Article
Attempted Crime
and the Crime Drop
International Criminal Justice Review
2016, Vol. 26(1) 21-30
ª 2015 Georgia State University
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1057567715615392
icj.sagepub.com
Graham Farrell1
Abstract
This study contributes to crime drop research on the security hypothesis. Using data from the
Crime Survey for England and Wales, it finds that the decline in attempted vehicle-related theft and
domestic burglary was delayed by 2–4 years. Between 1993 and 1997, completed domestic burglary
fell 21% but attempts by only 2.1%, while between 1993 and 1995 theft of cars fell 8% but attempts
increased 3%. The delay is interpreted as consistent with rational choice theory and with some
offenders continuing to try, but failing, to complete these crimes when faced with improved security.
The subsequent decline in attempts is consistent with offenders, particularly new cohorts of adolescents, having been discouraged.
Keywords
crime drop, crime decline, security hypothesis, debut crime hypothesis, situational crime
prevention, attempted crime
Introduction
‘‘If at first you don’t succeed, give up’’ was Homer Simpson’s insightful take on humanity’s
frequent failure to persevere. When it comes to teenage novices committing crime—particularly car
crimes and burglary—it may denote a largely unrecognized fact. The primary conjecture of the present
study is that when such offenders are faced with significant security improvements, some may try, try,
try again, but then they give up. This study looks at the patterns of attempted property crime when the
volume of completed crime fell dramatically in the 1990s in England and Wales. It finds that the fall in
attempts was delayed by 2–4 years relative to completed crimes and suggests that this is consistent
with rational choice theory and what would be expected as the result of improved security.
The theoretical context for the study is the security hypothesis which posits that crime fell in the
1990s due to the spread of more and better security. It is framed within the crime opportunity theories of rational choice and routine activities. Simply put, offenders choose to commit less crime
when it becomes more difficult to do so. Thus, the security hypothesis is consistent with prior crime
increases because both are products of changing crime opportunities (Farrell, 2013).
1
Centre for Criminal Justice Studies, School of Law, University of Leeds, Leeds, United Kingdom
Corresponding Author:
Graham Farrell, Centre for Criminal Justice Studies, School of Law, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, United Kingdom.
Email:
[email protected]
22
International Criminal Justice Review 26(1)
Security hypothesis research to date has focused mainly on the high-volume property crimes. The
decline in theft of vehicles has been linked to vehicle security improvements in Australia, England
and Wales, the Netherlands, and the United States (Brown, 2004; Brown & Thomas, 2003; Farrell,
Tseloni, Mailley, & Tilley, 2011; Farrell, Tseloni, & Tilley, 2011; Fujita & Maxfield, 2012; Kriven
& Zeirsch, 2007; van Ours & Vollaard, 2013), with electronic immobilizers also found effective in
Germany (Bassman, 2011; Brown, 2013).
Improvements in household security have been linked to the decline in household burglary generally in many countries (van Dijk, 2008; van Dijk, Manchin, van Kesteren, Nevala, & Hideg, 2007)
and by a series of more detailed studies of England and Wales (Tilley, Farrell, & Clarke, 2015;
Tilley, Farrell, Tseloni, & Evans, 2015; Tilley, Tseloni, & Farrell, 2011; Tseloni & Thompson,
2015; Tseloni, Thompson, Grove, Tilley, & Farrell, 2014). In Australia, where property crime fell
from 2001 (Mayhew, 2012), a recent study based on interviews with offenders asked their views on
why crime had declined. The most popular response from offenders was to attribute the crime drop
to improvements in security (Brown, 2015).
Young offenders are known to commit property crimes as their primary early-career debut crimes
from which they learn and from which a small cohort graduate to commit more diverse crimes (Owen
& Cooper, 2013; Svensson, 2002). It has been suggested that the disproportionate decline in adolescent crime that underpinned the crime drop in the United States is consistent with inexperienced young
offenders being more easily deterred by improved security (Farrell, Laycock, & Tilley, 2015). This is
consistent with Clarke and Cornish’s (1985) formulation of rational choice theory, wherein offender
initial decisions to become involved in crime are distinct from those relating to continuance.
Security hypothesis research to date has largely utilized an analytic approach comprising triangulation of data signatures (Farrell, Tilley, & Tseloni, In press). Data signatures are empirical indicators that may prove consistent or inconsistent with theory. The use of signatures per se is not new
but their application in the context of situational crime prevention has been spurred by Eck and
Madensen (2009), who concluded that ‘‘The analysis of crime signature change, as part of crime
prevention evaluations, can improve the internal validity of evaluation findings’’ (p. 59). While a
scientific hypotheses can never be said to be formally proven, evidence that fails to falsify a hypothesis and is consistent with what theory would suggest lends support to its credibility (see Tilley, In
press). This is the case for replication studies and for work that adds additional signatures to the evidence base, of which the present study is the latter type and the first to distinguish patterns of
attempted from completed crimes in the context of the security hypothesis.
Tables 1 and 2 summarize security hypothesis data signatures identified to date in the works of
Farrell, Tseloni, Mailley, and Tilley (2011); Farrell, Tseloni, and Tilley (2011); Farrell, Tilley, and
Tseloni (2014); Farrell, Laycock, and Tilley (2015); Tilley, Tseloni, and Farrell (2011); Tilley,
Farrell, and Clarke (2015); Tilley, Farrell, Tseloni, and Evans (2015); Tseloni, Thompson, Grove,
Tilley, and Farrell (2014); and Tseloni and Thompson (2015).
Security hypothesis research to date has concentrated mainly on car crime and household burglary. However, it has outlined how security and other situational crime prevention measures are
likely to have been critical to reductions in other crime types. Shoplifting, for example, is a highvolume property crime that also features prominently in the debut crimes of young offenders (Owen
& Cooper, 2013). Tilley (2010) identifies 31 situational crime prevention tactics from meet-andgreet policies to radio-frequency identification tagging and locked cabinets, improved sightlines and
management policies, which present a prima facie case for such measures having played a key role
in reducing shoplifting (see also Ross, 2013; Farrell, Tilley, & Tseloni, 2014). The security hypothesis identifies two routes by which violence has declined. The first is the direct effect of security
measures of different types. Robbery of businesses such as banks is the clearest example where
improved security is likely to have had a strong direct impact. The second is that violence fell as
an indirect diffusion of the benefits of reduced property crime. This includes the argument, dubbed,
Farrell
23
Table 1. Data Signatures Consistent With Security Having Reduced Vehicle-Related Theft.
The timing and spread of security fits with the trajectory of declines in vehicle-related theft in different
countries
Different security devices impact differently against different crime types, consistent with their preventive
mechanisms
Preventive effects are much stronger when multiple security devices are in place.
The average age of stolen vehicles increased over time when crime fell because new vehicles have better
security
There were quicker and larger effects on temporary theft (joyriding and transportation) than on permanent
theft (for resale or chopping)
Offenders’ modus operandi changed, with door lock forcing declining disproportionately, consistent with
better quality deadlocks
In Australia and Canada, car theft fell earlier in regions that introduced electronic immobilizers earlier, then
fell nationally in line with their broader spread
Table 2. Data Signatures Consistent With Security Having Reduced Domestic Burglary.
A steep decline in households without security coincided with burglary’s decline
Variation in the effectiveness of different security devices is consistent with their preventive mechanisms
Preventive effects are much stronger when multiple security devices are in place
The drop was mainly a decline in forced entry through doors and windows, consistent with improved
security
Unforced entries—push pasts, keys used, deception—increased slightly for a short period when forced
entries (and all burglary) decreased, consistent with partial short-term displacement as a result of effective
security
Door forcing at the rear of properties fell first and fastest, consistent with security at the previously most
vulnerable entry point
the debut crime hypothesis, that if teenage potential offenders are prevented from committing the
‘‘easy’’ property crimes, then they do not progress to violence (Farrell et al., 2014, 2015; Farrell, Tseloni, Mailley, et al., 2011).
Data and Method
The study uses all available sweeps of the Crime Survey for England and Wales (CSEW, formerly the British Crime Survey). For the 1980s, the CSEW measured crimes occurring in the years
1981, 1983, and 1987. For the 1990s, it measured crime for every other year: 1991, 1993, 1995,
1997, and 1999. It became a continual survey from 2001 and from 2001 switched from calendar
to fiscal years. For simplicity and consistency, only calendar years are referred to here, with the
majority part of the fiscal year used (e.g., 2004 refers to 2004/2005).
The CSEW only asks respondents about attempted crimes for two types of property crime:
domestic burglary and vehicle-related thefts. Serendipitously this focus fits with security hypothesis
research to date outlined above. Domestic burglary in the CSEW is subdivided into domestic burglary at a dwelling and domestic burglary at a nonconnected building to a dwelling. The latter refers
mainly to burglaries of sheds and garages that are not connected to the household by an adjoining
door (which means burglary in a dwelling includes garages, sheds and similar with an adjoining
door). Vehicle-related theft is discussed further below.
Some further conventions are adopted here for brevity and clarity. The ‘‘domestic’’ in domestic
burglary tends to be dropped as redundant. Burglary in a nonconnected building to a dwelling is
24
International Criminal Justice Review 26(1)
Figure 1. All burglary incidence per 1,000 households.
Figure 2. Burglary in a dwelling incidence per 1,000 households.
sometimes referred to as burglary in nonconnected buildings. Burglary with entry, either dwelling or
a nonconnected building, is sometimes referred to as completed burglary to distinguish it more
clearly from attempts.
Findings
Burglary
Figure 1 shows incidence rate trends per 1,000 households for all burglary, that is, both types combined. Figure 2 shows burglary in a dwelling and Figure 3 shows burglary in nonconnected buildings.
There are proportionally far more attempted burglaries in dwellings than nonconnected buildings.
This is probably indicative of better security and higher occupancy (guardianship) of dwellings than
Farrell
25
Figure 3. Burglary in a nonconnected building.
nonconnected buildings and perhaps greater underreporting of attempts that may go unnoticed more
often for nonconnected buildings. However, in both instances, the basic finding with respect to the
trend is similar: attempts peak and plateau between 1993 and the late 1990s (1997 for dwellings and
1999 for nonconnected buildings), whereas completed burglaries decline from a 1993 peak.
The delayed effect on attempted burglary is most pronounced by 1997. Within the first 4 years of
burglary’s decline, the fall in completed burglaries was 10 times greater at 21% than that of attempts
which fell by 2.1%. This effect is consistent between the two subtypes of domestic burglary: By
1997, burglary of dwellings with entry fell 19% and attempts by 2%, and burglary of nonconnected
buildings fell 22% while attempts increased by 4%.
Vehicle-Related Theft
The CSEW gathers information on two categories of motor vehicle-related theft: theft of vehicles
and theft from vehicles. For attempts though, it is not possible to distinguish one from the other: Both
leave a visible sign that someone sought to enter the vehicle but the specific motivation (theft of
or theft from) cannot be determined. Hence there is only one combined attempts category. Most
vehicle-related theft is theft from vehicles, but the trend in both is remarkably similar (Figure 4) and
so it is not considered misleading to group them for present purposes and is the appropriate means of
comparing them to attempts.
At first blush, the vehicle-related theft rate trend (Figure 5) appears broadly similar to that of
burglary: It peaks in 1993 and then declines. Attempted theft of and from vehicles continues to
increase until 2 years later and then declines. Between 1993 and 1995, theft of and from vehicles
fell 4% (theft of by 8% and theft from by 3%) while attempts increased 3%. The delay between the
fall in completed and attempted crime is most evident in the comparison of the first differences
(Figure 6).1 The first differences clarify that the rate of increase of completed vehicle-related theft
was already decelerating by 1987, but attempts did not do so until 1993.
Attempts per 100 Completed Crimes
Another way of cutting the analytic cloth is to look at trends in the number of attempts per 100
completed crimes. Figure 7 shows this for the two burglary subcategories and vehicle-related theft.
26
International Criminal Justice Review 26(1)
Figure 4. Indexed trends in theft of motor vehicle and theft from motor vehicle.
Figure 5. Motor vehicle-related thefts incidence per 1,000 households.
As might be expected, the proportion of attempted burglary dwellings and vehicle-related thefts
peaks in the 1990s, but, reflecting the delay, these peaks come after completed crimes have begun
to fall: in 1997 for burglary dwelling and 1995 for vehicle-related thefts. The proportion of attempts
then declines over time in each case, somewhat more steadily for motor-vehicle theft. From the peak
of 30 attempts per 100 completed motor vehicle-related thefts in 1995, the proportion of attempts
had almost halved to 16 by 2013, although this was still well above the 1981 value of 10 attempts
per 100 completions.
Discussion
The delay between the fall in completions and attempts of between 2 and 4 years suggests distinct
patterns of offending behavior and decision making. The delays are consistent with some offenders
continuing to attempt crime but being thwarted by improved security. The subsequent fall in
Farrell
27
Figure 6. Motor vehicle-related thefts—annual first difference in incidence.
Figure 7. Attempts per 100 completed crimes.
attempts is consistent with those offenders giving up, not at their first lack of success but at repeated
failure. Thus, the delayed fall in attempted crimes is here interpreted as consistent with both the
rational choice theory and the security hypothesis.
Trends in the proportion of attempted crimes (Figure 7) square well with the security
hypothesis. The proportion of attempts was lowest when crime was easiest in the 1980s,
increasing as crime became more difficult due to increased security in the 1990s. Since then,
the declining proportion of attempts reflects offenders learning that crime is more difficult.
Less crime is being undertaken by slightly more expert offenders: They are better at choosing
suitable targets or gaining entry (being more skilled or better equipped) and fail less often. This
interpretation is consistent with the finding of far greater declines in the offending rates of adolescents compared to older more experienced offenders identified for the United States (Farrell
et al., 2015).
28
International Criminal Justice Review 26(1)
This paragraph further outlines why the delay is interpreted as consistent with rational choice
theory. Individuals make bounded or quasi-rational decisions to offend that appear—to them at the
time—to offer greater potential reward than cost. Rewards include psychological and monetary
components, while costs include risk, time, and effort as well as any psychological cost such as
pangs of conscience (see, e.g., Farrell, 2010). Improved security increases the cost, causing some
offenders, particularly novices, to quit. The remainder continue, particularly those who have
already determined to continue beyond their initial involvement decision (see Clarke & Cornish,
1985). Thus framed, the aggregate delay in attempts is a form of partial and impermanent displacement and a signature of effective prevention (Barr & Pease, 1990). After subsequent failure,
other offenders perceive costs to outweigh likely reward, causing them to also quit whereupon
attempts decline. The result is that the fall in the rate of attempts lags behind that of completed
crimes.
Conclusion
When the going gets tough, the tough gets going. Fortunately most adolescent offenders are
not that tough, even though they disproportionately commit crime compared to other agegroups. The well-established offender age–crime curve suggests many committed only one or a
few crimes in adolescence even when crime was at its peak. The delayed fall in attempted property crimes identified here is consistent with many giving up when faced with improved security,
while others continued then gave up after further failed attempts. This in turn would leave a much
smaller cohort of potential life-course persistent offenders than previously, consistent with recent
findings for the United States (Farrell et al., 2015). The offenders who continued to attempt crime
may be those who already had a little experience and were less easily deterred, but another possible explanation is that it simply took some time until burglary and car crime began to ‘‘fall out
of fashion’’ among teenagers. If so, then ‘‘falling out of fashion’’ is essentially a technique of neutralization that saves face rather than acknowledging that burglary and car crime were more difficult. Thus, contemporary youth may claim to prefer to stay indoors or undertake other activities,
but this could also be because the easy crimes opportunities encountered by previous cohorts were
never available.
In addition to the security hypothesis, a range of alternate crime drop hypotheses have been
proposed. One of the strengths of the triangulation of data signatures is that each signature
offers a fresh challenge to each. Rival hypotheses are now required to offer an explanation consistent with a delayed decline in attempted crimes. It is difficult to see how demographic
change, changes in policing, imprisonment or illicit drug markets, or other rival hypotheses can
account for this delay. Neither can any of them account for the broader range of data signatures
identified in Tables 1 and 2, while most are also contradicted by a range of other evidence (Farrell, 2013).
To the extent that there is increasingly strong support for the security hypothesis, there are significant implications for criminological theory and crime policy. It suggests that situational crime
prevention is far more important than many commentators realized. If the most major declines in
crime experienced in modern history were due to situational factors, then there is a clear case for
policy to pursue situational crime prevention far more widely and persistently than it has to date
to address the crime types that continue or have increased in recent years.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/
or publication of this article.
Farrell
29
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship,
and/or publication of this article: The author acknowledges the contribution of grants RES-00022-2386, ES/K003771/1, ES/K003771/2 and ES/L014971/1 from the Economic and Social
Research Council.
Note
1. As the CSEW did not collect data every year, the first differences are adjusted before 2001 depending on the
number of years between sweeps: when there was a 2-year gap between sweeps the difference was halved
and when there was a 4-year gap the difference was quartered.
References
Barr, R., & Pease, K. (1990). Crime placement, displacement and deflection. In M. Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.),
Crime and justice: A review of research (Vol. 12, pp. 277–318). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Bassman, J. (2011). Vehicle theft reduction in Germany: The long-term effectiveness of electronic immobilisation. European Journal of Criminal Policy and Research, 17, 221–246.
Brown, R. (2004). The effectiveness of electronic immobilisation: Changing patterns of temporary and permanent vehicle theft. In M. G. Maxfield & R. V. Clarke (Eds.), Understanding and preventing car theft (pp.
101–119). Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.
Brown, R. (2013). Reviewing the evidence of electronic vehicle immobilisation: Evidence from four countries.
Security Journal. doi:10.1057/sj2012.55
Brown, R. (2015). Explaining the property crime drop: The offender perspective (Trends and Issues in Crime
and Justice Paper No. 495). Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology.
Brown, R., & Thomas, N. (2003). Aging vehicles: Evidence of the effectiveness of new car security from the
home office car theft index. Security Journal, 16, 45–54.
Clarke, R. V., & Cornish, D. B. (1985). Modeling offenders’ decisions: A framework for research and policy’.
In M. Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.), Crime and justice: A review of research (Vol. 6, pp. 147–185). Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
van Dijk, J. J. M. (2008). The world of crime. New York, NY: Sage.
van Dijk, J. J. M., Manchin, R., van Kesteren, J., Nevala, S., & Hideg, G. (2007). The burden of crime in the EU.
Research report: A comparative analysis of the European survey of crime and safety (EUICS) 2005. Brussels, Belgium: Gallup Europe.
Eck, J. E., & Madensen, T. D. (2009). Using signatures of opportunity structures to examine mechanisms in
crime prevention evaluations. In J. Knutsson & N. Tilley (Eds.), Evaluating crime reduction initiatives
(pp. 59–48). Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.
Farrell, G. (2010). Situational crime prevention and its discontents: Rational choice and harm reduction versus
‘cultural criminology’. Social Policy and Administration, 44, 40–66.
Farrell, G. (2013). Five tests for a theory of the crime drop. Crime Science, 2, 1–8.
Farrell, G., Laycock, G., & Tilley, N. (2015). Debuts and legacies: The crime drop and the role of adolescencelimited and persistent offending. Crime Science, 4, 1–10.
Farrell, G., Tilley, N., & Tseloni, A. (2014). Why the crime drop? In M. Tonry (Ed.), Why crime rates fall and
why they don’t: Vol. 43. Crime and justice: A review of research (pp. 421–490). Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
Farrell, G., Tilley, N., & Tseloni, A. (In press). Signature dish: Triangulation from data signatures to examine
the role of security in falling crime. Methodological Innovations.
Farrell, G., Tseloni, A., Mailley, J., & Tilley, N. (2011). The crime drop and the security hypothesis. Journal of
Research in Crime and Delinquency, 48, 147–175.
30
International Criminal Justice Review 26(1)
Farrell, G., Tseloni, A., & Tilley, N. (2011). The effectiveness of car security devices and their role in the crime
drop. Criminology and Criminal Justice, 11, 21–35.
Fujita, S., & Maxfield, M. (2012). Security and the drop in car theft in the United States. In J. J. M. van Dijk,
A. Tseloni, & G. Farrell (Eds.), The international crime drop: New directions in research (pp. 231–249).
Basingstoke, England: Palgrave Macmillan.
Kriven, S., & Ziersch, E. (2007). New car security and shifting vehicle theft patterns in Australia. Security
Journal, 20, 111–122.
Laycock, G. (2004). The U.K. car theft index: An example of government leverage. In M. Maxfield & R. V.
Clarke (Eds.), Understanding and preventing car theft, crime prevention studies (Vol. 17, pp. 25–44).
Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.
Mayhew, P. (2012). The case of Australia and New Zealand. In J. J. M. van Dijk, A. Tseloni, & G. Farrell (Eds.),
The international crime drop: New directions in research (pp. 76–102). Basingstoke, England: Palgrave
Macmillan.
van Ours, J. C., & Vollaard, B. (2013). The engine immobilizer: A non-starter for car thieves (CESifo Working
Paper: Public Choice, No. 4092). Munich: Centre for Economic Studies and Ifo Institute, University of
Munich.
Owen, N., & Cooper, C. (2013). The start of a criminal career: Does the type of debut offence predict future
offending? London, England: Home Office.
Ross, N. (2013). Crime: How to solve it—and why so much of what we’re told is wrong. London, England:
Biteback.
Svensson, R. (2002). Strategic offences in the criminal career context. British Journal of Criminology, 42,
395–411.
Tilley, N. (2010). Shoplifting. In F. Brookman, M. Maguire, H. Pierpoint, & T. Bennett (Eds.), Handbook on
crime (pp. 48–67). Cullompton, England: Willan.
Tilley, N. (In press). Realism, radicalism and situational crime prevention. In A. Edwards, G. Hughes, &
R. Matthews (Eds.), What is to be done about crime and punishment? London, England: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Tilley, N., Farrell, G., & Clarke, R. V. (2015). Target suitability and the crime drop. In M. A. Andresen &
G. Farrell (Eds.), The criminal act: The role and influence of routine activities theory (pp. 59–76).
London, England: Palgrave Macmillan.
Tilley, N., Farrell, G., Tseloni, A., & Evans, E. (2015). Did security cause the drop in domestic burglary?
Presentation to the Stockholm Criminology Symposium, 9 June, 2015, Stockholm, Sweden.
Tilley, N., Tseloni, A., & Farrell, G. (2011). Income disparities of burglary risk: Security availability and the
crime drop. British Journal of Criminology, 51, 296–313.
Tseloni, A., & Thompson, R. (2015). Securing the premises. Significance, 12, 32–35.
Tseloni, A., Thompson, R., Grove, L. E., Tilley, N., & Farrell, G. (2014). The effectiveness of burglary security
devices. Security Journal. doi:10.1057/sj.2014.30
Author Biography
Graham Farrell is Professor of International and Comparative Criminology at the University of Leeds. His
research is mainly in the area of Crime Science and situational crime prevention. Seeking to understand the role
of security in declining crime has been a particular interest in recent years.