Harper (2017) Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 13
BEYOND INDIVIDUAL THERAPY1
David Harper
[email protected]
University of East London, UK
Psychological distress has increasingly been recognised as an important
health and social problem – the 2014 Health Survey England reported
that 26 per cent of adults said they had been given a diagnosis of ‘at least
one mental illness’ in their lifetime (Bridges, 2015). The last 20 years
have seen a significant increase in the availability of mental health
interventions, primarily medication and individual psychological therapy.
However, such interventions are predominantly reactive (rather than
preventative) and focus at the level of the individual (rather than at the
level of the family, community or society).
In this article I will argue that applied psychologists could draw on
traditions like public health medicine and community psychology to
develop proactive preventative interventions, and to inform public debate
so as to address the distal causes of distress.
The rise of individualised mental health interventions
Recent decades have seen a year-on-year increase in the provision of
primarily individually focused mental health interventions: psychiatric
medication and psychological therapy. Psychiatric medication is still the
default intervention in mental health – the Healthcare Commission
(2007) noted that 92 per cent of their service-user sample had taken
medication. The cost of antidepressant medication rose from over £50m
in 1991 to nearly £400m in 2002 (Social Exclusion Unit, 2004). This
cannot simply be due to an increase in the size of the population (the
population in England increased by only 2 per cent between 1991 and
2001) or inflation (7.5 per cent in 1991 dropping relatively steadily to 1.3
per cent in 2002). This trend has continued: Ilyas and Moncrieff (2012)
report that there were 15,000 prescriptions of antidepressants in 1998
but over 40,000 in 2010 (during this period the population increased by
only 5.5 per cent), and they note that the total amount spent on all
psychiatric drugs, adjusted for inflation, rose from over £544m in 1998
to £881m in 2010. There have been rapid increases in the prescription of
other drugs too – prescriptions of methylphenidate for children (better
known by one of its trade names: Ritalin) have risen from 6000 in 1994
(Timimi, 2004) to over 922,000 in 2014 – a 153-fold increase in just over
20 years – costing over £34m a year (Health & Social Care Information
Centre, 2015a). It is important to note that these figures only relate to
community prescribing (by GPs and as outpatients) and don’t cover
medication prescribed in hospital.
Are we happy as citizens that we live in a society where there is an
increasing reliance on medication, with its concomitant side-effects?
First published as: Harper, D. (2016) Beyond individual therapy: Towards a
psychosocial approach to public mental health. The Psychologist, 29 (June), 440-444.
ISSN: 0952-8229
https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-29/june/beyond-individual-therapy
1
1
Harper (2017) Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 13
What might be the causes of such an increase, and what alternatives
might there be?
Comparable year-on-year figures aren’t available for psychological
therapy though we know that, in 2014–15, 1,250,126 people were
referred and 815,665 people began receiving therapy under the
Improving Access to Psychological Therapies initiative (Health and Social
Care Information Centre, 2015b). As clinical psychologists predominantly
provide individual therapy (Norcross & Karpiak, 2012), we can use the
numbers of clinical psychologists as a proxy measure of the increasing
availability of psychological therapy over time. There were 362 members
of the British Psychological Society’s Division of Clinical Psychology in
1970 (Hall et al., 2002) but this had risen to 10,202 by 2011 (British
Psychological Society, 2012), a 28-fold increase. There were 11,279
clinical psychologists registered with the Health and Care Professions
Council in January 2015 (HCPC, 2015).
Despite these increases, it is clear that many people still do not
have access to psychological therapy (Mental Health Taskforce, 2016), a
situation unlikely to change with ongoing cuts to public sector budgets.
Whilst psychological therapy is relatively benign in comparison with the
side-effects of many psychiatric medications, is it feasible to offer therapy
to everyone who might need it? And how ethical is it for psychologists to
focus primarily on providing reactive rather than preventative
interventions and to fail to advocate for social and economic policies that
might address the ‘causes of the causes’ of mental health problems? It is
to these questions that we turn next.
The limitations of individualised and reactive interventions
There have been two primary sources of concern about an over-emphasis
on psychological therapy as an intervention. First, whilst it might be
effective on an individual level, it will never be available to all those who
need it. Second, since therapies are reactive interventions, they do not
proactively address the causes of distress. The late George Albee pithily
summarised these concerns: ‘Individual psychotherapy is available to a
small number only. No mass disorder has ever been eliminated by
treating one person at a time’ (Albee, 1999, p.133).
Despite significant increases in spending on psychiatric
medication and individual therapy, demand still outstrips supply. Rather
than focusing our efforts on intervening once problems develop, perhaps
we need to focus more effort on preventing problems arising in the first
place. In the field of prevention a distinction is drawn between primary
and secondary prevention. Secondary prevention refers to attempts to
ameliorate problems at an early stage, once they have developed. This is
the approach adopted by many early-intervention services with which
applied psychologists are familiar. Primary prevention, on the other
hand, aims to prevent problems before they arise, often through
structural changes like social policy and legislation (e.g. the UK’s
legislation in 2007 to prevent smoking in enclosed public places). Keith
Humphreys argued nearly 20 years ago that clinical psychology had overemphasised psychological therapy at the expense of alternatives:
Psychotherapy lured the field into an overemphasis on individual
psychology and individual-level treatment as the best approach to
society’s ills and an underemphasis on preventive interventions and
2
Harper (2017) Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 13
sociocommunity-level conceptualizations of human behaviour. (1996,
p.193)
If we are to intervene we need to understand the social patterning
of distress. There is now substantial evidence that social inequality has a
powerful effect on mental and physical health (Cromby et al., 2013;
Friedli, 2009; Marmot, 2010; Mirowsky & Ross, 2003; Read & Sanders,
2010; Wilkinson & Pickett, 2009) – see Psychologists Against Austerity
(2015) for a useful summary of this research. A recent survey indicates
that men and women living in lower-income households are more likely
to have received a psychiatric diagnosis than those living in higherincome households: 27 per cent of men and 42 per cent of women in the
lowest income quintile compared with 15 per cent of men and 25 per cent
of women in the highest (Bridges, 2015). Treatment is also socially
patterned: Anderson et al. (2009) reported that 31 per cent of the poorest
quarter of the population (i.e. a household income less than £12,000)
have used medication, compared with only 17 per cent of the richest
quarter (i.e. household income of £38,000 or more).
In their 2009 book The Spirit Level: Why More Equal Societies
Almost Always Do Better, Wilkinson and Pickett show the strong
correlation at a national level between income inequality (i.e. the
difference between the richest and poorest in society) and WHO mental
health surveys. Countries such as the UK or USA, with the highest levels
of income inequality, have high levels of mental health problems; others
such as Japan or Belgium, with more equality of income, have lower
levels of distress (see www.equalitytrust.org.uk/mental-health). Of
course, poverty itself is strongly correlated with a range of physical
health and social problems, but Wilkinson and Pickett argue that the
evidence is strongest for an association between the size of the income
gap and mental and physical health (see also Burns, 2015). This is not to
ignore the role of biology but, rather to emphasise that, as biological
processes are in a constant interrelationship with the person and their
environment, those processes can often arise as the result of
environmental causal influences (Cromby et al., 2013).
The causal influences most applied psychologists encounter in their
everyday lives tend to be what the late David Smail termed ‘proximal’
causes (e.g. personal relationships, domestic and work situation,
education, family). We tend not to consider what Smail termed ‘distal’
causes – economic climate, dominant political ideologies and the media
(see the April 2014 special issue of The Psychologist for further
http://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-27/editiondiscussion:
4/charting-mind-and-body-economic).
The influence of social factors has been increasingly recognised by
leading cognitive behavioural researchers. For instance, in their text on
paranoia, Freeman and Freeman (2008) discuss links between income
inequality and distrust and call for ‘governmental policies to reduce
inequalities of wealth’, the benefits of which would be ‘lower levels of
social exclusion, stress, insecurity – and paranoia’ (2008, p.141).
Similarly in a debate about the merits of cognitive behavioural therapy in
the
British
Medical
Journal,
Nick
Tarrier
notes:
Much of mental distress no doubt has its roots in, or is at least
exacerbated by, social deprivation and inequality and their psychological
consequences. A good dose of social justice and redistribution of wealth
would do the world’s health a lot of good. In the meantime, any
3
Harper (2017) Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 13
psychological treatment can only be a sticking plaster over the wound of
such inequality… (Tarrier, 2002, p.292)
Why might income inequality be linked to psychological distress?
Pickett and Wilkinson (2010) suggest that distress is affected by societal
levels of trust and community life and that these, in turn, are worsened
by income inequality. These processes are magnified in industrialised
societies where the ability to consume is seen as a key aspect of identity
and where a failure to meet perceived social status norms can lead to
exclusion (e.g. Croghan et al., 2006). Worryingly, UK income inequality –
the so-called Gini coefficient – rose sharply in the late 1970s and has
plateaued at a high level since 1990 (see www.equalitytrust.org.uk/howhas-inequality-changed). A significant factor is that the income of the top
1 per cent and top 0.1 per cent of earners has outpaced other groups in
society and, unfortunately, this money is often lost to the real economy.
Given the substantial evidence for the influence of social factors and
negative life events on psychological distress, how ethical is it to
predominantly focus on this ‘sticking plaster’ approach of individual
treatment, rather than attempting to prevent these problems in the first
place? If psychologists were to respond to distress with a fuller range of
interventions than individual psychotherapy, what might they have to
offer?
Some suggestions
Although individual therapy is an important part of the tradition of
applied therapeutic psychology, we are trained in a range of other skills.
Jim
White
(2008,
p.844)
has
argued:
Why are we so hung up on individual therapy? What about equally
important care areas (for which psychologists are eminently suited) such
as mental health awareness raising, early intervention and prevention,
working with others, and delivering mental health help in varied media?
Other suggestions could include:
Improve epidemiological methods
Identify patterns and take action
Develop a range of preventative strategies and evaluate them
Consider different ways of delivering services
Improve epidemiological methods
Much mental health epidemiology utilises functional psychiatric
diagnostic categories, many of which are bedevilled by problems of
reliability and validity. Psychologists could help improve epidemiological
research by developing better survey methods, using more reliable and
valid constructs.
Identify patterns and take action
At a population level, psychologists might follow the tradition of medical
geography illustrated by the example of the physician John Snow in
order to map the distribution of forms of distress. In the mid-19th
century Snow was sceptical of the then dominant theory that diseases
like cholera were caused by pollution or ‘bad air’, and following a number
of deaths from cholera in the Soho district he talked to local residents in
order to map the outbreak back to an infected water pump. His
investigations helped to persuade the local council to disable the pump.
If improving sanitation systems could lead to such improvements in
4
Harper (2017) Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 13
physical health, what might be the analogous change in relation to
mental health?
Perhaps we could take up Paul Gilbert’s (2002) suggestion ‘to have
a “Defeat abuse”, rather than “Defeat depression” campaign’ (Boyle,
2003, p.30). Richard Bentall and colleagues (2014, p.1011) write that
‘childhood sexual abuse has been particularly implicated in auditoryverbal hallucinations, and attachment-disrupting events (e.g. neglect,
being brought up in an institution) may have particular potency for the
development of paranoid symptoms’. So as well as helping children to
become more resilient, we could also try to reduce the incidence of
childhood sexual abuse. How could we use our research-based
knowledge and theory to achieve such a goal?
Psychologists could advocate for changes to policy and legislation
much as physicians and health campaigners have done in relation to
smoking. We could influence the current climate of ideas by engaging
policymakers both directly and indirectly through think tanks and the
media (including social media).
Of course, there are conceptual and methodological challenges
associated with interpreting the implications for the individual of
epidemiological research (Burns, 2015). We would need to work with
local authorities (e.g. Kinderman, 2014) and a wide range of agencies and
planning infrastructure. We can look to history for encouragement: in the
mid-19th century Dr William Henry Duncan became the country’s first
Medical Officer of Health, appointed following the Liverpool Sanitary Act
of 1846. He worked closely with engineers and public officials to improve
sanitation so that ‘the worst of the sanitary evils were swept away’
(Chave, 1984, p.68), leading to dramatic reductions in mortality rates.
Develop a range of preventative strategies and evaluate them. Much
exciting and innovative preventative work is going on (see, for example,
Newton, 2013), but much more needs to be done in developing new
approaches to prevention and evaluating them (e.g. in developing safer,
more nurturing and trusting neighbourhoods). In order to facilitate this,
though, research priorities and service commissioning incentives need to
change.
A 2013 report by the government’s Chief Medical Officer lamented
the paucity of preventative research in mental health, but a key problem
is that research funders do not prioritise it. The charity MQ (2015)
recently reported that, in relation to depression research, £2.71m was
spent on aetiology, £1.05m on treatment but only £0.3m on prevention.
Similarly, for psychosis research, £1.67m went to aetiology, £0.3m on
treatment with only £0.19m spent on prevention. A great deal of
aetiological research is primarily bio-genetic rather than psychosocial,
and Bentall and Varese (2012) have argued that the latter is judged by
tougher standards than the former. If we are to understand interpersonal
and social processes in families, groups and communities whilst
remaining sensitive to the varied subjective personal and cultural
meanings of experience, we will need to involve service users and engage
in more pluralistic and multidisciplinary research. We will also need to
influence NHS commissioning incentives so that community-based
preventative initiatives are rewarded, not just individual therapy.
Consider different ways of delivering services. In the shorter term,
psychologists from a range of theoretical traditions might also consider
5
Harper (2017) Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 13
ways in which they could deliver therapy differently – for example, what
might a preventative intervention informed by a socially contextualised
cognitive behaviour therapy look like? Could individual therapists adapt
ideas from community psychology?
Psychologists could start by going out more to where people onduct their
everyday lives (e.g. where they live, study or work). We could encourage
more ‘bottom-up’ rather than expert-driven ‘top-down’ approaches, like
supporting the development of self-help and peer support groups. And we
could seek to reduce income inequality. This requires action in the
political realm, not only as individual citizens but also using our
knowledge and status as professionals who are familiar with this
research and the pernicious effects social injustice has on the lives of
those
who
use
our
services
(Mallinckrodt et
al., 2014).
Of course, psychologists may feel powerless to influence such distal
factors, but it is important to remind ourselves that change is possible.
Think of the social changes that have occurred in recent years where new
social norms have developed in relation to attitudes about sexuality or, in
the health field, in relation to smoking. The public appears to hold
unfounded and contradictory beliefs about income inequality, poverty
and welfare fraud, perhaps influenced by negative media coverage. How
might psychologists intervene to better inform public debate to support
policy moves to reduce inequality? Psychologists Against Austerity (2016)
offer some research-based suggestions.
Problems and prospects
It is important to note that I am not arguing for the abandonment of
individual therapy – it has a legitimate place as an intervention. Rather,
as others have argued, it is probably never going to be available for all
those who need it for as long as they need it. My argument should also
not be interpreted as a justification for cuts to current services. Rather,
we need significant investment in prevention in addition to current
services, together with a transformation in those services (e.g. so they are
incentivised for preventative work as well as reactive ameliorative work). I
am also not intending to criticise the work of the many psychologists
involved in providing individual therapy. They are doing a difficult job in
challenging circumstances – indeed, many psychological therapists
themselves are feeling under significant stress as a result of increased
targets and cuts to services (British Psychological Society, 2016).
Some psychologists may say that they do not have the skills to
engage in these types of activities or they may feel that, without
increasing demand for psychological therapy, there will be no funding for
psychology posts. Jim White argues that psychologists ‘are worth the
money as long as we exploit all our skills, not just the therapeutic ones’
(White, 2008, p.847). Many applied psychologists may see their
disciplines as synonymous with individual psychological therapy, but our
work has changed radically over time (Hall et al, 2002), and the
increasing centrality of therapy is the result, at least in part, of advocacy
by professional bodies and NGOs. Humphreys (1997) argues that we
could engage policy makers in a similar fashion to advocate for the
adequate funding of public mental health. The government’s recent
announcement of a £200m cut to public health budgets (Price, 2015)
makes the need for such advocacy even more urgent.
6
Harper (2017) Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 13
A community approach
Sue Holland is a clinical psychologist who developed a small women’s
mental health project on a council housing estate in White City in West
London in the 1980s. It followed a three-stage model where individual
therapy was nested within group- and community-based approaches:
1. Assessment followed by 10 weekly sessions of psychodynamic
psychotherapy helping the women to understand their subjective
experience and to understand the meaning of their ‘symptoms’ (e.g. as
understandable
reactions
to
their
life
experiences).
2. Groupwork with other women where each person’s individual
experiences were shared and often common themes in the women’s
experiences
emerged.
3. In a more transformative stage, many of the project’s participants set
up a self-help counselling and advocacy group called Women’s Action for
Mental Health which enabled them to challenge the wider ‘social systems
and structures that… limit people’s needs and choices’ (Holland 1992,
p.72).
Holmes’s (2010) ‘Psychology in the Real World’ project adapted Holland’s
model as a way of conceptualising all types of groupwork: people learnt
how to cope with individual problems but then moved on to exploring the
roots of their problems, subsequently taking action to transform local
communities and aspects of national and international policy that are
‘the causes of the causes’ of distress.
References
Albee, G.W. (1999). Prevention, not treatment, is the only hope. Counselling Psychology
Quarterly, 12(2), 133–146.
Anderson, S., Brownlie, J. & Given, L. (2009). Therapy culture? Attitudes towards
emotional support in Britain. In A. Park, J. Curtice, K. Thomson et al. (Eds.)
British social attitudes: The 25th report (pp.155–172) London: Sage/National
Centre for Social Research.
Bentall, R.P., de Sousa, P., Varese, F. et al. (2014). From adversity to psychosis. Social
Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(7), 1011–1022.
Bentall, R.P. & Varese, F. (2012). A level playing field? Psychosis, 4(3), 183–190.
Boyle, M. (2003, April). The dangers of vulnerability. Clinical Psychology, No. 24, pp.27–
30.
Bridges, S. (2015). Mental health problems. In Health survey for England 2014. London:
Health & Social Care Information Centre.
British Psychological Society (2012). Annual Report 2011. Leicester: Author.
British Psychological Society (2016). Psychological therapies staff in the NHS report
alarming levels of depression and stress – their own [Press release]. Retrieved
from tinyurl.com/j6ddrbs
Burns, J.K. (2015). Poverty, inequality and a political economy of mental health.
Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 24(2), 107–113.
Chave, S.P.W. (1984). The Duncan Memorial Lecture ‘Duncan of Liverpool – and some
lessons for today’. Journal of Public Health, 6(1), 61–71.
Croghan, R., Griffin, C., Hunter, J. & Phoenix, A. (2006). Style failure: Consumption,
identity and social exclusion. Journal of Youth Studies, 9(4), 463–478.
Cromby, J., Harper, D. & Reavey, P. (2013). Psychology, mental health and distress.
Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
7
Harper (2017) Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 13
Freeman, D. & Freeman, J. (2008). Paranoia: The 21st century fear. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Friedli, L. (2009). Mental health, resilience and inequalities. Copenhagen: World Health
Organization.
Gilbert, P. (2002, July). Understanding the biopsychosocial approach II: Individual and
social interventions. Clinical Psychology, No. 15, pp.28–32.
Hall, J., Lavender, T. & Llewelyn, S. (2002). A history of clinical psychology in Britain.
History & Philosophy of Psychology, 4, 32–48.
Health & Care Professions Council (2015, 26 January). Number of practitioner
psychologists – January 2015 [Freedom of Information Act disclosure]. London:
Author.
Health & Social Care Information Centre (2015a). Prescription cost analysis England
2014. London: Author
Health and Social Care Information Centre (2015b). Psychological therapies: Annual
report on the use of IAPT services. England, 2014/15. London: Author.
Healthcare Commission (2007). Talking about medicines: The management of medicines
in trusts providing mental health services. London: Author.
Holland, S. (1992) .From social abuse to social action. In J. Ussher & P. Nicolson (Eds.)
Gender issues in clinical psychology. London: Routledge.
Holmes, G. (2010). Psychology in the real world: Community-based groupwork. Ross-onWye: PCCS Books. Humphreys, K. (1996). Clinical psychologists as
psychotherapists: History, future, and alternatives. American Psychologist, 51,
190–197.
Humphreys, K. (1997). Money and the mission of clinical psychology. American
Psychologist, 52, 182.
Ilyas, S. & Moncrieff, J. (2012). Trends in prescriptions and costs of drugs for mental
disorders in England, 1998–2010. British Journal of Psychiatry, 200(5), 393–398.
Kinderman, P. (2014). A prescription for psychiatry: Why we need a whole new approach
to mental health and wellbeing. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Mallinckrodt, B., Miles, J.R. & Levy, J.J. (2014). The scientist-practitioner-advocate
model. Training and Education in Professional Psychology, 8(4), 303–311.
Marmot, M. (2010). Fair society, healthy lives: The Marmot Review Executive Summary.
London: The Marmot Review.
Mental Health Taskforce (2016). The five year forward view for mental health. London:
NHS England.
Mirowsky, J. & Ross, C.E. (2003). Social causes of psychological distress (2nd edn). New
York: Aldine de Gruyter. MQ (2015). Mental health research funding: Landscape
report. London: Author.
Newton,
J.
(2013).
Preventing
mental
ill-health.
London:
Routledge.
Norcross, J.C. & Karpiak, C.P. (2012). Clinical psychologists in the 2010s: 50
years of the APA Division of Clinical Psychology. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice 19(1), 1–12.
Pickett, K.E. & Wilkinson, R.G. (2010). Inequality: An underacknowledged source of
mental illness and distress. British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(6), 426–428.
Price, C. (2015, 5 June). Government announces £200m cuts to public health budget.
Pulse.
Psychologists Against Austerity (2016). Improving public discussion about inequality: A
briefing paper. London: Author.
Psychologists Against Austerity (2015). The psychological impact of austerity: A briefing
paper. London: Author.
Read, J. & Sanders, P. (2010). A straight talking introduction to the causes of mental
health problems. Ross-on-Wye: PCCS Books. Social Exclusion Unit (2004).
Mental health and social exclusion. London: Office of the Deputy Prime Minister.
Tarrier, N. (2002). Commentary: Yes, cognitive behaviour therapy may well be all you
need. British Medical Journal, 324, 291–292. Timimi, S. (2004). Developing
nontoxic approaches to helping children who could be diagnosed with ADHD and
their families. Ethical Human Sciences and Services, 6(1), 41–52.
White, J. (2008). Stepping up primary care. The Psychologist, 21, 844–847.
Wilkinson, R. & Pickett, K. (2009). The spirit level: Why more equal societies almost
always do better. London: Allen Lane. For slides see: tinyurl.com/5s6vqjl
8