expected; this occurs, for example, among
2 The newborn intensive-carenursery is a relativelyrecent development. Physicianswho specializein the care and treatment of sick and
tiny infants, called neonatologists,have additional training in newborn
function and care.
3There are more similaritiesthan differencesin the rates that are
estimatedto occur in major industrializednations. However,these rates
generallyare inapplicableto developingnations.
4 The averagerate givenfor Fetal AlcoholSyndromeobscuresthe
considerabledisparityfound in the United States. The rate is manyfold
higheramongEskimosand NativeAmericans.The averagerate for Down
syndrome also obscures actual rates by age of pregnant women. The
incidence for a woman in her 20s is often givenas I in 1,000 births,
whereas for a woman over40 the rate is I in 50 births.
227
some low.birth-weight children and most frequently
among boys (Kopp, 1983).
Problematic intellectual functioning and difficulty
in school are serious matters for society. Increasingly, data
indicate that intellectual problems, irrespective of cause,
can be associated with a variety of social and emotional
ills (e.g., Olweus, Block, & Radke-Yarrow, 1986).
Assessments and Interventions
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Assessment of Infant Developmental Status
Infants are typically categorized as those with an uneventful early course and whose development is normal,
those exposed to risk events and whose development is
either suspect or requires observation, and those exposed
to risks that are invariably associated with mental retardation and whose development is delayed. Assessment of
developmental status, and the implications thereof, for
each of these groups has been fraught with challenges.
Nonetheless, assessment takes on heightened meaning
because of recently enacted Education of the Handicapped Act Amendments of 1986 (PL 99-457), which
provide financial assistance to states for implementing
intervention services for infants and families (birth to
two years) where a known handicapping condition exists
or where development is perceived to be at risk. Developmental assessment is mandated prior to provision of
preventive or remedial services. The following discussion
focuses primarily on developmental testing for infants
whose development may be suspect or at risk.
Assessment by a good clinician provides a reasonable
evaluation of infant capability; this is an ongoing process
in which infant and toddler (up to age three years) performance is observed and a judgment is made about developmental status at the time of testing (e.g., normal,
suspect, delayed, or abnormal). Typically, assessment involves clinical evaluation and use of a developmental test,
for example the Gesell Schedules (Gesell & Amatruda,
1941) or the Bayley Scales (Bayley, 1969). The tests are
based on normative trends, that is, the kinds of mental,
motor, language, and social abilities most infants demonstrate at one or another month. A few items ask for
primitive problem solving, reasoning, and memory.
Developmental tests are not comparable to the intelligence and achievement tests given to school-aged
children. The latter ask for practical and abstract intelligence, factual knowledge, arithmetic skills, and sophisticated use of social language. The differences between
developmental assessments and intelligence tests highlight
the differences between infant capability and the capabilities demonstrated and needed by more mature children and adults.
Developmental tests have limitations. They do not
evaluate the efficiency by which mental processes are
used; how effectively particular behaviors are produced,
controlled, or terminated; the characteristics defining
growth of adaptive beha~ors; or even why a minor deviation from an expected response might be developmentally significant.
228
The most vocal challenge to developmental tests has
surfaced around the issue of prediction from infancy to
later childhood capabilities. Repeatedly, research findings
from these tests indicate that predictive accuracy is dubious unless the infant has a known handicap (e.g.,
Down syndrome) invariably associated with later mental
retardation or repeated assessments yield moderate to
severe developmental delays (KOPp & McCall, 1982). Attempts have been made to enhance predictions by combining infant test scores with social class indexes and
family conditions or by cumulating or weighting medical,
behavioral, and performance variables. Increases in levels
of success have been modest (Goodman, in press).
Recently, a new type of infant test has garnered considerable publicity because of claims for prediction. The
procedure involves measurement of infant informationprocessing abilities such as visual attention to and memory for patterns and photographs. Because infants have
some ability to process incoming information from the
earliest months of life, it is suggested that continuity may
exist between the processing abilities of infants and children. Further, it is assumed that individual differences in
infant processing capability will be related to differences
in child information-processing performance. In this regard, several researchers report moderate associations
between infancy and preschool language/intelligence test
performance.
Despite these promising data, caution is warranted
before the more traditional developmental tests are discarded. Some methodological and design issues have yet
to be addressed. However, developmental tests could also
be improved.
In the interim, sufficiently valuable judgments of
ongoing infant capability can be made with traditional
developmental tests if well-trained, developmentally
knowledgeable individuals do the assessments. For the
long term, it is unlikely that any test will provide absolute
definitions of future capabilities for all infants. This would
require skills that science does not have---determining
the degree of learning a brain is capable of and the ability
of children to produce adaptive behaviors and predicting
whether ameliorative or adverse factors will surface in a
child's life.
Interventionsfor Infants
Programs of intervention are based on the assumption
that there are recognizable conditions in infancy that, if
not dealt with appropriately, will result in poor developmental prospects. Interventions may involve services
geared exclusively to infants, to their caregivers, or to
both. Examples include the carefully controlled introduction of social experiences for preterm infants still in
isolettes, provision of counseling and support services for
parents who have an infant with a diagnosed handical>
ping condition, and interventions aimed at teaching an
adolescent mother how to provide play opportunities for
her toddler whose development is moving along slowly
because of a de novo perinatal risk.
All interventions have increased in the past decade.
February 1989 • American Psychologist
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Although few would quarrel with the need for program
services, concerns have risen about effective implementation and evaluation (e.g., Shonkoff & Hauser-Cram,
1987). In fact, interventions per se are being examined
for directions and effectiveness (e.g., Casto & Mastropieri,
1986; Dunst & Snyder, 1986; Guralnick & Bennett, 1987;
Marfo & Kysela, 1985).
The issues, particularly for infant interventions, do
not lend themselves to easy resolution. Aside from challenges that arise because of differences in program goals
and orientation, characteristics of the recipients of intervention, and methods of evaluation, infancy presents its
own contradictions. At the age when the brain is considered to be most open to learning and experiences, many
infant behaviors are remarkably resistant to modification.
Driven by strong biological givens, they have their own
timetable for growth. Thus teasing out effects requires
analysis of the content of intervention in terms of processes targeted for change along with an understanding
of processes that are most susceptible to change (Horowitz, 1987; Keogh & Kopp, 1978).
Because Public Law 99-457 provides financial assistance for infant interventions when development is or
might be compromised, there is opportunity to service
more needy infants and families than ever before. Yet
policies to determine who should be served vary widely.
In some instances, decisions are based on assessment data
and include inferences about when to intervene, the nature of the interventions that should be provided, and the
kinds of outcomes that can be anticipated. Other times,
services are provided on the basis of infant diagnosis (e.g.,
known handicap or risk condition), because of group
membership (e.g., economic distress), or even intervenor
philosophy (e.g., that all preterm infants require stimulation). This unsystematic approach has ramifications for
training and certification of personnel, quality programming~ and availability of resources at the state and community levels (Burke, McLaughlin, & Valdivieso, 1988,
provide a timely discussion of personnel issues).
The content and techniques of intervention should
differ relative to age and condition of the infant at risk,
the circumstances of risks, caregiver information and
emotional needs, program models, and personnel and
program resources. This diversity leads to questions about
how to measure program outcomes. Several have argued
for more focus on adaptive characteristics of families,
analysis of infant behavioral processes, and measurement
of infant social performance rather than extensive reliance
on developmental or intelligence test performance. We
would also add measures that capture growth of infant
adaptive skills, including those related to self-regulatory
abilities, emotional control, and anticipatory planning.
These are skills often found wanting among children with
developmental, educational, and social difficulties. Use
of well-defined sample groups for study of outcomes and
long-term follow-ups is also advised (Dunst & Snyder,
1986; Shonkoff & Hauser-Cram, 1987).
Although this brief discussion has highlighted stumbling blocks for infant intervention, it should not obscure
February 1989 • American Psychologist
the real and important progress made in the field. Infant
intervention is a fact, multiple services are available, and
researchers and clinicians are working together toward
clarifying the meaning of infant growth and development
in the face of risk. All of this did not exist a generation ago.
Future Directions
This article has focused on the origins of biological risk,
influences on infancy and later, assessment of infant developmental status, and interventions for infants. We have
called attention to progress that has been made as well
as to challenges that remain. Our discussion of risks and
developmental outcomes underscores the importance of
establishing policies so that this nation's infants and children can optimally function in tomorrow's society. Although it is unlikely that biological risks will be completely
eradicated, we can set reasonable goals for prevention
and thereby reduce the incidence of adverse outcomes.
Accordingly, prevention of avoidable prenatal, perinatal,
and postnatal risks should be a high priority.
It is our belief that prevention will be served best by
formulating information campaigns directed toward
young children, adolescents, high-risk family groups,
pregnant women, and new parents with a goal of informing without alarming. Innovative, dynamic media messages about nutrition, personal health care, drug and alcohol abuse, known and possible environmental pollutants, contraceptive options, pregnancy diagnosis, prenatal
care, and infant development could be systematically disseminated. Effective information models currently exist
(e.g., drugs and children, screening efforts for Tay Sachs
disease, and avoidance of rubella) and could be the basis
for additional efforts involving national agencies, community groups, school systems, health clinics, and recreational associations.
Finally, we suggest that the United States require a
nationally derived data base that would permit definition
of the scope of developmental problems, establishment
of research and service priorities based on that knowledge,
and determination of resource allocation for preventions
and interventions. With reference to infants at risk, a
national data base would provide better understanding of
infants who have been exposed to risk conditions, the
nature of the risks, the percentage of infants and children
who have known handicapping conditions, the percentage
of infants whose development is considered to be at risk,
and the percentage of infants who subsequently develop
educational and social problems in childhood. The multidisciplinary expertise required to mount such a coordinated effort is available.
REFERENCES
Abel,E. L. (1984).Fetal alcohol syndrome andfetal alcohol effects. New
York: Plenum.
Bayley,N. ( 1969). The Bayley Scales of Mental Development. NewYork:
PsychologicalCorporation.
Burke, P. J., McLaughlin, M. J., & Valdivieso,C. H. (1988). Preparing
professionalsto educatehandicappedinfantsand youngchildren:Some
229
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
policy considerations. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education,
8, 73-80.
Casto, G., & Mastropieri, M. A. (1986). The efficacy ofeady intervention
programs: A meta-analysis. Exceptional Children, 52, 417--424.
Christophersen, E. R. (1989). Injury control. American Psychologist, 44,
237-241.
Dorfman, S. E (1986). Age as a factor in pregnancy. Contemporary OB/
GYN. 24, 64-77.
Dunst, C. J., & Snyder, S. W. (1986). A critique of the Utah State University early intervention meta-analysis research. Exceptional Children,
52, 269-276.
The Education of the Handicapped Act Amendments of 1986, Pub. L.
No. 99-457.
Freeman, J. M. (1985). Summary. In J. M. Freeman (Ed.), Prenataland
perinatal factors associated with brain disorders. (NIH Publication
No. 85-1149, pp. 3-32). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health
and Human Services.
Gesell, A., & Amatruda, C. S. ( 1941 ). Developmental diagnosis: Normal
and abnormal child development. New York: Hoeber.
Goodman, J. (in press). Infant intelligence: Do we, can we, should we
assess it? In C. R. Reynolds & R. Kamphaus (Eds.), Handbook of
psychological and educational assessment. New York: Guilford.
Guralnick, M. J., & Bennett, E C. (1987). The effectiveness of early
interventionfor at-risk and handicapped children. Orlando: Academic
Press.
Hagberg, B. (1975). Ire-, peri- and postnatal prevention of major neuropediatric handicaps. Neuropaediatrie, 6. 331-338.
Hagberg, B. (1978). Severe mental retardation in Swedish children born
1959-1970: Epidemiolngical panorama and causative factors. In Major
mental handicap." Methods and costs of preventiou (Ciba Foundation
Symposium No. 59, pp. 29-51). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Haskins, R. (1986). Social and cultural factors in risk assessment and
mild mental retardation. In D. C. Farran & J. D. McKinney (Eds.),
Risk in intellectualand psychosocialdevelopment (pp. 29-60). Orlando:
Academic Press.
Horowitz, E D. (1987). Tar~:ting infant stimulation efforts: Theoretical
challenges for research and intervention. In N. Gunzenhauser (Ed.),
Infant stimulation: For whom, what kind, when, and how much? (pp.
97-108). Skillman, N J: Johnson & Johnson.
Hoy, E. A., Bill, J. M., & Sykes, D. H. (1988). Very low birthweight: A
long-term developmental impairment? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 37-67.
Hughes, D., Johnson, K., Rosenbaum, S., Butler, E., & Simons, J. (1988).
The health of America's children: Maternal and child health data
book. Washington, DC: Children's Defense Fund.
Kalter, H., & Warkany, J. (1983). Congenital malformations: Etiologic
factors and their role in prevention. New England Journal of Medicine,
308, 424-431,491--497.
Kcogh, B. K., & Kopp, C. B. (1978). From assessment to intervention:
an elusive bridge. In E D. Minifie & L. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Communicative
and cognitive abilities: Early behavioral assessment. Baltimore: University Park Press.
KOPp, C. B. (1983). Risk factors in development. In M. M. Haith &
230
J. J. Campos (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy
and developmentalpsychobiology (pp. 1081-1188). New York: Wiley.
Kol~, C. B., & McCall, R. B. (1982). Predicting later mental performance
for normal, at-risk, and handicapped infants. In P. B. Baltes & O. G.
Brim (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 33-60).
New York: Academic Press.
Kramer, M. S. (1987). Intrauterine growth retardation and gestational
duration determinants. Pediatrics, 80, 502-51 I.
Lozoff, B. (1989). Nutrition and behavior. American Psychologist, 44,
231-236.
Marfo, K., & Kysela, G. M. (1985). Early intervention with mentally
handicapped children: a critical appraisal of applied research. Journal
of Pediatric Psychology, 10, 305-324.
Mednick, S. A., Machon, R. A., Huttenen, M. O., & Bonett, D. (1988).
Adult schizophrenia following prenatal exposure to an influenza epidemic. Archives of General Psychiat~ 45, 189-192.
Miller, A. C. (1987). Maternal health and infant survival. Washington,
DC: National Center for Clinical Infant Programs.
National Center for Health Statistics. (1987). Advance report of final
natality statistics, 1985 (Monthly vital statistics report, Vol. 36).
Washington, DC: Author.
Olweus, D., Block, J., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1986). Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior:Research. theo~ and issues. Orlando:
Academic Press.
Rosen, M. G. (1985). Factors during labor and delivery that influence
brain disorders. In J. M. Freeman (Ed.), Prenatal andperinatalfactors
associated with brain disorders (NIH Publication No. 85-1149, pp.
239-262). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. J. (1975). Reproductive risk and the
continuum of earetaking casualty. In E D. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of
child development research(Vol. 4, pp. 187-244). Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Shonkoff, J. P., & Hanscr-Cram, P. (1987). Early intervention for disabled
infants and their families: A quantitative analysis. Pediatrics, 80, 650657.
Spellacy, W. N., Miller,S. J.,& Winegar, A. (1986). Pregnancy after40
years of age. Obstetricsand Gynecology,,68, 452--454.
Susser, M. B., Hauser, W. A., Kiely,J. L., Paneth, N., & Stein,Z. (1985).
Quantitative estimates of prenatal and perinatal risk factorsfor perinatal mortality, cerebral palsy, mental retardation, and epilepsy. In
J. M. Freeman (Ed.), Prenatal and perinatal factors associated with
brain disorders (NIH Publication No. 85-1149, pp. 359--439). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Vorhees, C. V. (1986). Origins of behavioral teratology. In E. P. Riley &
C. V. Vorhees (Eds.), Handbook of behavioral teratology (pp. 3-22).
New York: Plenum.
Vorhees, C. V., & Moilinow, E. (1987). Behavioral teratogenesis: Longterm influences on behavior from early exposure to environmental
agents. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd
ed., pp. 913-971). New York: Wiley.
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible. New
York: McGraw-Hill.
F e b r u a r y 1989 * A m e r i c a n Psychologist