652942
RERXXX10.3102/0034654316652942Scammacca et al.Reading Interventions 1914–2014
research-article2016
Review of Educational Research
Month 201X, Vol. XX, No. X, pp. 1–45
DOI: 10.3102/0034654316652942
© 2016 AERA. http://rer.aera.net
A Century of Progress: Reading Interventions for
Students in Grades 4–12, 1914–2014
Nancy K. Scammacca
The Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk,
The University of Texas at Austin
Garrett J. Roberts
The University of Denver
Eunsoo Cho
Michigan State University
Kelly J. Williams, Greg Roberts, Sharon R. Vaughn,
and Megan Carroll
The Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk,
The University of Texas at Austin
The history of research on interventions for struggling readers in Grades 4
through 12 dates back to 19th-century case studies of seemingly intelligent
children who were unable to learn to read. Physicians, psychologists, educators, and others were determined to help them. In the process, they
launched a century of research on a wide variety of approaches to reading
intervention. As shown in this systematic narrative review, much has
changed over time in the conceptualization of reading interventions and the
methods used to determine their efficacy in improving outcomes for struggling readers. Building on the knowledge gathered over the past 100 years,
researchers and practitioners are well-poised to continue to make progress
in developing and testing reading interventions over the next 100 years.
KEYWORDS:
struggling readers, reading disabilities, history of reading
interventions
The history of reading interventions began with the desire to solve a mystery.
In the late 19th century, physicians and teachers discovered that some children
with average or above-average IQs and normal vision and hearing were unable to
learn to read. This discovery launched a quest to learn the reasons why and to
develop ways to treat this enigmatic condition. One pioneer, physician W. Pringle
1
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
Morgan, wrote about the case of a bright, 14-year-old boy with a near-complete
inability to read in an article in the British Medical Journal that he titled
“Congenital Word Blindness” (Morgan, 1896). Noting that adults with damage to
the left angular gyrus of the brain found it difficult or impossible to read, Morgan
assumed that his patient suffered from a congenital defect in the same area. He
adopted the term word blind from Adolph Kussmaul, a German physician who
had used it nearly 20 years earlier to describe patients who had lost the ability to
read due to a brain injury or stroke (Kussmaul, 1877).
From the turn-of-the-20th-century case studies such as Morgan’s that documented attempts to understand and treat reading disabilities to 21st-century largescale randomized control trials (RCTs), the history of reading interventions has
been a colorful one. Theories of etiology and corresponding treatments from the
nascent days of the field may seem puzzling at best when viewed from the perspective afforded by decades of hindsight. However, synthesizing this history provides contemporary researchers and practitioners with valuable insight into how
the field arrived where it is today—including the origin of some prevailing practices. Knowing this history allows contemporary researchers and practitioners to
understand those whose shoulders we stand on and the thought processes and field
trials they undertook in attempts to help students who seemingly could not learn
to read through typical classroom instruction.
Surprisingly, no history of reading interventions has been published since A. J.
Harris (1967) summarized the highlights of intervention research published
between 1916 and 1965 in an address to the International Reading Association.
More recently, remedial reading has been included only as a subtopic in historical
treatments of the teaching of reading such as Smith’s (2002) American Reading
Instruction. A number of meta-analyses and systematic reviews are available that
synthesize studies from the mid-1970s onward (e.g., Edmonds et al., 2009;
Mastropieri, Scruggs, Bakken, & Whedon, 1996; Scammacca, Roberts, Vaughn,
& Stuebing, 2015; H. L. Swanson, 1999; Wanzek et al., 2013). However, no systematic review of reading interventions has been published that traces the history
of the field from the earliest efforts to understand reading disabilities to presentday randomized experimental investigations.
Given the ever-increasing importance of unlocking the mystery of reading disabilities and the need for continual improvement in developing and implementing
interventions for struggling readers, especially beyond the primary grades, we undertook the task of synthesizing 100 years of published research on reading interventions for students in Grades 4 to 12. Our goal was to systematically review the
published literature in a way that highlights the lessons of the past to inform present
and future research and practice. We took a decade-by-decade approach, focusing on
interventions for students in Grades 4 to 12 (ages 9–21) and seeking to document the
theories and practices of reading interventions researched over the past century.
Older struggling readers—students in Grades 4 to 12 whose reading ability is
below normative expectations—are the focus of our review because, historically,
students in this age-group were the first to be targeted for intervention. The attention
of pioneers in the field was riveted on upper elementary, middle, and high school
students who did not benefit from typical reading instruction received in the
primary grades despite having at least average hearing, eyesight, and intellectual
ability. Today, interventions aimed at helping struggling readers in Grades 4 to 12
2
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
remain critical to efforts to improve reading proficiency nationwide. The 2013
National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) reading assessment determined that 65% of fourth graders and 64% of eighth graders were not proficient readers. These statistics improved little from the previous decades, with the 2003 NAEP
reading assessment results showing that 69% of fourth graders and 68% of eighth
graders scored below the proficient level, and the 1992 NAEP reading assessment,
where 72% of fourth graders and 71% of eighth graders scored below proficient. A
look back to the origins of reading interventions and at the paths that researchers and
practitioners have traveled over the past 100 years presents an opportunity for 21stcentury reading intervention researchers to press forward in areas that have been fruitful using the technological and methodological advances now available. Additionally,
we present this 100-year review of reading interventions in honor of the 100th anniversary of the founding of the American Educational Research Association.
Analytical Approach
In determining our approach to synthesizing a century of research on reading
interventions, we chose a systematic review for several reasons. First, the standards of measurement in place during the first 65 or so years of research are more
amenable to summarization in a systematic review than a quantitative meta-analysis. Before 1980, researchers in nearly every published study of reading interventions reported their results using age-equivalent (AE) or grade-equivalent (GE)
scores from standardized assessments. Since these studies were published, reporting results in this manner has fallen out of favor because AE and GE scores are not
on an equal-interval scale (Berk, 1981). As a result, GE and AE scores cannot be
averaged across individuals to produce an accurate mean score and pretest GEs or
AEs cannot be subtracted from posttest GEs or AEs to compute meaningful gains,
two practices commonly found in reporting of results in reading intervention studies from the 1910s to 1970s. This limitation of GE and AE scores and their prevalence in the early reading intervention literature meant that for nearly all of the
studies located for this review that were published before 1980, no effect sizes
could be calculated to compare their outcomes. Therefore, we believe that synthesizing their results in a narrative format was the most appropriate approach.
Additionally, synthesizing the history of reading interventions through a systematic narrative review complements the available meta-analyses that focus on
reading interventions for struggling readers in Grades 4 to 12 between 1976 and
2011 (e.g., Mastropieri et al., 1996; Scammacca et al., 2015). These meta-analyses provide efficient quantitative syntheses of the effects of reading interventions;
our historical review addresses the broader context in which these studies were
conducted and synthesizes research from earlier decades when meaningful effect
sizes cannot be computed. We synthesize the findings of previous meta-analyses
as part of our narrative review and address the rise of meta-analysis as a theme in
the history of reading interventions.
Furthermore, a systematic narrative review is the ideal way to address the heterogeneity in research methods, participants, treatment approaches, and other factors
that is inevitably present when looking at a century of research in any field of study.
Research on reading interventions changed in substantial ways as it was affected by
advances in medicine, psychology, statistics, measurement, technology, and other
fields over the past 100 years. Within the United States, federal legislation and
3
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
funding priorities also have exerted a substantial influence over the type of research
conducted, especially over the past 20 years. When attempting to summarize a
group of studies with a great deal of heterogeneity, a narrative systematic review is
preferable (Petticrew & Roberts, 2006).
Finally, through this systematic review we expected to be able to answer the
following research questions:
Research Question 1: What types of interventions for struggling readers in
Grades 4 to 12 have been researched over the past 100 years and which ones
were shown to be effective in helping struggling readers?
Research Question 2: How has research on reading interventions evolved in
theory, method, and results over the past 100 years?
Research Question 3: What can be gleaned from the past century of reading
interventions research to inform and direct current and future researchers, educators, policymakers, and other stakeholders?
Method
Literature Search
To locate studies published between 1914 and 1975, we conducted a computer
search of ERIC and PsycINFO. Descriptors or root forms of these descriptors (reading
difficult*, learning disab*, LD, mild handi*, mild disab*, reading disab*, at-risk,
high-risk, reading delay*, learning delay*, struggling reader, dyslex*, read*, comprehen*, vocabulary, fluen*, word, decod*, English Language Arts) were used in combinations to capture the greatest possible number of studies. Our initial search resulted
in 2,557 abstracts. These abstracts were screened to determine if they met the inclusion criteria specified below. A search of other research syntheses was also done, and
their reference lists along with seminal studies and monographs were reviewed to
identify studies published in this time period that met the inclusion criteria.
The advent of meta-analysis, a term first coined by Glass (1976), represented a
fundamental shift in the history of reading interventions research. The first published
meta-analysis of reading interventions that included students in Grades 4 to 12 was
Mastropieri et al. (1996). They included research published between 1976 and 1996.
Listed in the order in which they were published, other meta-analyses followed: H.
L. Swanson (1999); Elbaum, Vaughn, Hughes, and Moody (2000); Scammacca et al.
(2007); Edmonds et al. (2009); Flynn, Zheng, and Swanson (2012); Wanzek et al.
(2013); and Scammacca et al. (2015). These meta-analyses represent rigorous efforts
to locate and synthesize available experimental and quasi-experimental research
from 1976 to 2011. Therefore, to locate studies published in this time period for this
systematic review, we searched the reference lists of these meta-analyses and selected
all studies that met the inclusion criteria described below. To locate studies published
between 2012 and 2014, years that have not yet been included in a published metaanalysis, we applied the same search criteria used to locate studies published between
1914 and 1975 and included those that met our inclusion criteria.
Inclusion Criteria
Studies found through the literature search were included in this systematic
review if they met all of the following criteria:
4
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Participants were English-speaking struggling readers. Struggling readers
were defined as those with low achievement in reading, unidentified reading difficulties, dyslexia, and/or with reading or learning disabilities (LDs).
Studies also were included if disaggregated data were provided for struggling readers regardless of the characteristics of other students in the study.
Participants were in Grades 4 to 12 (age 9–21). When a sample also included
older or younger students and it could be determined that the sample mean
age was within the targeted range or that 50% or more of the participants
were in the targeted age or grade range, the study was accepted. Studies also
were included if disaggregated data were provided for students in Grades 4
to 12 even if older and/or younger students also participated in the study.
The study’s research design used a multiple-group experimental or quasiexperimental treatment comparison design or a multiple treatment comparison design. Case studies and studies using a single-group pre/post
design published from 1914 to 1975 were retained to better understand the
theories and methods of treatment in the decades before multiple-group
experimental and quasi-experimental designs were widely used. Had we
not included these studies, very little research before 1976 would have met
our inclusion criteria.
The intervention provided any focus for reading intervention, including
word study, fluency, vocabulary, reading comprehension, or multiple components of reading instruction. We considered a study to have provided a
reading intervention if the treatment was viewed as a remedy for reading
disabilities at the time it was published.
Outcomes were assessed using at least one measure of one or more reading constructs and results for tests of effects on these measures were
reported at the group level in group design studies.
The study was published in an academic journal, compilation of conference proceedings, or monograph. We limited our searched to published
works due to the infeasibility of locating unpublished works across a 100year time span.
See Table 1 for the number of studies included in this review by decade and
research design.
Coding Procedures
The Vaughn, Elbaum, Wanzek, Scammacca, and Walker (2014) code sheet
was used for coding studies that met the inclusion criteria and were published
between 1976 and 2014. This code sheet includes elements specified in the
What Works Clearinghouse (WWC) Design and Implementation Assessment
Device (Institute of Education Sciences [IES], 2008) and used in previous
research (e.g., Scammacca et al., 2015; Wanzek et al., 2013). Data coded
included participant characteristics, description of the methodology and intervention, indicators of study quality, properties of measures, and data for calculating effect sizes. Researchers with doctorate degrees and doctoral students
with experience coding studies for other meta-analyses completed the code
sheets. All coders had completed training on how to complete the code sheet
5
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
TABLE 1
Number of intervention studies reviewed by decade and type
Decade
1914–1919
1920s
1930s
1940s
1950s
1960s
1970s
1980s
1990s
2000s
2010–2014
Case study/
Whole-class/
review
Pre/post
wholeof case
singleschool/
records
group whole-district
0
2
4
6
2
3
2
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
3
1
8
4
0
0
0
0
0
0
2
1
1
0
1
0
0
0
0
Treatment
comparison
experimental/quasi-experimental
Multiple
treatments
experimental/quasi-experimental Total
0
0
0
0
3
3
6
15
14
21
27
0
0
0
1
1
3
1
8
7
3
3
1
2
6
11
8
17
14
23
21
24
30
and had reached a high level of reliability with others coding the same article
independently. Two raters independently coded every study. When discrepancies were found, coders reviewed the article together and discussed the coding
until consensus was reached. Code sheets for the period between 1980 and
2011 had been completed for earlier meta-analyses covering this time period
(Scammacca et al., 2007; Scammacca et al., 2015; Wanzek et al., 2013). Code
sheets for 2012 to 2014 were completed for the present review using the same
procedures described above for the 1980 to 2011 studies.
We attempted to use the same code sheet and coding procedures for the studies meeting inclusion criteria for 1914 to 1975 but discovered that the nature of
the research published during this period and the way in which it was reported in
the literature did not fit a code sheet designed to capture details from more contemporary studies. The vast majority of the fields on the code sheet could not be
coded due to lack of detail in the published studies and differences in reporting
standards for journal articles over time. For the sake of coding efficiency, we
developed a shortened version of the Vaughn et al. (2014) code sheet to capture
the relevant details from the studies published between 1914 and 1975.
Results
In presenting the results, we take a decade-by-decade approach due to the historical nature of our systematic review. Our focus is on discussing the predominant themes that emerged in the research published in each decade. Taking this
approach allowed us to trace the developments in the field in a way that shows
how the research evolved over time based on both previous research and events
that occurred in the historical context of each decade. In the periods 1914 to 1919
6
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
and 1920 to 1929, the field of reading interventions was in its infancy and few
studies were published. Published research grew in volume beginning in the
1930s and the number of publications continues to expand through the present
day. To capture the characteristics and outcomes of the corpus of studies that met
our inclusion criteria without bogging down the narrative, we present the bulk of
the studies included in our review in tables by decade. These tables are available
as a supplement in the online version of this journal. They begin with Table S1 for
the 1930s and continue through Table S9 for 2010–2014 (see Supplementary
Tables S1-S9 in the online version of the journal).
1914–1919: Laying the Foundation
Several developments in the second decade of the 20th century focused attention
on older struggling readers. Thorndike (1914) highlighted the need for accurate,
normative assessments of reading to identify individuals who were struggling and
presented experimental scales for measuring vocabulary and reading comprehension. In 1915, Kelly developed the first standardized assessment of reading comprehension, the Kansas Silent Reading Tests, and published the measure the following
year (Kelly, 1916). The assessment had forms for measuring the reading ability of
children in Grades 3 to 5, 6 to 8, and 9 to 12. Gray (1916a, 1916b) also published
tests of silent and oral reading with grade-level standards for students in Grades 1 to
8. These innovations in assessment allowed teachers to determine via a very brief,
objective test whether students were performing as expected for their grade.
Uhl (1916) used findings from both the Kansas and Gray tests to individualize
treatment for poor readers in a summer program for students in Grades 3 to 8.
Uhl’s report is one of the earliest published studies on the efficacy of a programmatic intervention for a group of struggling readers. Uhl also is credited with
being the first to use the term remedial (Smith, 2002), which appeared in the title
of his report, to describe a reading intervention. The Kansas test was used to
screen all students enrolled in summer school; those with the lowest scores were
then tested individually with the Gray Oral Reading Test to confirm that they were
indeed struggling readers. Tutors worked with students daily for 6 weeks. The
treatment involved 15-minute drills focused on the deficits observed in the test
results. Tutors directed the student to work on remedying a specific aspect of oral
reading, such as reading in meaningful words groups (e.g., phrases and sentences)
instead of word-by-word, while absorbing the meaning of the text being read. The
students were tested at the conclusion of summer school, and the growth of those
in the reading program was compared to that of students who did not qualify for
intervention and received classroom instruction only. Results indicated that the
students who received intervention had similar pre–post gains as their peers who
did not require intervention.
An important development of this time period was the entry of the United
States into World War I in 1917. The U.S. military discovered that thousands of
soldiers were unable to comprehend simple written instructions, bringing the
issue of older struggling readers to the forefront as a matter of national security
(Smith, 2002). With the advent of standardized assessments and the initial publications of reading research, the 1910s laid the foundation for the 1920s to be a
decade of progress in interventions for older struggling readers.
7
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
1920s: Progress and Innovation
The dawn of the 1920s saw the opening of the first clinic focused on reading
disabilities, which was founded by Grace Fernald at the University of California,
Los Angeles’ University Training School (Smith, 2002). Here, she developed the
kinesthetic approach to reading intervention that she described in a publication
she coauthored with Helen Keller (Fernald & Keller, 1921). This kinesthetic intervention, which came to be known as the “Fernald method,” was implemented for
any students at the clinic who did not make progress when taught individually
using typical instructional methods. It began with having students master one
word at a time through first tracing the letters in the word written in cursive on a
chalkboard with their fingers and then writing the word themselves from memory
while pronouncing its syllables. Students progressed to learning to recognize the
same word in print and then repeated the process with words combined into sentences and paragraphs and finally to independent silent reading. Fernald and
Keller presented three case studies in which non-readers achieved grade-level
fluency and comprehension after four to six months of treatment.
Around the same time, Gray (1921), working with students at the University of
Chicago’s elementary school, advocated an approach to reading interventions that
contained many elements of what is now called a response to instruction (RTI)
framework. He called for universal screening of fluency and comprehension at the
start of each school year to determine which students needed reading interventions. For those found to be struggling readers, he recommended small group
instruction focused on remedying deficits detected through diagnostic assessments. Students who did not make adequate progress in small group instruction
were referred to a reading specialist for individualized instruction.
Gray described his procedure for the diagnosis and treatment of children with
reading disabilities and research findings in his 1922 monograph, Remedial Cases in
Reading: Their Diagnosis and Treatment. In this work, he reported on the specific
problems often seen in struggling readers, such as poor auditory memory, inadequate
spoken vocabulary, lack of interest in reading, and insufficient knowledge of phonics. Believing that proper remediation could only be provided once an accurate
understanding of the child’s reading problem was achieved, Gray’s treatment
approach devoted much time at the beginning of a case to assessment and observation of the child’s attempts at reading and other schoolwork to note the types of errors
the child was prone to make. Additionally, Gray and his team gathered a careful history of the child’s school experience, family life, and physical health. The information gathered guided the development of an individualized treatment plan. Gray
noted that his approach was so individualized that it was not possible to describe the
treatment in any general way; he referred readers to the case studies that comprise the
bulk of his monograph to learn what was done to help each child. He did state that
treatment included exercises to improve word and phrase recognition, story recall,
and reading comprehension. Gray also emphasized the importance of choosing reading material that appealed to the student’s interests. Many of Gray’s suggestions
remain key elements in reading interventions for struggling readers today.
While Fernald was treating students in Los Angeles and Gray in Chicago,
another reading intervention pioneer was at work at Columbia University’s
Teachers College in Manhattan. In 1922, Arthur Gates produced a monograph
8
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
based on his work in schools in the New York City area. Gates echoed Gray’s call
for thorough assessment to determine the specific nature of each child’s reading
problem. In the monograph, he documented his research on the causes of what he
called “backwardness” in reading. These included inadequate reading instruction
at school and/or home; lack of interest in reading leading to lack of effort; neurosis; uncorrected vision, hearing, speech, or motor problems; cognitive deficits that
lead to problems with memory or perception, including congenital word blindness; and a combination of these and other undiscovered factors.
Five years later, Gates (1927) published the first of what would become three
editions of his book The Improvement of Reading: A Program of Diagnostic and
Remedial Methods. The intended audience was teachers and remedial reading
specialists. Gates (1927) recommended strategies for remedying reading deficits
in word recognition, vocabulary acquisition, left-to-right eye tracking, and reading comprehension. He emphasized the importance of developing a complete
understanding of a struggling reader’s particular difficulties and designing an
individualized intervention. In addition to providing guidance for diagnosing and
intervening with struggling readers, Gates (1927) described the standardized oral
and silent reading tests he had developed and published in 1926. The following
year, Gates published diagnostic tests to help pinpoint the areas in which a student
was struggling in order that the proper intervention could be applied. Gates’s tests,
along with Gray’s measures, were used widely in research on interventions for
struggling readers in the decades that followed and remain in use in updated forms
today.
Gates and Gray represent the approach that educators and educational psychologists took to helping struggling readers in the 1920s. Physicians also studied
reading disabilities during this era, but from a different angle. Whereas education
researchers focused on developing tests that revealed skill deficits and finding
teaching techniques to remedy them, physicians’ first priority was to diagnose the
underlying physical cause and then to develop treatments targeting it (A. J. Harris,
1967). One such physician of the 1920s was Samuel Orton.
Orton (1925) presented the case of a 16-year-old boy who was able to read
very few words and had been diagnosed with congenital word blindness. To
Orton, this diagnosis seemed to be a poor fit for the boy’s symptoms, so he admitted the boy to the Iowa State Psychiatric Hospital for further evaluation. Orton
noted that the boy often read words from right to left, scrambled syllables within
words, and produced many reversals when asked to copy words. After assessing
this boy and a number of other children with extreme reading difficulty, Orton
noted that all could write about equally well with either hand and read text in a
mirror with about the same number of errors as standard text. Other common traits
noted among this group of children included a higher-than-average incidence of
stuttering, being left-handed, and having motor coordination and balance issues.
Additionally, 14 of the 15 students referred to Orton with severe reading difficulties were boys.
This constellation of factors, coupled with the knowledge available at the time
on the function of various regions of the brain and the effects of destruction of a
region in one or the other hemisphere of the brain, led Orton to an interesting
conclusion about the source of severe reading difficulty in the cases he examined.
9
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
Orton (1925) theorized that as youngsters begin learning the alphabet, the brain
stores the letters in the correct orientation in the language-dominant hemisphere
and stores the mirror image of the letters in the nondominant hemisphere. In the
process of learning to read, the typical child learns to ignore the mirror images and
focuses on the proper orientation of letters and words, and eventually, the memory
trace to the mirror images disappears. However, in children with severe reading
difficulty, neither hemisphere is dominant for language, and memory traces to
both hemispheres remain active. As a result, the child who is reading disabled
accesses letters and words both in correct and mirror image orientation at random.
Orton thus rejected the notion of congenital word blindness and instead labeled
this condition strephosymbolia from the Greek words for “twisted symbols.”
Orton’s (1925) theories concerning the etiology and treatment of reading disabilities were a watershed in the history of reading interventions. At a time when
the dominant view in the medical community was that a diagnosis of congenital
word blindness implied permanent brain damage that could not be remedied,
Orton convinced many researchers and physicians that there was hope for
improvement among students with reading disabilities (Traxler, 1941).
Strephosymbolia was seen as a functional impairment that would respond to treatment. One hemisphere of the brain could be trained to control reading using the
correctly oriented letters. Orton recommended treating children with strephosymbolia using extensive drill and practice in phonics to build up the memory traces
to the correct representations of words and letters and starve off the traces to the
mirror images. He based this recommendation on case reports where this method
was used successfully but emphasized that further research was needed. He would
later revise his treatment recommendations to promote a kinesthetic approach.
During the 1920s, as more school districts implemented standardized tests of
reading, superintendents became increasing aware that many of their students had
reading deficits that needed attention (Smith, 2002). In recognition of this issue,
the National Committee on Reading included a chapter titled “Diagnosis and
Remedial Work” in the 24th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of
Education (Gray, 1925). This chapter summarized intervention strategies that had
shown promise in case studies and other published and unpublished reports. The
bulk of the chapter consists of a chart listing specific reading difficulties, such as
hesitations or guessing of words while reading orally, lack of interest or effort in
reading, emotional/behavior disturbances manifested when asked to read, and
inability to retell what was read or to answer questions that demonstrate comprehension. The chart suggested an underlying reason for the difficulty and provided
recommended teaching strategies to help the student overcome the problem. This
report represented an attempt to synthesize early research on reading interventions for struggling readers. It also set in a motion changes in teachers’ views of
strugglers readers and ways to help them that would be more fully realized in the
next decade.
1930s: From the Lab to the Classroom
During the 1930s, reading intervention research moved from university labs to
classrooms as teachers were increasingly viewed as the best professionals to help
struggling readers (Traxler, 1941). One significant factor leading to this change
10
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
was the 24th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education (Gray,
1925), which, along with its chapter on remedial reading, emphasized the importance of continuing to teach reading in junior high and high school. Additionally,
published case studies of successful remediation of reading problems in school
settings gave teachers confidence that they could help the struggling readers in
their classrooms (Traxler, 1941). Increased availability of materials for reading
interventions also contributed to teachers’ ability and willingness to help struggling readers (Tinker, 1938). Popular among these was Gates’s 1935 edition of
The Improvement of Reading, which became the standard textbook on remedial
reading (A. J. Harris, 1967). Summaries of research on reading interventions that
were published during the 1930s focused on giving teachers ideas and tools to
help their students. Reviews by Tinker (1938) and Traxler (1941) emphasized putting the latest research findings into the hands of classroom teachers (A. J. Harris,
1967).
Although studies of interventions for struggling readers during the 1930s used
several different approaches, one similarity among them was that they were universally successful in raising reading GE scores. As noted by Traxler (1941) in his
review of 1930s research, whether the intervention occurred in a group or individually or with students with severe or moderate reading difficulties, results showed a
great deal of improvement over the course of treatment. Tinker (1934, 1938) also
noted that success was achieved across studies with varying treatment methodologies. In studies located for this review, the same universal success was noted
despite marked differences in the approach taken in the intervention. Fernald and
Keller’s (1936) kinesthetic approach, also used by Regensburg (1930), resulted in
gains of a year or more in GE scores in both studies after several months of treatment. Similarly, Gates and Bond (1936) reported impressive gains on a reading
intervention provided to a remedial reading class using readers and workbooks
developed to provide struggling readers with diverse and interesting material at
their reading level in an “activity program” that also involved field trips to study
topics in depth. In the Washington, D.C., public school system, Monroe (1937)
implemented a broad remedial reading program with great success across elementary, junior high, and high schools. Her approach involved teachers instructing
children in small groups while attending to the individual reading needs of each
student as identified through various reading tests and the use of reading material
that students would find interesting and be motivated to read.
Tinker (1934) explained the success of these different intervention approaches
by pointing to a common end result. All approaches helped struggling readers to
recognize words as whole units and to read words and lines of text from left to
right. Additionally, he stated that experiencing success in reading likely improved
students’ attitudes toward reading and ameliorated emotional struggles with the
learning situation and uncooperative behaviors that had developed due to previous experiences of failure. As a result, the struggling reader’s motivation improved
and the student remained engaged in the intervention. Tinker believed that motivation to cooperate with the intervention was the key element in the success of
differing approaches to reading interventions.
Another emerging theme in the 1930s was the comorbidity of behavior/emotional problems and reading difficulties. An examination of case records of children
11
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
with both reading and behavior problems (Damerau, 1934) showed that some students who had made marked gains in reading skills showed improvement in behavior also, whereas others did not. Those showing improvement in both areas typically
received treatment for both problems, leading to the conclusion that treating reading
problems in isolation was unlikely to resolve behavior issues. Robinson (1939) recommended beginning with the psychiatric intervention to reduce the child’s anxiety
and behavior problems so that the child would be better able to attend to the reading
intervention. Interventions for struggling readers that targeted emotional and behavioral disturbances would be a major focus of research in the decade to come.
1940s: New Perspectives on Reading Disabilities
The presence of emotional and behavioral disorders in struggling readers
received significant attention during the 1940s. In a widely cited report, Gates
(1941) reported that three in four students with reading disabilities also had an
emotional or behavioral disturbance. He noted that the comorbid disorder likely
caused the reading problem in 25% of cases. Many mental health clinicians
believed Gates underestimated the causal role of psychological disorders in reading disabilities (A. J. Harris, 1967). Hildreth (1942) asserted that the nature of
reading disabilities meant that psychologists could provide the most comprehensive assessment and treatment planning for struggling readers because they had an
understanding of the mind. After a careful diagnosis, the psychologist could prescribe a reading intervention for a teacher to implement and other needed interventions for the mental health, physiological, and other needs of the student.
Other psychologists and psychiatrists of the 1940s brought the perspective of
psychoanalytic theory to the treatment of reading problems. A number of psychoanalytic theories were proposed to explain the existence of reading disabilities
and to prescribe treatment. These theories included the following:
•• Because curiosity and the desire to explore motivate a child to learn to
read, if parents discourage or punish young children for expressing these
drives, anxiety arises out of fear of losing the parents’ love (Sylvester &
Kunst, 1943). Anxiety then interferes with the child’s ability to learn to
read.
•• The effort that struggling readers expend to repress sadistic fantasies and
aggressive urges toward their parents and teachers leaves them with too
little energy to learn to read (Blanchard, 1946). Reprimands and punishments received for reading failure relieve the child’s feelings of guilt over
wanting to harm his or her caregivers.
•• Girls are less likely than boys to have reading problems because they find
it easier to identify with and attach to their (usually female) reading teachers (Bell, 1945). Boys, however, typically experience transference of their
feelings toward their mothers onto their teachers. If those feelings are negative, boys will resist learning to read. Additionally, boys view reading as
a female activity that requires them to surrender some of their masculine
identity, stirring up guilt and anger. These reactions are especially severe in
boys who have overidentified with their mothers or who are overly aggressive or overly submissive in their relationships with their fathers.
12
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
Psychoanalytic viewpoints on reading disabilities prescribed psychotherapy as
the preferred intervention, either before or in addition to more traditional reading
interventions. They also recommended that parents create a home environment
that resolves the child’s aggressive urges and/or anxiety. Bell (1945) encouraged
teachers to learn about the home environments of struggling readers and be mindful of the key role of the student–teacher relationship in helping these students
improve. He viewed this relationship as the most critical element of a reading
intervention and noted that a strong bond between student and tutor may be the
reason why different reading intervention strategies achieved positive results.
Outside the psychoanalytic camp, other researchers combined reading and psychological interventions to treat struggling readers with comorbid emotional/behavioral disorders. These efforts included a 6-week residential summer program where
struggling readers received both psychotherapy and reading instruction using a nondirective approach that followed the interests of the students (Redmount, 1948).
Child guidance clinics of the 1940s used what is termed a “mental hygiene approach”
(Ellis, 1949), which involved a social worker who managed the child’s case, worked
with the child’s parents and teachers to improve the home and school environments,
arranged individualized tutoring as prescribed by the clinic’s psychologist, and
scheduled therapy sessions with the clinic’s psychiatrist if an emotional problem
was evident.
The 1940s also saw collaboration between public schools, universities, and mental health clinics to address reading problems. Tulane University created a course to
train in-service teachers in reading intervention strategies (Font, 1942) as part of an
effort to introduce interventions for struggling readers into the New Orleans public
school system for the first time. In another example of collaboration, the staff of a
community mental health clinic worked with the school district to establish a schoolbased summer program for struggling readers referred to the clinic for treatment
(Ullmann, 1949). After the program, clinic staff helped school leaders connect children with medical issues to health professionals and also helped district educators
develop a more comprehensive understanding of the needs of struggling readers.
A turning point in reading intervention research occurred in the late 1940s,
when reading intervention researchers began using experimental research designs
that implemented quantitative methods such as analysis of variance that had not
been widely used in education research. Burt and Lewis (1946) conducted two
experimental studies to compare the kinesthetic, visual/whole-word, phonics, and
mixed-methods approaches. In the first study, struggling readers were randomly
assigned to one of the four treatment types. In the second study, a randomized
blocks design was implemented. The results of both studies indicated larger gains
for the visual approach and smaller gains for the phonics approach. Burt and
Lewis encouraged other researchers to follow their example in comparing the
effects of different interventions in an experimental framework to remedy the
confusion resulting from studies where each approach, tested on its own without
a comparison group, showed positive results.
1950s: From Exploration to Efficacy
The 1950s brought a burst of new thinking and research on interventions for
struggling readers. One major factor came out of U.S. involvement in World War
13
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
II. As with World War I, the military again discovered that many soldiers were
functionally illiterate (Smith, 2002). With the war over and prosperity returning to
America, universities had the resources to open or expand reading clinics and
begin new graduate programs to train reading specialists in hopes of improving
instruction for struggling readers (A. J. Harris, 1967).
Researchers in the 1950s increased the rigor of their investigations, moving the
field forward by testing new research questions. Curr and Gourlay (1953) designed
a quasi-experimental study to determine if the positive results from previous studies that did not include a comparison group were due to practice effects and
regression to the mean rather than true gains in reading skills. To address this
research question, they tested the effects of their reading intervention against a
comparison group. Treatment students were selected and then matched to control
students on reading ability, IQ, gender, and classroom teacher. Their results suggested that practice and regression effects likely played a role in the positive findings of previous research. Schonfield (1956) sought to expand the knowledge
base on reading interventions by examining the impact of differences in the frequency of intervention sessions and the initial reading level of participants.
Results indicated that students with AE scores of 8.5 or better at the start of treatment made the greatest gains and that frequency of sessions did not make a difference. In a randomized experimental study, Seeman and Edwards (1954) compared
the effects of a psychotherapeutic experience provided at school for fifth and sixth
graders with both reading and emotional/behavioral problems to a business-asusual (BAU) control group. At the conclusion of the study, the average reading
gain for children who received treatment was greater than that of the matched
comparison group.
In another new twist on reading intervention research, Freed, Abrams, and Peifer
(1959) conducted an experimental study to determine if combining a reading intervention with an anti-psychotic medication improved the results of the intervention.
Their results indicated that all students who received reading instruction made statistically significant gains, but those given both reading instruction and medication
had larger gains than those who received either a placebo or medication only. Freed
et al. attributed the results to the calming effect of the medication, which they
believed mollified the emotional and behavioral disturbance at the root of the child’s
reading disability, allowing the child to benefit from instruction.
Despite the emergence of more experimental approaches to reading intervention research, some psychiatrists continued to view reading disabilities from the
perspective of untested psychoanalytic theories that viewed family dysfunction as
the cause. After reviewing children’s records from a public school, a child guidance clinic, a foster child placement agency, a children’s psychiatric unit, and a
clinic for children with delinquent behavior, Fabian (1955) declared that pathological home environments caused reading disabilities. Kunst (1959) saw reading
problems as a symptom of a neurosis involving anxiety about learning. She attributed the anxiety to parents who push their child too hard to learn, unloving or
rejecting parents who fail to inspire their child to learn, worries about an issue
going on in the child’s home, or some other disturbance in the parent–child relationship. Both Kunst and Fabian saw psychotherapy as a necessary part of a reading intervention and believed that teachers could prevent reading problems if they
14
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
understood the psychodynamics of children and created warm relationships with
them. However, they did not test their theories with experimental or quasi-experimental research.
The most significant watershed of the 1950s was Flesch’s (1955) publication
of a book on reading problems in children that was directed primarily toward
parents rather than educators or researchers. Why Johnny Can’t Read placed the
blame for reading disabilities on schools and teachers who had embraced the
whole-word or look/say approach to reading instruction and abandoned phonics.
As the book became a best seller, parents began to realize that their child’s reading
problem might not be due to the child’s IQ or effort—the school’s curriculum
might be to blame, leading parents to demand that schools do more to help struggling readers (A. J. Harris, 1967). Flesch’s denunciation of the whole-word
method was harsh and used rhetoric to win the argument that phonics was the
answer to America’s reading problems (Smith, 2002). His critics pointed out that
reading disabilities existed in countries where phonics were taught exclusively
and where languages had more regular grapheme/phoneme correspondence than
English (A. J. Harris, 1967). Nevertheless, Flesch succeeded in changing the conversation about reading disabilities from theories that pointed to child- and family-centered causes to one that also questioned the efficacy of teaching methods
and asked schools to do more to help struggling readers.
1960s: Expanding in New Directions
The public awareness and debate around reading disabilities that Flesch began
with his 1955 best seller proliferated during the 1960s. One example of the ongoing discussion was an article published in The New Yorker (Tomkins, 1963) that
traced theories of the etiology of reading problems, the debate over phonics versus whole-word instruction, and the challenges schools faced in finding money to
hire reading specialists. Tomkins (1963) concluded that students with reading disabilities who received appropriate interventions could improve their reading skills
dramatically but that few had access to the kind of individualized interventions
they needed.
Despite Tomkins’s (1963) pessimism, the 1960s were a decade of expansion in
new directions of research and thinking about how to help struggling readers.
More phonics-based interventions were developed and tested, perhaps in response
to Flesch’s (1955) treatise. These included interventions by Grover (1962) and
Brown (1967), both of whom found that phonics-based interventions produced
meaningful growth in struggling readers. Other researchers, such as Shedd (1968)
and Kline, Kline, Ashbrenner, and Calkins (1968), integrated phonics instruction
with other approaches to reading interventions with positive results.
In marked contrast to the psychoanalytic theory that influenced reading interventions in the 1940s and 1950s, the 1960s brought new research on reading interventions that reflected behaviorist theories of learning and implemented operant
conditioning protocols. Researchers tried out operant conditioning approaches
involving positive reinforcement and avoidance conditioning (McKerracher,
1967) and token reinforcements provided by a teacher who only communicated
with students via headphones to give instructions and dispensed tokens that students could save and trade for highly reinforcing items (Haring & Hauck, 1969).
15
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
These early efforts produced positive results for the struggling readers they sought
to help, but the interventions were not tested with experimental designs.
Another innovation for treating struggling readers was proposed by Delacato
(1963). Criticizing education researchers for focusing on treating reading disabilities symptomatically instead of discovering and treating the neurological cause,
he argued that reading disabilities shared the same underlying cause as speech
problems: lack of proper neurological organization. Delacato agreed with Orton
who had proposed in the 1920s that reading problems were the result of mixed
hemisphere dominance; however, he thought Orton was misguided in attempting
to treat the problem with a reading intervention. Delacato’s treatment involved
having the reading-disabled child sleep in a prescribed position, spend time daily
crawling and eventually walking with opposite hand and leg movements, practice
visual exercises to encourage dominance in the eye and hand of the same side of
the body, listen to music and learn to discriminate between tones, jump on a trampoline, and avoid using the limbs on the nondominant side of the body.
One more development of the 1960s bears mention for its broad and longlasting impact on reading intervention research and practice: the passage of the
Elementary and Secondary Education Act of 1965. This act provided funding to
schools serving low-income children that allowed them to provide reading interventions and other supports. However, trained reading specialists were in short
supply, leaving many schools without qualified personnel to implement interventions (A. J. Harris, 1967). As a result, reading was added to the areas of study that
could receive federal support under the National Defense Education Act of 1958,
leading to the creation of many new training programs and fueling an even greater
expansion of research on interventions for struggling readers in the decades that
followed (A. J. Harris, 1967).
1970s: A Time of Transition
In the early 1970s, researchers continued to explore many of the avenues of
research that had been launched during the 1960s. Phonics interventions continued to be tested, especially as interventions for students with no or very low reading ability. In 1974, A. Lane tested a phonics intervention that used the Initial
Teaching Alphabet as a transitional tool to break the cycle of reading failure for a
group of sixth-grade students with severe reading disabilities, with positive
results. E. Richardson and Collier (1971) also had success with a phonics intervention that consisted of very brief sessions focused on teaching a progression of
decoding skills.
Another continuing theme from the 1960s was the application of behaviorist
principles to reading interventions, albeit in new and subtler ways than operant
conditioning studies of the 1960s. Behaviorist techniques were used to help struggling readers resolve their reading anxiety, which was seen as resulting from
viewing the reading teacher as a source of stress (Cameron, Borst, Fifer, LaVigne,
& Smith, 1972) or from a phobic response to being asked to read aloud (Word &
Rozynko, 1974). More traditional applications of behaviorism were implemented
in other studies of reading interventions, such as Camp and van Doorninck’s
(1971) randomized experiment in which students who received tokens for completing drill-and-practice exercises scored significantly higher than a comparison
16
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
group that was reinforced only for correct responses during pre- and posttesting.
Applied behavior analysis also was tested as a treatment for struggling readers in
the 1970s, with some success (Lovitt, 1975).
Other researchers tested Delacato’s (1963) approach to remedying reading disabilities and the efficacy of perceptual and motor skills interventions overall.
Balow (1971) reviewed research on the Delacato exercises and noted that most
studies found that they were not effective in remediating reading problems.
However, the research conducted in the 1970s was equivocal. Some studies demonstrated benefits from adding perceptual and motor skills activities to a reading
intervention (Meikle & Kilpatrick, 1971; Van Osdol, Johnson, & Geiger, 1974),
whereas others found no differences between interventions with a movement
component compared to more traditional remedial reading instruction (Jackson &
Dando, 1976; Sullivan, 1972). Jackson and Dando (1976) directly compared the
Delacato program to phonics instruction and found no benefit to the Delacato
approach.
In addition to expanding on areas of research from previous decades, researchers in the 1970s also broke new ground in reading interventions that focused on
improving comprehension. Research conducted prior to the 1970s tended to focus
predominantly on oral reading fluency and decoding. In the 1970s, researchers
increasingly focused on reading comprehension when studying the teaching of
reading to the general population as well as interventions for struggling readers
(Smith, 2002). Research on reading comprehension led to a growth in interest in
helping struggling readers develop the metacognitive processes involved in
understanding text. In an influential study, Torgesen (1977) compared the cognitive strategy use of fourth-grade students with reading disabilities and their nondisabled peers. Before any instruction was provided, the groups differed in their
spontaneous use of strategies to recall a set of pictures, with students with reading
disabilities being less likely to use a strategy and to recall the pictures correctly.
However, after receiving strategy instruction their recall improved to the point
that their scores were not significantly different from the nondisabled group. As
the 1970s drew to a close, Torgesen’s conclusion that it was possible to teach
students with reading disabilities to use metacognitive strategies effectively would
lead to additional rigorous research on interventions that targeted metacognitive
processes in struggling readers in the 1980s and beyond.
1980s: Influence of Cognitive Psychology
Though building on the knowledge gained over the preceding decades, the research
on interventions for struggling readers in Grades 4 to 12 in the 1980s represented a
significant shift from all that preceded it. As cognitive psychologists became more
interested in studying reading, their theories influenced education researchers to
develop new approaches to reading interventions (Smith, 2002). This influence began
in the late 1970s but grew rapidly in the 1980s. A widely cited study by Torgesen
(1982) helped apply cognitive psychology theories to the work of designing reading
interventions. He summarized research showing that students with reading disabilities
tended to take a passive approach to learning, failing to employ cognitive strategies to
help themselves recall information or tie concepts together to make meaning from
text. However, he believed that research showed that these students had the potential
17
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
to become more active learners, and he challenged researchers to develop interventions that would teach strategies that could be applied to multiple learning tasks.
Other researchers responded to this call, developing interventions to teach cognitive strategies to struggling readers. Reading comprehension interventions
focused on teaching strategies such as self-questioning (e.g., Wong & Jones,
1982), self-monitoring (e.g., Graves & Levin, 1989), and rehearsal (e.g.,
Borkowski, Weyhing, & Carr, 1988). Other metacognitive interventions of the
1980s focused on vocabulary and taught cognitive strategies that included semantic feature analysis (e.g., Anders, Bos, & Filip, 1984) and mnemonics to teach
struggling readers how to acquire new vocabulary words (e.g., Mastropieri,
Scruggs, Levin, Gaffney, & McLoone, 1985). Another approach involved the use
of advance organizers and graphic organizers (e.g., Darch & Gersten, 1986).
These interventions yielded positive effects for the reading comprehension and
vocabulary learning outcomes they targeted. However, researchers typically measured these outcomes on unstandardized measures that were proximal to the intervention, leaving open the question of whether struggling readers transferred the
metacognitive strategies to more distal reading tasks. For more details on these
studies, see Supplementary Table S6 in the online version of this journal.
Additionally, in the 1980s the number of studies conducted using random
assignment to conditions or matching of treatment and comparison group participants to determine the efficacy of their reading interventions expanded considerably. More experimental and strong quasi-experimental designs were implemented
in the 1980s than in the prior 65 years combined. Also around this time, GE and
AE scores felt out of favor as researchers such as Farr and Tuinman (1972) pointed
out that statistical tests of effects require scores with equal-interval scales and that
any quantitative analysis of AE and GE scores is inappropriate and should not be
published. These advancements in methodology allowed for greater precision in
measuring effects and for more robust conclusions to be drawn about the relative
effectiveness of different interventions. However, small sample sizes remained a
limitation that attenuated the statistical power of the research designs.
1990s: Intensive Focus on Comprehension
Reading intervention research during the 1990s further developed many of the
areas of focus from the 1980s. Interventions that targeted building reading comprehension skills in struggling readers became almost a singular focus of researchers in the 1990s, with many more experimental studies devoted to comprehension
skills. Reading comprehension interventions of the 1990s continued to concentrate on teaching metacognitive strategies such as self-questioning (e.g., K. S.
Chan, 1991), summarizing (e.g., Gajria & Salvia, 1992), creating a cognitive map
(Boyle, 1996), identifying the theme of a reading passage (Williams, Brown,
Silverstein, & de Cani, 1994), and increasing depth of cognitive processing (Bos
& Anders, 1990). Results of these studies generally indicated positive effects for
the treatment group. Standardized measures used in several of these metacognitive strategy studies indicated smaller effects than those seen on researcher-developed measures.
Other researchers who focused on metacognitive strategies also incorporated
reciprocal teaching, where students acquire proficiency in key reading
18
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
comprehension practices and then take turns assuming the role of the teacher in
guiding other students in their use (Alfassi, 1998; Moore & Scevak, 1995).
Expansions of reciprocal teaching interventions tested in the 1990s focused on
special populations and also addressed reading comprehension in collaborative
groups (e.g., Klingner & Vaughn, 1996). These studies yielded few significant
differences between treatment and comparison groups, though pre–post gains
were found for the treatment group. Additional research focused on building fluency (e.g., Homan, Klesius, & Hite, 1993) and on word study to improve decoding (e.g., Abbott & Berninger, 1999). The interventions focusing on vocabulary
instruction that were common in the 1980s all but disappeared from the experimental literature of the 1990s, though vocabulary-building activities were incorporated in some interventions along with reading comprehension instruction.
Though research reviews and syntheses were published beginning in the 1930s,
the first meta-analyses of reading intervention studies appeared in the late 1990s
and represented a significant step forward in understanding the relative efficacy
of different approaches to helping struggling readers. Mastropieri et al. (1996)
used meta-analysis to synthesize reading comprehension interventions for students identified with LD in elementary, middle, and high school, calculating effect
sizes for 68 interventions published between 1976 and 1996. The mean effect size
for these studies was 0.98, though for results based on standardized measures only
the mean effect was smaller, 0.40. Larger effects were associated with studies that
taught self-questioning strategies compared to those that enhanced the text or provided general reading comprehension skills instruction. Mean effect sizes from
studies targeted at students older and younger than 152 months were similar in
magnitude.
A second meta-analysis of reading interventions, conducted by H. L. Swanson
(1999), included studies published through 1997 that sought to improve word
recognition or reading comprehension in students with LD. The sample-weighted
mean effect size for the 54 studies that measured word recognition was 0.57; for
the 58 studies that measured comprehension, the weighted mean effect size was
0.72. Mean effects were smaller for both word recognition (0.62) and comprehension (0.45) when results from only standardized measures were meta-analyzed.
The weighted mean effect for word recognition was somewhat larger for students
under age 12 (0.73) than for those ages 12 to 17 (0.44); the effects for comprehension were similar for both groups. H. L. Swanson concluded that direct instruction
was the most effective approach for improving word recognition, with a mean
effect of 0.70, while a combination of direct instruction and strategy instruction
yielded the largest mean effect, 1.15, for comprehension.
A third meta-analysis (Elbaum et al., 2000) synthesized effects from one-onone reading interventions for at-risk readers in elementary grades conducted
between 1975 and 1998. Just five studies in this meta-analysis involved students in
Grades 4 to 6; their weighted mean effect size was 0.06. Over the next 15 years,
more meta-analyses would be published focused on honing in on the effect of reading interventions targeted specifically for struggling readers in Grades 4 to 12 and
investigating variables that moderated their effectiveness. For more details on the
reading interventions for students in Grades 4–12 included in the meta-analyses
cited above, see Supplementary Table S7 in the online version of this journal.
19
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
2000s: Legislative Impacts on Intervention Research
In the first decade of the new millennium, the U.S. Congress shaped research
on reading interventions with passage of the Education Sciences Reform Act of
2002 and the Individuals With Disabilities Education Improvement Act (IDEIA)
of 2004. The Education Sciences Reform Act led to the creation of the IES, which
set out to fund educational research meeting high standards of rigor and established the WWC that developed requirements for the research IES would support.
In its 2005 Biennial Report to Congress, IES noted that it would prioritize funding
for educational research that conducted RCTs similar to those found in science
and medicine (IES, 2005). As a result, after 2005 many more RCTs of reading
interventions were published, sample sizes increased, and the use of standardized
measures became more prevalent.
The other landmark legislation of the 2000s, the passage of IDEIA in 2004, allowed
schools to identify students for special education services through a multitiered instructional approach known as response to instruction/intervention, thus changing the
framework for providing reading interventions as both prevention and remediation.
Now, all students not responding to general education instruction, rather than just those
identified with LD, could receive interventions characterized by evidence-based curriculums, smaller group sizes, increased dosage, and greater individualization (D.
Fuchs, Fuchs, & Vaughn, 2014). This framework changed the landscape of reading
intervention research in two ways: Students qualifying to participate in research could
include all those not responding to general education curriculum (not just those with a
discrepancy between IQ and achievement test scores) and schools were now implementing interventions with a larger percentage of students, thus changing the nature of
the BAU comparison condition in many intervention studies (Scammacca et al., 2015).
Along with these legislative changes, there was a growing awareness in the
2000s of the critical need to improve the reading proficiency of students in Grades
4 to 12, including a consensus report by experts about the need for an action plan
guiding reading comprehension for students at and after Grade 4 (Biancarosa &
Snow, 2006). Following up on Biancarosa and Snow’s (2006) report, two metaanalyses distilled the findings of research on reading intervention for students in
Grades 4 to 12 from 1980 through 2004: Scammacca et al. (2007) and Edmonds
et al. (2009). Scammacca et al. (2007) reported a weighted mean effect size of
0.95 across 31 reading interventions and a somewhat smaller 0.42 mean effect
based on a subset of findings that assessed outcomes using standardized measures
from the 11 studies that included such measures; results were similar when only
comprehension measures were included in the analysis. They found that intervention type was a significant moderator of effect size, with comprehension strategy
and vocabulary interventions having the largest mean effects and word study and
multicomponent interventions having moderate effects. Effects also were larger
for students in Grades 4 to 8 than those in Grades 9 to 12. Edmonds et al. (2009)
meta-analyzed 13 interventions that sought to improve reading comprehension in
struggling readers in Grades 6 to 12. They found a weighted mean effect size of
0.89 on comprehension measures, with a smaller effect of 0.47 when only standardized measures were included in the analysis. Both meta-analyses provided
convincing evidence that interventions for older struggling readers could result in
substantive improvements in reading ability for these students.
20
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
2010–2014: Rapid Expansion, Declining Effects
The federal legislation passed in the 2000s continued to impact research into
the 2010s. Table 2 summarizes characteristics of the group comparison experimental and quasi-experimental studies included in this review. These findings are
reported here because they are not available in published documents referenced in
the previous decades. The 5 years between 2010 and 2014 stand apart from any
preceding decade in both the rigor and the intensity of the intervention studies
published. The total number of studies exceeds those published in any previous
decade, the average sample size in these studies is triple that of the 2000s, 50% of
studies used only standardized measures, and 60% provided more than 25 hours
of intervention. The continued effect of IDEIA also is seen in changes in the participants in intervention research. The percentage of intervention studies focused
only on students with LD has decreased from 83% of studies in the 1980s to 57%
in the 1990s, 29% in the 2000s, and just 7% of studies published between 2010
and 2014.
As noted by Scammacca et al. (2015), this change in target population, along
with the increased rigor in research designs and a shift toward standardized measures that followed the founding of IES in 2002, may be partly responsible for the
declining effect sizes seen in their meta-analysis of interventions for struggling
readers that included research published between 1980 and 2011. They found a
significant effect for year of publication in a metaregression of effect sizes from
interventions for struggling readers in Grades 4 to 12, with more recent studies
having smaller effect sizes. The weighted mean effect size for studies published
between 2005 and 2011 was 0.49, compared to 0.95 for studies published between
1980 and 2004; a decline in magnitude also was seen when comparing effect sizes
from standardized measures only (from 0.42 to 0.21), though year of publication
was not a statistically significant predictor of effect sizes from standardized
measures.
Scammacca et al. (2015) also pointed to changes in the nature of the comparison group over time as a possible reason for the decline in effect sizes. As shown
in Table 2, researchers have increasingly used a “business-as-usual” comparison
group since group comparison designs became more prevalent beginning in the
1980s. In this review, we coded studies as BAU when the comparison group did
not receive any treatment from researchers but received reading instruction provided by the school independent of the researchers’ involvement. For studies published between 2010 and 2014, these types of comparison groups were used 80%
of the time. However, the nature of BAU reading instruction has changed over
time due to RTI paradigms that call for research-based interventions for students
who are not responding adequately to classroom reading instruction. In at least
some cases, contemporary experimental interventions are being compared to a
BAU condition that involves an alternate treatment that has already demonstrated
efficacy. As a result, effect sizes from these studies would be smaller than those
from studies that compared an experimental intervention to no intervention (see
Lemons, Fuchs, Gilbert, & Fuchs, 2014, for further discussion of this issue).
A number of recent meta-analyses have struggled to find moderator variables
other than year of publication that explain variation in effect sizes from the corpus
of studies they sought to synthesize. In a meta-analysis of outcomes from
21
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
22
TABLE 2
Summary of characteristics of experimental and quasi-experimental group-comparison studies, 1940–2014a
Study Characteristics
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Average sample size
Total hours of intervention, %
≤5
6-15
16-25
>25
Not reported
Intervention typeb, %
Vocabulary
Comprehension strategy
Fluency
Multicomponent
Word study
Multisensoryc
Visual trainingc
Psychiatric/behavior/emotional treatmentc
Other
Types of comparison group, %
No treatment
Business as usual
1940s, k = 1
1950s, k = 4
1960s, k = 6
1970s, k = 7
1980s, k = 23
1990s, k = 21
2000s, k = 24
2010–2014,
k = 30
80
106
57
37
39
44
123
382
0
100
0
0
0
0
0
0
75
25
17
33
17
33
0
14
29
0
29
29
39
30
0
0
30
43
24
5
19
10
4
21
29
42
4
3
27
10
60
0
0
0
0
100
0
100
100
0
0
0
0
75
0
0
0
50
0
0
0
17
0
17
17
17
0
0
0
0
57
57
29
0
30
39
9
22
0
0
0
0
5
57
10
14
14
14
14
0
0
33
13
42
13
4
0
0
10
20
7
63
0
0
0
3
0
25
83
43
0
0
0
0
0
0
50
25
33
0
29
29
9
9
19
24
4
58
3
80
(continued)
TABLE 2 (CONTINUED)
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Study Characteristics
Attention control
Absence of selected component
Different treatment
Types of outcome measures, %
Standardized
Unstandardized
Both
Unclear
Grades, %
4–8
9–12
Other/not clear
LD status, %
All LD
Some LD
No LD
Other/not clear
1940s, k = 1
1950s, k = 4
1960s, k = 6
1970s, k = 7
1980s, k = 23
1990s, k = 21
2000s, k = 24
2010–2014,
k = 30
0
0
0
25
17
17
0
14
17
17
5
19
4
13
7
3
100
0
33
29
48
33
21
7
0
0
0
100
50
25
25
0
67
0
33
0
29
14
43
14
4
78
17
0
19
62
19
0
46
21
33
0
50
17
3
0
100
0
0
75
25
0
67
17
17
86
0
14
78
22
0
81
19
0
92
8
0
70
20
10
0
0
0
100
25
0
0
75
0
0
0
100
0
0
0
100
83
9
0
9
57
19
5
19
29
33
13
25
7
30
10
53
Note. LD = learning disability.
aNo studies published before 1940 used a group comparison experimental or quasi-experimental design. bSome studies contrasted more than one intervention type, leading to totals in
excess of 100% for some decades. cIn addition to a reading intervention.
23
Scammacca et al.
standardized measures in 12 studies aimed at students with reading disabilities in
Grades 5 to 9, Flynn et al. (2012) found a weighted mean effect size of 0.41, but
the statistically significant heterogeneity associated with the mean effect could
not be explained by the focus or length of the intervention, student age, or grade
level. Wanzek et al. (2013) included a larger corpus of studies, including all those
that provided 75 or more sessions of intervention to struggling readers in Grades
4 to 12, but found statistically significant heterogeneity only when meta-analyzing 22 effect sizes from reading comprehension measures. This heterogeneity
could not be explained by differences in number of hours of intervention, number
of students in the instructional group, or grade level.
All of the studies in Wanzek et al. (2013) were included in Scammacca et al.
(2015), along with additional studies of the same population that provided fewer
than 75 sessions of intervention. Scammacca et al. found statistically significant
heterogeneity in effect sizes from studies published between 2005 and 2011 only
when looking across effect sizes from standardized and unstandardized outcomes
combined. LD status of participants, grade level, number of hours of intervention,
and whether a researcher or teacher implemented the intervention were all examined as possible moderators, but no differences based on these variables were
found. The only statistically significant moderator variable was intervention type,
with comprehension interventions having a significantly larger mean effect size
than multicomponent interventions.
Intervention research in the 2010s continues to be influenced by research syntheses and meta-analyses that have concluded that improving reading comprehension
outcomes for older readers is best accomplished through interventions that include a
reading comprehension component, as compared to those that focus on word-level
skills (Scammacca et al., 2015; Wanzek, Wexler, Vaughn, & Ciullo, 2010). More
than 80% of the 2010 to 2014 studies focused on comprehension strategies or
included comprehension in multicomponent interventions. No studies focused solely
on word study, and just 7% focused solely on fluency. An emphasis on comprehension instruction in reading interventions is a trend that began in the 1970s and 1980s
and appears likely to continue into the next century of reading intervention research.
Discussion
Efforts to improve outcomes for students with reading disabilities have been
under way for more than a century. Over the past 100 years, there has been considerable change in the way in which reading disabilities are perceived, the
hypothesized causes and consequences, and the types of treatments thought to
improve outcomes. In concluding this narrative review, we highlight key themes
and discuss the implications of a century of progress in research in reading interventions for researchers, practitioners, and policymakers of today and tomorrow.
Multidisciplinary Involvement
The history of interventions for struggling readers reflects the contributions of
researchers and practitioners from multiple disciplines. Pioneers in developing
methods to help struggling readers included physicians, neurologists, ophthalmologists, psychologists, psychiatrists, education researchers, and teachers. Some
of their theories and treatments may seem misguided with 21st-century hindsight,
24
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
reflecting the limited nature of the knowledge base available before innovations
such as desktop computers and brain imaging were commonplace. However, the
differences in perspective afforded by professionals with diverse specialties
helped move the field forward by highlighting the multicausal nature of reading
disabilities and allowing for experimentation with treatments that varied from
visual training exercises to psychotherapy to phonics drills. The cooperation
between university researchers and local schools that began in the 1920s and
1930s continues to be critical to the development and testing of new interventions
and the training of teachers, reading specialists, and other professionals.
Advances in research by neuroscientists and cognitive psychologists support
the continued importance of a multidisciplinary approach to helping struggling
readers succeed. For example, recent neuroimaging research has found changes in
the structure and functions in the brain following reading interventions (e.g.,
Barquero, Davis, & Cutting, 2014; Krafnick, Flowers, Napoliello, & Eden, 2011).
Cognitive psychologists are contributing theoretical frameworks that call for
reading interventions to focus on underlying deficits in the reading process instead
of teaching discrete skills (e.g., Compton, Miller, Elleman, & Steacy, 2014).
Maximizing the effectiveness of reading interventions over the next century will
require an ongoing dialogue with and a continued appreciation for the contributions of researchers and practitioners from many different fields.
Shift to Standardized Group Interventions
In recent decades, structured, standardized group interventions associated with
RCTs and other more rigorous designs have largely replaced individualized interventions typically implemented as case studies. Throughout the early history of
reading intervention research, pioneers such as Gray (1922), Gates (1927), and
others emphasized the importance of tailoring a reading intervention to the specific
strengths and needs of each struggling reader in one-on-one interventions. They
developed diagnostic tests specifically for the purpose of guiding individually
developed and provided interventions. This focus also can be seen in the plethora
of case studies published from 1914 to 1960, when the focus began to shift toward
more structured, group-based interventions. Although case studies and single-subject designs continue to be published and play a key role in testing new approaches
to reading intervention, the most recent 10 years of research have been marked by
rapid growth in the number of RCTs published. The establishment of more rigorous requirements for research-based interventions to be used in schools and for
federally funded education research to embrace large-scale randomized trials
fueled the growth in structured approaches to helping struggling readers.
However, the WWC notes that there is strong research evidence to recommend
that individualized instruction be made available to struggling readers who need
more help than can be provided in more standardized group-based interventions
(Kamil et al., 2008). This is especially true for students who do not respond adequately to reading interventions. Research aimed at understanding the cognitive
attributes of these low responders (e.g., Cho et al., 2015; Miciak et al., 2014) and
determining how best to address their severe and seemingly unyielding reading
difficulties (e.g., Pyle & Vaughn, 2012) is currently on the frontier of reading
intervention research. More individualized interventions, like those tested in
25
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
earlier decades, might hold clues for contemporary researchers seeking to understand how to help students who benefit little from a standardized approach.
Increased Focus on Reading Comprehension
Over the past century, interventions have evolved to focus more attention on
reading comprehension. Interventions tested during the first two thirds of the 20th
century focused primarily at the word level, emphasizing improving struggling
readers’ word recognition and oral reading fluency. Given that the original diagnosis
of reading disability was congenital word blindness, it is not surprising that wordreading skills were the predominant focus of early research. However, beginning in
the 1970s, teaching struggling readers to make meaning from text became more of
a priority. Cognitive psychology contributed theories on the ways in which mental
schema are built from vocabulary and background knowledge and on the metacognitive abilities that are key to constructing meaning from text. These theories led
education researchers to test new interventions that taught strategies for acquiring
and using vocabulary and background knowledge to aid comprehension.
More recently, meta-analyses and syntheses have highlighted the effectiveness of
interventions that either include a reading comprehension component or focus exclusively on comprehension over those at the word level. Additionally, contemporary
researchers are focusing more attention on providing reading interventions and supports in content area classes such as social studies and science as a means of helping
struggling readers improve their reading comprehension (e.g., Boardman, Klingner,
Buckley, Annamma, & Lasser, 2015; Kaldenberg, Watt, & Therrien, 2015; E.
Swanson et al., in press). With the Common Core Standards (National Governors
Association Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers,
2010) requiring students to comprehend more complex texts and master more
advanced academic vocabulary, interventions will increasingly be needed across the
school curriculum to help struggling readers reach proficiency in these areas.
Declining Effect Sizes
Interventions prior to the mid-1970s typically did not report results using
equal-interval scores and rarely included a counterfactual condition, making it
difficult to know if early reading interventions showed larger or smaller effects
than more recent ones. Studies from this time period in the published literature
generally reported positive effects in case studies and single-group design
research, with some reporting dramatic gains in reading skills. However, experimental and quasi-experimental studies from the 1980s through the 2010s produced effect sizes that have declined sharply and consistently over the decades.
This finding seems counterintuitive; one might expect interventions to demonstrate larger effects over time as a benefit of learning from previous research and
refining procedures to isolate those that best improve outcomes for struggling
readers. However, many aspects of intervention research (e.g., designs, measures)
have changed in ways that could account for the decline in observed effects.
In the 1980s and 1990s, interventions generally were brief and involved small
samples; as a result, only a large treatment effect would have resulted in sufficient
power to yield a statistically significant difference in outcomes between the treatment and comparison groups. Therefore, it may be that other studies with small
26
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
sample sizes where treatment effects were smaller were not published because of
null results. More recent studies were adequately powered to detect statistically
significant small effects as a result of having large sample sizes. Additionally, during in the late 20th century, researchers most often determined treatment effects
using researcher-developed, unstandardized measures rather than standardized
measures. As funders began to require more rigorous methodologies in the 2000s,
researchers increasingly used standardized measures to estimate effects. As noted
by Willingham (2007), the content of researcher-designed measures often aligns
more closely to the content of the intervention, meaning these measures are more
proximal estimates of the treatment effect. Standardized measures typically
require some degree of transfer of learning and/or generalization to new types of
content, making them more distal to the intervention and likely to show a smaller
effect.
Additionally, smaller effects from more recent interventions may be in part the
result of federal legislation allowing schools to broaden the population of students
who qualified for reading interventions to include those who did not respond adequately to general education instruction. This change in population may have altered
the sample in ways that both yielded a more intractable group of students with reading disabilities and a comparison group provided more robust reading treatments
compared to studies in the 1980s and 1990s. Students randomized to the comparison group in studies completed in the past 10 years typically received the school’s
BAU reading intervention rather than no or minimal instruction as was common in
earlier research. Therefore, effect sizes from more recent studies reflect the extent to
which the experimental intervention outperforms an existing intervention.
Finally, the increasing prominence of reading comprehension also may explain
some of the decrease in effects. The largest effects seen in intervention research
from 1980 to 2011 were found in early studies of vocabulary interventions
(Scammacca et al., 2015). Findings from these studies indicated that students who
were taught the meanings of words typically learned these meanings, but generalization to improved reading was not often documented. More recently, vocabulary
instruction has been incorporated into multicomponent interventions, with very
few experimental studies of a purely vocabulary-focused intervention published in
the past 10 years. Instead, reading comprehension interventions and multicomponent interventions with a reading comprehension component predominate, with
comprehension effects seen as a key outcome when intervening with students in
Grades 4 to 12. Results from the meta-analysis by Scammacca et al. (2015) indicate that 47 studies published between 2005 and 2011 reported reading comprehension outcomes, compared to 25 studies published between 1980 and 2004. The
mean effect size for more recent studies was 0.24 for all types of measures and 0.19
for standardized measures, indicating that effects of interventions on reading comprehension outcomes tend to be relatively small. Therefore, the increased focus on
reading comprehension may be a significant factor that, when combined with other
changes noted above, may account for the declining effect sizes seen over time.
Implications for Future Research, Practice, and Policy
One might argue that the most compelling reason for studying history is to
inform the future—to build on its successes and avoid repeating its mistakes. In
27
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
this vein, the history of reading interventions has much to say to current and future
researchers, practitioners, and policymakers who will carry on the work of
researching and implementing interventions for struggling readers over the next
100 years. In an effort to speak specifically to each of these stakeholders, we discuss the implications for research, practice, and policy separately below. However,
many of our conclusions are relevant to more than one of these groups.
Implications for Researchers
The history of the past century of reading interventions yields a number of suggestions for future research. One important issue for researchers to address is the
trend of declining effect sizes observed over the past 35 years. A number of possible reasons have been suggested in this review, but further research is needed to
determine if these declining effect sizes represent true diminished benefits from
reading interventions or reflect changes in the school environment (e.g., overall
improved classroom instruction), the rigor of contemporary research (e.g., RCTs
vs. case studies), or other factors. The use of standardized measures and reporting
of pretest standard scores by intervention researchers can aid future reviewers of
research in determining if changes in study participants’ preintervention reading
ability are occurring over time and playing a role in diminishing treatment effects.
Additionally, more detailed description of fidelity of implementation and of the
BAU instruction provided in the counterfactual condition also would be helpful in
determining the relative strength of the intervention the treatment and comparison
groups received.
Another avenue that future researchers should explore is a less standardized
treatment approach that aims to improve the knowledge and skills of tutors/teachers so that they can use student data to understand struggling readers’ needs and
implement more impactful treatments. This type of tailored intervention approach
was fairly common in the early decades of research and demonstrated effectiveness, but it is found infrequently in contemporary studies. Future intervention
studies also might consider testing protocols that adjust treatments to better align
with students’ reading and learning needs on an ongoing basis. These types of
studies can be more difficult to carry out as an RCT than using the case study and
single-group design methodologies that proliferated in the past, but testing this
approach to intervention using a robust research design is critical to determining
its efficacy.
Contemporary researchers should consider the example of reading researchers
of the past who embraced insights and developments in diverse fields of study and
tested their efficacy in reading interventions. Some of these efforts may seem
questionable in hindsight, but others, such as the insights gained from cognitive
psychology around metacognition, have borne fruit. Although early researchers
sought explanations for reading problems in the psyches of youngsters without
much success, it is important to consider ways to further investigate related psychosocial and behavioral issues such as the role of attention, motivation, selfefficacy, self-regulation, family dynamics, and other psychosocial and behavioral
factors that are often associated with reading problems. History shows that there
is much to gain in embracing a principle of openness to learning from and collaborating with scholars outside one’s own area of study. Exploring the efficacy of
28
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
a reading intervention designed by education researchers paired with a socialemotional learning intervention developed and implemented by psychologists or
school counselors is one example of a research partnership that might yield new
insights.
Finally, for those who continue to research the efficacy of reading interventions and will create the history of the next century, there are still issues to resolve
in determining why some children do not benefit sufficiently from classroom
reading instruction in the primary grades and how to help them become proficient
readers. In a similar vein, more research is needed to develop interventions for
struggling readers who do not respond adequately to evidence-based interventions provided with high treatment fidelity. Also, remediating reading difficulties
in high school students needs additional attention as few experimental studies
have been published focused on this age-group and treatment effects in these studies have tended to be quite small. Researchers also must continue to explore the
impact of changes in government policy, such as the introduction of the Common
Core Standards, on struggling readers and test interventions to help these students
meet expectations for proficiency in reading across the curriculum and throughout
upper elementary, middle school, and high school.
Implications for Teachers and Other Practitioners
Perhaps history’s greatest lesson is that the need for reading interventions and
the opportunity to learn to read proficiently does not end after the primary grades;
students in Grades 4 to 12 who are not reading at the level expected can also grow
in their reading skills. There is much in the past century of reading interventions
research to suggest that students with reading difficulties can make measureable
and meaningful progress when given appropriate interventions. Although the
effects may be small, it is reasonable to expect that their cumulative impact over
time would have meaningful practical benefits. Therefore, teachers and others
who work closely with struggling readers should persevere in implementing evidence-based interventions even when student gains are incremental. Some struggling readers may require interventions over a long period of time and may benefit
more from individualized interventions. With a history that dates back to William
Gray’s work in the 1920s, the RTI framework allows for increasingly intensive
interventions when students do not respond adequately to initial efforts to intervene, something that students with significant reading difficulties may need.
Teachers and those who train future teachers should be knowledgeable about
frameworks such as RTI as well as about what the history of reading interventions
reveals about the content of effective interventions. Much of the early history of reading interventions focused on word-level skills to improve fluency. More contemporary research has demonstrated the importance of instruction focused on
comprehension strategies with word-level instruction included as needed for students
who have not mastered decoding. Therefore, teachers of students in Grade 4 and
above need to be prepared to help students learn word-level skills, rather than expecting that students would have mastered them in the primary grades. For those students
struggling with reading comprehension, content area teachers can play an important
role in helping students learn strategies to make meaning from the texts that they
expect students to comprehend in order to learn the subject matter being taught.
29
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
Additionally, teachers and other practitioners should be aware of what the history of reading interventions has to say about the importance of psychosocial
factors in the effectiveness of reading interventions. Early research suggested that
the relationship between the reading instructor and student played a part in the
efficacy of interventions. Our historical review documented that factors such as
motivation to read, anxiety around reading, the need to overcome mental blocks
created by previous experiences of reading failure, and considering students’
interests when choosing reading materials are important when intervening with
struggling readers. The comorbidity of behavior disorders and reading difficulties
also requires teachers to attend to factors beyond the instructional components of
an intervention and calls for the involvement of other professionals who can assist
in formulating a comprehensive approach to helping a struggling reader.
Undoubtedly, the emotional, relational, and instructional aspects of a reading
intervention are all important to helping struggling readers become more
proficient.
Implications for Policymakers
Another important lesson from the history presented here is the role the U.S.
government plays in prioritizing reading interventions and shaping the nature of
intervention research. From raising the issue to national prominence during World
Wars I and II, to the Elementary and Secondary Education Act of 1965 that allocated funds under Title I to provide reading interventions, to the Education
Sciences Reform Act of 2002 that created IES and raised the bar for experimental
research on reading interventions, to the IDEIA of 2004 that expanded access to
reading interventions in schools, the U.S. government has had a hand in the ways
in which the field of intervention research has grown and changed over the past
century. It is unlikely that the research currently available on how to help struggling readers would be available if not for government support of reading intervention research. Government regulations also have raised the quality of the
research that has been conducted in the 2000s and 2010s.
For those who will write or influence the legislation of the future, the lessons
of history provide guidance for considering focusing future funding on issues
such as the mechanisms for identifying and intervening with struggling readers
across the age span and designing and evaluating treatments for students who
respond inadequately to current reading interventions. Additionally, policymakers
might prioritize studies addressing the dearth of longitudinal research on the additive effects of treatment over multiple years and the outcomes associated with
robust early treatments and their long-terms effects. Few studies over the past
century have followed students to determine if treatment effects are maintained
over time periods longer than a few months after the conclusion of an intervention. Replication studies, which also are infrequently seen in the history presented
here, also are important to fund to further validate the effectiveness of reading
interventions that show promise.
Finally, keeping in mind the role legislation has played across the history of
reading interventions research, policymakers should be cognizant of the potential
implications of present, past, and future regulations and policies on new research
and its outcomes. For example, many of the factors discussed above as possible
30
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
reasons for the decline in effect sizes over time stemmed from federal legislation
and policies. Judging the size of treatment effects found in current research by
comparing them to the effects of past, less rigorous studies would ignore the
effects of these regulations. The impacts of newer policies like the Common Core
Standards and legislation such as the Every Student Succeeds Act of 2015 have
yet to be realized, but the lessons of history suggest that both will change the
landscape of research on reading interventions for students in Grades 4 to 12.
The Next Century
Between 1914 and 2014, the research on reading interventions for students in
Grades 4 to 12 grew from initial efforts to diagnose and treat struggling readers on
a case-by-case basis to large-scale experimental trials of multicomponent interventions. Undoubtedly, fresh insights will be discovered over the next 100 years
through further research on new ways to help struggling readers, wisdom gained
from teachers and others who work closely with students, and public policy that
encourages progress in the field. Developments on the near horizon include
potential advances in neuroscience, genetics, psychology, education, and other
fields that may reveal more about the underpinnings of reading disabilities, document neurological and/or genetic differences in students’ response to evidencebased interventions, address questions surrounding the malleability of reading
disabilities, and develop treatments that are effective for students who have attention or behavior disorders or who struggle in math as well as reading. The story of
the next century of progress likely will be shaped by other developments that are
as unforeseeable to contemporary researchers as large-scale RCTs yielding data
analyzed with sophisticated statistical modeling would have been to pioneers such
as William Gray and Grace Fernald. However, throughout history the guiding
purpose of intervention research has remained the same: to help struggling readers overcome their difficulties and achieve reading success.
Note
This research was supported by Grant P50 HD052117 from the Eunice Kennedy
Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and by the Institute of
Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, through Grant R305F100013 to The
University of Texas at Austin as part of the Reading for Understanding Research Initiative.
The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the
official views of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development, the National Institutes of Health, the Institute of Education Sciences, or the
U.S. Department of Education.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate intervention studies included in the narrative review.
*Abbott, S. P., & Berninger, V. W. (1999). It’s never too late to remediate: Teaching
word recognition to students with reading disabilities in Grades 4-7. Annals of
Dyslexia, 49, 223–250. doi:10.1007/s11881-999-0025-x
*Alfassi, M. (1998). Reading for meaning: The efficacy of reciprocal teaching in fostering reading comprehension in high school students in remedial reading classes.
31
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
American
Educational
Research
Journal,
35,
309–332.
doi:10.3102/00028312035002309
*Allinder, R. M., Dunse, L., Brunken, C. D., & Obermiller-Krolikowski, H. J. (2001).
Improving fluency in at-risk readers and students with learning disabilities. Remedial
and Special Education, 22, 48–45. doi:10.1177/074193250102200106
*Amble, B. R., & Butler, G. (1966). Phrase-reading training and the reading achievement of slow learners: An experimental study. Journal of Special Education, 1,
119–123. doi:10.1177/002246696600100203
*Anders, P. L., Bos, C. S., & Filip, D. (1984). The effect of semantic feature analysis
on the reading comprehension of learning-disabled students. In J. S. Niles & L. A.
Harris (Eds.), Changing perspectives on reading/language processing and instruction (pp. 162–166). Rochester, NY: The National Reading Conference.
*Atkinson, J. K., Cochrane, K. J., & Elkins, J. (1968). Retarded readers at high school
start: An investigation in diagnosis and treatment. Slow Learning Child, 15, 67–83.
doi:10.1080/0156655680150202
*Balow, B. (1965). The long-term effect of remedial reading instruction. The Reading
Teacher, 18, 581–586.
Balow, B. (1971). Perceptual-motor activities in the treatment of severe reading disability. The Reading Teacher, 24, 513–525.
Barquero, L. A., Davis, N., & Cutting, L. E. (2014). Neuroimaging of reading intervention: A systematic review and activation likelihood estimate meta-analysis. PloS
one, 9, e83668.
*Bartlet, D., & Shapiro, M. B. (1956). Investigation and treatment of a reading disability in a dull child with severe psychiatric disturbances. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 26, 180–190. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1956.tb01378.x
Bell, J. E. (1945). Emotional factors in the treatment of reading difficulties. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 9, 125–131.
*Bemboom, C. M., & McMaster, K. L. (2013). A comparison of lower- and higherresourced tier 2 reading interventions for high school sophomores. Learning
Disabilities Research & Practice, 28, 184–195. doi:10.1111/ldrp.12020
Berk, R. A. (Ed.). (1981). Educational evaluation methodology: The state of the art.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
*Berkeley, S., Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (2011). Reading comprehension
strategy instruction and attribution retraining for secondary students with learning
and other mild disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 18–32.
doi:10.1177/0022219410371677
*Bhat, P., Griffin, C. C., & Sindelair, P. T. (2003). Phonological awareness instruction
for middle school students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly,
26, 73–87.
*Bhattacharya, A., & Ehri, L. C. (2004). Graphosyllabic analysis helps adolescent
struggling readers read and spell words. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 331–
348.
Biancarosa, C., & Snow, C. E. (2006). Reading next—A vision for action and research
in middle and high school literacy: A report to Carnegie Corporation of New York
(2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education.
*Biggs, M. C., Homan, S. P., Dedrick, R., Minick, V., & Rasinski, T. (2008). Using an
interactive singing software program: A comparative study of struggling middle
school readers. Reading Psychology, 29, 195–213. doi:10.1080/02702710802073438
32
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
*Billingsley, B. S., & Wildman, T. M. (1988). The effects of prereading activities on
the comprehension monitoring of learning disabled adolescents. Learning
Disabilities Research, 4, 36–44.
Blanchard, P. (1946). Psychoanalytic contributions to the problems of reading disabilities. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 2, 163–187.
*Block, C., Parris, S. R., Reed, K. L., Whiteley, C. S., & Cleveland, M. D. (2009).
Instructional approaches that significantly increase reading comprehension. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 101, 262–281. doi:10.1037/a0014319
*Bloomer, R. H. (1964). Progressive choice reading techniques in a remedial reading
clinic. Journal of Educational Research, 57, 486–488.
Boardman, A. G., Klingner, J. K., Buckley, P., Annamma, S., & Lasser, C. J. (2015).
The efficacy of Collaborative Strategic Reading in middle school science and social
studies classes. Reading and Writing, 28, 1257–1283. doi:10.1007/s11145-0159570-3
*Bookbinder, K. F., & Flierl, N. T. (1968). A plan for teaching pupils with severe reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1, 140–147.
doi:10.1177/002221946800100207
*Borkowski, J. G., Weyhing, R. S., & Carr, M. (1988). Effects of attributional retraining on strategy-based reading comprehension in learning-disabled students. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 80, 46–53. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.1.46
*Bos, C. S., & Anders, P. L. (1990). Effects of interactive vocabulary instruction on the
vocabulary learning and reading comprehension of junior-high learning disabled
students. Learning Disability Quarterly, 13, 31–42.
*Bos, C. S., & Anders, P. L. (1992). Using interactive teaching and learning strategies
to promote text comprehension and content learning for students with learning disabilities. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 39, 225–
238. doi:10.1080/0156655920390305
*Bos, C. S., Anders, P. L., Filip, D., & Jaffe, L. E. (1989). The effects of an interactive
instructional strategy for enhancing reading comprehension and content area learning for students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22,
384–390.
*Boyle, J. R. (1996). The effects of a cognitive mapping strategy on the literal and
inferential comprehension of students with mild disabilities. Learning Disabilities
Quarterly, 19, 86–98.
*Brailsford, A., Snart, F., & Das, J. P. (1984). Strategy training and reading comprehension. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 287–290. doi:10.1177/002221948401700508
*Brown, R. I. (1967). A remedial reading program for the adolescent illiterate. Journal
of Special Education, 1, 409–417. doi:10.1177/002246696700100411
*Buerger, T. A. (1968). A follow-up of remedial reading instruction. The Reading
Teacher, 21, 329–334.
*Burns, M. K., Hodgson, J., Parker, D. C., & Fremont, K. (2011). Comparison of the
effectiveness and efficiency of text previewing and preteaching keywords as smallgroup reading comprehension strategies with middle-school students. Literacy
Research and Instruction, 50, 241–252. doi:10.1080/19388071.2010.519097
*Burt, C., & Lewis, R. B. (1946). Teaching backward readers. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 16, 116–132.
*Butler, S. R. (1991). Reading program—remedial, integrated, and innovative. Annals
of Dyslexia, 41, 119–127. doi:10.1007/BF02648081
33
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
*Calhoon, M. (2005). Effects of a peer-mediated phonological skill and reading comprehension program on reading skill acquisition for middle school students with
reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 424–433. doi:10.1177/00
222194050380050501
*Calhoon, M., Sandow, A., & Hunter, C. V. (2010). Reorganizing the instructional
reading components: Could there be a better way to design remedial reading programs to maximize middle school students with reading disabilities’ response to
treatment? Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 57–85. doi:10.1007/s11881-009-0033-x
*Cameron, J. L., Borst, C. A., Fifer, W. P., LaVigne, G. L., & Smith, S. A. (1972).
Remedial reading: A psychoanalytic and operant approach. British Journal of
Medical Psychology, 45, 273–278. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1972.tb02209.x
*Camp, B. W., & van Doorninck, W. J. (1971). Assessment of “motivated” reading
therapy with elementary school children. Behavior Therapy, 2, 214–222.
*Cantrell, S., Alamsi, J., Carter, J., Rintamaa, M., & Madden, A. (2010). The impact of
a strategy-based intervention on the comprehension and strategy use of struggling
adolescent readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 257–280. doi:10.1037/
a0018212
*Chan, L. K., & Cole, P. G. (1986). The effects of comprehension monitoring training
on the reading competence of learning disabled and regular class students. Remedial
and Special Education, 7, 33–40. doi:10.1177/074193258600700407
*Chan, L. K., Cole, P. G., & Morris, J. N. (1990). Effects of instruction in the use of a
visual-imagery strategy on the reading-comprehension competence of disabled and
average readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 13, 2–11. doi:10.2307/1510388
*Chan, K. S. (1991). Promoting strategy instruction generalization through selfinstructional training in students with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 24, 427–290.
Cho, E., Roberts, G. J., Capin, P., Roberts, G., Miciak, J., & Vaughn, S. (2015).
Cognitive attributes, attention, and self-efficacy of adequate and inadequate responders in a fourth grade reading intervention. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice,
30, 159–170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ldrp.12088
*Clarke, P. J., Snowling, M. J., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children’s reading-comprehension difficulties: A randomized controlled trial.
Psychological Science, 21, 1106–1116. doi:10.1177/0956797610375449
*Coleman, J. C. (1953). Results of a ‘total-push’ approach to remedial education.
Elementary School Journal, 53, 454–458. doi:10.1086/458523
*Collyer, M. A. (1940). Improving reading in the ninth grade. The English Journal, 29,
37–43.
Compton, D. L., Miller, A. C., Elleman, A. M., & Steacy, L. M. (2014). Have we forsaken reading theory in the name of “quick fix” interventions for children with
reading disability? Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 55–73.
*Conte, R., & Humphreys, R. (1989). Repeated readings using audiotaped material
enhances oral reading in children with reading difficulties. Journal of Communication
Disorders, 22, 65–79.
*Cowin, P., & Graff, V. (1969). Comprehensive treatment of the older disabled reader.
Bulletin of the Orton Society, 19, 100–112. doi:10.1007/BF02653566
*Curr, W. M., & Gourlay, N. (1953). An experimental evaluation of remedial education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 23, 45–55.
*Damerau, R. (1934). Influence of treatment on the reading ability and behavior disorders of reading disability cases. Smith College Studies in Social Work, 5, 160–183.
34
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
*Darch, C., & Eaves, R. (1986). Visual displays to increase comprehension of high
school learning-disabled students. Journal of Special Education, 20, 309–318.
*Darch, C., & Gersten, R. (1986). Direct setting activities in reading comprehension:
A comparison of two approaches. Learning Disability Quarterly, 9, 235–243.
*Darch, C. B., & Kameenui, E. J. (1987). Teaching LD students critical reading skills:
A systematic replication. Learning Disability Quarterly, 10, 82–91.
doi:10.2307/1510215
Delacato, C. H. (1963). The diagnosis and treatment of speech and reading problems.
Springfield, IL: Thomas.
*DiCecco, V. M., & Gleason, M. M. (2002). Using graphic organizers to attain relational knowledge from expository texts. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 306–
320.
*Diliberto, J. A., Beattie, J. R., Flowers, C. P., & Algozzine, R. F. (2009). Effects of
teaching syllable skills instruction on reading achievement in struggling middle
school readers. Literacy Research and Instruction, 48, 14–27.
doi:10.1080/19388070802226253
Edmonds, M. S., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Reutebuch, C., Cable, A., Tackett, K. K., &
Schnakenberg, J. W. (2009). A synthesis of reading interventions and effects on reading comprehension outcomes for older struggling readers. Review of Educational
Research, 79, 262–300. doi:10.3102/0034654308325998
Education Sciences Reform Act of 2002. Pub. L. No. 107-279, § 116, Stat. 1941 (2002).
Elementary and Secondary Education Act of 1965. Pub. L. No. 89-10, § 79, Stat. 27
(1965).
Elbaum, B., Vaughn, S., Hughes, M. T., & Moody, S. (2000). How effective are oneto-one tutoring programs in reading for elementary students at risk for reading failure? A meta-analysis of the intervention research. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 92, 605–619. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.605
*Ellis, A. (1949). Results of a mental hygiene approach to reading disability problems.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 13, 56–61.
*Englert, C. S., & Mariage, T. V. (1991). Making students partners in the comprehension process: Organizing the reading “posse.” Learning Disability Quarterly, 14,
123–138. doi:10.2307/1510519
Every Student Succeeds Act of 2015. Pub. L. No. 114-95, § 1000 et seq., Stat.
1814 (2015).
Fabian, A. A. (1955). Reading disability: An index of pathology. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 25, 319–329.
*Faggella-Luby, M., & Wardwell, M. (2011). RTI in a middle school: Findings and
practical implications of a tier 2 reading comprehension study. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 34, 35–49.
Farr, R., & Tuinman, J. J. (1972). The dependent variable: Measurement issues in reading research. Reading Research Quarterly, 7, 413–423.
Fernald, G. M., & Keller, H. (1921). The effect of kinaesthetic factors in the development of word recognition in the case of non-readers. Journal of Educational
Research, 4, 335–377.
Fernald, G. M., & Keller, H. (1936). On certain language disabilities: Their nature and
treatment. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
*Fleisher, L. S., & Jenkins, J. R. (1983). The effect of word- and comprehensionemphasis instruction on reading performance. Learning Disability Quarterly, 6,
146–154. doi:10.2307/1510792
35
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
Flesch, R. F. (1955). Why Johnny can’t read and what you can do about it. New York,
NY: Harper.
Flynn, L. J., Zheng, X., & Swanson, H. L. (2012). Instructing struggling older readers:
A selective meta-analysis of intervention research. Learning Disabilities Research
& Practice, 27, 21–32. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2011.00347.x
*Font, M. M. (1942). Orientation in clinical approach through remedial reading
instruction. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 12, 324–334.
*Forlano, G., & Writestone, J. W. (1956). Measuring the effectiveness of special reading instruction in selected vocational high schools. High Points, 38, 31–36.
*Freed, H., Abrams, J., & Peifer, C. (1959). Reading disability: A new therapeutic
approach and its implications. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Psychopathology
& Quarterly Review of Psychiatry and Neurology, 20, 251–259.
Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., & Vaughn, S. (2014). What is intensive instruction and why is
it
important?
Teaching
Exceptional
Children,
46,
13–18.
doi:10.1177/0040059914522966
*Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Kazdan, S. (1999). Effects of peer-assisted learning strategies on high school students with serious reading problems. Remedial and Special
Education, 20, 309–319.
*Gajria, M., & Salvia, J. (1992). The effects of summarization instruction on text comprehension of students with learning disabilities. Exceptional Children, 58, 508–
516.
Gates, A. I. (1922). Teachers College contributions to education: Vol. 129. The psychology of reading and spelling with special, reference to disability. New York, NY:
Teachers College.
Gates, A. I. (1927). The improvement of reading (1st ed.). New York, NY: Macmillan.
Gates, A. I. (1935). The improvement of reading (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Macmillan.
Gates, A. I. (1941). The role of personality maladjustment in reading disability. Journal
of Genetic Psychology, 59, 77–83.
*Gates, A. I., & Bond, G. L. (1936). Some outcomes of instruction in the Speyer
Experimental School (P.S. 500). Teachers College Record, 38, 206–217.
*Given, B. K., Wasserman, J. D., Chari, S. A., Beattie, K., & Eden, G. F. (2008). A
randomized, controlled study of computer-based intervention in middle school
struggling readers. Brain and Language, 106, 83–97. doi:10.1016/j.
bandl.2007.12.001
Glass, G. V. (1976). Primary, secondary and meta-analysis of research. Education
Researcher, 10, 3–8.
*Gormly, J., & Nittoli, M. J. (1971). Rapid improvement of reading skills in juvenile
delinquents. Journal of Experimental Education, 40, 45–48.
*Graves, A. W. (1986). Effects of direct instruction and metacomprehension training
on finding main ideas. Learning Disabilities Research, 1, 90–100.
*Graves, A. W., Duesbery, L., Pyle, N. B., Brandon, R. R., & McIntosh, A. S. (2011).
Two studies of tier II literacy development: Throwing sixth graders a lifeline.
Elementary School Journal, 111, 641–661. doi:10.1086/659036
*Graves, A. W., & Levin, J. R. (1989). Comparison of monitoring and mnemonic textprocessing strategies in learning disabled students. Learning Disability Quarterly,
12, 232–236. doi:10.2307/1510693
Gray, W. S. (1916a). Methods of testing reading: I. Elementary School Journal, 16,
231–246.
36
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
Gray, W. S. (1916b). Methods of testing reading: II. Elementary School Journal, 16,
281–298.
Gray, W. S. (1921). Diagnostic and remedial steps in reading. Journal of Educational
Research, 4, 1–15.
Gray, W. S. (1922). Remedial cases in reading: Their diagnosis and treatment. Chicago,
IL: University of Chicago.
Gray, W. S. (Ed.). (1925). Report of the national committee on reading (24th yearbook
of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part 1). Bloomington, IL: Public
School Publishing.
*Grover, V. M. (1962). The suitability of small group teaching in cases of reading and
spelling disability among young primary school children. Journal for Social
Research, 13, 19–28.
*Günzburg, H. C. (1948). The unsuccessful reader. Mental Health, 8(2), 34–37.
*Guthrie, J. T., McRae, A., Coddington, C. S., Klauda, S., Wigfield, A., & Barbosa, P.
(2009). Impacts of comprehensive reading instruction on diverse outcomes of lowand high-achieving readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 195–214.
doi:10.1177/0022219408331039
*Haring, N. G., & Hauck, M. A. (1969). Improved learning conditions in the establishment of reading skills with disabled readers. Exceptional Children, 35, 341–352.
Harris, A. J. (1967, May). Five decades of remedial reading. Paper presented at the
meeting of the International Reading Association conference, Seattle, WA.
*Harris, M., Schumaker, J., & Deshler, D. (2011). The effects of strategic morphological analysis instruction on the vocabulary performance of secondary students
with and without disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 34, 17–33.
*Hasselbring, T. S., & Goin, L. I. (2004). Literacy instruction for older struggling readings: What is the role of technology? Reading & Writing Quarterly, 20, 123–144.
*Hester, K. B. (1946). Dade County meets the reading problem. Elementary School
Journal, 47, 148–156.
*Hicks, R. A., Hicks, M. J., Kellogg, M., & Honnen, R. (1968). Reading gains and
instructional sessions. The Reading Teacher, 21, 738–739, 744.
Hildreth, G. (1942). The psychologist investigates reading disability. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 6, 212–217.
*Homan, S. P., Klesius, J. P., & Hite, C. (1993). Effects of repeated readings and nonrepetitive strategies on students’ fluency and comprehension. Journal of Educational
Research, 87, 94–99.
Individuals With Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004. Pub. L. No. 108446, § 118, Stat. 2647 (2004).
Institute of Education Sciences. (2008). What works: Clearinghouse study review standards. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/reference_resources/wwc_
version1_standards.pdf
*Jackson, M., & Dando, F. (1976). An evaluation of the Delacto method for the facilitation of reading behavior. The Exceptional Child, 23, 113–118.
*Jitendra, A. K., Hoppes, M. K., & Xin, Y. P. (2000). Enhancing main idea comprehension for students with learning problems: The role of a summarization strategy and
self-monitoring instruction. Journal of Special Education, 34, 127–139.
*Johnson, G., Gersten, R., & Carnine, D. (1987). Effects of instructional design variables on vocabulary acquisition of learning disabilities students: A study of computer-assisted instruction. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 206–213.
37
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
Kaldenberg, E., Watt, S., & Therrien, W. (2015). Reading instruction in science for
students with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Learning Disability Quarterly,
38, 160–173.
Kamil, M. L., Borman, G. D., Dole, J., Kral, C. C., Salinger, T., & Torgesen, J. (2008).
Improving adolescent literacy: Effective classroom and intervention practices: A
practice guide (NCEE #2008-4027). Washington, DC: National Center for Education
Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S.
Department of Education.
Kelly, F. J. (1916). The Kansas silent reading tests. Journal of Educational Psychology,
7, 63–80.
*Kennedy, K. M., & Backman, J. (1993) Effectiveness of the Lindamood auditory in
depth program with students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities
Research & Practice, 8, 253–259.
*Kennedy, M. J., Thomas, C. N., Meyer, J. P., Alves, K. D., & Lloyd, J. W. (2014).
Using evidence-based multimedia to improve vocabulary performance of adolescents with LD: A UDL approach. Learning Disability Quarterly, 37, 71–86.
doi:10.1177/0731948713507262
*Kim, A., Vaughn, S., Klingner, J. K., Woodruff, A. L., Reutebuch, C., & Kouzekanani,
K. (2006). Improving the reading comprehension of middle school students with
disabilities through computer-assisted collaborative strategic reading. Remedial and
Special Education, 27, 235–249. doi:10.1177/07419325060270040401
*Kim, J. S., Samson, J. F., Fitzgerald, R., & Hartry, A. (2010). A randomized experiment of a mixed-methods literacy intervention for struggling readers in Grades 4–6:
Effects on word reading efficiency, reading comprehension and vocabulary, and oral
reading fluency. Reading and Writing, 23, 1109–1129. doi:10.1007/s11145-0099198-2
*Kline, C. L., Kline, C. L., Ashbrenner, M., & Calkins, S. (1968). The treatment of
specific dyslexia in a community mental health centre. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 1, 456–465. doi:10.1177/002221946800100805
*Klingner, J. K., & Vaughn, S. (1996). Reciprocal teaching of reading comprehension
strategies for students with learning disabilities who use English as a second language. Elementary School Journal, 96, 275–293.
Krafnick, A. J., Flowers, D. L., Napoliello, E. M., & Eden, G. F. (2011). Gray matter volume
changes following reading intervention in dyslexic children. Neuroimage, 57, 733–741.
Kunst, M. S. (1959). Learning disabilities: Their dynamics and treatment. Social Work,
4, 95–101.
Kussmaul, A. (1877). Cyclopedia of the practice of medicine: Vol. 14. Disturbances of
speech: An attempt in the pathology of speech (H. von Ziemssen, Ed., & J. A.
McCreery, Trans). New York, NY: William Wood.
*Laffey, R. (1950). A program in remedial reading. Elementary English, 27, 230–
239.
*Lane, A. (1974). Severe reading disability and the initial teaching alphabet. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 7, 479–483. doi:10.1177/002221947400700803
*Lane, P. R. (1970). Educational therapy for adolescent nonreaders. Intervention in
School and Clinic, 6, 155–159. doi:10.1177/1053451270006000205
*Lang, L., Torgesen, J., Vogel, W., Chanter, C., Lefsky, E., & Petscher, Y (2009).
Exploring the relative effectiveness of reading interventions for high school students. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2, 149–175.
doi:10.1080/19345740802641535
38
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
Lemons, C. J., Fuchs, D., Gilbert, J. K., & Fuchs, L. S. (2014). Evidence-based practices in a changing world: Reconsidering the counterfactual in education research.
Educational Researcher, 43, 242–252. doi:10.3102/0013189X14539189
*Lesaux, N. K., Kieffer, M. J., Kelley, J. G., & Harris, J. R. (2014). Effects of academic
vocabulary instruction for linguistically diverse adolescents: Evidence from a randomized field trial. American Educational Research Journal, 51, 1159–1194.
doi:10.3102/0002831214532165
*Lloyd, J., Cullinan, D., Heins, E. D., & Epstein, M. H. (1980). Direct instruction:
Effects on oral and written language comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly,
3, 70–76. doi:10.2307/1510677
*Louden, B., & Arthur, G. (1940). An application of the Fernald method to an extreme
case of reading disability. Elementary School Journal, 40, 599–606.
doi:10.1086/457811
*Lovell, K., Johnson, E., & Platts, D. (1962). A summary of a study of the reading ages
of children who had been given remedial teaching. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 32, 66–71. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1962.tb01734.x
*Lovett, M. W., Lacerenza, L., De Palma, M., & Frijters, J. C. (2012). Evaluating the
efficacy of remediation for struggling readers in high school. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 45, 151–169. doi:10.1177/0022219410371678
*Lovitt, T. C. (1975). Applied behavior analysis and learning disabilities: Part II:
Specific research recommendations and suggestions for practitioners. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 8, 504–518. doi:10.1177/002221947500800809
*Macaruso, P., & Rodman, A. (2009). Benefits of computer-assisted instruction for
struggling readers in middle school. European Journal of Special Needs Education,
24, 103–113. doi:10.1080/08856250802596774
MacGinitie, W. H. (1980). Gates-MacGinitie Reading Tests. Toronto, Ontario, Canada:
Nelson.
*Malone, L. D., & Mastropieri, M. A. (1992). Reading comprehension instruction:
Summarization and self-monitoring training for students with learning disabilities.
Exceptional Children, 58, 270–279.
*Manset-Williamson, G., & Nelson, J. M. (2005). Balanced, strategic reading instruction for upper-elementary and middle school students with reading disabilities: A
comparative study of two approaches. Learning Disability Quarterly, 28, 59–74.
doi:10.2307/4126973
Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T., Bakken, J., & Whedon, C. (1996). Reading comprehension: A synthesis of research in learning disabilities. Advances in Learning and
Behavioral Disabilities, 10B, 201–227.
*Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., Levin, J. R., Gaffney, J., & McLoone, B. (1985).
Mnemonic vocabulary instruction for learning disabled students. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 8, 57–63.
*Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T., Mohler, L., Beranek, M., Spencer, V., Boon, R. T., &
Talbott, E. (2001). Can middle school students with serious reading difficulties help
each other and learn anything? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 16, 18–27.
*Mathes, P. G., & Fuchs, L. S. (1993). Peer-mediated reading instruction in special
education resource rooms. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 8, 233–243.
*McCallum, R., Krohn, K. R., Skinner, C. H., Hilton-Prillhart, A., Hopkins, M., Waller,
S., & Polite, F. (2011). Improving reading comprehension of at-risk high-school
students: The Art of Reading program. Psychology in the Schools, 48, 78–86.
doi:10.1002/pits.20541
39
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
McGann, M. (1947). Dramatic dialogues for simultaneous treatment of reading and
personality problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 38, 96–104. doi:10.1037/
h0055961
*McKerracher, D. W. (1967). Alleviation of reading difficulties by a simple operant
conditioning technique. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 51–56.
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1967.tb02180.x
*McLoone, B. B., Scruggs, T. E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Zucker, S. F. (1986). Memory
strategy instruction and training with learning disabled adolescents. Learning
Disabilities Research, 2, 45–52.
*Meikle, S., & Kilpatrick, D. L. (1971). Changes in perceptual, motor, and reading test
scores in a remedial reading group. The Canadian Psychologist, 12, 254–269.
*Meyer, B. F., Wijekumar, K. K., & Lin, Y. (2011). Individualizing a web-based structure strategy intervention for fifth graders’ comprehension of nonfiction. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 103, 140–168. doi:10.1037/a0021606
Miciak, J., Stuebing, K. K., Vaughn, S., Roberts, G., Barth, A. E., & Fletcher, J. M. (2014).
Cognitive attributes of adequate and inadequate responders to reading intervention in
middle school. School Psychology Review, 43, 407–427. doi:10.17105/SPR-13-0052.1
*Monroe, M. (1937). Remedial reading: A monograph in character education. Boston,
NY: Houghton Mifflin.
*Moore, P. J., & Scevak, J. J. (1995). The effects of strategy training on high school
students’ learning from science texts. European Journal of Psychology of Education,
10, 401–410.
Morgan, W. P. (1896). A case of congenital word blindness. British Medical Journal,
2(1871), 1378.
National Defense Education Act of 1958, Pub. L. No. 85-864, § 72 Stat. 1580.
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State
School Officers. (2010). Common Core State Standards. Washington, DC: Author.
*Niedo, J., Lee, Y.-L., Breznitz, Z., & Berninger, V. W. (2014). Computerized silent
reading rate and strategy instruction for fourth graders at risk in silent reading rate.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 37, 100–110. doi:10.1177/0731948713507263
*O’Brien, F. J. (1938). A child guidance approach to the treatment of a severe reading
disability. Understanding the Child, 7, 22–26.
*Orton, S. T. (1925). Word blindness in school children. Archives of Neurology and
Psychiatry, 14, 581–615.
*O’Shea, L. J., Sindelar, P., & O’Shea, D. J. (1987). Effects of repeated readings and
attentional cues on the reading fluency and comprehension of learning disabled
readers. Learning Disabilities Research, 2, 103–109.
*Pany, D., Jenkins, J. R., & Schreck, J. (1982). Vocabulary instruction: Effects on word
knowledge and reading comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly, 5, 202–215.
doi:10.2307/1510288
*Penney, C. G. (2002). Teaching decoding skills to poor readers in high school. Journal
of Literacy Research, 34, 99–118.
Petticrew, M., & Roberts, H. (2006). Systematic reviews in the social sciences: A practical guide. Malden, MA: Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9780470754887
Pyle, N., & Vaughn, S. (2012). Remediating reading difficulties in a response to intervention model with secondary students. Psychology in the Schools, 49, 273–284.
doi:10.1002/pits.21593
*Rasinski, T., Samuels, S., Hiebert, E., Petscher, Y., & Feller, K. (2011). The relationship between a silent reading fluency instructional protocol on students’ reading
40
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
comprehension and achievement in an urban school setting. Reading Psychology,
32, 75–97. doi:10.1080/02702710903346873
*Redmount, R. S. (1948). Description and evaluation of a corrective program for reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 39, 347–358.
*Regensburg, J. (1930, June). Retraining in reading: A new method. Paper presented
at the annual meeting of the American Association of Psychiatric Social Workers,
Boston, MA.
*Richardson, E., & Collier, L. (1971). Programmed tutoring of decoding skills with
third and fifth grade non-readers. Journal of Experimental Education, 39, 57–64.
*Richardson, D. C., & Havlicek, L. L. (1975). High school students as reading instructors. Elementary School Journal, 75, 389–393. doi:10.1086/460922
*Ritchey, K. D., Silverman, R. D., Montanaro, E. A., Speece, D. L., & Schatschneider,
C. (2012). Effects of a tier 2 supplemental reading intervention for at-risk fourthgrade students. Exceptional Children, 78, 318–334. doi:10.1177/001440291207800304
*Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., Fletcher, J., Stuebing, K., & Barth, A. (2013). Effects of a
response-based, tiered framework for intervening with struggling readers in middle
school. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 237–254. doi:10.1002/rrq.47
Robinson, H. (1939). Types of deficient readers and methods of treatment.
Supplementary Educational Monographs, 49, 159–169.
*Robinson, H. (1949). Problems of seriously retarded readers from the Chicago community. In Clinical studies in reading (Vol. 1, Supplementary Educational
Monograph, No. 68) (pp. 114–173). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
*Ryback, D., & Staats, A. W. (1970). Parents as behavior therapy-technicians in treating reading deficits (dyslexia). Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Psychiatry, 1, 109–119. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(70)90034-0
Scammacca, N., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., Edmonds, M., Wexler, J., Reutebuch, C. K.,
. . . Torgesen, J. K. (2007). Reading interventions for adolescent struggling readers:
A meta-analysis with implications for practice. Portsmouth, NH: RMC Research
Corporation, Center on Instruction.
Scammacca, N., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., & Stuebing, K. K. (2015). A meta-analysis
of interventions for struggling readers in Grades 4-12: 1980-2011. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 48, 369–390. doi:10.1177/0022219413504995
*Schonfield, D. (1956). Special difficulties at a reading age of 8+. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 26, 39–50.
*Schworm, R. W. (1979). The effects of selective attention on the decoding skills
of children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 12,
639–644.
*Seeman, J., & Edwards, B. (1954). A therapeutic approach to reading difficulties.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 18, 451–453.
*Shedd, C. L. (1968). Dyslexia and its clinical management. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 1, 171–184. doi:10.1177/002221946800100302
*Shippen, M. E., Houchins, D. E., Steventon, C., & Sartor, D. (2005). A comparison
of two direct instruction reading programs for urban middle school students.
Remedial and Special Education, 26, 175–182. doi:10.1177/07419325050260030
501
*Shippen, M. E., Morton, R. C., Flynt, S. W., Houchins, D. E., & Smitherman, T.
(2012). Efficacy of a computer-based program on acquisition of reading skills of
incarcerated youth. Remedial and Special Education, 33, 14–22.
doi:10.1177/0741932510362512
41
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
*Silver, A. A., Hagin, R. A., & Hersh, M. F. (1967). Reading disability: Teaching
through stimulation of deficit perceptual areas. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
37, 744–752. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.1967.tb00516.x
*Simpson, S. B., Swanson, J. M., & Kunkel, K. (1992). The impact of an intensive
multisensory reading program on a population of learning-disabled delinquents.
Annals of Dyslexia, 42, 54–66. doi:10.1007/BF02654938
Smith, N. B. (2002). American reading instruction. Newark, DE: International Reading
Association.
*Snider, V. E. (1989). Reading comprehension performance of adolescents with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 12, 87–96.
*Somers, M., Corrin, W., Sepanik, S., Salinger, T., Levin, J., & Zmach, C. (2010). The
enhanced reading opportunity study final report: The impact of supplemental literacy courses for struggling ninth-grade readers. Washington, DC: Institute of
Educational Sciences, National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional
Assistance, U.S. Department of Education.
*Somerville, D. E., & Leach, D. J. (1988). Direct or indirect instruction? An evaluation
of three types of intervention programme for assisting students with specific reading
difficulties. Educational Research, 30, 46–53. doi:10.1080/0013188880300106
*Spencer, S., & Manis, F. (2010). The effects of a fluency intervention program on
fluency and comprehension outcomes of middle-school students with severe reading
deficits. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 25, 76–86.
doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2010.00305.x
*Staats, A. W., Minke, K. A., & Goodwin, W. (1967). Cognitive behavior modification:
“Motivated learning” reading treatment with subprofessional therapy-technicians.
Behavior Research and Therapy, 5, 283–299. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(67)90020-4
*Steiner, R., Wiener, M., & Cromer, W. (1971). Comprehension training and identification for poor and good readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 506–513.
*Stoddard, K., Valcante, G., Sindelar, P., & O’Shea, L. (1993). Increasing reading rate
and comprehension: The effects of repeated readings, sentence segmentation, and
intonation training. Literacy Research and Instruction, 32, 53–65.
*Sullivan, J. (1972). The effects of Kephart’s perceptual motor-training on a reading
clinic sample. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 5, 545–551.
Swanson, E., Wanzek, J., Vaughn, S., Fall, A.-M., Roberts, G., Hall, C., & Miller, V.
(in press). Middle school reading comprehension and content learning intervention
for below average readers. Reading & Writing Quarterly.
Swanson, H. L. (1999). Reading research for students with LD: A meta-analysis in
intervention outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 504–532.
doi:10.1177/002221949903200605
*Swenby, C., & Zielsdorf, M. (1951). A remedial-reading program in a senior high
school. The School Review, 59, 350–357.
*Sylvester, E., & Kunst, M. S. (1943). Psychodynamic aspects of the reading problem.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 13, 69–76.
*Thames, D. G., Reeves, C., Kazelskis, R., York, K., Boling, C., Newell, K., & Wang,
Y. (2008). Reading comprehension: Effects of individualized, integrated language
arts as a reading approach with struggling readers. Reading Psychology, 29, 86–115.
doi:10.1080/02702710701853625
*Therrien, W. J., Wickstrom, K., & Jones, K. (2006). Effect of a combined repeated
reading and question generation intervention on reading achievement. Learning
Disabilities Research & Practice, 21, 89–97. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2006.00209.x
42
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
Thorndike, E. L. (1914). The measurement of ability in reading. Teachers College
Record, 15, 1–70.
Tinker, M. A. (1934). Remedial reading for non-readers. School & Society, 40, 524–
526.
Tinker, M. A. (1938). Trends in diagnostic and remedial reading as shown by recent
publications in this field. Journal of Educational Research, 32, 293–303.
Tomkins, C. (1963, September 14). A reporter at large: The last skill acquired. The New
Yorker, 39(30), 127–128.
Torgesen, J. K. (1977). Memorization processes in reading-disabled children. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 69, 571–578.
Torgesen, J. K. (1982). The learning disabled child as an inactive learner: Educational
implications. Topics in Learning & Learning Disabilities, 2, 45–52.
*Torgesen, J. K., Stancavage, F., Myers, D., Schirm, A., Stuart, E., Vartivarian, S., . . .
Haan, C. (2006). Closing the reading gap: First year findings from a randomized
trial of four reading interventions for striving readers: Final report. Washington,
DC: U.S. Department of Education.
*Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Alexander, A. W., & Conway, T.
(1997). Preventive and remedial interventions for children with severe reading disabilities. Learning Disabilities, 8, 51–61.
Traxler, A. E. (1941). Ten years of research in reading: Summary and bibliography.
New York, NY: Educational Records Bureau.
Uhl, W. L. (1916). The use of the results of reading tests as bases for planning remedial
work. Elementary School Journal, 17, 266–275.
Ullmann, C. A. (1949). A public health approach to improving community mental
health through the schools. Public Health Reports, 64, 1655–1660.
*Vadasy, P. F., & Sanders, E. A. (2008). Benefits of repeated reading intervention for
low-achieving fourth- and fifth-grade students. Remedial and Special Education, 29,
235–249. doi:10.1177/0741932507312013
*Van Osdol, B. M., Johnson, D. M., & Geiger, L. (1974). The effects of total body
movement on reading achievement. Australian Journal of Mental Retardation, 3,
16–19. doi:10.3109/13668257409018751
*Vaughn, S., Cirino, P., Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Fletcher, J., Denton, C., . . . Francis, D.
(2010). Response to intervention for middle school students with reading difficulties: effects of a primary and secondary intervention. School Psychology Review, 39,
3–21.
Vaughn, S., Elbaum, B. E., Wanzek, J., Scammacca, N., & Walker, M. A. (2014). Code
sheet and guide for education-related intervention study syntheses. Austin, TX: The
Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk.
*Vaughn, S., Klingner, J. K., Swanson, E. A., Boardman, A. G., Roberts, G.,
Mohammed, S. S., & Stillman-Spisak, S. J. (2011). Efficacy of collaborative strategic reading with middle school students. American Educational Research Journal,
48, 938–964. doi:10.3102/0002831211410305
*Vaughn, S., Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Barth, A., Cirino, P., Fletcher, J., …Francis, D. (2010).
The relative effects of group size on reading progress of older students with reading difficulties. Reading and Writing, 23, 931–956. doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9183-9
*Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Leroux, A., Roberts, G., Denton, C., Barth, A., & Fletcher, J.
(2012). Effects of intensive reading intervention for eighth-grade students with persistently inadequate response to intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45,
515–525. doi:10.1177/0022219411402692
43
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Scammacca et al.
*Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Roberts, G., Barth, A. A., Cirino, P. T., Romain, M. A., . . .
Denton, C. A. (2011). Effects of individualized and standardized interventions on
middle school students with reading disabilities. Exceptional Children, 77, 391–407.
*Veit, D. T., Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (1986). Extended mnemonic instruction with learning disabled students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 300–
308.
*Wanzek, J., & Roberts, G. (2012). Reading interventions with varying instructional
emphases for fourth graders with reading difficulties. Learning Disability Quarterly,
35, 90–101. doi:10.1177/0731948711434047
*Wanzek, J., Vaughn, S., Roberts, G., & Fletcher, J. M. (2011). Efficacy of a reading
intervention for middle school students with reading disabilities. Exceptional
Children, 78, 73–87.
Wanzek, J., Vaughn, S., Scammacca, N. K., Metz, K., Murray, C. S., Roberts, G., &
Danielson, L. (2013). Extensive reading interventions for students with reading difficulties after grade 3. Review of Educational Research, 83, 163–195.
doi:10.3102/0034654313477212
Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., & Ciullo, S. (2010). Reading interventions for
struggling readers in the upper elementary grades: A synthesis of 20 years of
research. Reading and Writing, 23, 889–912. doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9179-5
*Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., Roberts, G., & Denton, C. A. (2010). The efficacy of repeated
reading and wide reading practice for high school students with severe reading disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 25, 2–10.
doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2009.00296.x
*Wilder, A. A., & Williams, J. P. (2001). Students with severe learning disabilities can
learn higher order comprehension skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93,
268–278.
*Williams, J. P., Brown, L. G., Silverstein, A. K., & deCani, J. S. (1994). An instructional program in comprehension of narrative themes for adolescents with learning
disabilities. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 17, 205–221.
Willingham, D. T. (2007). Ask the cognitive scientist: The usefulness of brief instruction in reading comprehension strategies. American Educator, 30(3), 39–45, 50.
*Wong, B. Y., & Jones, W. (1982). Increasing metacomprehension in learning disabled
and normally achieving students through self-questioning training. Learning
Disability Quarterly, 5, 228–240. doi:10.2307/1510290
*Word, P., & Rozynko, V. (1974). Behavior therapy of an eleven-year-old-girl with
reading problems. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 7, 551–554.
doi:10.1177/002221947400700904
Authors
NANCY K. SCAMMACCA, PhD, is a project manager and research associate at the
Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk in the College of Education at The
University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station D4900, Austin, Texas, TX 78712;
e-mail:
[email protected]. Her primary research interests include
meta-analysis, psychometrics and other statistical methods, and reading disabilities.
GARRETT J. ROBERTS, PhD, is an assistant professor of Teaching and Learning Sciences
at The University of Denver, Morgridge College of Education, 1999 E. Evans Ave,
Denver, CO 80210-1700; e-mail:
[email protected]. His research
44
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016
Reading Interventions 1914–2014
interests include academic, reading, and behavioral interventions, as well as effective
instructional practices to support all learners.
EUNSOO CHO, PhD, is an assistant professor at Michigan State University, College of
Education, Erickson Hall, 620 Farm Ln, East Lansing, MI 48824; e-mail: eunsoo.e.cho@
gmail.com. Her research interests include statistical modeling of growth in reading
skills and identification and intervention of reading disabilities.
KELLY J. WILLIAMS, EdS, is a graduate research assistant and doctoral student at the
Meadows Center for Preventing Education Risk in the College of Education at the
University of Texas at Austin, SZB 228, University Mail Code D4900, Austin, Texas,
TX 78701; e-mail:
[email protected]. Her research interests are in adolescent
reading interventions and dropout prevention.
GREG ROBERTS, PhD, is director of the Vaughn Gross Center for Reading and Language
Arts and the associate director of the Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk,
both at The University of Texas at Austin, SZB 228, University Mail Code D4900,
Austin, Texas, TX 78701; e-mail:
[email protected]. His interests include
statistical modeling, program evaluation, and reading disability.
SHARON R. VAUGHN, PhD, is the H. E. Hartfelder/Southland Regents Chair and the
executive director of the Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk at The
University of Texas at Austin, SZB 228, University Mail Code D4900, Austin, Texas,
TX 78701; e-mail:
[email protected]. Her research addresses academically
related interventions, primarily in reading, for students with reading and learning
difficulties.
MEGAN CARROLL, MA, is a research associate at the Meadows Center for Preventing
Educational Risk in the College of Education at The University of Texas at Austin, SZB
228, University Mail Code D4900, Austin, Texas, TX 78701; e-mail: meganmckenty@
austin.utexas.edu. Her primary research interests include reading disabilities, middle
school reform, and instructional interventions for high-risk populations.
45
Downloaded from http://rer.aera.net at University of Texas Libraries on June 3, 2016