Behav Genet (2013) 43:161–167
DOI 10.1007/s10519-013-9585-y
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
Heritability of Boldness and Aggressiveness in the Zebrafish
Tolulope O. Ariyomo • Mauricio Carter
Penelope J. Watt
•
Received: 8 October 2012 / Accepted: 15 January 2013 / Published online: 25 January 2013
Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013
Abstract Behavioural traits that are consistent over time
and in different contexts are often referred to as personality
traits. These traits influence fitness because they play a
major role in foraging, reproduction and survival, and so it
is assumed that they have little or no additive genetic
variance and, consequently, low heritability because, theoretically, they are under strong selection. Boldness and
aggressiveness are two personality traits that have been
shown to affect fitness. By crossing single males to multiple females, we estimated the heritability of boldness and
aggressiveness in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. The additive
genetic variance was statistically significant for both traits
and the heritability estimates (95 % confidence intervals)
for boldness and aggressiveness were 0.76 (0.49, 0.90) and
0.36 (0.10, 0.72) respectively. Furthermore, there were
significant maternal effects accounting for 18 and 9 % of
the proportion of phenotypic variance in boldness and
aggressiveness respectively. This study shows that there is
a significant level of genetic variation in this population
that would allow these traits to evolve in response to
selection.
Edited by Stephen Maxson.
T. O. Ariyomo (&) P. J. Watt
Department of Animal and Plant Sciences, University of
Sheffield, Western Bank, Sheffield S10 2TN, UK
e-mail:
[email protected];
[email protected]
M. Carter
Center for Ecology and Conservation, College of Life and
Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter,
Cornwall Campus, Penryn, UK
Keywords Heritability Additive genetic variance
Boldness Aggressiveness Maternal effects
Personality traits
Introduction
Individuals can express distinctive behaviours that are
consistent over time and context (Wilson et al. 1994) and
these are referred to as personality traits, coping style or
temperament (Koolhaas et al. 1999; Gosling 2001; Réale
et al. 2007). These behavioural traits have been found in
many vertebrate groups (Boissy 1995; Dall et al. 2004; Sih
et al. 2004a, b; Réale et al. 2007; Bell et al. 2009) and may
have significant fitness consequences (Smith and Blumstein
2008; Ariyomo and Watt 2012). Consistent behaviour
within individuals of a population may be due to genetic or
environmental effects or a combination of both (Brown
et al. 2007). The additive genetic variance of a trait, that is,
its heritability, is a requirement for predicting its response
to selection (Falconer and Mackay 1996). Heritability is
estimated by measuring the similarity between relatives
and is often expressed as the proportion of phenotypic
variance that can be attributed to additive genetic variance
(Falconer and Mackay 1996; Wilson et al. 2010). However,
similarity between relatives is not limited to genetic variation; environmental effects may also contribute (Falconer
and Mackay 1996). There are various sources of environmental variation and one that is considered to be important
is that of the mother’s phenotype and its influence on her
offspring’s phenotype; so called maternal effects (Falconer
and Mackay 1996; Kruuk and Hadfield 2007). Maternal
effects can help influence the behavioural responses of
offspring to the environment (Bonduriansky and Day 2009;
Stamps and Groothuis 2010) in ways that differ from
123
162
genetic inheritance, for example, through maternal provisioning during ontogeny, which may affect development
and behaviour (Qvarnström and Price 2001). Maternal
effects may persist throughout the life of the offspring and
they may inflate the additive genetic variance and heritability if not accounted for (Wilson et al. 2010).
Two widely studied personality traits are boldness and
aggressiveness. Boldness is the propensity of an individual
to take risks (Wilson et al. 1994), whereas aggression is
any behaviour that may be deemed confrontational (Olivier
and Young 2002). Aggressiveness can be used to gain
access to mates, resources and maintain dominance (Larson
et al. 2006; Ang and Manica 2010). Boldness and aggressiveness are behavioural traits that can facilitate the success
of foraging (Stamps 2007; Dyer et al. 2009), dispersal
(Réale and Festa-Bianchet 2003), reproduction (Godin and
Dugatkin 1996; Ariyomo and Watt 2012) and survival
(Biro and Stamps 2008; Smith and Blumstein 2010),
though they may have a negative impact on reproductive
success and longevity (Sih et al. 2004b; Sih and Watters
2005; Stamps 2007; Smith and Blumstein 2008). Furthermore, boldness and aggressiveness are often closely linked
to fitness (Réale and Festa-Bianchet 2003; Dingemanse and
Realé 2005; Smith and Blumstein 2008; Ariyomo and Watt
2012) and they may alter the selective environment of other
traits, such as morphological and physiological traits, and
by so doing influence their evolution (Blomberg et al.
2003).
For a trait to respond to selection, it must show phenotypic variation among individuals, have a significant
genetic component and affect fitness (Dingemanse and
Realé 2005; Bijma 2011). Selection favours alleles that
show higher fitness so traits under intense selection should
have little or no additive genetic variance as the selected
alleles become fixed in a population thus resulting in low
heritability estimates (Elimination hypothesis: Jones 1987;
Merilä and Sheldon 1999; Hoffmann 1999; Stirling et al.
2002). The aim of this study was to estimate the proportion
of additive genetic variance underlying phenotypic variation in boldness and aggressiveness in the zebrafish and to
estimate the variance attributable to maternal effects in
both traits. Previous work has shown that, in the zebrafish,
boldness and aggressiveness vary among individuals and
that both traits are highly repeatable (Wright et al. 2003;
Robison and Rowland 2005; Wilson et al. 2009; Norton
et al. 2011; Ariyomo and Watt 2012), which suggests that
they are genetically determined. Furthermore, both traits
significantly affect reproductive fitness (Smith and Blumstein 2008; Ariyomo and Watt 2012). Therefore, we predicted that boldness and aggressiveness would have low
additive genetic variance and thus low heritability estimates. We also predicted that maternal effects would be
123
Behav Genet (2013) 43:161–167
weak since zebrafish are egg scatterers with no parental
care (Spence et al. 2008).
Materials and methods
Adult zebrafish were used (LWT strain) in this study and
they came from laboratory maintained stock. Twenty males
(standard length ± SE 27.30 ± 0.23 mm) and twenty
females (standard length ± SE 26.98 ± 0.22 mm) were
housed separately in two tanks (30 cm 9 15 cm 9 24 cm)
in a re-circulatory system maintained at 27 °C, under a
12:12 h light/dark photoperiod with a 40 min dusk and
dawn period. The fish were fed twice daily with dry fish
food and brine shrimp.
Behavioural testing
Behavioural testing for boldness and aggressiveness was
similar to that described in Ariyomo and Watt (2012). The
open field test has been shown to be the most valid measure
of boldness (Burns 2008) and was therefore used to
determine boldness in this study. A high rate of movement
and the extent of the utilisation of a novel tank were used
as a measure of boldness because it showed the exploratory
and risk taking ability of a fish (Burns 2008; Ariyomo and
Watt 2012) in an unfamiliar environment. Previous studies
have measured boldness and exploratory behaviour (Norton et al. 2011; Wisenden et al. 2011) and found a correlation between the two, which suggests these behaviours
are similar. Subjects were placed in a novel tank
(48 cm 9 26 cm 9 22 cm), with the base marked into 24
rectangles (each 8 cm 9 5.5 cm) and filled with 3 L of
dechlorinated water heated to 26 °C. The number of lines
crossed in 180 s after a 60 s acclimation period was
recorded for each individual. Individuals that crossed more
lines and utilized the inner and outer rectangles on the base
of the tank were deemed bold, while those that crossed the
fewest lines and did not utilize the inner and outer rectangles on the base of the tank and the tank as a whole were
deemed shy. Lighting during the experiments was provided
by two 18 W daylight fluorescent tubes placed approximately 34 cm above the tank.
Individuals tested for boldness in the open field assays
were rested and then tested after 48 h for aggressiveness
using the mirror test (Gerlai et al. 2000; Moretz et al.
2007a, b; Ariyomo and Watt 2012). In zebrafish, aggressive acts include nipping, biting and short bouts of fast
swimming towards an opponent (Gerlai et al. 2000). The
number of aggressive interactions made by a fish towards
its mirror image was recorded. Individuals with the most
number of aggressive interactions were deemed aggressive
Behav Genet (2013) 43:161–167
while those with few aggressive interactions were deemed
non aggressive.
Size matched males and females were paired up randomly for behavioural traits. Pairs were placed in tanks
(23.5 cm 9 12.5 cm 9 17.5 cm) with two layers of marbles on the bottom to serve as a spawning site, and they
were left overnight. Pairs were separated at the same time
every morning and the eggs that had been spawned
between the marbles were pipetted into Petri dishes. Any
unfertilized or dead eggs were removed after 24 h. Fish
were left for 1 week to recover and they were mated again
with different partners so that each male spawned with two
females. In total 40 spawnings were conducted resulting in
40 broods. Eggs that had been transferred to Petri dishes
were left to hatch. After 15 days, larvae were transferred
into tanks (23.5 cm by 12.5 cm by 17.5 cm) on a recirculatory system, with centrally filtered and heated water kept
at 27 °C. The fry were fed commercial fry food ad libitum
for 8 weeks. At 2 months old, the offspring in the broods
were tested for boldness and aggressiveness. For logistical
reasons, and to reduce the time taken to test the offspring, if
a brood was less than thirty, all were tested, but if a brood
was greater than thirty then a subset of twenty individuals
were randomly selected and tested for boldness and
aggressiveness. This amounted to 482 offspring, approximately 63 % of the offspring. Offspring were not sexed.
The number of offspring tested ranged from 3 to 27 within
a brood.
163
and aggressiveness were fitted with the default parameters,
and the plots showed some autocorrelation. Therefore,
models were rerun with longer iterations (Wilson et al.
2010) based on nitt = 650,000, thinning interval = 100,
burn in period = 150,000. We checked whether the priors
specified earlier had any influence on the outcome of the
analysis by dividing the phenotypic variance between the
priors of genetic and residual effects, so that a larger proportion was assigned to the genetic effect (0.95) and the
rest to the residual (0.05) (Wilson et al. 2010). Rerunning
the models showed that the outcome of the analysis was not
different from the previous models.
Maternal effects (VM) were estimated by adding the
maternal identity to the first models specified for boldness
and aggressiveness as random effect and we added another
prior for a random effect (Kruuk 2004; Wilson et al. 2010)
so that VP was further partitioned into VA ? VM ? VR.
The posterior distributions of the estimates of the additive
genetic (animal), maternal and residual (units) variances
were also rerun with the longer iterations used above.
Deviance information criteria (DIC) were used to detect
the statistical significance of the additive genetic and
maternal components of the models. The statistical significance of the genetic estimates (i.e. how different they
were from zero) was assessed using 95 % confidence
intervals (CI) for the heritability estimates, and the
maternal effects were calculated from the posterior distributions using the highest-posterior-density function (HPD
interval, library coda; (Hadfield 2010).
Statistical analysis
Results
Data analysis was carried out using the R statistical package version 2.11.2 (R Development Core Team 2011). Data
on the boldness and aggressiveness of the parent fish were
anaylsed using a two sample t test to compare the personality traits of the male and female parents. Spearman’s
rank correlation (q) was used to test whether brood size
was correlated with boldness and aggression levels.
We estimated the genetic component of the phenotypic
variation in boldness and aggressiveness using the animal
model, and the whole pedigree (i.e. parents and offspring)
was used. We estimated the narrow sense heritability of
boldness and aggressiveness (h2) as the proportion of
phenotypic variance (VP) that could be attributed to additive genetic variance (VA) (that is, h2 = VA/VP) with
univariate animal models using the R package
MCMCglmm. We specified the priors for the genetic
variance (VA) and residual effects (VR) by splitting
the observed phenotypic variance (VP) evenly between the
genetic variance (VA) and the residual effects (VR). The
posterior distributions of the estimates of the additive
genetic (animal) and residual (units) effects for boldness
Overall, the adult males were found to be bolder than the
adult females (t = 2.816, p = 0.008 (two-tailed),
df = 32.38), but there was no significant difference
between males and females in aggressiveness (t = -0.555,
p = 0.58 (two-tailed), df = 47.08). There was no correlation between brood size and boldness (Spearman correlation: q = 0.61, p = 0.10) or brood size and aggressiveness
(Spearman correlation: q = 0.56, p = 0.10).
For boldness and aggressiveness, all variance estimates
were significantly different from zero (Tables 1, 2) and the
posterior DIC comparison with the model versus the
restricted genetic model was statistically significant
(boldness: 4,518.979—only residuals; 4,478.145—additive
genetic and maternal components; aggressiveness:
2,996.9—only residuals; 2,862.7—additive genetic and
maternal components).
For boldness, the additive genetic variance accounted
for 76 % of the total phenotypic variance while the
maternal effects accounted for 18 % (Table 1). For
aggressiveness, the additive genetic variance accounted for
123
164
Behav Genet (2013) 43:161–167
Table 1 Estimates of the variance components of boldness, the
proportion of variances from a univariate model (with maternal
identity as a random effect) and their corresponding 95 % confidence
intervals
Component
Variance
Var. 95 % CI
Prop. Var.
Prop. Var.
95 % CI
VA
4,149.47
2,990.83,
5,042.77
0.76 = h2
0.49, 0.90
VM
661.52
0.18
0.05, 0.43
VR
341.47
136.65,
3,098.17
107.97,
930.85
0.06
0.02, 0.17
h2 proportion of variance in boldness attributable to additive genetic
variance
Table 2 Estimates of the variance components of aggressiveness, the
proportion of variances from a univariate model (with maternal
identity as a random effect) and their corresponding 95 % confidence
intervals
Component
Variance
Var. 95 % CI
Prop. Var.
Prop. Var.
95 % CI
VA
13.97
3.09, 36.23
0.36 = h2
0.10, 0.72
VM
3.32
0.75, 8.73
0.09
0.02, 0.19
VR
21.61
9.84, 29.95
0.55
0.19, 0.79
2
h proportion of variance in aggressiveness attributable to additive
genetic variance
36 % of the total phenotypic variance in the second model
while the maternal effects accounted for 9 % (Table 2).
Discussion
In this study, male zebrafish were found to be bolder than the
females but both sexes were equally aggressive, which is in
line with previous findings by Moretz et al. (2007a). However, it appears that detecting sex differences in aggression is
a function of the measure of aggression used, and some
studies have shown differences in aggressiveness between
the sexes (Filby et al. 2010; Paull et al. 2010; Norton et al.
2011; Dahlbom et al. 2011; but see Dahlbom et al. 2012)
based on social ranks rather than the mirror test used here and
by Moretz et al. (2007a, b). It has been argued that the mirror
test may not recreate all aspects of an aggressive encounter
because of differences in brain activity and hormonal
responses between animals exposed to a mirror image and
those exposed to a real opponent (Oliveira et al. 2005;
Hirschenhauser et al. 2008; Desjardins and Fernald 2010;
Dijkstra et al. 2012). However, these authors seem to agree
that the mirror image elicits aggressive responses.
The heritability estimate for boldness was found to be
high (0.76) while that of aggressiveness was moderate
123
(0.36), and both estimates are comparable to heritability
estimates of personality traits in studies conducted on other
vertebrates (exploratory scores: great tits, Parus major,
h2 = 0.22–0.61, Dingemanse et al. 2002; h2 = 0.10–0.78,
Drent et al. 2003; but see Chervet et al. 2011) and invertebrates (boldness: h2 = 0.2–0.8; dumpling squid, Euprymna
tasmanica, Sinn et al. 2006). Contrary to our prediction, our
results indicate that there were significant additive genetic
components underlying phenotypic variation in boldness and
aggressiveness in the zebrafish tested. Given that the population used in this study was relatively small and far removed
from the original stock, these estimates are still high enough to
cause the behavioural traits to evolve in response to selection
(Waldmann 2001; Blomberg et al. 2003).
In this study, the fact that the heritability estimate for
aggressiveness was lower than the heritability estimate for
boldness suggests that aggressiveness is under more intense
selection in this strain of zebrafish (Hoffmann 1999; Careau
et al. 2011). Zebrafish are known for their aggressive behaviour towards conspecifics (Spence et al. 2008; Paull et al.
2010) and aggressive behaviours, such as displays and chases,
are used by zebrafish to maintain dominance (Spence et al.
2008; Watt et al. 2011). Being dominant is important in
zebrafish because it allows individuals to gain access to territories, mates and resources (i.e. spawning sites and food)
(Spence et al. 2008). This means that non-aggressive individuals may not have access to resources and consequently
have reduced fitness. Alternatively, the lower heritability
estimate for aggressiveness could have been due to high
residual variance associated with this trait. Although, we did
not partition the residual variance into its components, this
high variance is likely to be due to environmental variance not
accounted for in this study. Given the nature of aggression and
its function in zebrafish, the social environment could have
had a more significant impact on the phenotypic variance in
this trait (Moore et al. 1997; Wolf et al. 1998, 1999; Wilson
et al. 2009; McGlothlin et al. 2010) than for boldness. The
high estimate of heritability recorded in this study for boldness
may be because in the laboratory there is little or no environmental variation since food and the physicochemical
properties of the water are controlled, which reduces the
residual variance and so results in high heritability estimates
in captive animals (Lush 1949; Falconer and Mackay 1996;
Drent et al. 2003; Kruuk 2004). Brood size did not appear to
have any effect on boldness and aggressiveness.
Our results show significant maternal effects accounting
for 18 and 9 % of the proportion variances in boldness and
aggressiveness respectively, indicating that the mother has
an influence on the development of boldness and aggressiveness in her offspring. From this study, we do not know
the relative contributions of genetic and environmental
maternal effects to the estimate. In a study conducted on the
exploratory-boldness behavioural syndrome in zebrafish,
Behav Genet (2013) 43:161–167
Wisenden et al. (2011) found that irrespective of the
father’s personality, offspring of exploratory mothers
always showed high exploratory behaviour, suggesting that
maternal effects for behavioural traits may be commonly
found in zebrafish. Significant maternal effects on the
expression of behavioural traits of offspring have been
shown to occur in a variety of animals (great tits: P. major:
Van Oers et al. 2004; sticklebacks: Gasterosteus aculeatus:
Giesing et al. 2011) through the deposition of various
substances into the eggs, and these effects can persist into
adulthood and across generations (Taylor et al. 2012).
Zebrafish are egg scatterers that show no parental care
(Spence et al. 2008) and so maternal effects in such a
system may occur through egg provisioning before
spawning (Wright et al. 2003). Studies have shown that the
rearing conditions that the mother experiences can influence the degree to which hormones are integrated into her
eggs and consequently affect the behavioural phenotype of
her offspring (Schwabl 1993; Stratholt et al. 1997;
McCormick 1998; Eising et al. 2001; Gil 2008; Leatherland et al. 2010; Storm and Lima 2010; Giesing et al. 2011)
in a way that may enhance their fitness. For example,
Giesing et al. (2011), in a recent study on three-spined
sticklebacks (G. aculeatus), showed that the eggs of
mothers exposed to a predator had elevated concentrations
of cortisol and the juveniles of predator exposed mothers
formed tighter shoals.
In conclusion, we have shown that in this population of
zebrafish, boldness and aggressiveness have heritable components and so are able to respond to selection. Furthermore,
there are significant maternal effects on the expression of
these behavioural traits by the offspring. Maternal experience from her interaction with the environment, shaped by
natural selection, may cause the transmission of information
through the eggs and consequently, directly or indirectly,
shape the fitness of her offspring. Thus, maternal effects on
offspring phenotype may be adaptive (Heath and Blouw
1998; Mousseau and Fox 1998; Giesing et al. 2011).
Acknowledgments We thank Jon Slate for comments on the manuscript, Phil Young for technical assistance and two anonymous
reviewers for helpful comments and constructive criticism.
References
Ang TZ, Manica A (2010) Benefits and costs of dominance in the
angelfish Centropyge bicolour. Ethology 116(9):855–865
Ariyomo TO, Watt PJ (2012) The effect of variation in boldness and
aggressiveness on the reproductive success of zebrafish, Danio
rerio. Anim Behav 83(1):41–46
Bell AM, Hankison SJ, Laskowski KL (2009) The repeatability of
behaviour: a meta-analysis. Anim Behav 77(4):771–783
165
Bijma P (2011) A general definition of the heritable variation that
determines the potential of a population to respond to selection.
Genetics 189(4):1347–1359
Biro PA, Stamps JA (2008) Are animal personality traits linked to
life-history productivity? Trends Ecol Evol 23(7):360–368
Blomberg SP, Garland TJR, Ives AR (2003) Testing for phylogenetic
signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile.
Evolution 57(4):717–745
Boissy A (1995) Fear and fearfulness in animals. Q Rev Biol
70(2):165–191
Bonduriansky R, Day T (2009) Nongenetic inheritance and its
evolutionary implications. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst 40:
103–125
Brown C, Burgess F, Braithwaite VA (2007) Heritable and experiential effects on boldness in a tropical poeciliid. Behav Ecol
Sociobiol 62(2):237–243
Burns JG (2008) The validity of three tests of temperament in
guppies, Poecilia reticulata. J Comp Psychol 122(4):344–356
Careau V, Thomas D, Pelletier F, Turki L, Landry F, Garant D, Réale
D (2011) Genetic correlation between resting metabolic rate and
exploratory behaviour in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus).
J Evol Biol 24(10):2153–2163
Chervet N, Zöttl M, Schürch R, Taborsky M, Heg D (2011)
Repeatability and heritability of behavioural types in a social
Cichlid. Int J Evol Biol, Article ID 321729
Dahlbom SJ, Lagman D, Lundstedt-Enkel K, Sundström FL, Winberg
S (2011) Boldness predicts social status in zebrafish (Danio
rerio). PLoS One 6:e23565
Dahlbom SJ, Backstrom T, Lundstedt-Enkel K et al (2012) Aggression and monoamines: effects of sex and social rank in zebrafish
(Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 228(2):333–338
Dall SRX, Houston AI, McNamara JM (2004) The behavioural
ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an
adaptive perspective. Ecol Lett 7(8):734–739
Desjardins JK, Fernald RD (2010) What do fish think of their mirror
images? Biol Lett 6(6):744–747
Dijkstra PD, Schaafsma SM, Hofmann HA, Groothuis TG (2012)
‘Winner effect’ without winning: unresolved social conflicts
increase the probability of winning a subsequent contest in a
cichlid fish. Physiol Behav 105(2):489–492
Dingemanse NJ, Realé D (2005) Natural selection and animal
personality. Behaviour 142(9–10):1165–1190
Dingemanse N, Both C, Drent PJ, Van Oers K, Van Noordwijk AJ
(2002) Repeatability and heritability of exploratory behaviour in
great tits from the wild. Anim Behav 64(6):929–938
Drent PJ, Van Oers K, Van Noordwijk AJ (2003) Realized heritability
of personalities in the great tit (Parus major). Proc R Soc Lond B
Biol Sci 270(1510):45–51
Dyer JR, Croft DP, Morrell LJ, Krause J (2009) Shoal composition
determines foraging success in the guppy. Behav Ecol 20(1):
165–171
Eising CM, Eikenaar C, Groothuis AGG (2001) Maternal androgens in
black-headed gull (Larus ridibundus) eggs: consequences for chick
development. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 268(1469):839–846
Falconer DS, Mackay TFC (1996) Introduction to quantitative
genetics, 4th edn. Longmans Green, Harlow, Essex, UK
Filby AL, Paull GC, Hickmore TFA, Tyler CR (2010) Unravelling the
neurophysiological basis of aggression in a fish model. BMC
Genomics 11:498
Gerlai R, Lahav M, Guo S, Rosenthal A (2000) Drink like
a fish: zebra fish (Danio rerio) as a behaviour genetic model to
study alcohol effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 67(4):773–
782
Giesing ER, Suski CD, Warner RE, Bell AM (2011) Female
sticklebacks transfer information via eggs: effects of maternal
123
166
experience with predators on offspring. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol
Sci 278(1712):1753–1759
Gil D (2008) Hormones in avian eggs: physiology, ecology and
behavior. Adv Stud Behav 38:337–398
Godin J-GJ, Dugatkin LA (1996) Female mating preference for bold
males in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
93(19):10262–10267
Gosling S (2001) From mice to men: what can we learn about
personality from animal research? Psychol Bull 127(1):45–86
Hadfield J (2010) MCMC Methods for multi-response generalized
linear mixed models: The MCMCglmm R Package. http://cran.
r-project.org/web/packages/MCMCglmm/vignettes/Overview.
pdf
Heath DD, Blouw DM (1998) Are maternal effects in fish adaptive or
merely physiological side effects? In: Mousseau TA, Fox CW
(eds) Maternal effects as adaptation. Oxford University Press,
New York, pp 178–201
Hirschenhauser K, Wittek M, Johnston P, Mostl E (2008) Social
context rather than behavioral output or winning modulates postconflict testosterone responses in Japanese quail (Coturnix
japonica). Physiol Behav 95(3):457–463
Hoffmann AA (1999) Is the heritability for courtship and mating
speed in Drosophila (fruit fly) low? Heredity 82(2):158–162
Jones JS (1987) The heritability of fitness: bad news for good genes?
Trends Ecol Evol 2(2):35–38
Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, Van Reenen
CG, Hopster H, Hopster H, De Jong IC, Ruis MAW, Blokhuis
HJ (1999) Coping styles in animals: current status in behaviour
and stress-physiology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 23(7):925–935
Kruuk LEB (2004) Estimating genetic parameters in natural populations using the ‘animal model’. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
359(1446):873–890
Kruuk LE, Hadfield JD (2007) How to separate genetic and
environmental causes of similarity between relatives. J Evol
Biol 20(5):1890–1903
Larson ET, O’Malley DM, Melloni RH (2006) Aggression and
vasotocin are associated with dominante subordinate relationships in zebrafish. Behav Brain Res 167(1):94–102
Leatherland JF, Li M, Barkataki S (2010) Stressors, glucocorticoids
and ovarian function in teleosts. J Fish Biol 76(1):86–111
Lush JL (1949) Heritability of quantitative characters in farm animals.
Hereditas 35(S1):356–375
McCormick MI (1998) Behaviorally induced maternal stress in a fish
influences progeny quality by a hormonal mechanism. Ecology
79(6):1873–1883
McGlothlin JW, Moore AJ, Wolf JB, Brodie ED III (2010) Interacting
phenotypes and the evolutionary process. III. Social evolution.
Evolution 64(9):2558–2574
Merilä J, Sheldon BC (1999) Genetic architecture of fitness and
nonfitness traits: empirical patterns and development of ideas.
Heredity 83(2):103–109
Moore AJ, Brodie ED III, Wolf JB (1997) Interacting phenotypes and
the evolutionary process. I. Direct and indirect genetic effects of
social interactions. Evolution 51(5):1352–1362
Moretz JA, Martins EP, Robison BD (2007a) Behavioral syndromes
and the evolution of correlated behavior in zebrafish. Behav Ecol
18(3):556–562
Moretz JA, Martins EP, Robison BD (2007b) The effects of early and
adult social environment on zebrafish (Danio rerio) behavior.
Environ Biol Fishes 80(1):91–101
Mousseau TA, Fox CW (1998) The adaptive significance of maternal
effects. Trends Ecol Evol 13(10):403–407
Norton WH, Stumpenhorst K, Faus-Kessler T, Folchert A, Rohner N,
Harris MP, Callebert J, Bally-Cuif L (2011) Modulation of
Fgfr1a signaling in zebrafish reveals a genetic basis for the
aggression-boldness syndrome. J Neurosci 31(39):13796–13807
123
Behav Genet (2013) 43:161–167
Oliveira RF, Carneiro LA, Canário AVM (2005) No hormonal
response in tied fights. Nature 437(7056):207–208
Olivier B, Young LJ (2002) Animal models of aggression. In:
Davis KL, Charney D, Coyle JT, Nemeroff C (eds) Neuropsychopharmacology: the fifth generation of progress. Lippincott
Williams and Wilkins, Philadelphia, pp 1699–1708
Paull GC, Filby AL, Giddins HG, Coe TS, Hamilton PB, Tyler CR
(2010) Dominance hierarchies in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and their
relationship with reproductive success. Zebrafish 7(1):109–117
Qvarnström A, Price TD (2001) Maternal effects, paternal effects and
sexual selection. Trends Ecol Evol 16(2):95–100
R Development Core Team (2011) R: a language and environment for
statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing,
Vienna. http://www.rproject.org
Réale D, Festa-Bianchet M (2003) Predator-induced natural selection
on temperament in bighorn ewes. Anim Behav 65(3):463–470
Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse N (2007)
Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution.
Biol Rev 82(2):291–318
Robison BD, Rowland W (2005) A potential model system for
studying the genetics of domestication: behavioral variation
among wild and domesticated strains of zebra danio (Danio
rerio). Can J Fish Aquat Sci 62(9):2046–2054
Schwabl H (1993) Yolk is a source of maternal testosterone for
developing birds. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90(24):11446–
11450
Sih A, Watters J (2005) The mix matters: behavioural types and group
dynamics in water striders. Behaviour 142(9–10):1417–1922
Sih A, Bell A, Johnson J (2004a) Behavioral syndromes: an ecological
and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol Evol 19(7):372–378
Sih A, Bell A, Johnson J, Ziemba R (2004b) Behavioral syndromes:
an integrative overview. Q Rev Biol 79(3):241–277
Sinn D, Apiolaza L, Moltschaniwskyj N (2006) Heritability and
fitness-related consequences of squid personality traits. J Evol
Biol 19(5):1437–1447
Smith BR, Blumstein DT (2008) Fitness consequences of personality:
a meta-analysis. Behav Ecol 19(2):448–455
Smith BR, Blumstein DT (2010) Behavioral types as predictors of
survival in trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol
21(5):919–926
Spence R, Gerlach G, Lawrence C, Smith C (2008) The behaviour
and ecology of the zebrefish Danio rerio. Biol Rev 83(1):
13–34
Stamps JA (2007) Growth-mortality tradeoffs and ‘personality traits’
in animals. Ecol Lett 10(5):355–363
Stamps JA, Groothuis TGG (2010) Developmental perspectives on
personality: implications for ecological and evolutionary studies
of individual differences. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
365(1560):4029–4041
Stirling DG, Réale D, Roff DA (2002) Selection, structure and the
heritability of behaviour. J Evol Biol 15(2):277–289
Storm JJ, Lima SL (2010) Mothers forewarn offspring about
predators: a transgenerational maternal effect on behaviour.
Am Nat 175(3):382–390
Stratholt ML, Donaldson EM, Liley NR (1997) Stress induced
elevation of plasma cortisol in adult female coho salmon
(Oncorhynchus kisutch), is reflected in egg cortisol content,
but does not appear to affect early development. Aquaculture
158(1–2):141–153
Taylor RW, Boon AK, Dantzer B, Réale D, Humphries MM, Boutin
S, Gorrell JC, Coltman DW, McAdam AG (2012) Low
heritabilities, but genetic and maternal correlations between
red squirrel behaviours. J Evol Biol 25(4):614–624
Van Oers K, Drent PJ, de Jong G, van Noordwijk AJ (2004) Additive
and nonadditive genetic variation in avian personality traits.
Heredity 93(5):496–503
Behav Genet (2013) 43:161–167
Waldmann P (2001) Additive and non-additive genetic architecture of
two different-sized populations of Scabiosa canescens. Heredity
86(Pt6):648–657
Watt PJ, Skinner A, Hale M, Nakagawa S, Burke T (2011) Small
subordinate male advantage in the zebrafish. Ethology 117(11):
1003–1008
Wilson DS, Clark AB, Coleman K, Dearstyne T (1994) Shyness and
boldness in humans and other animals. Trends Ecol Evol
9(11):442–446
Wilson AJ, Gelin U, Perron M-C, Réale D (2009) Indirect genetic
effects and the evolution of aggression in a vertebrate system.
Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 276(1656):533–541
Wilson AJ, Réale D, Clements MN, Morrissey MM, Postma E,
Walling CA, Kruuk LEB, Nussey DH (2010) An ecologist’s
guide to the animal model. J Anim Ecol 79(1):13–26
167
Wisenden BD, Sailer CD, Radenic SJ, Sutrisno R (2011) Maternal
inheritance and exploratory-boldness behavioural syndrome in
zebrafish. Behaviour 148(14):1443–1456
Wolf JB, Brodie ED III, Cheverud JM, Moore AJ, Wade MJ (1998)
Evolutionary consequences of indirect genetic effects. Trends
Ecol Evol 13(2):64–69
Wolf JB, Brodie ED III, Moore AJ (1999) Interacting phenotypes and
the evolutionary process. II. Selection resulting from social
interactions. Am Nat 153(3):254–266
Wright D, Rimmer LB, Pritchard VL, Krause J, Butlin RK (2003)
Inter and intra-population variation in shoaling and boldness in
the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Naturwissenschaften 90(8):374–377
123