Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Pathways toward progress

University of Iowa Masthead Logo Iowa Research Online Theses and Dissertations Summer 2011 Pathways toward progress: examining the relationships among racial identity, academic intrinsic motivation, and perceived suppport on African American students' academic achievement Antonia Maria Kennedy Szymanski University of Iowa Copyright 2011 Antonia Maria Kennedy Szymanski This dissertation is available at Iowa Research Online: https://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/1270 Recommended Citation Szymanski, Antonia Maria Kennedy. "Pathways toward progress: examining the relationships among racial identity, academic intrinsic motivation, and perceived suppport on African American students' academic achievement." PhD (Doctor of Philosophy) thesis, University of Iowa, 2011. https://doi.org/10.17077/etd.1d1q7wou Follow this and additional works at: https://ir.uiowa.edu/etd Part of the Educational Psychology Commons PATHWAYS TOWARD PROGRESS: EXAMINING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG RACIAL IDENTITY, ACADEMIC INTRINSIC MOTIVATION, AND PERCEIVED SCHOOL SUPPORT ON AFRICAN AMERICAN STUDENTS' ACADEMIC ACHIEVMENT by Antonia Maria Kennedy Szymanski An Abstract Of a thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy degree in Psychological and Quantitative Foundations in the Graduate College of The University of Iowa July 2011 Thesis Supervisors: Professor David Lohman Assistant Professor Malik Henfield 1 Academic as achievement reflects the accumulation over time of interactions between a student and the demands and affordances of the situations in which they attempt to learn particular academic knowledge and skills. Aptitude theory describes the cognitive, affective, and conative variables that moderate the success these interactions. Racial identity may be an essential aptitude variable for explaining academic achievement for non-white students. Black racial identity is described as the importance and understanding that African American students ascribe to belonging to a Black racial group in a society that is dominated by non-Black members. Investigating how the factors of racial identity (private regard, public regard, and racial centrality) are associated with the other key aspects of performance may guide efforts to support African American students in improving their academic performance. The purpose of this study was to investigate relationships among racial identity, academic intrinsic motivation, perceived school support, and academic achievement for African American students. Participants were students (N=56) in grades 11 and 12 from two schools in a single Midwestern metropolitan district. Students completed questionnaires regarding their perceptions of school support, academic intrinsic motivation, and racial identity. The school provided information on current grade point average, number of advanced placement courses taken, and sixth grade standardized achievement test scores. Hierarchical linear regression models were used to investigate whether the racial identity factors moderated the relationships between academic intrinsic motivation and perceived school support on either grade point average or the number of advanced placement courses taken, controlling for sixth-grade academic achievement. 2 Significant interactions were found between racial centrality and perceived school support for academic intrinsic motivation. Significant interactions were also found for racial centrality and private regard moderating the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and the number of advanced placement courses taken, controlling for prior academic achievement. These findings suggest that racial centrality and private regard may be important moderators of academic success for students with lower levels of academic intrinsic motivation and lower perceived school support. These students may benefit from interventions that support feeling positive about being Black and recognize the importance of race in students’ self-concepts. Abstract Approved: ____________________________________ Thesis Supervisor ____________________________________ Title and Department ____________________________________ Date ____________________________________ Thesis Supervisor ____________________________________ Title and Department ____________________________________ Date PATHWAYS TOWARD PROGRESS: EXAMINING THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG RACIAL IDENTITY, ACADEMIC INTRINSIC MOTIVATION, AND PERCEIVED SCHOOL SUPPORT ON AFRICAN AMERICAN STUDENTS’ ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT by Antonia Maria Kennedy Szymanski A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy degree in Psychological and Quantitative Foundations in the Graduate College of The University of Iowa July 2011 Thesis Supervisors: Professor David Lohman Assistant Professor Malik Henfield Graduate College The University of Iowa Iowa City, Iowa CERTIFICATE OF APPROVAL _______________________ PH.D. THESIS _______________ This is to certify that the Ph.D. thesis of Antonia Maria Kennedy Szymanski has been approved by the Examining Committee for the thesis requirement for the Doctor of Philosophy degree in Psychological and Quantitative Foundations at the July 2011 graduation. Thesis Committee: ___________________________________ David Lohman, Thesis Co-Supervisor ___________________________________ Malik Henfield, Thesis Co-Supervisor ___________________________________ Robert Ankenmann ___________________________________ Megan Foley-Nicpon ___________________________________ Laurie Croft To My Family, without you I would never have had the courage to go down this path. You are the reason for everything I do. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank Dr. Croft for her incredible support of gifted education and those who want to help. Dr. Croft was the first person who encouraged me to pursue a PhD in Educational Psychology which led me to Iowa when she didn’t even know me. Her support and friendship helped me complete this journey when there were many times I would have quit. Thank you also to Dr. Malik Henfield who offered support and mentoring before I had even completed comps. Deepest thanks go to Dr. Ankenmann who spent an entire summer working on chapter three with me. I could not have gotten through this process without Dr. Ankenmann’s expertise and patience. I would like to thank my advisor David Lohman for his patience and support throughout my PhD process. Thank you also to Dr. Foley-Nicpon for her willingness to add yet another commitment to her already incredible schedule. Dr. Coohey, School of Social Work was integral in the conceptualization of my dissertation questions and gave tremendous support in the development of my proposal. As always, my family, Chris, Amanda, and Tristan are my real heroes who help me so much. Thank you for drying my tears, believing in me when I didn’t, and your willingness to give up everything for my chance to fulfill my dreams. If it weren’t for you, I could have never done this. Thank you to my mom and extended family for your love, support, and understanding that I had to move to find space to grow and to Char who gives me courage everyday to live life as best I can. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................................................. viii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION................................................................................................. 1 Racial Identity ...................................................................................................................... 4 Motivation ............................................................................................................................ 6 School Climate and Teacher Relations .................................................................................. 8 Intelligence ........................................................................................................................... 8 Research Questions ............................................................................................................... 9 Importance and Relevance for the Field of Education ......................................................... 11 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW .................................................................................... 13 Academic Achievement ...................................................................................................... 13 Aptitude Theory.................................................................................................................. 15 Cognitive Ability ................................................................................................................ 17 Racial Identity .................................................................................................................... 22 Intrinsic Motivation ............................................................................................................ 35 School Support ................................................................................................................... 45 Summary ............................................................................................................................ 55 CHAPTER 3 METHODS ......................................................................................................... 58 Participants and Sampling ................................................................................................... 58 Determination of the Sample ............................................................................................... 59 IRB and Plan to Maintain Confidentiality ........................................................................... 60 Instruments ......................................................................................................................... 61 Data Analysis ..................................................................................................................... 65 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS ........................................................................................................... 70 Missing Data....................................................................................................................... 70 Preliminary Statistical Analyses .......................................................................................... 71 Statistical Analyses Used to Answer the Primary Research Questions ................................. 73 CHAPTER 5 CONCLUSIONS AND DISCUSSION ................................................................ 90 Conclusions ........................................................................................................................ 90 Discussion .......................................................................................................................... 91 Study Limitations ............................................................................................................... 94 Recommendations for Further Research .............................................................................. 96 REFERENCES ......................................................................................................................... 98 iv APPENDIX A LETTER OF INFORMED CONSENT ........................................................... 108 APPENDIX B DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE ........................................................... 112 APPENDIX C PERCEIVED SCHOOL SUPPORT QUESTIONNAIRE ................................ 114 APPENDIX D MULTIDIMENSIONAL INVENTORY OF BLACK IDENTITY .................. 116 APPENDIX E ACADEMIC MOTIVATION QUESTIONNAIRE .......................................... 118 APPENDIX F TABLES ......................................................................................................... 119 v LIST OF TABLES TABLE 3.1. Key Demographics of Schools Participating in the Study .............................................. 58 F1. Summary Statistics of Sample Recruitment District .................................................... 119 F2. Student Demographic Data.......................................................................................... 120 F3. Chi-Squared Analysis of Demographic Data ............................................................... 121 F4. Summary Statistics of Outcome and Control Measures ............................................... 122 F5. Summary Statistics of Scores on Self-Report Measures ............................................... 122 F6. Correlations Between Scores on Self-Report Measures ............................................... 123 F7. Regression Model Summary for Relationships Among Perceived Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB) and Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) ........................................................................................................................ 124 F8. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships Among Perceived Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB) and Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) ...................................................................................................... 125 F9. Regression Model Summary for Relationships Among Perceived Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB) and Academic Intrinsic Motivation – Reduced Models ......................................................................................................... 126 F10. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships Among Perceived Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) – Reduced Models (A, B, and C) ................................................... 128 F11. Regression Model Summary for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Grade Point Average (GPA) ....................................... 131 F12. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Grade Point Average (GPA)........................... 132 F13. Regression Model Summary for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced Placement Courses (NAP) ........ 133 F14. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced Placement Courses (NAP) ......................................................................................................................... 134 vi F15. Regression Model Summary for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced Placement Courses (NAP) – Reduced Models (A, B, and C).................................................................................... 135 F16. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced Placement Courses (NAP) – Reduced Models (A, B, and C)...................................................................... 137 F17. Regression Model Summary for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Perceived Academic Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Grade Point Average (GPA) .................................................... 140 F18. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Perceived Academic Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Grade Point Average (GPA)........................... 141 F19. Regression Model Summary for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Perceived Academic Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced Placement Courses (NAP)..................... 142 F20. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships Among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Perceived Academic Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced Placement Courses (NAP) ......................................................................................................................... 143 vii LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE 4.1. Graph of Interaction Between Private Regard and Perceived Support on Academic Motivation .................................................................................................................... 77 4.2. Graph of Interaction Between Centrality and Motivation on Number of Advanced Placement Courses ........................................................................................................ 82 4.3. Graph of Interaction Between Private Regard and Motivation on Number of Advanced Placement Courses ....................................................................................... 83 viii 1 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION The election of the first African American president of the United States may give the impression that African American students receive the education and support that they need to develop their potential. However, research shows the opposite is true. African American students are disproportionately overrepresented in remedial classes and underrepresented in programs for gifted students (Skiba, Poloni-Staudinger, Gallini, Simmons, & Feggins-Azziz, 2006; Elhoweris, Mutua, Alsheikh, Hollowya, 2005; Ferri & Connor, 2005; Harris III, Brown, Ford, & Richardson, 2004; Patton, 1998). Studies show that the underrepresentation in gifted programs is so substantial that it has been called, ―The most segregated portion of education in this nation.‖(Ford, 1995, p. 52). A possible explanation for this disproportionate representation may be that African American students behave differently from European American students in the classroom due to differences in motivation and perceived social context. Barton and Coley (2010), report that the NAEP achievement gap in reading and mathematics has not diminished since 1980 with the exception of the reading gap for 9-year-old students. The goal of this research is to build upon current conceptualizations of the factors that affect academic achievement for African American students. Information from this study may help teachers, administrators and parents understand the unique needs of African American students and thereby create interventions that may aid the development of these learners. Colangelo, Kerr, Maxey and Christensen (1992) noted that 0.8% of students who scored at the 95th percentile on the ACT were African American. This dismal state of academic achievement for African American students has not shown much improvement 2 over the past 20 years. ACT reports that only 35% of all African American students who took the ACT in 2009 met the benchmark score (50% chance of obtaining a B or higher, or a 75% chance of obtaining a C or higher in corresponding college course work; ACT, 2010). In 2009, the average ACT composite score of White students was 23.0, 1.1 standard deviations higher than the average African American score of 17.6. This difference provides evidence that an achievement gap persists between academically advanced Black and White students. Higher scores provide opportunities afforded by of scholarships and admission to prestigious colleges. Elementary and secondary students need support, encouragement, and appropriate academic preparation to take advanced coursework that will lay the foundation for post-secondary studies. Better understanding of the psychosocial foundations of academic learning could enable educators to develop better methods for assisting students to maximize their academic achievement. Academic achievement, as estimated by scores on standardized tests and grade-point average, has always been the primary concern of those interested in education. The achievement disparity between Black and White students led to the passage of Federal mandates that aim to increase achievement for African American students such as the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001. Although some progress has been made such as the decrease in reading score differences on NAEP tests, the achievement gap between African American and White students is still substantial (Barton & Coley, 2010). Even small group differences at the mean of a distribution may translate into large group differences at the tails of the distribution. Recently, researchers have drawn attention to the large group differences at the upper end of the performance distribution (Plucker, Burroughs, & Song 2009; Loveless, 2009). One of the findings in advanced 3 mathematics has been that the gaps between Black and White students have increased from 1996 to 2007 ranging from 6.8% - 8.5%. Another finding was that Grade 4 reading showed the percentage of White students scoring at advanced levels increased by 1.4 percentage points whereas Black students scoring at advanced levels increased by 0.7% (Plucker, Burroughs, & Song, 2009). Various theories have been developed to explain differences between Black and White students on standardized measures of performance such as NAEP and ACT tests. Most modern theories view the scores as reflecting the accumulated influence of many different factors operating over the years that children prepare for and participate in the educational system (Corno, Cronbach, Kupermintz, Lohman, Mandinach, Porteus & Talbert, 2002; Snow, 1992). Some researchers argue that ―stereotype threat‖ creates increased anxiety on African American students which reduces their test performance (Steele, & Aronoson, 1995). Stereotype threat, which has been defined as ―being at risk of confirming as self-characteristic, a negative stereotype about one’s group‖ (Steele & Arononson, 1995, p.797), has been found to produce additional anxiety for members of the marginalized group. The anxiety of anticipating a negative interaction or a feeling of having to represent a race of people may impair performance. Fordham and Ogbu (1986) claim that academic achievement is seen as a ―White‖ domain and a refusal to participate in school is a rejection of White culture. Other researchers point to the heritability of general intelligence with lower IQ group means as indicative of genetic restrictions on intelligence leading to poor academic performance (Jensen, 1998). Since the inception of intelligence testing, theories have existed to explain intelligence and individual variation 4 of performance. It is important to understand the various dimensions of intelligence in order to understand individual differences in academic performance. Racial Identity Ethnic or racial identity theories may help explain the lower achievement of African American students. Worrell (2009) suggests that education is not merely an individual experience but is situated in a social context and is influenced by one’s self perception. Racial identity, while not often recognized by the majority culture, is a major factor in the creation of self-perception of members of subgroups of the population. Situations are interpreted in part through the perceptions created by racial identity and thus may be interpreted differently by members of the minority subgroups (Grantham & Ford, 2003; Cokley, 2001). One of the problems with much research on the impact of racial identity on academic achievement in African American students is that it takes a unidimensional view of both constructs. Researchers hold that there is a direct, linear relationship between how a student views his or her racial identity and academic achievement though results of investigations have been mixed. However, years of research have shown that academic achievement is a multidimensional outcome that reflects the influence of a person’s cognitive ability, his or her level of motivation, affective characteristics, and the situation. Reducing the explanation to two unidimensional variables, racial identity and academic achievement, may miss important information. It is the combinations of contributions from different facets of racial identity to different aspects of achievement that have been ignored which may resolve the inconsistencies in the findings and provide valuable insights for researchers. 5 Black racial identity has been described by Sellers, Smith, Shelton, Rowley, and Chavous as ―the significance and qualitative meaning that individuals attribute to their membership within the Black racial group within their self-concepts‖ (1998, p. 23). Academic institutions and classrooms are typically led by Caucasians. As such, school may represent the dominant culture to racially diverse student. The perceptions regarding the importance of race, how the public perceives African American students, and how the individual feels about being identified as belonging to a particular racial group, may influence interactions between the individual, peers, and teachers thus impacting the academic experience. The Multi-dimensional Model of Racial Identity holds that identity is multifaceted and situational (Sellers, et al., 1998). Different situations may highlight various identities of individuals. Racial identity research seeks to understand the role of race in individual’s interpretation and response in social situations. Components of racial identity such as centrality and regard have been demonstrated to be stable within the individual. Understanding how these components may operate differently for people and be manifested in different behaviors may help understand individual differences in response to interventions. Furthermore, understanding that both racial identity and academic achievement have multiple dimensions allows researchers to explore the more nuanced relationships between the dimensions of each that may be missed by using overall general measures. Some researchers assert that in situations where racial differences are highlighted, (e.g. one minority in a group of majority race people) that some African Americans may feel anxiety due to their perceptions regarding majority cultures’ existing stereotypes 6 (Steele & Aronson, 1995). The situation may increase the centrality of race in the individual’s identity because of a feeling of being different from the rest of the group. If the individual holds a negative perception of public regard, it could influence performance because anxiety regarding Caucasian expectations may be increased if individuals feel that they have to disprove negative stereotypes or serve as representatives for an entire race. Other researchers contend that negative perceptions of public regard may serve as a catalyst that leads individuals to reject the dominant culture (Fordham & Ogbu, 1986). In an academic setting, school may be seen as the dominant culture and academic achievement may be rejected as a means of coping with prior negative experiences. A person for whom race is important in defining who they are and who has experienced the negative stereotypes that may be held by majority culture may feel compelled to reject White culture as a means of demonstrating racial solidarity. Examining academic performance through the lens of racial identity may provide new insights. Refining racial identity into components of racial centrality – the importance of race in who a person is, perceived public regard, and perceived private regard allows a more nuanced understanding regarding which component(s) interact with other performance variables. This level of understanding has been lacking in other research regarding the role of identifying with a particular race and its relationship with academic achievement. Motivation Many theorists distinguish between internal and external sources of motivation. For example, Ryan and Deci (2004) describe intrinsic motivation as an internal drive that 7 compels individuals to engage in specific behavior such as learning. Extrinsic motivation, on the other hand, is the result of external sources that influence behavior, often through the use of threats and rewards. Both sources of motivation may influence academic achievement by reinforcing behaviors that increase student success. Individuals may differ in their sources and levels of motivation. Findings regarding motivation of African American students have been mixed. Some researchers found that African American students seem to be more externally motivated than White students. However, this does not lead to negative performance consequences (Cokley, 2003; Cokley, 2002; Graham, 1994). Conversely, Shernoff and Schmidt (2008) found that African American students had higher intrinsic motivation and affect scores than White students even when their achievement was lower. The study also showed a negative interaction between engagement and grades for African American students (ES = -.20). Further investigation is warranted to understand how motivational components may operate differently for African Americans. Individuals vary in their beliefs regarding the extent to which they view cognitive ability as fixed or malleable which may affect motivation. Beliefs regarding cognitive ability may also affect academic achievement. Researchers have found that individuals who hold incremental beliefs regarding cognitive ability show higher persistence for challenging tasks and are willing to attempt more difficult tasks than individuals who hold entity beliefs regarding ability (Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007; Spray, Wang, Biddle, Chatzisarantis, & Warburton, 2006; Cury, Elliot, DaFonseca, & Moller, 2006; Gottfried, Gottfried, Cook & Morris, 2005). Cury, Elliot, DaFonseca and Moller (2006) found that students who held entity beliefs tended to focus on performance and 8 performance-avoidance goals depending on their personal evaluations of mathematical ability. Pepi, Faria and Alesi (2006) found significant interactions between cultural background and education level of parents in predicting whether an individual adopts a fixed or malleable view of cognitive ability. School Climate and Teacher Relations Students’ perceptions of teacher relations and school climate have also been shown to affect the motivation and self-concept of African American students (Ford, Grantham, & Whiting, 2008; Milner & Ford, 2005). Stereotype threat has been shown to extend beyond testing conditions (Steele, 1997). Students may perceive teachers’ lower expectations and respond in ways which fulfill the expectations at very young ages. Thus, the students who show the potential for advanced achievement may be influenced positively or negatively by their experiences with teachers and the teachers’ expectations. Worrell (2009) has indicated a need for further research to identify whether various identity profiles have unique consequences for learning. Intelligence Theories of intelligence recognize that intelligent behavior is a combination of developed cognitive abilities and other characteristics such as personality, motivation, and creativity (Sternberg, 2002; Ackerman, 1996; Wechsler, 1975; Cattell, 1943). By examining these clusters of characteristics as they are used in the situation in which an individual is asked to demonstrate intelligence, more of the variability in performance may be explained than a static measure of cognitive ability alone (Snow, 1992). A similar breadth of characteristics is commonly recognized among those who study giftedness. For example, Renzulli’s Triarchic Model contends that giftedness may be conceptualized 9 as three overlapping characteristics: cognitive ability, motivation, and creativity (Renzulli, 1977). For African American students, racial identity may be an important factor in understanding the unique issues faced by students of color. Racial identity may impact demonstrations of cognitive ability, affective characteristics, or motivation. Research Questions The purpose of this study is to understand the relationships among components of racial identity, academic intrinsic motivation, perceived school support, and prior academic achievement in predicting academic achievement for African American students. More specifically, this study will focus on the influence of racial identity on motivation, perception of school climate, and current academic achievement. Research to date has yielded mixed results on the relationship of racial identity and achievement. Part of the discrepancies observed in previous studies may be a result of measures of racial identity (Sellers, Rowley, Chavous, Shelton, & Smith, 1997). These inconsistencies may reflect the fact that studies have used various measures of racial identity, each of which emphasizes a somewhat different aspect of the construct. Using a measure of racial identity with three components will allow a more detailed examination of the variables that interact with the components of individual differences in academic achievement. Several research areas will be examined. First, the linear relationship between the three components of racial identity (centrality, public regard, and private regard) in Seller’s Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity and two components of academic motivation (intrinsic motivation, and perceived school support) among 11 th and 12th grade African American students will be investigated. Second, they ways in which the three factors of racial identity (centrality, public regard or private regard) singly or in 10 combination moderate the observed relationships between perceived academic support and intrinsic academic motivation will be explored. Third, an understanding of the role of each racial identity factor and whether it moderates a relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and academic achievement and between perceived school support and academic achievement will be sought. Accordingly, the research questions are: 1. What are the relationships between the set of racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and private regard) and perceived support? 2. What are the relationships between the set of racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and private regard) and intrinsic motivation? 3. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between perceived support and motivation? 4. Do racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and private regard) moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and cumulative grade point average after controlling for 6th grade standardized test scores? 5. Do the racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and private regard) moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and the number of advanced placement courses taken after controlling for 6th grade standardized test scores? 6. Do racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and private regard) moderate the relationship between perceived school support and cumulative grade point average after controlling for 6th grade standardized test scores? 7. Do the racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and private regard) moderate the relationship between perceived school support and the number of advanced placement courses taken after controlling for 6th grade standardized test scores? 11 Importance and Relevance for the Field of Education Studies to date have yielded mixed results when analyzing the relationship between racial identity and achievement. The planned analysis will add information to the understanding of this relationship by examining important variables that influence academic achievement in high-school in conjunction with racial identity. For example, students’ scores on reports of motivation will be used in conjunction with their reports of racial identity components to explain variance in grade point average. The students will also be compared with one another on their racial identity, intrinsic motivation, and perceived school support. By understanding similarities and differences between higher and lower academic achievers we may find areas that schools and teachers can strengthen to increase achievement. Most research on African American academic achievement focuses on differences between Black and White students. The tendency, then, is to adopt a deficit view in order to explain the problem that is preventing African American students from achieving. While solving problems is the nature of most research, this view may reinforce the cultural deficit model that may be in place systematically. Therefore, this research is focused on exploring relationships among racial identity factors, perceived school support, cognitive ability and motivation for African American students in an effort to illuminate those areas of strength that can be strengthened and supported by teachers and administrators. Researchers have learned a great deal regarding differences of the two groups; however, the time has come to focus on within-group variability. This study will add to the literature on intelligence theory and research by including racial identity as a key component in explaining achievement. The results of 12 this research may also be used by policy makers and administrators to create programs and environments that will support academic achievement for African American students. Organization This dissertation is organized into five chapters. Chapter one is an overview of the research problem and brief background explanation. Chapter two is a review of the literature on the relationship between achievement, cognitive ability, motivation, racial identity, and perceived school support for African American adolescents. The literature review begins with an overview of aptitude and academic achievement. The review then focuses on (a) cognitive ability as it relates to academic achievement, (b) racial identity formation and the impact of racial identity on academic achievement, (c) the effect of motivation on academic achievement, and (d) perceived social context or school support effects on academic achievement for all ethnic racial groups, and student perceptions of teacher expectations on academic achievement. The literature review ends with a summary of the state of the literature and a clear explanation of how the dissertation research adds to the current body of knowledge. Chapter three explains the methods that are used to carry out the proposed analysis. Chapter four provides the results of the data analysis. Chapter five discusses the results of the data and implications of the research. 13 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW This chapter will review the relevant literature in four areas: racial identity development for African Americans, motivation, teacher support and school climate, and academic achievement. The final section of the chapter explores relations among these factors and summarizes the research questions of the proposed study. Academic Achievement Educational achievement is a cumulative product of classroom interactions that begin in elementary school. Achievement is typically estimated by grade point average (GPA), scores on standardized achievement tests, or completion of a course of study. Other measures of academic achievement may include participation in Advanced placement or honors courses in high school. Low academic achievement in the K-12 setting limits the educational choices for students upon graduation. Academic achievement influences future earnings. Not surprisingly, more education results in higher income. The National Center for Education Statistics reports that adults, aged 2534, who earn a bachelor’s degree earn salaries that are 53 percent more than young adult high school completers, and 96 percent more than those who did not earn a high school diploma. (2010). Many students in the U. S. leave high school prior to graduation. As of 2000, African Americans dropped out at a rate of 13.1% compared to their White peers’ rate of 6.9%. When students leave school, it impacts the economic, social, and intellectual aspects of the community and of society as a whole. Loveless (2009) used NAEP data to investigate ethnic differences among high achievers. These students scored at or above the top 90th percentile for their grades in 14 reading or math. Although African Americans comprise 16% of the general population, only 2.6% of the high achieving students were African American compared to 81.5% who were White students. African American and Hispanic students scored in the low achieving range at twice the proportion of their representation in the population of eighth graders and one-fifth to one-fourth of the proportion of high achievers that would be expected given the composition of the group as a whole (Loveless, 2009). School achievement is a function of cognitive ability as well as other personal and environmental factors. Thus, predicting school achievement from ability alone ignores the influence of other important variables (Thorndike, 1963). Such ―over-prediction‖ of student achievement may result in students being labeled as ―underachieving‖ or failing to live up to expectations when the result is more accurately described as a failure in prediction (Thorndike, 1963). For example, students must sustain high levels of motivation and engagement in order to attain high levels of achievement. Including measures of the components of intrinsic motivation and the student’s perception of the educational experience may increase the accuracy of the prediction. Researchers have shown that although students may have similar scores on ability tests at one time period, different levels of conative and affective characteristics may impact later achievement. Ackerman (2003) has argued that ―trait complexes‖ of cognitive and personality characteristics that may be used to better predict performance than cognitive ability alone. Individuals with different levels of intrinsic or extrinsic motivation and different goal orientation may demonstrate different levels of performance even if their cognitive abilities are similar. 15 Aptitude Theory More than multiple measures are required for predicting student achievement. The demands and affordances of the context in which learning occurs must also be considered. In a classic paper, Cronbach (1957) suggested that educational researchers simultaneously investigate the effect of both individual characteristics and the treatments. ―Applied psychologists should deal with treatments and persons simultaneously. Treatments are characterized by many dimensions; so are persons.‖ (Cronbach, 1957, p. 680). Collectively, characteristics of persons that predict success in some environment are called aptitudes. Cronbach also suggested using characteristics of the situation to understand how the environment may support or diminish expressions of motivation, affect, and cognitive ability. Aptitude theory as a way to understand individual differences received significant attention from Snow (1989). His conceptualization built upon Cronbach’s (1957) original understanding of aptitude and treatment interactions. Snow (1992) described aptitudes as ―a readiness‖ (p. 9) to learn. This readiness involved three main factors: 1) motivation to learn the material, 2) affective characteristics that make learning enjoyable (i.e. curiosity, openness, agreeableness, emotional stability), and 3) the cognitive ability to grasp the material. These three factors interacted with the characteristics of the learning environment to determine ―the degree to which persons are equipped to meet the affordances of particular performance situations.‖ (Snow, 1996, p. 538). The situation thus may help determine the readiness of the individual to learn in a particular environment. Put differently, defining the treatment is part of defining the aptitude (Snow & Lohman, 1984). 16 Snow (1992) described aptitude as including the ―conative and affective characteristics of persons, not just cognitive abilities‖ (p.9). The interaction between these traits of persons and the treatment environment (situation) in which they are trying to learn together explain achievement. ―Aptitude theory should be thought of as linking science, aimed at descriptive and explanatory concepts that connect the characteristics and capabilities of persons to features of treatment environments, real or desired, so as to reach goals of field achievement‖ (Snow, 1992, p. 16). Understanding the characteristics that an individual brings to the situation can assist in predicting which interventions will be the most successful in supporting academic achievement. Other researchers have extended the concept of aptitude-treatment interaction to describe how a person’s response tendencies or ―propensities‖ (Corno, et al., 2002, p. 47) interact with the situation. These propensities may be strengthened or weakened depending on the situation and the environmental response to the individual’s behavior. As an individual continues to encounter different situations, the propensities that are used continue to develop and eventually become more general aptitudes for learning in other situations. A key component in this conceptualization is the role of the environment in providing feedback that strengthens or weakens the natural tendencies. A child with little environmental variation may lack the opportunity to develop some propensities. Similarly, children who consistently receive negative feedback in specific situations (e.g. school) may develop generalizations about themselves or future situations that thwart learning. 17 The situative aspect of this conceptualization recognizes that persons bring to the situations many response tendencies that could assist or hinder their learning (Snow, 1996). However, individuals in an unfamiliar situation (e.g. different culture) may not perceive the nuanced clues of the environment that would allow them to bring to bear the knowledge, skills, and other characteristics that would help them succeed. Such individuals may perform poorly when compared to others who bring similar resources to the situation. Including situativity in an explanation of individual differences may add to understanding performance differences among individuals with similar aptitude complexes. Cognitive Ability The relationship between cognitive ability and academic achievement is well established. Although the term ―cognitive ability‖ is sometimes used as a synonym for intelligence, it may also be viewed as designating a broader collection of abilities (Jensen, 1998; Cattell, 1963). Indeed, all models of human abilities show that intelligence is a multidimensional construct. Spearman was the first researcher to empirically analyze intelligence test scores (Fancher, 1985). His creation of the statistical technique of factor analysis provided a new way to investigate the relationships between scores on different tests. Spearman’s g is still used as the overarching explanation for differences in ability. Jensen (1998) describes g as the ―distillate of the common source of individual differences in all mental tests, completely stripped of their distinctive features of information content, skill, strategy, and the like.‖ (p. 74). Jensen (1998) further claims that this common factor is invariant across different methods of factor analysis and has cross-cultural support. 18 Cattell (1943) decomposed Spearman’s general factor into two broad abilities: general fluid ability (Gf) and general crystallized ability (Gc). Gc represented an investment of fluid intelligence in domain specific knowledge. Individual differences in interests help determine which areas of domain knowledge are developed over time (Cattell, 1963). Horn (1980) and other researchers found that although Gf tends to decrease after young adulthood Gc tends to increase. Carroll’s (1993) hierarchical model of intelligence is the most widely accepted and widely validated model in use today. This model proposed a stratified hierarchy with the most psychologically transparent or narrow abilities at the bottom of the hierarchy. The second stratum is comprised of nine broad cognitive abilities and the final stratum is comprised of g or a general factor that is shared by the other abilities. The general factor is the most difficult to define because there are no specific abilities that can be observed as evidence for g alone. Further, the predictive power of the general factor is generally less than the predictive power of broad abilities that better match the performance demands of particular domains. Brunswikian symmetry provides one way to understand the lack of predictive power of the most general abilities (Ackerman &Lohman, 2006). This symmetry refers to matching the complexity or specificity of the ability that is being tested to the task that the individual is being asked to perform. Accuracy of prediction may be reduced if a broad ability, as measured by an ability test such as verbal ability, is used to predict performance on a narrow specific task such as reading fluency. Thus, using cognitive ability measures to predict performance is complicated both by the measurement instruments used and the performance task that is predicted. 19 Measuring Cognitive Abilities Measuring cognitive ability is a complex task. Alfred Binet has been credited with developing the first intelligence test. The tasks that made up Binet’s test were designed to be used clinically to determine the mental functioning capabilities of children who were performing below expectations in school. The most common measure of cognitive ability has been to the intelligence test. The most common IQ tests given to school-aged individuals are the Standford- Binet (SB) 5 and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) IV. Until the 1960s, most IQ standardization populations were White, middle-class and primarily male (Flynn & Weiss, 2007). During the feminist and civil rights movements it became apparent to society that White, middle-class children, and adults were often provided opportunities and educational experiences that other individuals were denied. These privileges may have resulted in higher scores on IQ tests therefore standardization samples need to include people who reflect the national population. The samples were changed with the WISC-R and SB-4 (Dickens & Flynn, 2006). However, simply including these individuals in the standardization does not somehow correct for differences in opportunity to develop the abilities measured by the test. Although difference between the performance of White and African American students on intelligence tests have declined; they are still substantial (Barton & Coley, 2010). Other environmental variables may contribute to lower scores of ethnic minority students on standardized IQ tests. Stereotype threat has been found to create anxiety in African American students which may lower test scores. Language may be an issue for English Language Learners or for students who have not learned to speak standard 20 academic English. Nonverbal ability tests have long been used in an effort to reduce the impact of culture on measures of cognitive ability. However, such measures at best reduce the impact of language and culture (Anastasi, 1980). Sometimes nonverbal tests are even more culturally loaded than tests that rely on language (Scarr, 1984). Genetics and Intelligence Throughout history, researchers have been interested in determining how people think and why some seem to be capable of advanced learning. Galton described intelligence as a highly heritable trait that was innate and fixed. Others such as John Stuart Mill have described intelligence and the creation of knowledge as highly influenced by nurture. In an attempt to disentangle the effects of the environment from genetics, studies, such as the Minnesota Twins Study (Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, Segal & Tellegen, 1990), have allowed researchers to estimate the heritability of cognitive abilities. Bouchard et al., (1990) estimated that 50% of the variation in IQ scores could be attributed to genetic factors. Of course, this finding may also be interpreted as 50% of the variation in IQ scores was attributed to the environment. When Turkheimer, Haley, Waldorn, D’Onofrio and Gottesman (2003) studied twins of various levels of SES, they found that environment played a larger role in explaining IQ variance for low SES individuals and the exact opposite was true for high SES individuals. Specifically, heritability (h2) estimates ranged from .10 for low SES children to .72 for high SES children. ―All abilities – physical and cognitive- are developed through exercise and experience. There are no exceptions.‖ (Lohman, 2005b, p. 119). Thus, while nature may provide the potential, nurture provides the opportunities for exercise and experience that allow for the development of potential. 21 The common tendency to view ability and achievement as qualitatively distinct concepts can in part be attributed to the use of different terms to label the constructs. Kelley (1927) called this the jangle fallacy. Because the concepts have different names we assume that they must be different. The jangle fallacy gives the false impression that these are two distinct constructs and that measurement tools can be created that measure each unambiguously. Cognitive abilities are often conceptualized as the raw materials which students bring to academic institutions. Achievement tests are seen as the measurement by which evaluations can be made as to the extent of using given cognitive abilities, students were able to understand and learn grade level information. Lohman (2005b) provides an appropriate analogy for the fallacy of trying to create such a distinction. Arguing that a good measure of reasoning ability should be independent of motivation, experience, education, or culture is like saying that a good measure of physical fitness should be independent of every sport or physical activity in which the person has engaged. (p. 119) Corno, et al. (2002), provide another view of cognitive abilities and achievement test scores. In their view, achievement test performance, or performance at any time is a function of the interaction of past experiences, motivation, and cognitive ability or in a word, readiness. The readiness of an individual is an evaluation of the extent that past experiences and cognitive abilities have impacted an individual’s capacity to benefit from instruction in an area at a particular time. Any measure of performance is a function of these three components and therefore it is not possible to separate cognitive ability from the other components or from past experiences of opportunities to learn and past motivation to learn. 22 Many theorists describe an ability-achievement continuum by which measurement instruments may be described as measuring more crystallized or more fluid intelligence (Lohman, 1993; Sternberg, 1985; Cronbach & Furby, 1970). This continuum places ability tests which measure general problem solving or non-content specific tests at one end. Achievement tests which are measures of specific content taught at various grade levels of school are at the opposite end of the continuum. Thus, the exposure to content is the determining factor of where on the continuum a measurement would lie. Measures of academic achievement therefore not only reflect the cognitive ability of the students but their ability to make use of the instruction provided to master components of the curriculum. Cognitive ability has been linked to almost every measurable outcome. The most common in educational research are academic achievement, school behavior, and family characteristics. As would be expected, strong positive relationships have been found between cognitive ability and academic achievement. Research on cognitive ability and academic achievement has shown that g or IQ can account for approximately 50% of the variance associated with academic achievement as reflected in GPA (Jensen, 1998). While this is a significant percentage of variance, it leaves another 50% that may be impacted by other factors. Therefore, it is important to examine other possible contributors to academic achievement to understand how to help students maximize their performance. Racial Identity Recent research has examined the role of racial identity in school achievement. Identity formation is thought to develop with age and become stable in adulthood. Marcia 23 developed a framework for categorizing identity formation based on the amount of exploration and commitment an individual demonstrates when investigating different identities (1994). Identity exploration typically begins in early adolescence and individuals reach a stable identity in adulthood although some life experiences (e.g. getting married, having a child) may change an individual’s identity perception. The result of participating in various roles leads individuals to create hierarchies of identities (Stryker & Serpe, 1994). An individual may have an identity as a man, an African American, an executive, and a father, and may behave differently based on the identity that is salient in a given situation. For example, Barack Obama has the identity of the President of the United States, the leader of the Democratic Party, an African American man, a husband, and a father to his children. Any of these roles will be salient in a given situation and his behavior will correspond to his perception of the identity that is most important at the time. Similarly, an African American student may exhibit codeswitching, behaving differently in home and school settings, if these environments require different ways of speaking and use different vocabulary. Racial identity differs from most personal identities because of its invariance and the public nature of its expression. An individual would have to take deliberate steps to conceal or change his or her racial identity. ―Unlike a personal identity, such as occupation, ethnicity cannot be chosen by the individual, but rather it is determined at birth or assigned to one by others on the basis of ethnic background or phenotype.‖ (Phinney & Ong, 2007, p. 275). However, most models of racial and ethnic identity recognize that even though society may assign an individual to a particular group, his or her response to acceptance of that assignment may vary with age and experience 24 (Rowley, Sellers, Chavous, Shelton, & Smith, 1998; Cross, 1991; Phinney, 1989). Models of racial and ethnic identity formation may be similar to identity theory in identifying stages through which an individual is expected to progress (Cross, 1991) and that racial and ethnic identity is stable once established (Phinney, 1989; Sellers et al., 1997, Yip, Seaton, Sellers, 2006). Although there is overlap between racial and ethnic identity, they are separate constructs (Phinney, 1989; Sellers, et al., 1997). Ethnic identity may be defined as ―a sense of self, but it differs [from personal identity] in that it involves a shared sense of identity with others who belong to the same ethnic group.‖ (Phinney & Ong, 2007, p. 274). Membership in the group is based on common culture, language and history (Cokley, 2007). For example, Irish and Italian are ethnic identities. Race is based on phenotypic characteristics. In the previous example, the race would be White. Black racial identity is described by Sellers, et al., (1998) as ―the significance and qualitative meaning that individuals attribute to their membership within the Black racial group within their self-concepts.‖(p. 23). Thus while both address group membership, ethnic identity is more focused on sense of an individual’s identification with a particular group (a measure of degree). Racial identity is more focused on the importance and meaning that group membership has for the individual (a qualitative measure). Cokley (2007) advises the choice of the appropriate lens by which to view ethnic and racial identities depends on the focus of the research. When researchers are interested in how individuals see themselves relative to their cultural beliefs, values, and behaviors, ethnic identity is the appropriate construct to study. However, when researchers are more interested in how individuals construct their identities in response to an oppressive and highly racialized society, racial identity is the more appropriate construct to study. ( p. 225) 25 Because racial and ethnic identities are different constructs, different measurement tools have been created to operationalize these variables. Ethnic identity refers to a sense of belongingness to a group or culture (Phinney & Ong, 2007). Consequently, measures of ethnic identity also have measures of racelessness or other group orientation to describe individuals for whom belonging to the ethnic group is not a significant factor in their identity. Racial identity reflects the role that society imposes on an individual as a member of a group (Sellers, et al., 1997). Society’s response to an individual of color sends messages regarding the acceptance and value that is placed on individuals who are not a part of the majority culture. Racial identity instruments seek to measure the degree to which individuals accept the labels that society places on them, the degree to which positive and negative societal messages are accepted, and how meaning is made by individuals. Sellers, Smith, Shelton, Rowley, and Chavous (1998), developed the Multidimensional Model of Racial Identity (MMRI) and subsequently the Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity (MIBI) to measure specific components of racial identity in African Americans. Unlike the Cross Racial Identity Scale (CRIS) (Vandiver, Cross, Worrell, & Fhagen-Smith, 2002) which is stage based and whose scores reflect judgments about an individual’s identity development, the MIBI regards racial identity as both situational and stable. ―Little longitudinal research has been produced that demonstrates the validity of the concept of individuals’ cycling through the four stages proposed by the model.‖ (Sellers, et al., 1998, p. 22). The Multidimensional Model of Racial Identity (MMRI) focuses on the status or strength of the areas of identity 26 rather than the progression of the development of racial identities. There is no final racial identity to be achieved. There are some components in racial identity that are measured by the MIBI, such as regard and centrality, which are more stable within an individual. Other components such as salience reflect the situativity of race being more or less salient in a given situation (Sellers, et al., 1998). Thus, because situations change, salience cannot be measured by survey instruments. The MIBI allows measurements based on subtest scores regard, centrality, and ideology. Regard is the individual’s perception of public opinion of African Americans in general as well as the individual’s personal perception of African Americans. These perceptions are developed through life experiences and environmental interactions. Judgments of public opinion of African Americans may be influenced through societal messages received through the media, in school or when interacting with other members of the community. Personal regard for African Americans reflects the individual’s view towards other African Americans and membership in the group. The extent of positive or negative feelings and affect comprise the component of regard. Centrality refers to the importance of racial identity to the individual. Sellers, et al., (1998) indicated the hierarchical structure of identity. Centrality describes the extent that racial identity is a core component in the individual’s identity. It reflects the importance of race in how individuals define themselves. Ideology describes individual perceptions of how African Americans should behave in political, economic, and social interaction with other racial groups. This component may be subdivided based on the extent of the philosophy of interaction on the 27 continuum of nationalist to humanist where behavior is characterized by more or less assimilation and interaction with the dominant group. Certain ideologies are not judged to be better than others in the MMRI; rather ideology is recognized as a component in the racial identity that may help to explain behavior. Using the three dimensions, clusters of scores have been used to develop profiles of individuals. These clusters may be formed on the basis of scoring high or low on centrality and regard and the preferred ideology. The profiles give researchers the ability to use a more fine grained approach in understanding the specific components of racial identity that are interacting with the independent variables. This refined approach may help explain academic achievement by developing understanding of the roles of racial identity factors for high and low achievers when cognitive ability and other factors are held constant. ―What is not yet known is whether different identity profiles have differential relationships with self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation, with concomitantly different implications for achievement.‖ (Worrell, 2009, p. 145). Thus, research is needed that will help explain how different racial identity factors may interact with other important variables to impact academic achievement. Chavous, Bernat, Schmelk-Cone, Caldwell, Kohn-Wood, & Zimmerman (2003) found centrality, private, and public regard clustered to create four profiles; Buffering – 1) high centrality, high private regard, low public regard, 2) Low Connectedness – low centrality, high private regard, low public regard, 3) Idealized – high centrality, private regard, and public regard, 4) Alienated – low centrality, extremely low private regard and low public regard. These four clusters were found to have significant group differences in educational attainment. The individual components of racial identity may provide 28 insight to the differences in performance. Chavous et al. (2003) found that public and private regard were significantly correlated to school attachment and school relevance for 12th grade African American students. Private regard was also significantly correlated with school efficacy and school importance. School relevance and school efficacy were also significantly positively correlated with racial centrality. In a similar study, Harper and Tuckman (2006) used the MIBI to create cluster profiles for 9th and 12th grade students. The four profiles demonstrated significant differences in academic achievement with students possessing the alienated profile displaying higher grade-point averages for both 9th and 12 grades. The situativity of the racial identity may be reflected in this study by low centrality of race because the students may have a pro-achievement academic orientation. In the hierarchical ordering of identities, the students appeared to give more salience to academic achievement than to race (Harper & Tuckman, 2006). Worrell has examined ethnic identity and other group orientation as it relates to academic achievement for academically gifted African American students using the MEIM developed by Phinney (1989). His research on ethnic identity and its relationship to academic achievement is important; one finding of the investigations of stereotype threat was that individuals who have high confidence in the domain may be particularly vulnerable to situational cues that indicate a threat may be present. In his study of gifted students, Worrell (2007) found that only African American students’ scores on other group orientation predicted self-esteem. Ethnic identity was not a significant predictor, although it was negatively correlated with achievement for gifted African American students. Similar to the students in the Harper and Tuckman (2006) study, Worrell 29 proposed that perhaps the need for academic achievement was stronger in these high ability students than was the need to identify with their ethnic group. The situativity of the educational process was re-emphasized by Worrell (2009), ―Academic achievement is not merely an individual endeavor; rather, it occurs in a social context and is framed by one’s perceived position in the social structure of the society‖(p. 138). While many researchers purport that the best indicant of achievement in the immediate future is achievement in the immediate past (Lohman, 2005a), for students from minority groups, the social structure of previous educational conditions may have erected barriers that impeded the students’ ability to ―experience and exercise‖ their abilities (Lohman, 2005b, p. 119). Educators play an important role in helping to remove the structural barriers, such as stereotype threat and teacher expectations, and provide opportunities for students to develop academic aptitudes. When people are members of a group about which a negative stereotype is known to exist, and they encounter a situation where there is a risk of confirming the negative stereotype, a stereotype threat occurs (Steele & Aronson, 1995). The threat is perceived as a self-evaluation, a threat that may affect performance. Individuals may experience increased anxiety when they encounter difficulties in the task as a result of the known stereotype. When performance diminishes because of the stress on working memory due to increased anxiety, individuals may lower expectations about performance. Over time, these lowered expectations may translate into decreased motivation or diminished value of the activity. The cumulative effect of schooling and prolonged exposure to stereotype threat may manifest itself in increased feelings of anxiety and decreased motivation (Seaton, 30 Yip, & Sellers, 2009). In an effort to protect their self-concepts, individuals may develop disidentification (Steele, 1997). Disidentification removes the domain area from importance in self-concept. The individual no longer cares about the domain. Performance is no longer a reflection of the person’s capabilities. Disidentification can negatively impact a person’s identification with school and over time result in underachievement for those in the stereotyped group. Steele and others have indicated that stereotype threat exists in situations beyond standardized tests and may cause non-White students to feel less able to participate in an academic setting (Purdie-Vaughns, Steele, Davies, & Ditlmann, 2008; Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronoson, 1995). ―Different groups experience different forms and degrees of stereotype threat because the stereotypes about them differ in content, in scope, and in situations to which they apply‖(Steele, 1997, p. 618). The situational and individual differences that are experienced make treatments to treat stereotype threat difficult to design and test. Stereotype threat echoes Snow’s (1989) aptitude-treatment interaction conceptualization that identified individual aptitudes interact with situational demands and opportunities with different manifestations. The ability to recognize the constraints and affordances of a situation is a defining factor in the development of aptitudes (Snow, 1996). Stereotype threat may be seen as an aptitude for recognizing threatening aspects of situations that are salient to those of minority populations. Purdie-Vaughns, et al., (2008) showed how settings provided cues that signaled the degree of threat or safety an African American could expect to experience. Understanding the cues that a school setting may be sending to minority students may help to explain academic achievement. ―When cues convey the threat of 31 identity-contingent evaluations, trust in the setting can be undermined. When cues signal affirming contingencies or evaluations that are not identity-contingent, trust can be sustained‖ (Purdie-Vaughns, et al., 2008, p. 626). Students who have high confidence in the domain may experience even greater stereotype threat (Steele, 1997). Thus those students who have high cognitive ability and high motivation may be even more susceptible to stereotype threat. Their high cognitive ability may make them more aware of broadly held negative views of others. Their high motivation may make them more aware of barriers, such as the negative opinions of others, which may be preventing them from reaching their goals. Fordham and Ogbu (1986) purport that minority groups develop a cultural frame of reference that defines situational activities, behaviors, events and symbols as representing the dominant (White) culture. Academic achievement may be viewed as a White activity and thus would be seen as something minorities would not expect to attain. Individuals who attempt to participate in White culture (i.e. achieve in school) or ―act White‖ may receive negative feedback from their peers or community. Fordham and Ogbu (1986) offer this cultural framework as one possible explanation of seemingly able African American students performing below their ability levels. While ―acting White‖ has received much attention in the literature, there have been mixed results in empirical replication of the finding. Tyson and Darity (2005) argued that adolescents from all races display oppositional attitudes as they separate from parents in Western cultures. Peers become important and academic mediocrity is commonplace. These researchers believe that certain school cultures may increase negative interactions between high and low achievers or within and between racial and 32 socioeconomic groups. Witherspoon, Spreight, and Thomas (1997), reported that African American high school students who had immersion attitudes – involving oneself in Black culture and withdrawing from interacting with persons form the dominant culture, displayed a statistically significant negative correlation between this attitude and gradepoint average. Thus, the implication of the students’ perception of the school climate toward academic achievement and racially diverse students would benefit from further review. Measuring Racial Identity Sellers, et al., (1997) performed initial analysis of the Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity (MIBI) using 474 African American college students from two medium-sized universities. One university was predominantly Black (185 participants) and the other was predominantly White (289). Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted using the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin test to evaluate the appropriateness of a three factor solution. The results indicated that the MIBI measured three interrelated constructs. Factor analysis was performed using maximum likelihood extraction of three factors with a Promax rotation. Questions which loaded less than .30 on any factor were eliminated from the instrument. Factor analyses were then conducted for the items loading on each subscale to determine that the questions were measuring only one construct. The one-factor structure was supported for Centrality however two items were dropped due to factor loadings below .30. A two-factor structure was investigated for Regard, however, three of the public regard questions had to be dropped due to low factor loadings thus public regard was eliminated from the scale it Sellers et al noted that further development of this scale was necessary. All of the private regard questions loaded at 33 appropriate levels thus the Regard scale consisted of these seven items. The ideology scales were further analyzed using a four factor matrix and questions were eliminated that failed to load above .30. The result was the 51 item MIBI. Cronbach’s alpha for the scales were as follows: Centrality (.77), Regard – Private (.60), Assimilation (.73), Humanist (.70), Minority (.76) and Nationalist (.79). Correlations between the scales were examined to test the hypothesized relationships. An example of a hypothesized relationship was that ―individuals for whom race was central were also likely to have positive private regard for African Americans‖ (Sellers, et al, 1997, p. 810). This relationship had a correlation of r=.37 which was significant. Other expected relationships involved the different ideologies and their relationships to racial centrality and private regard. All of the predicted relationships were supported by correlations, most of which were significant. Criterion validity was assessed by measuring relationships predicted by the MIBI and having an African American best friend or taking Black studies courses. As expected, those students who had high racial centrality and high private regard reported having frequent contact with other African Americans. Walsh, (2001) Walsh (2001) conducted a validation study of the 51 item MIBI with 95 Black Britons living in London. These participants ranged in age from 18-60 with the majority of participants aged 18-30. The correlations between the subscales were very similar to those found by Sellers, et al., 1997. Centrality and Private Regard had a correlation of r =.40 in this sample whereas Sellers et al. (1997) all found r = .37. All other correlations were similarly close in value. For both groups, Private Regard had the highest mean 34 rating which could be interpreted as both groups had strong positive feelings about being a member of the Black community. A difference between the American and Briton groups was found on the ratings of Centrality. The American group rated this subscale as the second highest mean score whereas the Britons rated it as second lowest. In an attempt to replicate the predictive validity study by Sellers et al.,1997, Walsh used contact with Black people (amount of time spent with Black and White people) as the dependent variable. Contact with Black people followed the American pattern of positive, significant correlations with Centrality, Private Regard, and Nationalist Ideology. Contact with White people was somewhat different between the groups in that the Britons’ correlations were smaller and not significant although the directionality remained the same. Based on this study, Walsh concluded that the MIBI was a valid measure of racial identity for Black Britons (2001). Cokley and Helm, (2001) Cokley and Helm (2001) assessed the validity of the MIBI with additional items for the Public Regard scale using confirmatory factor analysis and item analysis. Cokley and Helm also used the African Self-Consciousness Scale to determine the concurrent validity of the MIBI. A total of 279 African American college students from three predominantly White universities and four historically Black universities comprised the sample group. The age range of this group was 18-46. Cronbach’s alpha for the subscales in this study were as follows: Centrality (.73), Private Regard (.76), Public Regard (.74), Assimilationist (.75), Humanist (.72), Oppressed Minority (.83) and Nationalist (.78). These alpha coefficients were similar to those found by Sellers et al. (1997). Confirmatory factor analysis was performed in which seven factors were extracted from 35 the 56 variables. The model allowed items to correlate based on previous studies. Eleven items stood out as misfitting and the model statistics were less than predicted Chi-square (1465, N=279) = 2921.200, p<.001; NFI = .56; CFI = .71; SMR = .09). Concurrent validity of the MIBI was supported by comparing scores on subscales such as Centrality and Private Regard to the African Self-Consciousness Scale. The hypothesized relationships were supported. For example, individuals for whom race centrality and private regard was high also reported high scores in African selfconsciousness. Similar relationships were found in the ideology subscales and correlations with the African Self-Consciousness Scale. Confirmatory factor analysis was performed in a similar manner as Sellers, et al. including the Public Regard subscale items. In this analysis, all but one of the items for the Public Regard subscale had factor loadings >=.30. The item that did not load above .30 was ―Blacks are not respected by the broader society.‖ Thus Cokley and Helms (2001) provide additional support for including the Public Regard subscale in the instrument. The item analysis focused on the Ideology subscales and noted several problems with the different philosophies not being clearly defined. The authors did not mention any areas of inconsistency or difficulties for the subscales of Centrality, Public or Private Regard. Intrinsic Motivation Several researchers have developed theories to explain motivation, especially in academic settings. Expectancy-value theory (Berndt & Miller, 1990) states that the expectation of a positive outcome and the value that is placed on the outcome influences the amount of effort that is exerted towards the goal. Meece and Holt (1993) and other 36 researchers suggest that individuals who adopt performance goals or mastery goals may display different motivational effects such as persistence and risk-taking (Blackwell, et al., 2007; Spray, et al., 2006; Cury, et al., 2006). Beliefs about the nature of academic ability also impact the effort put forth and types of goals that the individual pursues (Dweck, 1999). The social cognitive theory of motivation argues that an individual’s selfefficacy plays a large role in the energy an individual is willing to invest in pursuit of attaining a goal (Bandura, 1982). Self Determination Theory regards components of intrinsic motivation as human needs and the degree to which these needs are met will determine the level of motivation that an individual displays (Ryan & Deci, 2004). Beliefs that academic ability is either fixed or malleable significantly affect motivation, persistence and ultimately academic achievement (Frunham, ChamorroPermuzic & McDougall, 2002; Dweck, 1999). Individuals who hold entity belief view intelligence as fixed, a natural endowment that cannot be changed. Those who hold incremental beliefs view intelligence as malleable and able to be improved with practice and experience. Studies have shown that students’ beliefs regarding abilities directly impact performance (Blackwell et al., 2007). Students who hold incremental beliefs are more likely to engage in practice such as homework that will improve skills and performance. Students who hold entity beliefs are more likely to have more performance goals and avoid tasks in which they may fail (Blackwell et al., 2007; Meece & Holt, 1993). Incremental and entity beliefs may affect students’ ability to set goals and to persist in efforts to achieve those goals even when the environment is not supportive. An important component of intrinsic motivation is perceived competence, or selfconcept. Self-concept is regarded as a multidimensional construct with an overarching 37 general component that is useful for predicting general behaviors and specific components such as academic, physical and social that is useful for examining and predicting specific behaviors such as persistence in homework and training, and the ability to interact with others. Academic self-concept has been found to have a significant impact on achievement (Marsh, Trautwein, Ludtke, Koller, 2008; Davis-Kean, Husemann, Jager, Collins, Bates, Lansford, 2008; Cokley & Chapman, 2008; Awad, 2007). However, due to limitations on sample size and the length of the survey instrument, this area will not be investigated in this study. Self Determination Theory, a model of motivation developed by Ryan and Deci (2004) argues that intrinsic motivation is comprised of three components; perceived competence, perceived autonomy and perceived connectedness. Intrinsic motivation is a powerful factor that influences persistence and achievement. Ryan and Deci (2004) describe intrinsic motivation as self-directed action to achieve a goal for the pleasure that is derived from participating in the task. Individuals who are intrinsically motivated for academic learning seek out new opportunities to explore unfamiliar subjects for the inherent enjoyment in learning. High ability students may experience decreased intrinsic motivation because they do not feel intellectually connected with their peer group or feel constrained by the pace or level of material being taught in the classroom. Research on the relationship between academic achievement and the intrinsic motivation of African American students has shown mixed results. Connell, Spencer, and Aber (1994) used three samples of African American students (n = 114 - 399) to investigate relationships between components of intrinsic motivation for racial minority groups and components of intrinsic motivation. Although the significance levels and 38 incremental R2 varied by sample, perceived competence and perceived relatedness correlated significantly with behavioral and emotional engagement. These engagement measures significantly predicted attendance, test scores, and grades. Perceptions of parental support, competence, and relatedness were found to have stronger relationships with students’ actions than neighborhood socioeconomic status or gender. Corpus, McClintic-Gilbert, Hayenga (2009) found a positive, symmetric relationship over time between intrinsic motivation and academic achievement for third through eighth graders. Fall intrinsic motivation significantly and positively predicted spring GPA (β = .06) and that fall GPA significantly and positively predicted spring intrinsic motivation (β = .06). The authors suggested one interpretation of these findings. They argued that positive internal drive of intrinsic motivation for academic learning may assist students in high persistence and performance on academic tasks. Similarly, the positive effects of high GPA or academic honors may increase perceived competence which would increase intrinsic motivation. This synergistic relationship may promote a positive cycle of achievement and increased self-concept for learners who experience positive educational environments. Conflicting results were found by Gagné and St. Père (2002). Their study of high school females found that once IQ was controlled, intrinsic motivation did not significantly improve the prediction of achievement. In this study IQ and intrinsic motivation were not correlated (r = .03). Spinath, Spinath, Harlarr, & Plomin (2006) found that although intrinsic values, how much children indicated they enjoyed the activities of the subject, were positively correlated with math and English (.20 and .26), they did not significantly add to predicting academic achievement after controlling for 39 IQ. The correlation of g and intrinsic values in their study of 9 year olds was .08-.13. Intrinsic values were correlated with math and English at .20-.26 respectively. However, intrinsic values (interest) did not add significantly to the prediction of achievement. Cury, Elliot, DaFonseca and Mooler (2006) used a social cognitive model to explore the relationships between implicit theories of ability and achievement goals, and the role of perceived competence as a moderator of these relationships in two studies. In the first study, entity theory was a negative predictor of math performance (β = -.14) for 465 French students with an age range of 12-14 years. Incremental theory of ability was a positive predictor (β = .19) of mastery goals. Perceived competence significantly predicted mastery-approach and performance approach goals. However, perceived competence did not moderate the relationships between the students’ personal theories regarding abilities and achievement goals. The second study examined the relationship between intrinsic motivation and achievement goals using regression analysis. In this study, mastery-goals were significant positive predictors. Furthermore, the relationship between beliefs regarding ability and intrinsic motivation were mediated by achievement goals, namely mastery-approach and performance-approach goals, which reduced the strength of the relationship. The Cury et al. (2006) study supports Dweck’s theory (1999) regarding beliefs and performance. However, this study also underscores the complexity of the relationship between motivation and performance. The mediating role of mastery-approach goals on the relationship between ability beliefs and intrinsic motivation may illustrate the need to understand the roles that goals may play in explaining performance. In this study, perceived competence was a significant predictor of achievement goals. However, 40 perceived competence did not moderate test performance or intrinsic motivation. This finding does not support the assumption in Self-Determination Theory that perceived competence is a key element in intrinsic motivation. Other research has explored differences in motivation among minority groups. Areepattamannil and Freeman (2008), explored motivation differences in 573 immigrant (n=266) and non-immigrant (n=307) grade 11 and 12 Canadian students. This research found significant group differences on measures of extrinsic motivation-external regulation, extrinsic motivation-introjected regulation, intrinsic motivation to know, intrinsic motivation to accomplish, and intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation. The strongest variables for differentiating between the groups were the intrinsic motivation measures with immigrants scoring higher than non-immigrants. Despite significant group differences, none of the motivation variables significantly predicted English or Math grade point average. This study is important when trying to understand motivation because it highlights differences in motivation that may exist between members of the dominant culture (non-immigrants) and members of a minority culture (immigrants). Unrau and Schlackman (2006) found a small but statistically significant relationship between ethnicity and intrinsic motivation for 1,032 students (n=768 Hispanic, n=264 Asian). In this study, ethnicity also significantly predicted reading achievement. The interaction of the ethnicity and intrinsic motivation on reading achievement was also statistically significant. In this study, intrinsic motivation for Hispanic students had a lower standardized path coefficient (.19) than for Asian students (.30). These findings highlight how intrinsic motivation may function differently for 41 different groups. The levels of intrinsic motivation were significantly different (d = -.15) by group as were the strengths of the relationship between intrinsic motivation and reading achievement. Some researchers have found an unusual relationship between some components of motivation and academic achievement for African American students. For the past decade, researchers have been investigating the attitude-engagement paradox (Mickelson, 1990). Specifically, African American students reporting higher levels of academic engagement and more positive attitudes towards education than European American students still perform at a lower level in school. In his review of literature on the motivation of African American students, Cokley (2003) found results that support this finding. Long, Monoi, Harper, Knoblauch, and Murphy (2007) found that for eighth and ninth grade African American students, academic self-efficacy was high while achievement was low. This study found that some students chose to underachieve due to their perceptions of a negative school climate and lack of school support. However, the same students were able to maintain a strong belief in their own capabilities. African American students who adopt oppositional identities to combat the negative impact of actual and perceived discrimination within their school setting may feel confident in their ability to successfully execute a given academic task or be interested in an academic domain and still simultaneously express a deliberate disdain for academic behaviors associated with successful outcomes. (Long et al., 2007, p. 214). Shernoff and Schmidt (2008), used data from the Sloan Study of Youth and Social Development to investigate group differences in academic engagement and attitudes among U.S. high school students (N=586). This study reported African American students’ grade point averages as .18 grade lower than European American students (ES 42 = .72). Engagement of African American students was higher than that of European American students (effect size = .51). African American students in this study also reported higher levels of intrinsic motivation than European American students (effect size = .43). An interesting finding of this study is the negative interaction of the term black * engagement for predicting grades. Thus in this case, engagement may be operating differently with grades based on the race of the participants. Relationships that appear to make intuitive sense, such as higher engagement being strongly correlated with higher achievement, may not be universal and may even go in the opposite direction for some students. This requires researchers to explore the role of specific components of motivation for African American students. Measuring Academic Motivation The Academic Motivation Scale was created to measure academic motivation in high school students. The measure was originally created in French. As a means of establishing reliability and validity of the English instrument, 745 university students from Ontario completed the survey (Vallerand, Pelletier, Blais, Briere, Senecal, & Vallieres, 1992). Racial composition of the participants is not given in the study. Cronbach’s alpha from this study were as follows: Amotivation (.85), External Regulation (.83), Introjected Regulation (.84), Identified Regulation (.62), Intrinsic Motivation to Know (.84), Intrinsic Motivation to Accomplish (.85), Intrinsic Motivation to Experience Stimulation (.86). Confirmatory factor analysis was utilized to support the structure of the model. A seven factor model that allowed correlations between pairs of 26 measured variable residuals showed that the model fits the data as expected, NFI = .93,GFI = .94, X2 = 748.64, df = 303, p<= .001. Additional analysis showed that the 43 addition of the residuals to the model did not bias the interpretation. This factor analysis was similar to the original French-Canadian version. The three intrinsic motivation subscales of Intrinsic Motivation to Know, Intrinsic Motivation to Accomplish and Intrinsic Motivation to Experience Stimulation correlated r = .58 - .62 with each other. The extrinsic subscales of External Regulation, Introjected Regulation, and Identified Regulation correlated r = .29 - .48 with each other. When ANOVA analysis was performed on the basis of participant’s sex, a significant sex * instrument interaction was noted F(6,738) = 3.87. Females scored higher on all three intrinsic motivation subscales than male participants (Vallerand, et al., 1992). Cokley, (2000) Cokley (2000), sought to examine the validity of the AMS by replicating the study within a diverse population of American students. The participants were 263 undergraduates (39 African Americans, 181 Euro-Americans, 6 European Internationals, 2 Asian Americans, 8 Asian Internationals, 6 Hispanic Americans, 16 individuals who described their ethnicity as ―other‖, and five participants who did not disclose their ethnicity). This study showed significant, positive correlations within the two main areas of the scale – intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. The intrinsic motivation subscales had correlations of r =..58 - .68. The three extrinsic subscales had correlations ranging from r = .45-.50. These correlations are similar to those obtained by Vallerand et al. (1992). Cokley (2000) hypothesized that the continuum of self-determination should be reflected in scores that ranged from low to high moving from Amotivation to Extrinsic Motivation Subscales to Intrinsic Motivation Subscales. In this study, there was a stronger relationship between the Intrinsic Motivation to Accomplish subscale and Identified 44 Regulation and Introjected Regulation than the three extrinsic motivation subscales had with one another. While this result could indicate the possibility of overlap within the subscales, the study did find a clear distinction between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. Cokley, Bernard, Cunningham and Motoike, (2001) In an effort to provide further psychometric evidence for the validity of using the AMS with U.S. students, Cokley, Bernard, Cunningham, and Motoike (2001), conducted factor analysis and concurrent validation studies using data from the 2000 study. Cronbach’s alphas reported in the 2001 study were as follows: Amotivation (.86), Extrinsic Motivation External Regulation (.81), Extrinsic Motivation Introjected Regulation (.86), Extrinsic Motivation Identified Regulation (.70), Intrinsic Motivation to Know (.83), Intrinsic Motivation to Experience Stimulation (.81), and Intrinsic Motivation to Accomplish (.85). Thus the internal consistency of the survey was very similar to the results obtained by administering the AMS to the Canadian participants. Confirmatory factor analysis was utilized to assess the validity of the internal structure of the AMS. The seven factor model revealed the best fit with the data (CFI = .90, NFI =.83) however the chi-square value was statistically significant. These findings were also similar to those found by Vallerand et al. (1993). Cokley et al. (2001) used the AMS subscale correlations with academic self-concept to establish construct validity. Significant, positive correlations were found between the three intrinsic motivation subscales and academic self-concept (r = .39, .32, and .25). There were no correlations between the extrinsic motivation subscales and academic self-concept. The amotivation subscale correlated with academic self-concept r = -.47 which was significant. None of the AMS subscales correlated significantly with self-reported GPA, which is contrary to 45 the results of Vallerand et al. (1993). Construct validity was further tested by investigating group differences using t-tests with a Bonferroni correction. The only group difference on seven subscale scores that was noted was that of African Americans scoring significantly higher on Extrinsic Motivation External Regulation than other participants. There were no other gender or ethnic differences found. School Support Self-concept, racial identity, intrinsic motivation, and aptitude theory all place significant importance on the frames of reference, or situations in which an individuals are interacting. The situation, as perceived by the individuals, influences the behavioral response. It is important to recognize the affordances and constraints of any given situation as perceived by the individual. ―Context is critical for both the development of ability and the expression of it.‖ (Lohman, 2005b, p. 118). Due to the lack of individual control, academic settings play an influential role in perceived social context for the individual. Those students who perceive the social context of school as supportive and nurturing will have a different educational experience than those who perceive it as limiting or antagonistic. The National Research Council (2004) found that ―students’ motivation develop partly as a consequence of the educational environments they experience.‖ (p. 33). Thus the social context or school climate that students experience may influence academic achievement. Numerous studies have been conducted on the role of perceived social context and academic achievement (McMahon,Wernsman, & Rose, 2009; Stewart, 2008; Suldo, Shaffer, & Shaunessy, 2008; Sanders, 1997). Perceived teacher support, perceived school acceptance and nurturance, and perceived autonomy have been found to play significant 46 roles in motivation and achievement (Cokley & Chapman, 2008; Ames, 1992). Marshall and Weinstein (1986) found that students as young as first grade were able to identify teachers who treated students differently based on perceived academic ability. Differences noted by teacher behaviors, include, positive relationships, positive display, praise, buffered criticism, positive academic evaluation, and positive behavioral evaluation. Teachers who emphasized effort and did not treat groups of students differently were rated more positively by students for teacher-students interactions. Conversely, on average, teachers who emphasized ability and treated students differentially were rated low for teacher-student interaction. An interesting aspect of this study involved the variance in scores by grade-level for similar teacher behaviors. This difference suggests that the age of the child may indicate the level of awareness the he or she possesses to recognize differences in the environment. School Climate School climate may be considered as the ―quality and consistency of interpersonal interactions within the school community that influence children’s cognitive, social, and psychological development.‖ (Haynes, Emmons, & Ben-Avie, 1997, p. 322). Classroom structures may contribute to the overall school climate. Structures typically include task evaluation and instructional demands (Ames, 1992). The classroom structures that have been found to increase motivation and promote mastery learning are ―the design of tasks and learning activities, evaluation practices and use of rewards, and distribution of authority or responsibility.‖ (Ames, 1992, p. 263). Designing tasks, learning activities and evaluation practices to allow for diversity in process and product has been found to 47 reduce peer comparison among students and differential treatment by teachers (Ames, 1992). By examining school climate, researchers move beyond cognitive, motivational, and familial characteristics of students to explore academic achievement. This additional understanding of the roles of social interactions between teachers and students in the school context may provide opportunities for interventions with school personnel to improve academic achievement. When considering a student’s perception of the classroom, research has shown that there is variation both within classrooms and within students (Marshall & Weinstien, 1986). Marshall and Weinstein documented significant within-class differences in student perception of autonomy and the level of work in the classroom (1986). Students’ perceptions of themselves and of their classroom are different because of both past experiences and the way they interpret interactions between themselves and the teacher. Ames, points out quite clearly, ―Thus to predict and examine motivated cognition, affect, and behavior of a student, it is necessary to attend to how that student perceives and gives meaning to classroom experiences.‖ (1992, p.267). Student perceptions of the level of support and teacher expectations may explain reasons for engagement and offer opportunities for interventions at an individual perception level once differences have been noted. The importance of school climate for racially diverse students has been found to be significant (McMahon, Wernsman, & Rose, 2009; Stewart, 2008; McKown & Weinstein, 2008; McKown & Weinstein, 2002). McMahon, et al., (2009) found significant positive relationships between perceptions of school belonging, satisfaction, 48 and cohesiveness on language arts self-efficacy for racially diverse fourth and fifth graders (N=149). In this study, school belonging had the strongest influence (β = .19) when all measures of school climate (satisfaction, cohesion, difficulty, friction, and competitiveness) were included in a model to predict language arts self-efficacy. On an individual model level, the significant positive relationship between language arts selfefficacy and school belonging (.22) show an interaction between factors of motivation and students’ perception of school as a safe, welcoming place. Stewart (2008) used the National Educational Longitudinal Study (NELS) 1990 data to investigate school attachment and cohesion for African American tenth graders (N = 1,238). School attachment was measured as the extent to which students felt positive about interactions with teachers and other students. School cohesion measured students’ feelings about whether teachers and other students care about each other and the extent to which students from different ethnicities interacted with one another. School cohesion was significantly, positively correlated with grade point averages (r = .14). School attachment was a significant predictor of GPA (t = 5.50) in a model that included attachment, school commitment, school involvement, positive peers, parent-child discussion, parental school involvement, gender, family structure, and family SES. In a model of school structural variables, school cohesion was the only statistically significant predictor (b = .014, t = 3.50) after controlling for individual level variables. These studies show that school climate may be perceived differently for racially diverse students and may have a stronger relationship to academic achievement. 49 Teacher Support The situative nature of aptitude theory to explain performance requires a close examination of the students’ perception of the learning environment. The high school experience is quite different from college or life in the adult world. It is important to conduct research in this area to help educators and parents understand the roles of teachers and the school environment in influencing academic achievement. Studies have shown that students’ perceptions of teacher expectations and attitudes have a significant impact on student achievement (McKown & Weinstein, 2008; Suldo, et al., 2008; McKown & Weinstein, 2003; McKown & Weinstein, 2002; Sanders, 1997). Teachers who hold high expectations and challenge their students while providing support and encouragement motivate students to higher achievement. Teacher referrals are often the first step in the process of identifying students for gifted or remedial learner programs. As such, the teacher’s attitudes and understanding of African American learners may play a pivotal role in selecting these students for special programs. Way, Reddy, and Rhodes (2007) examined sixth grade students’ (N=1,451) perceptions of school climate and teacher support to explore relationships between students’ perception of school climate and psychosocial outcomes of behavior problems, self-esteem, and depressive symptoms. This study found significant relationships (medium effect size) between the intercepts and slopes of perceived teacher support and measures of well being. Large effect sizes were reported for the slopes of perceived teacher support and the slopes of problem behavior and depressive symptoms. There were no significant differences found for gender or ethnicity. However, the sample was 91% European American. Students who reported more behavior problems in school also 50 had lower teacher support intercepts initially and the trajectory showed a negative slope which indicates that as students with behavior problems progressed through the school year, they perceived less teacher support. Konstantopoulos (2009), evaluated the effects of teachers on early academic achievement (K-3). Each grade had over 6,000 students and over 300 teachers participate. The student groups were 33% minority. The main effect of belonging to a minority group had a statistically significant negative relationship with mathematics achievement (-.229 -.413). Prior year effective teachers were positively significant for first and second grade math achievement. Although the teacher effect * minority interaction was not significant, there was a significant negative teacher * minority interaction for reading achievement in first grade (-.169). There were significant main effects for minority status on reading achievement in second and third grade (-.189, -.223). Teachers provide scaffolding to students to help them learn and accomplish those tasks that are beyond the students’ capabilities while working alone. Therefore, the role of the teacher is critical for influencing students’ development based on the amount of scaffolding provided. Teachers who hold a cultural deficit model for African American students may not believe that these students are capable of high academic achievement. ―When children come to understand that others endorse stereotypic beliefs, they gain an insight into others’ social motives that profoundly affects their relationship to other individuals, social settings, and society.‖ (McKown & Weinstein, 2003, p. 498). McKown and Weinstein (2002), sought to understand the moderating role of ethnicity in teacher expectancy effects. The study had 30 teachers and 561 students grades 3-5 (n= 228 Black, n = 231 White) participate. In the fall teachers were asked to 51 rank order students in terms of expected year-end achievement in reading and math based on the previous year’s achievement results. This ranking was compared to students’ actual current year-end performance using hierarchical linear modeling. McKown and Weinstein found that teachers consistently underestimated the performance of African American students compared to European American students (2002). The researchers also found that in third and fifth grade, African American students were statistically significantly more likely to perform at levels that matched teacher underestimates of performance. In this study, the African American students demonstrated that they were adversely affected by teacher underestimates of their ability whereas European American students benefited from the teacher overestimates of their ability. This study highlights the need to recognize that African American students may be more sensitive to perceptions of low teacher expectations. It is important to examine the relationships among perceived school support, awareness of negative stereotypes, motivation, and academic achievement to better understand the roles of teachers and the school for African American students. In a follow-up to this study, McKown and Weinstein sought to examine the development of stereotype consciousness, or negative public regard, in 202 students of heterogeneous ethnicity and ages 6 - 10 (2003). The researchers found that children from ethnic minority groups perceived broadly held stereotypes at a younger age than Caucasian students. Eighty percent of the ethnically diverse students at age 10 were aware of negative stereotypes compared to 63% of Caucasian students. This finding aids the understanding of identity development by illustrating that at a young age, children are aware that they are different from those of the majority culture and membership in a 52 minority ethnic group may render them susceptible to others’ preconceived notions. This study is also important for schools because it shows that children as young as six may be aware of racial stereotypes held by others. Interactions with teachers and administrators can serve to confirm the perceived negative public regard or to provide counter examples to previous experiences and reinforce that while ethnic differences are recognized and respected, the student is seen as an individual and teachers hold high expectations of all students. Measuring School Support The Inventory of School Climate – Student Version (ISC-S) is an instrument used to measure student perceptions of the school context (Brand, Felner, Shim, Seitsinger,& Dumas, 2003). The initial instrument was created from 125 items that had been identified in the literature as important to student adjustment (Brand, et al., 2003). These items were reviewed by 1,000 students to determine the ease of readability and the question comprehension. Following the pilot study, exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis was conducted over two years. The samples for this analysis consisted of 16,600 students in 42 middle schools. The ethnicity of the participants was not included in the study description. The samples were randomly split into two groups; one was used for the exploratory factor analysis, and one was used for confirmatory factor analysis. Items were eliminated from the instrument if: they did not have similar factor loadings in both subsamples in both years, they had factor loadings less than .325, or if they loaded on more than one factor greater than .35 (Brand, et al., 2003). The results of this study led to a revised survey with 61 items. Following the initial analysis, a 10 dimensional model comprised of: Disciplinary Harshness, Negative Peer Interactions, Positive Peer 53 Interactions, Structure and Clarity of Rules and Expectations, Student Commitment to Achievement, Teacher Support, Instructional Innovation, Student Participation in Decision Making, Support for Cultural Pluralism, and Safety Problems was used to assess the model fit to the data. Results from this analysis were a further reduction of 11 items due to low factor loadings on the target factor and further refinement of the instrument. The second study conducted by Brand, et al., (2003) was aimed at confirming the structure of the scales and establishing reliability in a larger and more diverse sample of schools. This study took place over three years and involved 161,000 students from 300 schools in 16 states. The participants in this study were 44% from minority racial-ethnic groups, and 46% participated in free/reduced lunch. This large, diverse sample afforded the researchers the opportunity to test the appropriateness of the instrument for diverse students. Cronbach’s alpha for the subscales were as follows: Teacher Support (.76), Clarity of Rules and Expectations (.74), Student Commitment to Academic Achievement (.81), Negative Peer Interactions (.73), Positive Peer Interactions (.70), Disciplinary Harshness (.67), Participation in Decision Making (.70), Innovation (.63), Support for Cultural Pluralism (.68), and Safety Problems (.71). Correlations were computed between the subscale scores across year 1 and year 2 to assess the stability of the climate ratings in each building. The correlations of year one had a median r = .76 and year two had a median r = .52 which indicates that the scores within each school were reasonably stable across different cohorts of students. The samples were divided by gender, race, gradelevel and participating in free/reduced lunch to determine the reliability of the instrument for different groups (Brand, et al., 2003). Cronbach’s alpha for each subscale for each group were above .70. Factor loadings were examined by group and no substantial 54 differences were found for ethnicity or lunch status. Some small differences were found for gender however the factor loadings differed by .10 or less. The researchers also evaluated the subgroups at the building level using hierarchical linear modeling and did not find significant differences among groups for ratings on the subscales. The third study sought to understand how differences between schools on the ISC-S were related to academic achievement and other student adjustment indicators (Brand, et al., 2003). This study was conducted in conjunction with the previous study and asked participants to provide additional information on ―academic efficacy, delinquency, drug use attitudes – behavior, self-esteem, anxiety, and depression‖ (Brand, et al., 2003, p. 577). The additional information was used to understand the predictive validity of the ISC-S using school-level climate subscales. Hierarchical linear modeling was used to test the subscales’ relationships to academic achievement and adjustment, behavior problems and substance use, socioemotional adjustment, and minority and majority students’ perceptions of school climate and adjustment. The strongest predictor of academic achievement was Student Commitment to Academic Achievement. Other subscales that showed consistent relationships with academic adjustment were Teacher Support, Structure, Positive Peer Interactions, and Instructional Innovation (Brand, et al., 2003). Support for Cultural Pluralism showed a consistent, positive relationship with reported self-expectations and academic aspirations, which suggests that perceived support for diversity may be an important aspect in motivation for academic achievement. As one of the first scales to measure the school environment’s support for diverse students, the ISC-S showed that minority students in schools that were rated by 55 students as having high levels of Support for Cultural Pluralism had higher academic expectations and lower levels of delinquency and substance use. Summary Most of the literature on the psychosocial aspects of academic achievement investigates each component individually. Researchers focus on their areas of interest – motivation, cognitive ability, and other characteristics. Most of these studies did not include participant racial information or were mainly conducted with White, European American participants. The lack of research on the relationship among cognitive ability, and motivation characteristics for African American students makes generalizations to this group difficult. African Americans have a qualitatively different experience in school and the community than their White counterparts. The phenotypic differences of this group are present from birth and may influence their self-concept and perceptions of those around them. Racial identity seeks to understand the salience of race for individuals and the meaning African Americans attach to the role of being Black in a predominantly White society. Racial identity has typically been examined in conjunction with academic achievement and thus other components of the psychosocial development are ignored. This study will extend prior theories of aptitude development by investigating the influence of racial identity factors as important aptitudes for academic learning. Cronbach’s initial conceptualization of aptitude-interaction changed the way that treatment affects were interpreted (1957). Snow (1989), Ackerman (1996), and Lubinski (2000) recognized that different combinations of affective, conative and cognitive traits interacted and enabled individuals to be more or less successful in different situations. 56 This research added a new factor to the concept, namely racial identity. If the other three components are thought to interact with one another within a situation, adding another factor which also develops as an individual interacts within a situation is reasonable to extend the conceptualization. Accordingly, this research considered the following questions to help understand the role of racial identity: 1. What are the relationships between the racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard) and perceived support? 2. What are the relationships between the racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard) and intrinsic motivation? 3. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between perceived support and motivation? 4. Do racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard) moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and cumulative grade point average after controlling for 6th grade standardized test scores? 5. Do the racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard) moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and the number of Advanced placement courses taken after controlling for 6th grade standardized test scores? 6. Do racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and public regard) moderate the relationship between perceived school support and cumulative grade point average after controlling for 6th grade standardized test scores? 7. Do the racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard) moderate the relationship between perceived school support and the number of 57 advanced placement courses taken after controlling for 6th grade standardized test scores? 58 CHAPTER 3 METHODS Participants and Sampling The purpose of this study was to understand the relationship among the factors of racial identity, prior academic achievement, academic motivation, and perceived school support in the prediction of academic success for African American students. Academic success will be measured by 11th and 12th grade cumulative grade point average (GPA) and the number of advanced placement courses taken. Predictors included measures of racial identity, academic intrinsic motivation, perceived school support, and achievement in elementary school. Participants who identified themselves as African American and were currently enrolled in 11th and 12th grade were included in this study. Due to the large number of required participants, this study was conducted in a geographical area that has large student bodies. Two high schools in a large, Midwestern metropolitan area agreed to participate in the study. This district has a history of students taking a high number of honors courses. The school demographics are summarized in Table 1. 59 Table 3.1. Key Demographics of Schools Participating in the Study Measure School A School B Total Population 2,124 1,528 African American 32% 28% Caucasian 34% 29% Asian American 28% 33% Hispanic 4% 9% Native American 1% 1% Eligible for Free or Reduced Lunch 58% 72% African American Graduation Rate 56% 76% Number of honors courses passed by African American students 432 302 Students who took the ACT 64% 52% Determination of the Sample A letter describing the study and a parental consent form were mailed to a 233 11th grade students from the school district office. Students were given two weeks to respond by mailing the signed consent forms back to the researcher in a pre-addressed, stamped envelope. As a follow up measure, the school counselors at each school redistributed the forms to 11th grade students and passed out forms to 12th grade students and asked students to return the signed consent forms to them. A total of 84 students returned the consent forms and were administered the survey. The district provided the researcher with the 6th grade reading and math test scores on the Standford Achievement 60 10 test, current GPA, and number of honors and number of advanced placement courses taken by the student for each student who returned a signed consent form. Students who returned consent forms were called into a conference room in groups of 10 to 15 where the researcher briefly explained the survey. Students were told that they could skip any questions that they did not feel comfortable answering and that they could stop participating at any time. Students were encouraged to ask questions about the survey or for clarification on any items. At the end of the survey administration, the researcher drew one name from the participants at each school and that individual received a $45 Visa gift card. The drawing was held to thank students for their time and effort in completing the survey. A one-hour survey session was needed for the principal investigator to administer the questionnaire to the students. The instruments were numbered and a record was kept with individual names and the survey numbers in order to match individual student test scores and grade point averages with survey results. However, there was no other identifying information on the instrument. After the data were entered into a spreadsheet for analyses, the record connecting student names and survey numbers was destroyed. IRB and Plan to Maintain Confidentiality This study was approved by The University of Iowa Institutional Review Board. There were minimal risks to the participants. The short survey asked their perceptions of their own motivation, affective characteristics, racial identity, and perceptions of school climate. The only foreseeable possible risk was feeling uncomfortable when asked questions about how they feel about themselves or their school experience. The participants may have benefited by examining how they felt regarding their performance 61 in school and the level of support they felt that they are receiving in their classroom. All required steps such as providing confidentiality, explaining that they may end their participation at any time without penalty, and fully explaining the purpose of the research were taken to insure appropriate minimization of risks to participants. Data was stored on a password protected computer that was not a part of a network. The computer was maintained in a home office that was only accessible by the principal investigator. Hard copy files were maintained in a locked file cabinet and the record that links participant names to instrument identification numbers was destroyed when the data was entered into spreadsheets. Instruments In order to answer the research questions, it is important to have quality measures of each of the factors. Accordingly, this study required the use of three standardized instruments to assess the key components of interest: academic intrinsic motivation (15 questions), racial identity (17 questions), and perceived school support (12 questions). The survey instrument also asked five demographic questions. Academic Intrinsic Motivation The Academic Motivation Scale (AMS) –English version was developed based on the self-determination theory of motivation (Vallerand, et al., 1992). The questionnaire asks students to respond to the question ―Why do you go to school?‖ using a seven-point Likert scale that indicates agreement with various responses. The original instrument was created in French and consisted of 28 items and seven subscales. The subscales were: intrinsic motivation to know, intrinsic motivation toward accomplishments, intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation, external regulation, introjected regulation, 62 identification, and amotivation. Each subscale consisted of four items. The scale was translated into English following the preferred approach of parallel-back translation where the original instrument is translated into English by a bilingual person. A second bilingual person then translated the English version back into French. Agreement between the original document and the document translated back into French is used as the measure of appropriateness of the translated instrument. Following the translation, the items were assessed by a committee to determine the appropriateness of the translation. Once agreement had been reached on the 28 items, a pilot-test was performed with 10 college students to assess the meaning and clarity of the items. This study used the three subscales that measure intrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation is a key component for understanding why people with similar cognitive ability may show differences in academic performance. Thus, questions regarding the overlap of subscales of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation of the AMS will not be pertinent in this investigation. Racial Identity Racial identity was measured using the Multi-Dimensional Inventory of Black Identity (MIBI) (Sellers, et al., 1998). As developmental theories of ethnic identity continue to be refined and revised, researchers have begun to move away from the stage theory of ethnic identity development that was originally proposed by Cross and Phinney. The MIBI is a measure of racial identity that is not focused on stages but accepts that development takes place on a continuum. The MIBI assesses Black identity along three dimensions: centrality, regard, and ideology. The MIBI was originally constructed using items from existing scales on African American racial and ethnic identity, as well as 63 social identity if they met the criteria for the conceptual framework of centrality, regard, and ideology. Students are asked to respond using a seven-point Likert scale to the extent that they agree with the statements given in the measure. The Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity (MIBI) was developed by Sellers, Rowley, Chavous, Shelton and Smith in 1997. The original instrument consisted of 71 questions based on the Multidimensional Model of Racial Identity. (Sellers, et al., 1997). The model proposes that racial identity can be measured among four main areas: Salience, Centrality, Regard, and Ideology. Salience refers to the situativity of racial identity and as such varies with the context. This aspect of racial identity was not included in the MIBI due to its transitory nature. Centrality refers to the importance raters place on identifying themselves by race. The Multidimensional Model of Racial Identity was developed using theories of hierarchical identity development. As such, it acknowledges that racial identity may vary in centrality depending on the situation and the other identity roles that an individual may play. Regard consists of two subscales, private and public regard. This aspect refers to an individual’s judgment regarding how others perceive Black people and how the individual feels about being Black. The Ideology subscales measure individuals’ beliefs regarding how members of the Black community should conduct themselves as far as interracial relationships, economics, and living in a multiracial society. These beliefs were further categorized into four different attitudes: Assimilationist, Humanist, Nationalist, and Oppressed Minority. An important difference between the MIBI and other measures is that the MIBI does not combine scores into one overall racial identity score. Also, the MIBI allows different components 64 of racial identity to be operationalized to determine the specific components that may be moderating relationships between other factors. The original instrument consisted of 71 questions that attempted to capture the three stable dimensions of racial identity: Centrality, Regard, and Ideology. The centrality subscale consisted of 10 items. The private regard subscale consisted of seven items. The public regard subscale consisted of four items. Private and Public regard were combined to form one scale labeled regard. The Ideology subscale consisted of 50 items. Consistent with recent studies (Chavous, et al., 2003; Harper & Tuckman, 2006) the ideology subsection of the MIBI will not be used in data collection. Due to the age of the participants in this study and the areas of interest, only the Centrality, Public and Private Regard subscales of the MIBI will be used. Although the Ideology subscale may provide other important information, it is beyond the scope of this study to include these questions. Furthermore, due to the limited experience that students may have had in participating in the larger society, many of the ideological questions may be beyond the scope of their understanding. Perceived School Support School climate was assessed to determine the students’ perceptions of how supportive their school is in terms of promoting academic achievement and multicultural awareness. This study utilized the Inventory of School Climate (ISC) – Student subscales of Teacher Support and Support For Cultural Pluralism by Brand, et al.,(2003). The ISC was created in an effort to provide an instrument that would measure the school environment for middle and high school students. Prior to the creation of this instrument, most scales had measured school climate at the elementary level where students typically 65 spend the entire school day with one teacher. Brand et al. recognized that such an instrument would not be suitable for students who change classes throughout the day and therefore sought to create a measure that would assess school climate at the school level rather than the class level. The researchers conducted three studies to assess the appropriateness, reliability, and validity of the instrument. Prior Academic Achievement Measures of academic achievement prior to entry to high school were obtained from sixth grade scores on the Standford Achievement Test 10. Subsection scores for reading and math were used to create composite measures. Current school grade point average and number of honors courses taken were used to measure achievement in 11th and 12th grade. Data Analysis Cronbach’s alpha and item inter-correlations among all scales were examined to ensure that the instruments are functioning as expected and to establish construct validity. Descriptive measures such as means and standard deviations were reported for each of the variables of interest. The data from the MIBI were reported as scores on Centrality, Private Regard, and Public Regard. Sixth grade standardized reading and math test scores were combined to form a composite score for prior academic achievement. First, analyses explored the relationships among the racial identity factors (racial centrality, public regard, and private regard), academic intrinsic motivation, and perceived school support among African American students. Initial analysis was conducted to determine whether racial identity factors interact with other components of the model. Correlational analysis was used to determine the extent of relationships 66 between academic intrinsic motivation, perceived school support, and the racial identity factors among African American students. Regression analysis was conducted to determine variables which add incremental validity to the prediction of academic achievement as measured by grade point average and number of honors courses taken beyond 6th grade standardized test scores. The following questions were answered by examining the correlations among the components of racial identity, perceived school support, and intrinsic motivation. Significant correlations will be identified. 1. What are the relationships between the racial identity factors (Centrality, Public Regard, and Private Regard) and perceived support among 11 th and 12th grade African American students? 2. What are the relationships between the racial identity factors (Centrality, Public Regard, and Private Regard) and intrinsic motivation among 11 th and 12th grade African American students? Second, the effect of students’ perceptions of social context, teacher attitudes, and expectations and school climate, has been shown to influence academic achievement (Ford, Grantham, Whiting, 2008; Milner & Ford, 2005). Stereotype threat, which has been defined as ―being at risk of confirming as self-characteristic, a negative stereotype about one’s group‖ (Steele & Aronoson, 1995, p.797), has been shown to extend beyond testing conditions (Steele, 1997). Students may perceive teachers’ lower expectations and respond accordingly as early as fifth grade (McKown &Weinstein, 2003). Thus, the students who show the potential for advanced achievement may be influenced positively 67 or negatively by their experiences with teachers and the teachers’ expectations. The third research question was answered by entering data simultaneously in three blocks in a hierarchical regression model with intrinsic motivation as the dependent variable. Block one contained the main effects of perceived support. Block two contained each racial identity factor (centrality, private regard, public regard). Block three contained the twoway interactions of each of the racial identity factors with perceived support. The analysis was initially run with all three blocks. If no significant interaction was found within the third block, it was dropped from the model. The process was repeated in an attempt to find the most parsimonious model to explain the relationship. When significance was found, further tests were run to help clarify interpretations of the relationships. 3. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between perceived support and motivation? The final stage of analysis investigated each of the racial identity factors individually with the other predictor and outcome variables. Given the large number of variables and interactions, analyses was be performed separately on each of the two dependent variables: grade point average (GPA) and number of advanced placement courses taken. For each analysis, a composite measure (total reading and math scores combined) of 6th grade achievement was entered into the regression to control for differences in academic readiness at high school entry. The main effects of this study were racial identity factors, motivation, perceived school support, and cognitive ability. This study sought to improve the prediction of academic achievement by understanding 68 the relationships among these variables. Accordingly, the following questions were investigated: 4. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and grade point average after controlling for 6 th grade academic achievement? 5. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and the number of Advanced placement courses taken after controlling for 6th grade academic achievement? 6. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between perceived school support and grade point average after controlling for 6 th grade academic achievement? 7. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between perceived school support and the number of advanced placement courses taken after controlling for 6th grade academic achievement? These questions were investigated using hierarchical regression analysis. Due to the large number of variables involved, analyses were run separately for each dependent variable (grade point average and number of advanced placement courses taken). Furthermore, the racial components were tested individually to investigate any interactions between the racial component and either academic intrinsic motivation or perceived school support. Each model was stated in terms of variance accounted for as 69 well as improved prediction. The models consisted of four blocks. The first block contained a composite measure of 6th grade academic achievement (test score) to control for differences in readiness as the students begin their high school careers. The second block contained one of the racial identity variables. The third block contained one of the two remaining predictors (academic intrinsic motivation or perceived school support). The final block of the model contained an interaction term of the racial identity factor in the second block and the predictor used in block three. Analyses were run for each outcome measure, each racial identity factor and each predictor for a total of twelve models. 70 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS The purpose of this study was to explore relationships among racial identity factors, perceived school support, academic motivation, and academic achievement for African American 11th and 12th grade students. The final sample consisted of 56 11th and 12th grade students from two schools in the metropolitan mid-west. The participants completed a demographic questionnaire, plus a self-report survey on racial identity factors, perceived school support, and academic intrinsic motivation. Measures of cumulative grade point average, number of advanced placement courses taken, and sixth grade standardized test performance on reading and math were obtained from school records. Preliminary statistical analyses were conducted for the purposes of describing the sample, and confirming whether combining the two schools into one sample was justified. The research questions were then answered in a series of bi-variate correlation and hierarchical linear regression analysis. Missing Data Table F1 summarizes information concerning the total number of consent forms distributed to student’s families (233), the number of completed surveys (84), and the number of surveys deleted from analyses due to missing sixth grade achievement test scores (26). The final sample consisted of 56 11th and 12th grade students. The students were allowed to skip any questions that they did not want to answer and as such 11 responses out of a total of 2,296 or .04% were left blank. Scores were imputed for the missing data by averaging the individual’s subscale scores and using that average as the missing score. 71 Preliminary Statistical Analyses In the demographic section of the survey, students reported information on race, father’s education level and mother’s education level. The schools provided information on students’ sex, and grade. Table F2 summarizes the demographic profiles of students who participated in the present study. Demographic characteristics of students in the two schools were compared to determine if combining the data would be appropriate (Table F3). Parents’ education level was used as one indication of parental socioeconomic status as details regarding family income were not obtained. In addition to demographic characteristics, measures of students’ achievement were also compared. Independent samples t-tests for equality of means analyses were performed on the groups to compare each of the variables (Table F4). No statistically significant differences were detected; therefore it was deemed appropriate to examine the group as a whole. These scores were compared to national norms. Overall, students in this study scored between the 48 th and 64th percentile nationally (national mean = 1298) on the Standford Achievement Test Version 10. Given that the percentages of students from different grades varied for the two groups of students, it was deemed appropriate to combine the students from the schools into a single sample. Table F5 reports summary statistics for the self-report measures (racial centrality, private regard, public regard, perceived school support, and academic intrinsic motivation). The average racial centrality score for students in the sample was 15.40 (SDCENT = 2.78) on a scale that ranged from 5 to 25. The average private regard score for students in the sample was 26.25 (SDPRI = 3.40) on a scale that ranged from 6 to 30. The average public regard score for students in the sample was 16.26 (SD PUB = 3.61) on a 72 scale that ranged from 6 to 30. The average perceived school support score for students in the sample was 42.46 (SDSUPP = 7.27) on a scale that ranged from 12 to 60. The average academic intrinsic motivation score for students in the sample was 50.35 (SD MOT = 4.52) on a scale that ranged from 12 to 60. Table F5 also reports the Cronbach’s coefficient alpha (α), for each of the selfreport measures (racial centrality, private regard, public regard, perceived school support, and academic intrinsic motivation) as a measure of internal consistency reliability based on the sample of students in the present study, and compared to the several studies previously cited in Chapter 2. All of the sample measures for this study had Cronbach’s alpha coefficient above 0.60. Table F6 reports the Pearson product moment (zero-order) correlations and disattenuated correlations between scores on the self-report measures. A disattenuated correlation is a Pearson correlation that is divided by the square root of the product of the reliabilities of the variables being correlated. Thus, a disattenuated correlation may be conceptualized as a Pearson correlation that has been corrected for the measurement error in the scores; in effect it estimates the correlation between the true scores on the two measures. A disattenuated correlation close to 1 indicates that two scores are essentially measuring the same construct. The largest disattenuated correlation was between racial centrality and private regard (.461) which is about 21.3% shared true score variance. The low disattenuated correlations suggest very little overlap between the constructs being measured by the self-report questionnaire. Although statistically significant Pearson correlations were found between racial centrality and private regard, public regard and private regard, and perceived school 73 support and academic intrinsic motivation, none of the correlations were over .32 which indicates that although they are statistically significant, the correlation may not be practically significant. It is interesting to note that, although not statistically significant, racial centrality was negatively correlated with both perceived school support and academic intrinsic motivation. This suggests that as racial centrality increases, students’ perceptions of school support and academic intrinsic motivation may decrease. Table F4 shows the ranges, averages, and standard deviations for cumulative grade point average, number of advanced placement courses taken, and 6th grade standardized test scores. Statistical Analyses Used to Answer the Primary Research Questions The Relationships Between Racial Identity Factors and Perceived Support The first research question was: 1. What are the relationships between the racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and private regard) and perceived support among 11th and 12th grade African American students? This question was answered by examining the correlations among the variables in Table F6. There were no significant relationships between the racial identity factors and perceived support. The Relationships Between Racial Identity Factors and Academic Intrinsic Motivation The second research question was: 74 2. What are the relationships between the racial identity factors (centrality, public regard, and private regard) and academic intrinsic motivation among 11th and 12th grade African American students? This question was addressed by examining the correlations in Table F6. There were no statistically significant relationships between any of the racial identity factors and academic intrinsic motivation. The Relationships Among Racial Identity Factors, Perceived Support and Academic Intrinsic Motivation The third research question was: 3. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between perceived support and motivation among 11th and 12th grade African American students? The regression model for the outcome measure of academic intrinsic motivation is shown in Table F7. The full model that included separate racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard), perceived support and the three interactions, used a partial F test to determine the statistical significance of adding the interaction terms to model 3.2 which only included the main effects of the racial identity factors and perceived school support. As shown in the last row of table F7, the F[MOT|SUP, CEN, PRI, PUB, SUP*CEN, SUP*PRI, SUP*PUB] = 1.546 (p = 0.215) which indicated that the null hypothesis should be retained. The addition of the interaction terms to model 3.2 did contribute to academic intrinsic motivation beyond the contribution of the main effects of the variables. Two simpler models were also tested. Model 3.2 used a partial F test to determine if the addition of the racial identity factors would improve the prediction of academic 75 intrinsic motivation significantly beyond the independent variable of perceived school support. This model was not significant. Model 3.1 which only included perceived school support had a statistically significant relationship with academic intrinsic motivation. Specifically F[MOT|SUP] = 5.197 (p = .027). Model 3.1 explained 9% of the variance in academic intrinsic motivation. The addition of the racial identity variables (centrality, private regard, and public regard) contributed another 2.8% of the variance. Together, the interactions of the racial identity variables and perceived support, the main effects of each racial identity variable and perceived support explained 19.4% of the variance in academic motivation which is more than twice the variance explained by model 3.1. Table F7 illustrates the magnitude and direction of the relationships among the main effects of perceived school support and each of the racial identity variables, their interactions and academic intrinsic motivation. A statistically significant interaction was found between perceived school support and private regard B = 3.149 (p = .042). The zero order correlation of the interaction term (perceived school support * private regard) is .249 (Table F6) is considered to be a small effect size (0.1 < r < 0.2) by Cohen (1988). One is also mindful that effect sizes must be considered with a detailed understanding of the variables under investigation in order to make meaningful interpretations. The positive zero order correlation of perceived support is .296 which is just below Cohen’s criteria for a medium effect size (1988). As a result of a statistically significant interaction between private regard and perceived school support, reduced models including each interaction term individually are presented in Table F8. Of the three models, only the one using the interaction term of 76 private regard and perceived school support resulted in a statistically significant improvement. Specifically, the addition of only one interaction term (private regard * perceived school support) significantly improved model 3.4B over model 3.2 which only included the main effects of perceived support and private regard F [MOT|SUP, CEN, PRI , PRI * SUP] = 4.329 (p = .042). This result suggests that removing the nonsignificant interaction terms improved the model’s ability to predict motivation. The overall reduced model was able to explain 15.8% of the variance in motivation which while it is less than the full model of 19%, it is meaningfully different from the Model 3.2 that did not include any interaction terms. A graph of the interaction of private regard and perceived support on academic motivation is shown in Figure 1. It appears that differences in the levels of academic intrinsic motivation are more pronounced for students with lower levels of perceived school support. Private regard correlated positively with academic intrinsic motivation for students with perceived school support less than one standard deviation below the mean. The relationship between private regard and academic intrinsic motivation was not significant for students at the mean or above one standard deviation for perceptions of school support. 77 Figure 1. Graph of interaction of Private Regard and Perceived School Support on Academic Motivation Interaction of Private Regard and Perceived Motivation on Academic Intrinsic Motivation Academic Intrinsic Motivation 56 54 52 50 Private 48 -1SD Mean 46 +1SD 44 42 40 -2SD -1SD Mean +1SD Perceived School Support +2SD Thus, the answer to question three depends on the number of interaction terms included in the model. If the full model, that included all three interaction terms, is used then the answer is ―no‖; racial identity variables do not moderate the relationship between perceived school support and academic intrinsic motivation. However, if the models examine the relationship of each racial identity variable individually, then the answer to question three is ―yes‖; private regard moderates the relationship between perceived school support and academic intrinsic motivation. Specifically, the interactions were significantly different from one another for those students above and below one standard deviation on perceived support. 78 The Relationship Among Racial Identity Factors, Academic Intrinsic Motivation and Outcome Measures The fourth and fifth research questions explored the direction and magnitude of the relationships among the racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard), academic intrinsic motivation, and two outcome measures (grade point average and number of Advanced placement courses taken). Specifically, the questions were: 4. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and grade point average after controlling for 6 th grade academic achievement? 5. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and number of advanced placement courses taken after controlling for 6th grade academic achievement? Question 4 The regression model for the outcome measure of grade point average is shown in Table F11. The full model that included the separate factors and their interactions used a partial F test to determine if the addition of the interaction terms between academic motivation and each of the three racial identity factor (centrality, private regard, and public regard) significantly improved the prediction of grade point average over model 4.3 that included only the main effects of sixth grade academic achievement, academic intrinsic motivation, racial centrality, private regard, and public regard. Specifically, for Model 4.4 F[CEN*MOT,PRI*MOT,PUB*MOT|6TH, MOT,CEN,PRI,PUB] = .135, (p = 79 .939) indicating that the null hypothesis should be retained. The addition of the interaction terms did not contribute to the prediction of grade point average. Two simpler models were also tested. Model 4.3 used a partial F test to determine if the addition of the racial identity factors would improve the prediction of grade point average significantly beyond the independent variables of sixth grade achievement and academic intrinsic motivation. This model was not statistically significant. Similarly, model 4.2 used a partial F test to evaluate if academic intrinsic motivation would significantly improve the model beyond sixth grade academic achievement. This model also failed to provide statistically significant evidence that the addition of academic motivation or racial identity factors improved the prediction of grade point average beyond that of sixth grade achievement. Therefore, the answer to research question four is ―no‖; the racial identity variables do not moderate the relationship between academic motivation and grade point average after controlling for sixth grade achievement. The magnitude and direction of the relationships among sixth grade academic achievement, academic intrinsic motivation, racial identity variables (centrality, private regard, and public regard) and grade point average are show in Table F12. Only sixth grade academic achievement was a significant predictor in the any of the models. The strong correlation of r = .584 for sixth grade academic achievement meets Cohen’s (1988) threshold for a large effect size (r >.5). Question 5 Research question 5 was answered through hierarchical regression analysis shown in Table F13. A partial F test was used to determine if the adding interaction terms 80 between academic intrinsic and each of the three racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard) to model 5.3 significantly improved the prediction of the number of Advanced placement courses taken over model 5.3 that only included the main effects of sixth grade achievement, academic intrinsic motivation, and each racial identity factor. Specifically, for model 5.4, F[NAP, CEN*MOT, PRI*MOT,PUB*MOT|6TH, MOT, CEN, PRI, PUB] = 5.185 (p = .004) indicating that the null hypothesis should be rejected; that there was a statistically significant, linear relationship among the number of advanced placement courses taken and one or more of the interaction terms between academic motivation and each of the three racial identity factors. The full model, 5.4 explained 43.9% of the variance in the number of advanced placement courses taken which was significantly more than the 25.3% of the variance explained by the main effects of sixth grade achievement, motivation, centrality, private regard, and public regard. Therefore, the answer to research question five is ―yes‖; the racial identity variables moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and the number of Advanced placement courses students took. Table F14 shows the direction and magnitude of the variables in the models 5.1 – 5.4. Significant interactions were found between racial centrality and academic intrinsic motivation [standardized B = -3.827 (p = .025)] and between private regard and academic intrinsic motivation [standardized B = -3.971 (p = .034)]. This suggests that the effects of perceived racial centrality and private regard may have different relationships with academic intrinsic motivation on the number of advanced courses students took. Figures 2 and 3 show graphs of the interactions in the full model after controlling for the effects of sixth grade achievement. These graphs were created by calculating the 81 predicted number of advanced placement courses taken using sixth grade achievement. The residuals between observed and predicted number of advanced placement courses (AP) were used to plot the interactions to show the relationship between racial centrality, private regard and motivation when sixth grade achievement was controlled. Both figures show a similar pattern in which the differences in the number of AP courses taken are more pronounced for students with lower levels of academic intrinsic motivation. The standardized beta weights for racial centrality and private regard were quite similar (B racial centrality = 4.077, B interaction of centrality and motivation = -4.247; B private regard = 4.419, B interaction of private regard and motivation = -5.615) thus the lines are nearly identical. For students with lower levels of academic intrinsic motivation, racial centrality correlated positively with number of courses taken. The relationship between racial centrality and the number of advanced placement courses taken is significant for those students one standard deviation below the mean on academic intrinsic motivation. It is not significant for those students near or above the mean for motivation 82 Standardized Predicted Number of AP Courses Taken Figure 2. Graph of Interaction between Centrality and Motivation on Number of Advanced Placement Courses Taken Interaction of Racial Centrality and Motivation on Number of AP Courses Taken when 6th grade academic achievement is controlled 1.5 1 0.5 0 -1SD Centrality -0.5 Mean Centrality +1SD Centrality -1 -1.5 -2SD -1SD Mean +1SD +2SD Motivation Figure 3 also showed larger differences in the relationship between private regard and the number of advanced placement courses taken for students with lower levels of academic intrinsic motivation than for those with motivation above the mean. For students with low levels of academic motivation, private regard correlated positively with the number of advanced placement courses taken. Statistically significant differences in the relationship between private regard and the number of advanced placement classes taken were found for students with academic intrinsic motivation scoring at the mean and below. The relationship was not significant for students with academic intrinsic motivation levels above the mean. 83 Figure 3. Graph of Interaction between Private Regard and Motivation on Number of Advanced Placement Courses Taken Interaction of Private Regard and Motivation on Number of AP Courses Taken when 6th grade achievement is controlled Standardized Predicted Number of AP Courses 1.5 1 0.5 0 -1SD Private Regard Mean Private Regard -0.5 +1SD Private Regard -1 -1.5 -2SD -1SD Mean +1SD +2SD Motivation Question 5 Reduced Models Model 5.5 examined each racial identity variable and its interaction with academic motivation individually to test which variables significantly improved the ability to predict the number of AP courses taken over model 5.2 which only included the main effects of sixth grade achievement and academic intrinsic motivation. The reduced models allow each variable to claim the common variance that is shared by all three variables in the full model. Thus it is not surprising that each racial identity factor interaction was significant. The variance explained by the individual models ranged from 27.7% - 36%. All of the models showed improvement over Model 5.2 which explained 21% of the variance. When the size and magnitude of the relationships in the reduced models are compared to the full model which included all of the racial identity variables 84 and their interactions, the standardized coefficients for the interaction terms increased while the standardized beta for sixth-grade academic achievement decreased (TableF16). It is interesting to note that all of the other standardized coefficients for main effects were positive; however, the standardized coefficients for the interaction terms were negative for each of the reduced models 5.5. The answer to research question five is complex. When the full model that included all of the interaction terms was tested, no statistically significant differences were found which would suggest that the answer is ―no‖; racial identity variables do not moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and the number of advanced placement courses taken when all of the variables are included in the model. However, racial centrality and private regard did have a statistically significant interaction in the full model and each was significant in the reduced models. Therefore, the answer to question five would be ―yes‖; when considered individually, racial identity variables do moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and the number of advanced placement courses taken. Specifically, racial centrality and private regard have significant moderation effects. The results of the regression analysis of these two research questions provide interesting differences. In question four, sixth- grade academic achievement was the only significant predictor of grade point average and academic intrinsic motivation did not demonstrate a strong relationship with grade point average. Racial identity variables did not moderate the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and grade point average. However, the lack of significant interactions could be reflective of the weak relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and grade point average. This may 85 explain why other variables, such as teacher subjectivity, extrinsic motivation, and academic self-concept, may have more influential roles in grade point average than those examined in the current study. The results of question five provided evidence that racial identity variables moderated the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation and the number of advanced placement courses taken by students. Thus it appears that the role of academic intrinsic motivation may vary with different outcome measures. The Relationship Among Racial Identity Factors, Perceived Support and Outcome Measures The final research questions explored the direction and magnitude of the relationships among the racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard), perceived school support and two outcome measures (grade point average and number of advanced placement courses). Specifically, the questions were: 6. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between perceived school and grade point average after controlling for 6th grade academic achievement? 7. Do racial identity factors moderate the relationship between perceived school support and number of advanced placement courses taken after controlling for 6th grade academic achievement? Question 6 The regression model for the outcome measure of grade point average is shown in Table F17. The full model (6.4) that included the separate racial identity factors and their interactions with perceived school support used a partial F test to determine the statistical significance of the addition of the interaction terms between perceived school support and 86 each of the three racial identity factors (centrality, private regard, and public regard). Specifically, for model 6.4 F[CEN*SUP, PRI*SUP, PUB*SUP|6TH, SUP, CEN, PRI, PUB] = .880, (p = .458) indicating that the null hypothesis should be retained. The addition of the interaction terms did not statistically contribute to the prediction of grade point average. Two simpler models were also tested. Model 6.3 used a partial F test to determine if the addition of the racial identity factors would improve the prediction of grade point average beyond the independent variables of sixth grade academic achievement and perceived school support. This model was not statistically significant F[CEN, PRI, PUB|6TH, SUP] = 2.255, (p = .093) indicating that the null hypothesis should be retained for the alpha of .05. Model 6.2 used a partial F test to evaluate if perceived school support would significantly improve the model beyond sixth grade academic achievement. This model also failed to provide statistically significant evidence that the addition of perceived school support improved the prediction of grade point average beyond that of sixth grade achievement. Specifically, F[SUP|6TH] = 1.336 (p = .253). None of the models were able to predict grade point average significantly better than sixth grade achievement alone. Therefore, the answer to research question six is ―no‖; the racial identity variables do not moderate the relationship between perceived school support and grade point average after controlling for sixth grade achievement. The direction and magnitude of the relationships among sixth grade academic achievement, perceived school support, racial identity variables (centrality, private regard, and public regard) are detailed in Table F18. In examining all of the models, perceived school support has a statistically meaningful relationship with grade point 87 average (p = .062) in model 6.3 however, the standardized beta in this model is .213 which is less than half of the standardized beta for sixth grade academic achievement (B6TH = .548) and lower than the standardized beta for racial centrality (BCEN = .233, p = .057). This suggests that perhaps perceived school support does not have an influential relationship with grade point average. Question 7 The regression model for the outcome measure of number of advanced placement courses taken is shown in Table F19. The full model that included sixth grade academic achievement, perceived school support, the separate racial identity factors and their interactions used a partial F test to determine whether the addition of interaction terms between the racial identity variables and perceived support significantly improved the prediction of advanced placement courses taken over the prediction of model 7.3 which included only the main effects. Specifically, this model tests the partial F[CEN*SUP, PRI*SUP, PUB*SUP|6TH, SUP, CEN, PRI, PUB] = 1.071, p = .371. The results of this test indicate that no significant difference exists and thus the null hypothesis should be retained. The addition of the interaction terms did not statistically contribute to the prediction of the number of advanced placement courses taken. Two simpler models were also tested. Model 7.3 used a partial F test to determine if the addition of the racial identity factors to model 7.2, which only included the variables of perceived school support and sixth grade academic achievement, would improve the prediction of the number advanced placement courses taken. The results of this test F[CEN, PRI, PUB|6TH, SUP] = 1.307, p = .282. These results indicate that no meaningful evidence exists that racial identity variables add to the prediction of the 88 number of advanced placement courses that students take over the prediction given by perceived school support and sixth grade academic achievement. Model 7.2 used a partial F test to evaluate if perceived support would significantly improve the model beyond sixth grade academic achievement. This model also failed to provide statistically significant evidence. In examining all the models, only sixth grade academic achievement significantly predicted the number of advanced placement courses students took. The full model including interaction terms was able to explain 33.6% of the variance in the number of advanced courses student took; however, sixth grade achievement alone was able to explain 20.2% of the variance. The increase of 13.4% did not reach the level of statistical significance in contribution. Therefore, the answer to research question seven is ―no‖; the racial identity variables do not moderate the relationship between perceived school support and the number of advanced placement courses taken beyond sixth grade academic achievement. Examination of the regression and correlation coefficients for models 7.1 – 7.4 in Table F20 shows that none of the interaction terms between perceived academic support and racial identity variables were significant. In model 7.4, the full model, only sixth grade academic achievement was significant B = .520 (p = .000). Perceived school support has a significance of p = .065 in the full model. A comparison of the standardized betas for perceived school support in model 7.4 (B = 2.338) to Model 7.3 (B = .226) suggests that the racial identity variables may influence the relationship between perceived school support and the number of advanced placement courses students took. A similar phenomenon did not occur with sixth grade standardized academic achievement (B = .520, Model 7.4 vs. B = .511, Model 7.3). 89 Questions six and seven allowed the examination the relationship of perceived school support, racial identity variables, and outcome measures of grade point average and the number of advanced placement courses taken. In both questions, no significant evidence was found that racial identity variables moderate the relationship between perceived school support and the outcome measures. The zero-order correlation of perceived school support and the outcome measures in both questions was quite low rSUP, GPA = .091, rSUP, NAP = .151. Thus the weak relationship between perceived school support, grade point average, and number of advanced placement courses taken may be the main influence in the lack of statistical significant relationships. 90 CHAPTER 5 CONCLUSIONS AND DISCUSSION Conclusions The purpose of this study was to understand the relationships among components of racial identity (centrality, private regard, and public regard), prior academic achievement, academic intrinsic motivation, and perceived school support to predict academic achievement, as measured by grade point average and number of Advanced placement courses taken, for African American students. Correlations between the variables were examined to determine the strength of the relationship among each independent variable (see Table F4 for correlations among studied variables). While statistically significant relationships were noted among racial centrality and private regard, private regard and public regard, and perceived support and academic motivation, these correlations may not be practically significant. It was interesting that none of the racial identity variables had a significant relationship with perceived school support. A model was created to understand the roles of perceived school support, racial identity factors and academic intrinsic motivation. This model showed a significant interaction between private regard and perceived school support in explaining academic intrinsic motivation. Specifically, the students who had a lower perceived school support showed different relationships between private regard and academic intrinsic motivation than did students with mean or higher perceived school support. Models were created to control for prior academic achievement and explore the relationships among the racial identity factors, academic motivation, and perceived school support to increase an understanding of the variance in academic achievement. A total of four models were created to investigate whether racial identity variables moderated the relationship between academic intrinsic motivation, perceived school 91 support, and the outcome measures of grade point average and number of Advanced placement courses taken. The only statistically significant interactions were found in the model that investigated racial identity factors and academic motivation to explain the number of Advanced placement courses taken after controlling for prior academic achievement. Significant interactions were found between academic intrinsic motivation and racial centrality and between academic intrinsic motivation and private regard. An examination of the interaction graphs showed that for students with lower levels of academic intrinsic motivation, the racial identity variables of centrality and private regard were positively correlated with the number of Advanced placement courses taken. For students with academic motivation at the mean or higher, there were no statistically significant differences in the relationships between racial centrality and the number of courses taken. There were no other statistically significant interactions, which suggests that racial identity factors do not moderate the relationships between perceived school support and grade point average or the number of Advanced placement courses taken, and academic intrinsic motivation and grade point average, when prior academic achievement has been controlled. Discussion Racial Identity Variables The correlations of racial identity variables in this study were similar to those found by other researchers using the Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity (Chavous, et al, 2003; Walsh, 2001; Sellers et al., 1998). In comparison, similar findings of the relationship between private regard and academic intrinsic motivation have been found in previous studies. Chavous, et al., (2003) found that Private regard had a 92 significant relationship with school attachment. The results of this study contrast with those reported by Harper and Tuckman (2006) where students with low levels of racial centrality, public regard, and private regard achieved significantly higher grade point averages than students with high levels of the racial identity variables. One possible explanation may be the addition of the control variables of sixth grade academic achievement and the other independent variables of perceived school support and academic intrinsic motivation. This study added to existing literature by highlighting the importance of disentangling components of racial identity. In questions three and five, the racial component interactions taken together were not significant however; when examined individually, significant interactions existed. The importance of this finding illustrates that combining racial identity into one overarching component (i.e. ―ethnic identity‖ Phinney, 1989) may miss the subtle influences of individual factors such as racial centrality, private regard or public regard. For example, students with lower levels of academic motivation had higher achievement when they felt positive about being Black and belonging to the Black community (private regard). When the three racial factors were examined as one group, there were no strong relationships noted. If researchers only looked at the single group, they would miss the importance of the individual components. Academic Intrinsic Motivation and Perceived School Support When exploring the results of the study, it became apparent that the weak relationships between academic intrinsic motivation and perceived school support with the outcome measures limited the ability to determine the impact of racial identity variables. In hindsight, a problem may have been the ways in which academic intrinsic motivation and perceived school support were measured. Although both were measured 93 using existing, psychometrically sound instruments, they may not have been precise enough to notice differences with these students. Academic intrinsic motivation was measured using the Vallerand, et al., (1992) subscales of intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation, intrinsic motivation to accomplish, and intrinsic motivation to know. It is possible that limiting the variable to intrinsic motivation to accomplish would better suit the needs of this study because the outcome measures were measures of accomplishment (grade point average and number of Advanced placement courses taken). However, Gagné and St. Père (2002) found similar results with no statistically significant relationships reported between intrinsic motivation and academic achievement once IQ was controlled. Their study examined both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and neither was able to contribute to the prediction of academic achievement beyond IQ scores. Perceived School Support was measured using the Brand, et al. (2003) subscales of Teacher Support and Support for Cultural Pluralism. These two subscales may be measuring two distinct constructs, one of a personal nature (teacher support) and one of an overall administrative nature (support for cultural pluralism). In reality, students could have opposing perceptions of teacher interactions and administrative support for diversity which may be lost in combining the two scores. Thus, the small correlations between these independent variables and the outcome measures may be reflecting a need for more precise measurement. In regard to prior research, McKown and Weinstien (2003) found a relationship between perceived negative stereotypes (public regard) and a decrease in cognitive performance. The current research study did not reveal any significant relationships 94 between perceived public regard and academic achievement. Negative public regard, stereotypes, have received a great deal of attention in the literature (Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995), yet the students in this study did not report excessively negative perceptions of public regard (Mean = 16, range = 24). Perhaps because the students of this study did not perceive negative public perceptions, this aspect of racial identity did not contribute significantly in any of the models. Konstantopoulos (2009) found that there were no significant differences between race and teacher effect on academic achievement; these appear to be similar to the findings of this study. Perceived school support, comprised of teacher relationship and administration support for cultural pluralism in this study, did not show significant relationships with either grade point average or number of Advanced placement courses taken. Konstantopoulos found that all students, regardless of race, gender or social class, showed academic improvement when taught by effective teachers (2009). However, once prior achievement was controlled, the teacher effects were not statistically significant. The current study found a significant correlation between perceived school support and academic motivation. However, when sixth grade achievement was controlled, there was a very weak correlation between perceived school support and either grade point average or number of advanced placement courses taken. Thus, prior achievement appears to be such an important determinant of future performance that there is little variance left to be accounted for by perceived school support. Study Limitations There are several limitations to this study that warrant caution when interpreting the findings and attempting to generalize across settings. The size of this sample is quite small which brings into question the power of the ability to detect significant effects. 95 Power is the probability of making a type II error or failing to reject a false null hypothesis. Given that the effects of this study were in the small to medium range, using a sample size of 56 lends a power of .75 for simple correlations. Thus, significant effects for small correlations may exist and would have been detected if the sample size were larger. Another limitation of this study involves the use of self-report measures. Although the instruments used have documented validity and reliability, participants may have introduced possible bias through answering in socially desirable ways. Participants were asked questions about their school which may limit the generalizability because the responses are situationally specific. Not all schools are the same in terms of social support therefore different settings may elicit different responses by the students. Even among students attending the same school, respondents indicated variability in their perceptions of school support which makes generalizing this relationship difficult. The participants in this sample came from an urban school district in the Midwest that was heterogeneous in the racial make-up of the school population. Thus, the student experiences in terms of the racial factors could be quite different from those students who live in large metropolitan areas or areas where the schools are more racially homogeneous. Students who attend schools where the population is primarily African American may experience the racial identity factors quite differently than students in the study schools. Similarly, students who are among the few non-white students in a district may have very different experiences of teacher support, strength of racial centrality, and public regard. Although the school district in this study was quite large, it is not large in comparison to those on either coast of the United States. Thus the size of the district may 96 influence the educational experiences of the participants in unique ways that would limit the generalizability of the study. The final area of concern regarding the generalizability of this study is the number of Advanced placement courses taken by students. This district has a history of promoting African American students participation in advanced placement courses. The schools involved in the study have specific programs in place designed to support students in taking advanced coursework beginning in the ninth grade. As such, students in these districts may have access to more advanced placement courses and support to help African American students be successful in these courses when compared to other schools that do not have targeted programs. Therefore, interpreting the relationships with the number of advanced placement courses taken must be done cautiously. Recommendations for Further Research This study could be replicated with a greater sample size. It would be interesting to replicate this study across settings where the student population was more homogeneous and compare the racial identity results for differences. It would improve the generalizability of this study if it could be conducted with regional samples across the United States to compare differences among students in similar size schools to explore regional issues with regard to race. The understandings explored in this study could benefit from qualitative research such as ethnographic studies such as interviews with students to gain their viewpoints of the experience. Further research could benefit by adding other variables to the model. This research did not show significant relationships among the racial identity factors and perceived school support; perhaps other variables such as student self-concept or 97 personality would show stronger relationships with racial identity and academic achievement. Adding these variables to the model may yield important findings that would help educators to improve the educational experience and outcomes for African American students. 98 REFERENCES Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22, 227-257. Ackerman, P. L. (2003). Aptitude complexes and trait complexes. Educational Psychologist, 38(2), 85-93. Ackerman, P. L., & Lohman, D. F. (2006). Individual differences in cognitive functions. In P. Alexander, & P. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of Educational Psychology (2nd ed., pp. 139-149) American Psychological Association. Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(3), 261-271. Anastasi, A. (1980). Abilities and the measurement of achievement. New Directions for Testing & Measurement, 5, 1-10. Areepattamannil, S., & Freeman, J. G. (2008). Academic achievement, academic selfconcept, and academic motivation of immigrant adolescents in the greater Toronto area secondary schools. Journal of Advanced Academics, 19(4), 700-743. Awad, G. H. (2007). The role of racial identity, academic self-concept, and self-esteem in the prediction of academic outcomes for African American students. Journal of Black Psychology, 33(2), 188-207. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37(2), 122-147. Barton, P. E., & Coley, R. J. (2010). The Black-White Achievement Gap Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Berndt, T. J., & Miller, K. E. (1990). Expectancies, values, and achievement in junior high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(2), 319-326. Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit Theories of Intelligence Predict Achievement Across an Adolescent Transition: A Longitudinal Study and an Intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246-263. Bouchard Jr., T., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science, October, 223-228. 99 Brand, S., Felner, R. D., Seitsinger, A., Burns, A., & Bolton, N. (2008). A large scale study of the assessment of the social environment of middle and secondary schools: The validity and utility of teachers' ratings of school climate, cultural pluralism, and safety problems for understanding school effects and school improvement. Journal of School Psychology, 46(5), 507-535. Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Cattell, R. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1), 1-32. Cattell, R. B. (1943). The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 40(3), 153-193. Chavous, T., Bernat, D. H., Schmellk-Cone, K., Caldwell, C. H., Kohn-Wood, L., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2003). Racial identity and academic attainment among African American adolescents. Child Development, 74(4), 1076-1090. Cokley, K. O. (2000). Examining the validity of the Academic Motivation Scale by comparing scale construction to self-determination theory. Psychological Reports, 86, 560-564. Cokley, K. O. (2001). Gender differences among African American students in the impact of racial identity on academic psychosocial development. Journal of College Student Development, 42(5), 480-487. Cokley, K. O. (2002). Ethnicity, gender and academic self-concept: A preliminary examination of academic disidentification and implications for psychologists. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8(4), 378-388. Cokley, K. O. (2003). What do we know about the motivation of African American students? Challenging the "anti-intellectual" myth. Harvard Educational Review, 73(4), 524-558. Cokley, K. (2007). Critical issues in the measurement of ethnic and racial identity: A referendum on the state of the field. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54(3), 224234. Cokley, K. O., Bernard, N., Cunningham, D., & Motoike, J. (2001). A psychometric investigation of the Academic Motivation Scale using a United States sample. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34(2), 109-119. Cokley, K. O., & Chapman, C. (2008). The roles of ethnic identity, anti-White attitudes, and academic self-concept in African American student achievement. Social Psychology of Education, 11(4), 349-365. 100 Cokley, K. O., & Helm, K. (2001). Testing the construct validity of scores on the Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34(2), 80-95. Colangelo, N., Kerr, B., Christensen, P., & Maxey, J. (1993). A comparison of gifted underachievers and gifted high achievers. Gifted Child Quarterly, 37(4), 155-160. The Condition of College & Career Readiness (2010). Iowa City, IA: ACT. Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994). Educational risk and resilience in African-American youth: Context, self, action, and outcomes in school. Child Development, 65(2), 493-506. Corno, L., Cronbach, L. J., Kupermintz, H., Lohman, D. F., Mandinach, E. B., Porteus, A. W., & Talbert, J. (2003). In Cronbach L. J. (Ed.), Remaking the Concept of Aptitude: Extending the Legacy of Richard E. Snow. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Corpus, J. H., McClintic-Gilbert, M., & Hayenga, A. O. (2009). Within -year changes in children's intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations: Contextual predictors and academic outcomes. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 154-166. Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12(11), 671-684. Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure "change": Or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74(1), 68-80. Cross, W. E., Jr. (1991). Shades of black: Diversity in African-American identity Philadelphia, PA, US: Temple University Press. Cury, F., Elliot, A. J., Da Fonseca, D., & Moller, A. C. (2006). The social-cognitive model of achievement motivation and the 2 × 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(4), 666-679. Davis-Kean, P., Huesmann, L. R., Jager, J., Collins, W. A., Bates, J. E., & Lansford, J. E. (2008). Changes in the relation of self-efficacy beliefs and behaviors across development. Child Development, 79(5), 1257-1269. Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2006). Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence From Standardization Samples. Psychological Science, 17(10), 913-920. Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development New York, NY, US: Psychology Press. 101 Elhoweris, H., Mutua, K., Alsheikh, N., & Holloway, P. (2005). Effect of children’s ethnicity on teachers’ referral and recommendation decisions in gifted and talented programs. Remedial and Special Education, 26(1), 25-31. Fancher, R. E. (1985). The intelligence men, makers of the IQ controversy / Raymond E. Fancher. New York : Norton. Ferri, B.a., & Connor, D.J. (2005). In the shadow of Brown. Remedial and Special Education, 26(2), 93-100. Flynn, J. R., & Weiss, L. G. (2007). American IQ gains from 1932 to 2002: The WISC subtests and educational progress. International Journal of Testing, 7(2), 209-224. Ford, D. (1995). Desegregating gifted education: A need unmet. The Journal of Negro Education, 64(1), 52-64. Ford, D., Grantham, T., & Whiting, G. (2008). Another look at the achievement gap: Learning rom the experiences of gifted black students. Urban Education, 43(2), 216239. Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. U. (1986). Black students' school success: Coping with the "Burden of 'Acting White'". The Urban Review, 18(3), 176-206. Furnham, A., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & McDougall, F. (2002). Personality, cognitive ability, and beliefs about intelligence as predictors of academic performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 14(1), 47-64. Gagné, F., & St Père, F. (2002). When IQ is controlled, does motivation still predict achievement? Intelligence, 30(1), 71-100. Gottfried, A. W., Cook, C. R., Gottfried, A. E., & Morris, P. E. (2005). Educational Characteristics of Adolescents With Gifted Academic Intrinsic Motivation: A Longitudinal Investigation From School Entry Through Early Adulthood. Gifted Child Quarterly, 49(2), 172-186. Graham, S. (1994). Motivation in African Americans. Review of Educational Research, 64(1), 55-117. Grantham, T. C., & Ford, D. (2003). Beyond self-concept and self-esteem: Racial identity and gifted African American students. The High School Journal, , 18-27. Harper, B. E., & Tuckman, B. W. (2006). Racial identity beliefs and academic achievement: Does being black hold students back? Social Psychology of Education, 9, 381-403. 102 Harris III, J.J., Brown, E.L., Ford, D.Y., & Richardson, J.W. (2004). African Americans and multicultural education: A proposed remedy for disproportionate special education placement and underinclusion in gifted education. Education and Urban Society, 36(3), 304-341. Haynes, N. M., Emmons, C., & Ben-Avie, M. (1997). School climate as a factor in student adjustment and achievement. Journal of Educational & Psychological Consultation, 8(3), 321-329. Horn, J. L. (1980). Concepts of intellect in relation to learning and adult development. Intelligence, 4(4), 285-317. Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability. Westport, CT: Praeger. Kelley, T. L. (1927). Interpretation of educational measurements. Chicago: World Book. Konstantopoulos, S. (2009). Effects of teachers on minority and disadvantaged students’ achievement in the early grades. The Elementary School Journal, 110(1), 92-113. Lohman, D. F. (1993). Teaching and testing to develop fluid abilities. Educational Researcher, , 12. Lohman, D. F. (2005a). The role of nonverbal ability tests in identifying academically gifted students: An aptitude perspective. Gifted Child Quarterly, 49(2), 111-138. Lohman, D. F. (2005b). An aptitude perspective on talent: Implications for identification of academically gifted minority students. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 28(3-4), 333-360. Long, J. F., Monoi, S., Harper, B., Knoblauch, D., & Murphy, P. K. (2007). Academic motivation and achievement among urban adolescents. Urban Education, 42(3), 196-222. Loveless, T. (2009). An analysis of NAEP Data. High-Achieving Students in the Era of NCLB (pp. 13-48). Washington, DC: Thomas B. Fordham Institute. Lubinski, D. (2000). Scientific and social significance of assessing individual differences: "Sinking shafts at a few critical points.".Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 405-444. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.405 Marcia, J. E. (1994). The empirical study of ego identity. Identity and development: An interdisciplinary approach. (pp. 67-80) Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc. 103 Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., & Köller, O. (2008). Social comparison and big-fish-little-pond effects on self-concept and other self-belief constructs: Role of generalized and specific others. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 510524. Marshall, H. H., & Weinstien, R. S. (1986). Classroom context of student-perceived differential teacher treatment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 441-453. McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2002). Modeling the role of child ethnicity and gender in children's differential response to teacher expectations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(1), 159-184. McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2003). The Development and Consequences of Stereotype Consciousness in Middle Childhood. Child Development, 74(2), 498-515. McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2008). Teacher expectations, classroom context, and the achievement gap. Journal of School Psychology, 46(3), 235-261. McMahon, S. D., Wernsman, J., & Rose, D. S. (2009). The relation of classroom environment and school belonging to academic self-efficacy among urban fourthand fifth-grade students. The Elementary School Journal, 109(3), 267-281. Meece, J. L., & Holt, K. (1993). A pattern analysis of students' achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(4), 582-590. Mickelson, R. A. (1990). The attitude-achievement paradox among Black adolescents. Sociology of Education, 63(1), 44-61. Milner, H. R., & Ford, D. Y. (2007). Cultural considerations in the underrepresentation of culturally diverse elementary students in gifted education. Roeper Review: A Journal on Gifted Education, 29(3), 166-173. National Research Council. (2004). In Blank R., Dbady M. and Citro C. (Eds.), Measuring Racial Discrimination. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. Patton, J.M. (1998). The disproportionate representation of African Americans in special education: Looking behind the curtain for understanding and solutions. The Journal of Special Education 32(1), 25-31. Pepi, A., Faria, L., & Alesi, M. (2006). Personal Conceptions of Intelligence, SelfEsteem, and School Achievement in Italian and Portuguese Students. Adolescence, 41(164), 615-631. Phinney, J. S., & Ong, A. D. (2007). Conceptualization and measurement of ethnic identity: Current status and future directions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54(3), 271-281. 104 Phinney, J. S. (1989). Stages of ethnic identity development in minority group adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 9(1-2), 34-49. Plucker, J. A., Burroughs, N., & Song, R. (2010). Mind the (other) gap Indiana: Center for Evaluation and Education Policy. Purdie-Vaughns, V., Steele, C. M., Davies, P. G., & Ditlmann, R. (2008). Social identity contingencies: How diversity cues signal threat or safety for African Americans in mainstream institutions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 615630. Renzulli, J.S. (1977). The enrichment triad model: a guide for developing defensible programs for the gifted and talented. Mansfield Center, CT: Creative Learning Press. Rowley, S. A. J., Sellers, R. M., Chavous, T., & Smith, M. A. (1998). The relationship between racial identity and self-esteem in African American college and high school students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 715-724. Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (2004). An overview of self-determination theory: an organismic dialectical perspective. In R. Ryan, & E. Deci (Eds.), Handbook of selfdetermination research (pp. 31-59). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Sanders, M. G. (1997). Overcoming obstacles: Academic achievement as a response to racism and discrimination. Journal of Negro Education, 66(1), 83-93. Scarr, S. (1984). Intelligence: What an introductory psychology student might want to know. The G. Stanley Hall lecture series, Vol. 4. (pp. 63-99) Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association. Seaton, E. K., Yip, T., & Sellers, R. M. (2009). A longitudinal examination of racial identity and racial discrimination among African American adolescents. Child Development, 80(2), 406-417. Sellers, R. M., Rowley, S. A. J., Chavous, T., Shelton, J. N., & Smith, M. A. (1997). Multidimensional inventory of black identity: A preliminary investigation of reliability and construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 805-815. Sellers, R. M., Smith, M. A., Shelton, J. N., Rowley, S. A. J., & Chavous, T. (1998). Multidimensional model of racial identity: A reconceptualization of African American racial identity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(1), 18-39. Shernoff, D. J., & Schmidt, J. A. (2008). Further evidence of an engagement-achievement paradox among U.S. high school students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37(5), 564-580. 105 Skiba, R.J., Poloni-Staudinger, L. Gallini, S., Simmons, A.B., & Feggins, Azziz, R. (2006). Disparate Access: The disproportionality of African American students with disabilities across educational environments. Council for Exceptional Children, 72(4), 411-424. Snow, R. E. (1989). Aptitude-treatment interaction as a framework for research on individual differences in learning. Learning and individual differences: Advances in theory and research. (pp. 13-59) New York, NY, US: W H Freeman/Times Books/ Henry Holt & Co. Snow, R. E. (1992). Aptitude theory: Yesterday, today, and tomorrow. Educational Psychologist, 27(1), 5-32. Snow, R. E. (1996). Aptitude development and education. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2(3-4), 536-560. Snow, R. E., & Lohman, D. F. (1984). Toward a theory of cognitive aptitude for learning from instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(3), 347-376. Spinath, B., Spinath, F. M., Harlaar, N., & Plomin, R. (2006). Predicting school achievement from general cognitive ability, self-perceived ability, and intrinsic value. Intelligence, 34(4), 363-374. Spray, C. M., Wang, C. K. J., Biddle, S. J. H., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Warburton, V. E. (2006). An experimental test of self-theories of ability in youth sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7(3), 255-267. Steele, C. (1997). A Threat in the air. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613-629. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797-811. Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.). (2000). Handbook of Intelligence (1st ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Human intelligence: The model is the message. Science, 230(4730), 1111-1118. Stewart, E. B. (2008). Individual and school structural effects on African American high school students' academic achievement. The High School Journal, 91(2), 16-34. Stryker, S., & Serpe, R. T. (1994). Identity salience and psychological centrality: Equivalent, overlapping, or complementary concepts? Social Psychology Quarterly, 57(1), 16-35. 106 Suldo, S. M., Shaffer, E. J., & Shaunessy, E. (2008). An independent investigation of the validity of the School Attitude Assessment Survey--Revised. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 26(1), 69-82. Thorndike, R. L. (1963). The prediction of vocational success. Vocational Guidance Quarterly, 11(3), 179-187. Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623-628. Tyson, K., Darity, W.,Jr., & Castellino, D. R. (2005). It's Not "A Black Thing": Understanding the Burden of Acting White and Other Dilemmas of High Achievement. American Sociological Review, 70(4), 582-605. Unrau, N., & Schlackman, J. (2006). Motivation and Its Relationship With Reading Achievement in an Urban Middle School. Journal of Educational Research, 100(2), 81-101. U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2010). The Condition of Education 2010 (NCES2010-28), Indicator 17. Vallerand, R., Pelletier, L., Blais, M., Briere, N., Senecal, C., & Vallieres, E. (1992). The academic motivation scale: A measure of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in education. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 1003-1017. Vandiver, B. J., Cross, W. E., Jr., Worrell, F. C., & Fhagen-Smith, P. (2002). Validating the Cross Racial Identity Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49(1), 71-85. Walsh, J. J. (2001). The Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity: A validation study in a British sample. Journal of Black Psychology, 27(2), 172-189. Way, N., Reddy, R., & Rhodes, J. (2007). Students' perceptions of school climate during the middle school years: Associations with trajectories of psychological and behavioral adjustment. American Journal of Community Psychology, 40(3-4), 194213. Wechsler, D. (1975). Intelligence defined and undefined: A relativistic appraisal. American Psychologist, 30(2), 135-139. Witherspoon, K. M., Speight, S. L., & Thomas, A. J. (1997). Racial identity attitudes, school achievement, and academic self-efficacy among African American high school students. Journal of Black Psychology, 23(4), 344-357. Worrell, F. C. (2009). What does gifted mean? Personal and social identity persepctives in giftedness in adolescence. In F. D. Horowitz, R. Subotnik & D. J. Matthes (Eds.), 107 The Development of Giftedness and Talent across the Lifespan (pp. 131-152). Washington, DC,: American Psychological Association. Worrell, F. C. (2007). Ethnic identity, academic achievement, and global self-concept in four groups of academically talented adolescents. Gifted Child Quarterly, 51(1), 2338. Yip, T., Seaton, E. K., & Sellers, R. M. (2006). African American Racial Identity Across the Lifespan: Identity Status, Identity Content, and Depressive Symptoms. Child Development, 77(5), 1504-1517. 108 APPENDIX A LETTER OF INFORMED CONSENT INFORMED CONSENT DOCUMENT Project Title: Pathways toward Progress: Understanding the roles of cognitive ability, personality, motivation and racial identity in African American students Principal Investigator: Antonia Szymanski, ABD, University of Iowa Research Team Contact: Antonia Szymanski (319) 541-2653 email: [email protected]  If you are the parent/guardian of a child under 18 years old who is being invited to be in this study, you will be asked to read and sign this document to give permission for your child to participate.  If you are a teenager reading this document because you are being invited to be in this study, the word ―you‖ in this document refers to you. This consent form describes the research study to help you decide if you want to participate. This form provides important information about what you will be asked to do during the study, about the risks and benefits of the study, and about your rights as a research subject.    If you have any questions about or do not understand something in this form, you should ask the research team for more information. You should discuss your participation with anyone you choose such as family or friends. Do not agree to participate in this study unless the research team has answered your questions and you decide that you want to be part of this study. WHAT IS THE PURPOSE OF THIS STUDY? This is a research study. We are inviting you to participate in this research study because you are an African American 11th or 12th grader in St. Paul Public School District. The purpose of this research study is to understand how your ability to learn, motivation, and feelings about being Black affect the GPA and course selection of African American students. HOW MANY PEOPLE WILL PARTICIPATE? Approximately 600 people will take part in this study conducted by researchers from the University of Iowa. 109 HOW LONG WILL I BE IN THIS STUDY? If you agree to take part in this study, your involvement will last for approximately one hour. WHAT WILL HAPPEN DURING THIS STUDY? You will be given a short survey that asks about how you see yourself and your abilities. The survey asks questions about your feelings about yourself and your feelings toward your school environment. You may skip any questions that you do not wish to answer. The survey takes about one hour to complete. We will meet in a room at school to complete the survey. We will obtain your test scores for the standardized tests you took in 6th grade, your current GPA, and the total number of AP or honors classes you have taken. The school will provide this information to the researcher after receiving this consent form. WHAT ARE THE RISKS OF THIS STUDY? You may experience one or more of the risks indicated below from being in this study. In addition to these, there may be other unknown risks, or risks that we did not anticipate, associated with being in this study. It is possible that you may feel uncomfortable answering questions about yourself or your classroom experiences. You may skip any questions that you do not wish to answer and stop participating at any time. You may be concerned that your decision whether or not to be in the study or your responses on the survey may affect your class grades or your standing in school. We will not reveal your answers to the teachers or anyone in the school. WHAT ARE THE BENEFITS OF THIS STUDY? You will not benefit directly from being in this study. However, we hope that, in the future, other people might benefit from this study because we will provide new information about African American learners. WILL IT COST ME ANYTHING TO BE IN THIS STUDY? You will not have any costs for being in this research study. WILL I BE PAID FOR PARTICIPATING? Enclosed in the envelope for the return of your survey will be a ticket. Separate the ticket from the survey and write your name on the ticket. Place the ticket in the collection box at the front of the classroom. You will be entered into a drawing for a $45 Visa gift card. The winner will be drawn at the conclusion of the survey administration and the card will be delivered that day. 110 WHO IS FUNDING THIS STUDY? The University of Iowa and the research team are receiving no payments from other agencies, organizations, or companies to conduct this research study. WHAT ABOUT CONFIDENTIALITY? We will keep your participation in this research study confidential to the extent permitted by law. However, it is possible that other people such as those indicated below may become aware of your participation in this study and may inspect and copy records pertaining to this research. Some of these records could contain information that personally identifies you.  federal government regulatory agencies,  auditing departments of the University of Iowa, and  the University of Iowa Institutional Review Board (a committee that reviews and approves research studies) To help protect your confidentiality, we will use a number and not your name to identify your study information. Your survey form will have a number and will be returned in an envelope so that no one can see your answers. There will be a document that links your name with the survey number. This list and this consent document will be stored so that only the researcher can see them. We will destroy the list linking the number to your name once your data has been entered into the computer. We will store all study materials and information in locked files and password protected computer files. If we write a report or article about this study or share the study data set with others, we will do so in such a way that you cannot be directly identified. IS BEING IN THIS STUDY VOLUNTARY? Taking part in this research study is completely voluntary. You may choose not to take part at all. If you decide to be in this study, you may stop participating at any time. If you decide not to be in this study, or if you stop participating at any time, you won’t be penalized or lose any benefits for which you otherwise qualify. Your decision whether or not to be in this study will not affect the grades you receive in your courses. WHAT IF I HAVE QUESTIONS? We encourage you to ask questions. If you have any questions about the research study itself, please contact: Antonia Szymanski (319) 541-2653. If you experience a researchrelated injury, please contact Dr. David Lohman (319) 335-5574. If you have questions, concerns, or complaints about your rights as a research subject or about research related injury, please contact the Human Subjects Office, 105 Hardin Library, 600 Newton Road, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, 52242, (319) 335- 111 6564, or e-mail [email protected]. General information about being a research subject can be found by clicking ―Info for Public‖ on the Human Subjects Office web site, http://research.uiowa.edu/hso. To offer input about your experiences as a research subject or to speak to someone other than the research staff, call the Human Subjects Office at the number above. This Informed Consent Document is not a contract. It is a written explanation of what will happen during the study if you decide to participate. You are not waiving any legal rights by signing this Informed Consent Document. Your signature indicates that this research study has been explained to you, that your questions have been answered, and that you agree to take part in this study. You will receive a copy of this form. Subject's Name (printed): __________________________________________________________ Birthdate (MM/DD/YY)_______________ Student ID # (CIF)___________________ School______________ Do not sign this form if today’s date is on or after $STAMP_EXP_DT . ________________________________________________________________________ (Signature of Subject) (Date) Parent/Guardian’s Name and Relationship to Subject: ________________________________________________________________________ (Name - printed) printed) (Relationship to Subject - Do not sign this form if today’s date is on or after $STAMP_EXP_DT . ________________________________________________________________________ (Signature of Parent/Guardian) (Date) 112 APPENDIX B DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE 42 How do you identify yourself? □ □ □ □ African African American Puerto Rican Hispanic □ Other:_____________________________ 43 What is your father’s highest level of education completed? □ □ □ □ □ Some High School High School Graduate Some College College Graduate Don’t Know 113 44 What is your mother’s highest level of education completed? □ □ □ □ □ Some High School High School Graduate Some College College Graduate Don’t Know Have you ever been identified to 45 participate in programs for gifted or talented students? If “yes”, tell how many years you participated in programs for gifted or talented students. Yes □ No □ ______________________ 114 I am encouraged to work with students of 1 other races and cultures in school activities. I am encouraged to learn about different 2 races and cultures. My teachers show me that they think it is important for students of different races 3 and cultures at my school to get along with each other. Students from different races and cultures 4 are represented in important school activities. Very Rarely Rarely Sometimes Often How often do you have each of the following experiences at your school? Very Often APPENDIX C PERCEIVED SCHOOL SUPPORT QUESTIONNAIRE □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ 5 Teachers take a personal interest in students. 6 Teachers go out of their way to help students. 7 In classes, students find it hard to get along with each other. 8 If students want to talk to teachers about something, teachers will find time to do it. 9 Students really enjoy their classes. 10 Teachers ask students what they want to learn. 11 Teachers help students to organize their work. 12 Teachers help students catch up when they return from an absence. □□□ □□□ □□□ □□□ □□□ □□□ □□□ □□□ Strongly Disagree Disagree Neither Agree or Disagree Agree How much do you agree with each of the following statements about your school? Strongly Agree 115 □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ 116 14 I feel that Blacks have made major accomplishments and advancements. 15 I feel good about Black people. 16 Society views Black people as an asset. 17 In general, being Black is an important part of my self-image. 18 Overall Blacks are considered good by others. 19 Being Black is unimportant to my sense of what kind of person I am. 20 I often regret that I am Black. 21 I am proud to be Black. 22 Most people consider Blacks, on average, to be less effective than other racial groups. 23 Blacks are not respected by the broader society. 24 In general, other groups view Blacks in a positive manner. □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ Strongly Disagree Disagree Overall, being Black has very little to do with how I feel about myself. Neither Agree or Disagree 13 Agree How much do you agree with each of the following statements about how you feel being Black? Strongly Agree APPENDIX D MULTIDIMENSIONAL INVENTORY OF BLACK IDENTITY 117 25 In general, others respect Black people. 26 I am happy that I am Black. 27 I feel that the Black community has made valuable contributions to this society. 28 Being Black is an important reflection of who I am. 29 Being Black is not a major factor in my social relationships. □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ 118 31 I feel good when I improve in my studies. 32 School is fun for me. 33 I like to learn more about interesting subjects. 34 I really like going to school. 35 High school helps me to feel good about working towards excellence in my studies. 36 I feel good while reading about various interesting subjects. 37 I enjoy discussions with my teachers. 38 I feel good when I know that I am doing something better than before. 39 I feel satisfied when I succeed at difficult academic tasks. 40 My classes help me to learn about things that interest me. 41 It feels good to learn about new things. Disagree I enjoy learning new things. Agree Neither Agree or Disagree 30 Strongly Agree How much do you agree with each of the following statements about why you go to school? Strongly Disagree APPENDIX E ACADEMIC MOTIVATION QUESTIONNAIRE □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ □□ 119 APPENDIX F TABLES Table F1. Summary Statistics of Sample Recruitment District Wide Total Number of Consent Forms Sent Out and Distributed by the Schools (Estimated) Number of Completed Consent Forms Returned 271-300 84 Number of Surveys Completed 84 Response Rate 28 – 31% Number of Participants with 6th Grade Tests Scores Available Number of Surveys Deleted from Analyses 56 28 Final Sample Size 56 Response Rate 19-21% 120 Table F2: Student Demographic Data (N = 56) School A School B # # No Response 2 1 African 1 0 African American 31 12 Other – Mixed Race 8 1 Female 29 7 Male 13 7 11th 26 2 12th 16 12 Unknown 10 1 Some High School 3 3 High School Graduate 7 3 Some College 11 3 College Graduate 11 4 Unknown 1 1 Some High School 5 2 High School Graduate 7 6 Some College 16 4 College Graduate 13 1 Race By Which the Students Identified: Sex: Grade: Father’s Education Mother’s Education 121 Table F3. Chi-Squared Analysis of Demographic Data Race Χ2 1.564 P value .668 Effect Size .167 Sex 1.659 .198 .172 Grade 9.524 .002 .412 Father’s Education 3.736 .443 .25 Mother’s Education 6.464 .167 .339 Variable 122 Table F4. Summary Statistics of Scores on Outcome and Control Measures Mean Standard Deviation Mean Difference t Significance 6th Grade Achievement Test Scores School School A B 1326 1294 60 54 32.05 1.78 .082 Number of AP Classes Taken School A 4.39 3.48 Grade Point Average School B 3.39 3.79 School A 2.08 .695 .994 .905 .370 School B 1.75 .58 .324 1.57 .122 Table F5. Summary Statistics of Scores on Self-Report Measures Centrality (CEN) Private Regard (PRI) Public Regard (PUB) 6 Perceived School Support (SUP) 12 Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) 12 Number of Items 5 6 Minimum Possible 5 6 6 12 12 Maximum Possible 25 30 30 60 60 Mean 15.40 26.25 16.63 42.26 50.35 SD 3.78 3.40 3.61 7.27 4.52 6 13 8 26 41 Minimum Observed Maximum Observed α 23 31 24 75 60 .642 .740 .658 .748 .783 α* .75 .68 .74 .72 .83 Note: α* denotes the Cronbach Alpha reliability estimates obtained in previous research studies cited in Chapter 2. 123 Table F6. Correlations between Scores on Self-Report Measures Pearson Product Moment (Zero-Order) Correlations Centrality (CEN) Private Regard (PRI) Public Regard (PUB) Perceived Support (SUP) Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) Centrality Private Regard Public Regard Perceived Support Academic Intrinsic Motivation .316 (p = .018) .084 (p = .538) .269 (p = .045) -.178 (p = .189) .070 (p = .608) .220 (p = .103) -.089 (p = .514) .015 (p = .911) -.091 (p= .505) .296 (p = .027) Disattenuated Correlations Centrality Private Regard Public Regard Perceived Support Academic Intrinsic Motivation .386 1 Centrality Private Regard Public Regard Perceived Support Academic Intrinsic Motivation .458 .129 .385 -.257 .094 .313 -.125 .019 -.126 124 Table F7. Regression Model Summary for Relationships among Perceived Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) Change Statistics Std Model R R2 Adj R2 Error Est R2 Sig F F df1 Change Change df2 Change 3.1 .296a .088 .071 4.31 .088 5.197 1 54 .027 3.2 .340b .116 .046 4.37 .028 .536 3 51 .660 3.3 .440c .194 .076 4.30 .078 1.546 3 48 .215 a. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support b. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard c. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard, Centrality * Perceived Support, Private Regard * Perceived Support, Public Regard * Perceived Support 125 Table F8. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships among Perceived Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CENT, PRI, PUB), and Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) Unstandardized Coefficients Model 3.1 3.2 3.3 Stdzd Coeff Correlations t β Std Error Const 42.782 3.419 SUP .181 .080 Const 44.46 5.824 SUP .199 .085 CEN -.038 PRI Sig B ZeroOrder Partial Part .296 .296 .296 12.514 .000 2.280 .027 7.633 .000 .325 2.352 .023 .296 .313 .310 .168 -.032 -.227 .821 -.089 -.032 -.030 .064 .188 .049 .342 .734 .015 .048 .045 PUB -.212 .172 -.173 -1.236 .222 -.091 -.170 -.163 Const 85.327 34.687 2.460 .018 SUP -.729 .825 -1.191 -.884 .381 .296 -.127 -.115 CEN .428 1.270 .363 .337 .737 -.089 .049 .044 PRI -2.434 1.209 -1.856 -2.005 .051 .015 -.278 -.260 PUB .858 1.254 .698 .684 .497 -.091 .098 .089 SUP*CEN -.010 .030 -.379 -.326 .746 .099 -.047 -.042 SUP*PRI .057 .027 3.198 2.094 .042 .259 .289 .271 SUP*PUB -.026 .030 -1.216 -.847 .401 .099 -.121 -.110 .296 Dependent Variable: Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) 126 Table F9. Regression Model Summary for Relationships among Perceived Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) – Reduced Models R Change .088 Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 5.197 1 54 Sig F Change .027 3.1 .296 a .088 .071 Std Error Est 4.31 3.2A .299 b .089 .055 4.35 .001 .079 1 53 .779 3.4A .300 c .090 .038 4.39 .001 .063 1 52 .803 Model R R2 Adj R2 2 a. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support b. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support, Centrality c. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support, Centrality, Centrality * Perceived Support Dependent Variable: Academic Intrinsic Motivation R Change .088 Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 5.197 1 54 Sig F Change .027 3.1 .296a .088 .071 Std Error Est 4.31 3.2B .296 b .088 .053 4.35 .000 .002 1 53 .967 3.4B .397 c .158 .109 4.22 .070 4.329 1 52 .042 Model R R2 Adj R2 2 a. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support b. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support, Private Regard c. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support, Private Regard, Private Regard * Perceived Support Dependent Variable: Academic Intrinsic Motivation 127 Table F9 Continued R Change .088 Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 5.197 1 54 Sig F Change .027 3.1 .296 a .088 .071 Std Error Est 4.31 3.2C .337b .113 .080 4.29 .026 1.530 1 53 .222 3.4C ..340 c .116 .065 4.33 .002 .147 1 52 .703 Model R R2 Adj R2 2 a. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support b. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support, Public Regard c. Predictors: (Constant), Perceived Support, Public Regard, Public Regard * Perceived Support Dependent Variable: Academic Intrinsic Motivation 128 Table F10. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships among Perceived Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CENT, PRI, PUB), and Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) – Reduced Models (A, B, and C) Unstandardized Coefficients Stdzd Coeff Model 3.1 3.2A 3.3A Correlations t β Std Error Const 42.782 3.419 SUP .181 .080 Const 43.635 4.588 SUP .177 .082 CEN -.044 .157 Const 47.927 17.693 SUP .073 .422 CEN -.350 SUP*CEN .007 Sig B ZeroOrder Partial Part .296 .296 .296 12.514 .000 2.280 .027 9.510 .000 .290 2.174 .034 .296 .286 .285 -.038 -.282 .779 -.089 -.039 -.037 2.709 .009 .120 .174 .863 .296 .024 .023 1.226 -.297 -.285 .777 -.089 -.040 -.038 .030 .286 .251 .803 .099 .035 .033 .296 Dependent Variable: Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) 129 Table F10 Continued Unstandardized Coefficients Stdzd Coeff t Sig Correlations Model 3.1 3.2B 3.3B β Std Error Const 42.782 3.419 SUP .181 .080 Const 42.958 5.488 SUP .182 .081 PRI -.007 .172 Const 100.961 28.382 SUP -1.162 .651 PRI -2.225 SUP*PRI .051 B ZeroOrder Partia l Part .296 .296 .296 12.51 .000 2.28 .027 7.83 .000 .297 2.26 .028 .296 .296 .296 -.005 -.04 .967 .015 -.006 -.005 3.56 .001 -1.898 -1.79 .080 .296 -.240 -.227 1.079 -1.703 -2.06 .044 .015 -.275 -.262 .025 2.880 2.08 .042 .259 .277 .265 .296 Dependent Variable: Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) 130 Table F10 Continued Unstandardized Coefficients Stdzd Coeff Model 3.1 3.2C 3.3C Correlations t β Std Error Const 42.782 3.419 SUP .181 .080 Const 45.194 3.921 SUP .204 .081 PUB -.201 .163 Const 37.711 19.942 SUP .388 .489 PUB .259 SUP*PUB -.011 Sig B 12.52 .296 ZeroOrder Partial Part .296 .296 .296 .000 2.280 .027 11.53 .000 .332 2.51 .015 .296 .326 .324 -.164 -1.24 .222 -.091 -.167 -.160 1.89 .064 .634 .79 .431 .296 .109 .103 1.213 .211 .21 .832 -.091 .030 .028 .029 -.532 -.38 .703 .099 -.053 -.050 Dependent Variable: Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT) 131 Table F11. Regression Model Summary for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Grade Point Average (GPA) Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 27.891 1 54 .328 Std Error Est .555 R Change .341 .342 .317 .560 .001 .117 1 53 .734 .627c .393 .333 .553 .051 1.408 3 50 .251 .631d .398 .296 .568 .005 .135 3 47 .939 Model R R2 Adj R2 4.1 .584a .341 4.2 .585b 4.3 4.4 2 Sig F Change .000 a. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement b. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation c. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard d. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard, Centrality *Academic Motivation, Private Regard *Academic Motivation, Public Regard * Academic Motivation Dependent Variable: Grade Point Average (GPA) 132 Table F12. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Grade Point Average (GPA) Unstandardized Coefficients Model Const -6.731 Std Error 1.654 6TH .007 .001 Const -6.575 1.829 6TH .007 .001 MOT -.006 .017 Const -5.778 1.842 6TH .006 .001 MOT -.005 CEN β 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 Stdzd Coeff Β .584 Correlations t Sig ZeroOrder Partial Part .584 .584 .584 -4.070 .000 5.281 .000 -3.540 .001 .586 5.249 .000 .584 .585 .585 -.038 -.342 .734 -.003 -.047 -.038 -3.137 .003 .544 4.531 .000 .584 .540 .499 .017 -.033 -.297 .768 -.003 -.042 -.033 .033 .022 .183 1.499 .140 .352 .207 .165 PRI -.005 .024 -.024 -.198 .844 .143 -.028 -.022 PUB -.029 .021 -.157 -1.363 .179 -.094 -.189 -.150 Const -10.074 8.644 -1.165 .250 6TH .006 .001 .552 4.388 .000 .584 .539 .496 MOT .077 .164 .511 .473 .639 -.003 .069 .053 CEN .131 .296 .733 .441 .661 .352 .064 .050 PRI .025 .307 .129 .083 .934 .143 .012 .009 PUB .079 .257 .423 .306 .761 -.094 .045 .035 MOT*CEN -.002 .006 -.586 -.342 .734 .343 -.050 -.039 MOT*PRI -.001 .006 -.170 -.091 .928 .108 -.013 -.010 MOT*PUB -.002 .005 -.619 -.423 .674 -.097 -.062 -.048 Dependent Variable: Grade Point Average (GPA) 133 Table F13. Regression Model Summary for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced placement Courses (NAP) Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 13.701 1 54 .188 Std Error Est 3.203 R Change .202 .210 .181 3.217 .008 .539 1 53 .466 .503c .253 .178 3.222 .042 .944 3 50 .427 .662d .439 .343 2.880 .186 5.185 3 47 .004 Model R R2 Adj R2 5.1 .450a .202 5.2 .459b 5.3 5.4 2 Sig F Change .001 a. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement b. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation c. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard d. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard, Centrality *Academic Motivation, Private Regard *Academic Motivation, Public Regard * Academic Motivation Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken 134 Table F14. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CENT, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken (NAP) Unstandardized Coefficients Model 5.1 5.2 5.3 5.4 β Std Error Const -31.122 9.536 6TH .027 .007 Const -34.291 10.51 6TH .026 .007 MOT .071 .097 Const -32.787 10.72 6TH .029 .008 MOT .074 CEN Stdzd Coeff Correlations t Sig Β ZeroOrder Partial Part .450 .450 .450 -3.264 .002 3.701 .001 -3.264 .002 .444 3.634 .001 .450 .447 .444 .090 .734 .466 .117 .100 .090 -3.059 .004 .493 3.702 .001 .450 .464 .453 .098 .093 .752 .456 .117 .106 .092 .034 .127 .036 .265 .792 .128 .037 .032 PRI -.228 .141 -.220 -1.619 .112 -.069 -.223 -.198 PUB -.001 .124 .001 -.009 .993 -.020 -.001 -.001 Const -197.53 43.79 -4.511 .000 6TH .032 .007 .539 4.435 .000 .450 .543 .485 MOT 3.236 .828 4.077 3.907 .000 .117 .495 .427 CEN 3.42 1.501 3.654 2.278 .027 .128 .315 .249 PRI 3.178 1.553 3.065 2.046 .046 -.069 .286 .224 PUB 1.087 1.302 1.114 .835 .408 -.020 .121 .091 MOT*CEN -.068 .029 -3.827 -2.313 .025 .150 -.320 -.253 MOT*PRI -.066 .030 -3.971 -2.186 .034 -.018 -.304 -.239 MOT*PUB -.022 .026 -1.185 -.838 .406 .006 -.121 -.092 .450 Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken (NAP) 135 Table F15. Regression Model Summary for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced Placement Courses (NAP) Reduced Models (A, B, and C) Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 13.701 1 54 .188 Std Error Est 3.203 R Change .202 .210 .181 3.217 .008 .539 1 53 .466 .459 c .211 .165 3.247 .000 .023 1 52 .881 . 532d .283 .227 3.125 .072 5.127 1 51 .028 Model R R2 Adj R2 5.1 .450a .202 5.2 .459b 5.3A 5.5A 2 Sig F Change .001 a. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement b. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation c. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Centrality d. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Centrality, Centrality *Academic Motivation Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken Std Error Est Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 13.701 1 54 Model R R Adj R2 5.1 .450a .202 .188 3.203 R Change .202 5.2 .459b .210 .181 3.217 .008 .539 1 53 .466 5.3B . 502c .252 .208 3.161 .041 2.867 1 52 .096 5.5B .601 d .361 .311 2.950 .109 8.701 1 51 .005 2 2 Sig F Change .001 a. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement b. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation c. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Private Regard d. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Private Regard, Private Regard *Academic Motivation Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken 136 Table F15 Continued Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 13.701 1 54 .188 Std Error Est 3.203 R Change .202 .210 .181 3.217 .008 .539 1 53 .466 .462 c .213 .168 3.241 .003 .189 1 52 .665 .527 d .277 .221 3.137 .064 4.530 1 51 .038 Model R R2 Adj R2 5.1 .450a .202 5.2 .459b 5.3C 5.5C 2 Sig F Change .001 a. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement b. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation c. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Public Regard d. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Academic Intrinsic Motivation, Public Regard, Public Regard *Academic Motivation Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken 137 Table F16. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Academic Intrinsic Motivation (MOT), Racial Identity Factors (CENT, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken (NAP) Reduced Models (A, B, and C) Unstandardized Coefficients Model 5.1 5.2 5.3A 5.5A Stdzd Coeff Correlations t β Std Error Const -31.122 9.54 6TH .027 .007 Const -34.291 10.51 6TH .026 .007 .444 3.702 MOT .071 .097 .090 .734 Const -34.463 10.66 6TH .027 .008 .451 3.424 MOT .070 .099 .088 CEN -.019 .124 -.020 Const -93.092 27.85 6TH .029 .008 .496 3.860 MOT 1.154 .488 1.454 CEN 3.477 1.548 MOT*CEN -.069 .030 Sig ZeroOrder Partial Part .450 .450 .450 .001 .450 .447 .444 .466 .117 .100 .090 .000 .450 .429 .422 .705 .484 .117 .097 .087 -.150 .881 .128 -.021 -.019 .000 .450 .475 .458 2.363 .022 .117 .314 .280 3.715 2.246 .029 .128 .300 .266 -3.886 -2.264 .028 .150 -.302 -.269 Β -3.264 .002 .450 3.701 .001 -3.059 .002 -3.231 .002 -3.342 .002 Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken (NAP) 138 Table F16 Continued Unstandardized Coefficients Model 5.1 5.2 5.3B 5.5B Stdzd Coeff Correlations t β Std Error Const -31.122 9.54 6TH .027 .007 Const -34.291 10.51 6TH .026 .007 .444 3.702 MOT .071 .097 .090 .734 Const -33.157 10.35 6TH .030 .007 .504 4.023 MOT .071 .095 .089 .744 PRI -.219 .130 -.212 Const -143.32 38.576 6TH .028 .007 .477 4.074 MOT 2.236 .739 2.817 PRI 4.059 1.455 MOT*PUB -.083 .028 Sig ZeroOrder Partial Part .450 .450 .450 .001 .450 .447 .444 .466 .117 .100 .090 .000 .450 .487 .483 .460 .117 .103 .089 -1.693 .096 -.069 -.229 -.203 .000 .450 .495 .456 3.024 .004 .117 .390 .339 3.914 2.789 .007 -.069 .364 .312 -4.99 -2.950 .005 -.018 -.382 -.330 Β -3.264 .002 .450 3.701 .001 -3.059 .002 -3.205 .002 -3.715 .001 Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken (NAP) 139 Table F16 Continued Unstandardized Coefficients Model 5.1 5.2 5.3C 5.5C Stdzd Coeff Correlations t β Std Error Const -31.122 9.54 6TH .027 .007 Const -34.291 10.51 6TH .026 .007 .444 3.634 MOT .071 .097 .090 .734 Const Sig ZeroOrder Partial Part .450 .450 .450 .001 .450 .447 .444 .466 .117 .100 .090 Β -3.264 .002 .450 3.701 .001 -3.264 .002 -33.626 10.695 -3.144 .003 6TH .027 .007 .450 3.632 .001 .450 .450 .447 MOT .067 .098 .085 .683 .498 .117 .094 .084 PUB -.053 .121 -.054 -.435 .665 -.020 -.060 -.054 Const -80.052 24.143 -3.316 .002 6TH .027 .007 .452 3.769 .000 .450 .467 .449 MOT .990 .444 1.248 2.230 .030 .117 .298 .265 PUB 2.650 1.275 2.717 2.078 .043 -.020 .279 .247 MOT*PUB -.054 .025 -2.917 -2.128 .038 .006 -.286 -.253 Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken (NAP) 140 Table F17. Regression Model Summary for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Perceived School Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Grade Point Average (GPA) Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 27.891 1 54 .328 Std Error Est .555 R Change .341 .357 .333 .554 .016 1.336 1 53 .253 .658c .433 .377 .535 .077 2.255 3 50 .093 .681d .464 .372 .537 .030 .880 3 47 .458 Model R R2 Adj R2 6.1 .584a .341 6.2 .597b 6.3 6.4 2 Sig F Change .000 a. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement b. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Perceived School Support c. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Perceived School Support, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard d. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Perceived School Support, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard, Centrality *Academic Motivation, Private Regard *Academic Motivation, Public Regard * Academic Motivation Dependent Variable: Grade Point Average 141 Table F18. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Perceived School Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CENT, PRI, PUB), and Grade Point Average (GPA) Unstandardized Coefficients Model 6.1 6.2 6.3 6.4 β Std Error Const -6.731 1.654 6TH .007 .001 Const -7.351 1.734 6TH .007 .001 SUP .012 .010 Const -6.825 1.691 6TH .006 .001 SUP .020 CEN Stdzd Coeff Correlations t Sig Β ZeroOrder Partial Part .584 .584 .584 -4.070 .000 5.281 .000 -4.240 .000 .592 5.360 .000 .584 .593 .590 .128 1.156 .253 .091 .157 .127 -4.036 .000 .548 4.742 .000 .584 .557 .505 .010 .213 1.908 .062 .091 .260 .203 .042 .021 .233 1.949 .057 .352 .266 .207 PRI -.009 .023 -.045 -.380 .706 .143 -.054 -.040 PUB -.037 .021 -.200 -1.764 .084 -.094 -.242 -.188 Const -7.971 4.685 -1.701 .095 6TH .006 .001 .491 3.924 .000 .584 .497 .419 SUP .065 .103 .702 .632 .530 .091 .092 .068 CEN -.113 .159 -.632 -.712 .480 .352 -.103 -.076 PRI .031 .153 .159 .206 .838 .143 .030 .022 PUB .157 .165 .843 .948 .348 -.094 .137 .101 SUP*CEN .004 .004 .932 .968 .338 .395 .140 .103 SUP*PRI -.001 .003 -.279 -.217 .829 .152 -.032 -.023 SUP*PUB -.005 .004 -1.483 -1.187 .241 -.045 -.171 -.127 Dependent Variable: Grade Point Average (GPA) .584 142 Table F19. Regression Model Summary for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Perceived School Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CEN, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced Placement Courses Taken (NAP) Change Statistics F df1 df2 Change 13.701 1 54 .188 Std Error Est 3.203 R Change .202 .235 .206 3.167 .032 2.238 1 53 .141 .539c .290 .219 3.140 .056 1.307 3 50 .282 .579d .336 .223 3.133 .045 1.071 3 47 .371 Model R R2 Adj R2 7.1 .450a .202 7.2 .484b 7.3 7.4 2 Sig F Change .001 a. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement b. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Perceived School Support c. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Perceived School Support, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard d. Predictors: (Constant), 6th Grade Academic Achievement, Perceived School Support, Centrality, Private Regard, Public Regard, Centrality *Academic Motivation, Private Regard *Academic Motivation, Public Regard * Academic Motivation Dependent variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken 143 Table F20. Regression and Correlation Coefficients for Relationships among Sixth Grade Academic Achievement (6TH), Perceived School Support (SUP), Racial Identity Factors (CENT, PRI, PUB), and Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken (NAP) Unstandardized Coefficients Model 7.1 7.2 7.3 7.4 β Std Error Const -31.122 9.536 6TH .027 .007 Const -35.713 9.915 6TH .027 .007 SUP .088 .059 Const -34.197 9.922 6TH .030 .008 SUP .110 CEN Stdzd Coeff Correlations t Sig Β ZeroOrder Partial Part .450 .450 .450 -3.264 .002 3.701 .001 -3.602 .001 .461 3.830 .000 .450 .466 .460 .180 1.496 .141 .151 .201 .180 -3.446 .001 .511 3.946 .000 .450 .487 .470 .061 .226 1.802 .078 .151 .247 .215 .067 .125 .071 .534 .596 .128 .075 .064 PRI -.243 .137 -.234 -1.768 .083 -.069 -.243 -.211 PUB -.058 .124 -.060 -.473 .638 -.020 -.067 -.056 Const -77.321 -2.829 .007 6TH .031 .008 .520 3.736 .001 .450 .479 .444 SUP 1.137 .601 2.338 1.892 .065 .151 .266 .225 CEN 1.023 .926 1.093 1.105 .275 .128 .159 .131 PRI .418 .893 .403 .468 .642 -.069 .068 .056 PUB .667 .964 .684 .692 .493 -.020 .100 .082 SUP*CEN -.024 .022 -1.168 -1.090 .281 .199 -.157 -.130 SUP*PRI -.015 .020 -1.076 -.752 .456 .064 -.109 -.089 SUP*PUB -.018 .023 -1.065 -.766 .448 .046 -.111 -.091 .450 27.33 5 Dependent Variable: Number of Advanced placement Courses Taken (NAP)