European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 40, pp. 2971–2979, 2014
doi:10.1111/ejn.12652
BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE
Performance of compulsive behavior in rats is not a
unitary phenomenon – validation of separate functional
components in compulsive checking behavior
Mark C. Tucci, Anna Dvorkin-Gheva, Eric Johnson, Paul Cheon, Leena Taji, Arnav Agarwal, Jane Foster and
Henry Szechtman
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neurosciences, Health Science Centre, McMaster University, Room 4N82, 1280 Main
Street West, Hamilton, ON, Canada, L8S 4K1
Keywords: 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino) tetralin hydrochloride, analysis and synthesis, animal model, compulsive checking
behavior, nucleus accumbens core lesion
Abstract
A previous analysis of the quinpirole sensitisation rat model of obsessive-compulsive disorder revealed that the behavioral phenotype of compulsive checking consists of three constitutive components – vigor of checking performance, focus on the task of
checking, and satiety following a bout of checking. As confirmation of this analysis, the aim of the present study was to reconstitute, without quinpirole treatment, each of the putative components, with the expectation that these would self-assemble into compulsive checking. To reconstitute vigor and satiety, the employed treatment was a bilateral lesion of the nucleus accumbens core
(NAc), as this treatment was shown previously to exaggerate these components. To reconstitute focus, the employed treatment
was a low dose of the serotonin-1A receptor agonist 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino) tetralin hydrochloride (DPAT) (0.0625 mg/
kg), as high doses of this drug induce compulsive behavior and exacerbate focus. Results showed that injection of DPAT to NAc
lesion rats did yield compulsive checking. Neither the drug alone nor the NAc lesion by itself produced compulsive checking. The
demonstrated synthesis of compulsive checking by the combined treatment of low-dose DPAT and NAc lesion strengthened the
previous fractionation of the model obsessive-compulsive disorder phenotype into three constitutive components, and suggested
a role for serotonin-1A receptors outside the NAc in enhanced focus on the task of checking.
Introduction
There is a general consensus that the neurobiological substrate
underlying obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) stems at least in
part from a dysfunction that results in persistent neural activity
within a series of cascading cortical-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical
loops (Wise & Rapoport, 1989; Saxena et al., 1998; Graybiel &
Rauch, 2000; Stein, 2002; Aouizerate et al., 2004; Huey et al.,
2008; Szechtman et al., 2014), probably involving alterations in
serotonin (5-HT) and dopamine (DA) signaling (Goodman et al.,
1990; Zohar et al., 2000; Westenberg et al., 2007; Nikolaus et al.,
2010). However, relatively little is known about the specific behavioral process, or processes, that is altered in this neurocircuit to yield
OCD, and more generally, whether the OCD phenotype is the manifestation of a unitary functional system or a collection of constitutive components. To address this question, a recent study employed
the quinpirole sensitisation rat model of OCD (Szechtman et al.,
1998; Eilam & Szechtman, 2005), and revealed that quinpiroleinduced compulsive checking behavior is not a unitary phenomenon
Correspondence: Dr H. Szechtman, as above.
E-mail:
[email protected]
Received 20 January 2014, revised 6 May 2014, accepted 8 May 2014
but consists of three relatively independent components, all greatly
exaggerated by quinpirole: (i) vigor of checking; (ii) the focus on
checking; and (iii) rest or ‘satiety’ after a bout of checking (Dvorkin
et al., 2010). This decomposition exposed ‘compulsive’ behavior as
highly motivated performance but without apparent satiation (Dvorkin et al., 2010), and suggested the possibility of a similar explanation for OCD compulsions, theorised to stem from a dysfunction in
‘the sense of task completion’ (Pitman, 1989), ‘just right feeling’
(Leckman et al., 1994; Wahl et al., 2008), ‘feeling of incompleteness’ (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992; Summerfeldt, 2004; Zor et al.,
2011) or ‘feeling of knowing’ (Szechtman & Woody, 2004; Hinds
et al., 2012).
The hypothesis that exaggerated function in three elemental processes produces compulsive checking behavior would be strengthened if this model OCD-like phenotype could be ‘synthesised’
experimentally, without quinpirole treatment (Teitelbaum & Pellis,
1992; Teitelbaum, 2012). That is, if each component process could
be activated specifically by a non-quinpirole treatment, would the
simultaneous activation of all three yield compulsive checking
behavior? The present study addresses this question. It builds on the
reported findings that, in saline-treated rats, an excitotoxic lesion of
the nucleus accumbens core (NAc) increases the vigor of checking
and reduces the rest period after a bout of checking, without
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction
in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
2972 M. C. Tucci et al.
inducing compulsive checking behavior (Dvorkin et al., 2010). That
is, NAc lesion does not increase the concentration on the task of
checking, and affects only two of the three components needed for
compulsive checking. Hence, we hypothesised that a pharmacological treatment that increases the focus on checking would result, in
NAc lesion rats, in compulsive checking behavior. Because of the
body of evidence suggesting a role for 5-HT in OCD (Barr et al.,
1993; Zohar et al., 2000; Aouizerate et al., 2005), we employed as
the pharmacological treatment the selective 5-HT1A receptor agonist,
8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino) tetralin hydrochloride (DPAT). We
injected NAc lesion rats with DPAT and examined whether such
stimulation would increase focus, yielding compulsive checking
behavior. The rationale for the choice of this compound was as
follows.
The possible importance of 5-HT1A receptors in OCD is suggested by findings in two animal tests, the marble-burying test
(Njung’e & Handley, 1991) and DPAT-induced disruption of spontaneous alternation (Yadin et al., 1991). Both of these tests are useful in screening for anti-OCD compounds and have revealed that the
positive effects of 5-HT reuptake inhibitors involve activity at the 5HT1A receptor (Yadin et al., 1991; Ichimaru et al., 1995). Moreover, like quinpirole, high doses of DPAT can induce compulsive
checking behavior (Alkhatib et al., 2013) but low doses of DPAT
only increase the focus on checking, without inducing compulsive
checking (H. Szechtman, unpublished observations). Also of relevance are reports in the literature that attentional focus, which may
be related to the present concept of ‘focus on the task of checking’,
is enhanced in some behavioral tests by injection of DPAT (Winstanley et al., 2003; Carli et al., 2005). Hence, we hypothesised that
low-dose DPAT would increase focus and add to the NAc lesion
effects on vigor and satiety, yielding compulsive checking behavior.
Surgery
The excitotoxin, N-methyl-D-aspartate (Sigma Aldrich), was dissolved in phosphate-buffered saline at a concentration of 12 mg/mL
to produce neurotoxic lesions. For sham lesions, an equivalent volume of phosphate-buffered saline was injected. Intracranial injections of N-methyl-D-aspartate and phosphate-buffered saline were
made using a 10 lL non-coring Hamilton syringe (Hamilton Company, USA) mounted to a motorised Ultra Micro Pump (World Precision Instruments, USA) that was attached to the arm of a
Stereotaxic Apparatus (David Kopf Instruments, USA). Vaporised
isoflurane (Pharmaceutical Partners of Canada, Canada) was used to
anesthetise the animals, and lidocaine hydrochloride (0.002 mg;
Astra Zeneca, Canada) was injected subcutaneously at the surgical
site. The post-operative non-steroidal anti-inflammatory analgesic
Anafen (0.05 mg/kg; Merial, Canada) was administered subcutaneously at 10 min prior to the end of surgery. The coordinates for the
NAc lesion were – Anterior/Posterior, +1.2 mm from bregma;
Medial/Lateral, +1.9; Dorsal/Ventral, 7.0 mm from dura. At the
injection site, 0.3 lL of the solution was injected bilaterally at a rate
of 0.1 lL/min, and the needle was left in place for 5 min to allow
the neurotoxin to sufficiently diffuse away from the needle tip.
Histology
The subjects were 94 experimentally naive adult male Long Evans
rats weighing 250–300 g at the onset of the experiment. Animals
were housed in a climate-controlled rat colony room with a 12 h
light/dark cycle (lights on at 06:00 h, lights off at 18:00 h). Testing
occurred during the light phase. Food and water were freely available. Upon arrival at the animal facility, animals were allowed to
acclimatise to the facility for 7 days, followed by handling for
5 days for approximately 2–5 min each day before receiving stereotaxic surgery. Following surgery, rats received approximately
14 days of recovery and during the last 3 days before the start of
testing, they were handled daily for 2–5 min as before. Animals
were housed and tested in compliance with the regulations set forth
by the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care and
approved by the animal Research Ethics Board of McMaster
University.
After the final test, rats were killed using carbon dioxide. Brains
were removed and flash frozen in 60 °C methylbutane, placed on
dry ice for 1 min, wrapped in aluminum foil, and stored in a
80 °C freezer until sectioning. Brains were mounted for sectioning
using Tissue-Tek Optimum Cutting Temperature compound and
placed in a cryostat for 1 h to thaw to 18 °C. The coronal plane
was sectioned at 12 lm thickness, with approximately every ninth
section collected on a gelatin-coated slide and stored in a 35 °C
freezer until immunohistochemistry. The location and size of lesions
were visualised using neuronal nuclei protein immunohistochemistry; sections were stained using monoclonal mouse anti-neuronal
nuclei (1 mg/mL; Chemicon International, USA) as the primary
antibody, followed by a biotinylated monoclonal anti-mouse IgG
(0.5 mg/mL; Vector Laboratories, Canada) as the secondary antibody according to a previously described procedure (Jongen-Relo &
Feldon, 2002). Following neuronal nuclei staining, each section was
examined for the location and size of the lesion using an Axioskope
microscope and AXIOVISION 4.3 software system (Carl Zeiss Microimaging Inc., USA). Lesion boundaries inside the region of interest
(ROI) were demarcated, areas computed, and expressed as a percentage of the ROI area. To compute the ROI lesion area, brain sections
at (or nearest to) the pre-determined atlas plates (Paxinos & Watson,
1998) were taken (NAc – plates 12, 14 and 16) and the percent of
the ROI lesion at these plates were averaged to obtain the mean percent of the ROI lesion. To be included for behavioral analysis, the
minimum lesion size was 55% of the total ROI, following the lesion
criterion set in Dvorkin et al. (2010).
Drugs
Apparatus
8-Hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino) tetralin hydrochloride (Sigma
Aldrich, USA) was administered at a dose of 0.0625 mg/kg. This dose
was chosen because it increases the focus on checking (H. Szechtman,
unpublished observations) and higher doses induce full-blown
compulsive checking (Alkhatib et al., 2013). The drug was dissolved
in 0.9% physiological saline and administered by a subcutaneous
injection under the nape of the neck at a volume of 1 mL/kg. Control
animals received an equivalent volume of saline in a similar manner.
Animals were tested on a large open field (160 9 160 cm table
without walls) that was located in a non-colony experiment room, as
described previously (Dvorkin et al., 2006b, 2010). The table was
divided virtually into a grid of 25 rectangular places (locales), but
no lines were actually marked on the table surface. Four small Plexiglas/glass boxes (approximately 8 9 8 9 7.5 cm) were located at
the same fixed location on the open field throughout the experiment;
two were located at corners and two were located at places near the
Materials and methods
Animals
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 2971–2979
Synthesis of compulsive checking 2973
center of the open field. After each rat was tested, the table and
objects were wiped clean with a diluted solution of an antibacterial
cleaner (Lysol). The behavior of animals on the open field was videotaped continuously by a camera affixed to the ceiling (providing a
stationary top view of the entire open field and the rat in it). Videotapes were converted to MPEG files (Canopus MPEGPro EMR realtime MPEG-1 MPEG-2 encoder) and these digitised videos were
used to automatically track the trajectories of locomotion using
ETHOVISION 3.1 (Noldus Information Technology, Netherlands) software (Noldus et al., 2001; Spink et al., 2001).
Data analysis
From the digitised video files, ETHOVISION 3.1 software was used to
extract the time series of x, y coordinates of the rat in the open field
(Dvorkin et al., 2006b). To remove noise, digitised tracking data
were pre-processed (by applying appropriate filters to smooth the x,
y coordinates; Hen et al., 2004), and the obtained coordinates were
divided into episodes of forward locomotion (called progression)
and episodes of small movements or immobility (called lingering),
as described previously (Golani et al., 1993; Drai et al., 2000; Drai
& Golani, 2001). The coordinate system was mapped onto the 25
open field locales (places) (Szechtman et al., 1998), and the frequency of visits and duration of stops in each locale were computed
(the terms ‘visit’ and ‘stop’ are equivalent and are used interchangeably). Checking behavior was defined with reference to the mostvisited locale (labeled ‘key place’ or ‘key locale’; these terms are
equivalent), which in most instances was also the locale with the
longest total duration of stops (Eilam & Golani, 1989; Szechtman
et al., 1998). A visit to the key place was also referred to as a
‘check’ or ‘checking’, and the following set of four criteria measures
of checking behavior were computed. (i) Frequency of checking –
total number of visits to the key locale. (ii) Length of check – total
duration of stay at the key locale divided by the frequency of visits
there; this measure was also an indirect index of ritual-like behavior
as the appearance of motor rituals in quinpirole-treated rats is associated with a very short duration of stay in the key locale (Szechtman
et al., 1998; Ben Pazi et al., 2001). (iii) Recurrence time of checking – mean duration of return times to the key locale (‘return time’
is the interval from departure to next arrival at the locale). (iv) Stops
before returning to check – mean number of places visited between
returns to the key locale. Compulsive checking behavior was identified by the presence of a significant difference compared with saline-treated control animals; all four measures needed to differ from
controls for the claim of compulsive checking (Szechtman et al.,
1998), and hence the group of these four measures was termed
‘criteria measures’ for compulsive checking.
The profile of compulsive checking behavior has been dissociated
into a set of functional components in a lesion study (Dvorkin et al.,
2010). Specifically, a lesion to the NAc altered the amount of
checking behavior (as indexed by a change simultaneously in both
the ‘frequency of checking’ and ‘length of check’), whereas a lesion
to the orbital frontal cortex affected the delay between checks of the
key locale (as indexed by a change simultaneously in both the ‘time
to return to check’ and ‘number of stops before returning to check’).
This pattern of results suggested that the functional roles of the NAc
and orbital frontal cortex in checking behavior are to control the
vigor of motor performance and the focus on goal-directed activity,
respectively (Dvorkin et al., 2010). Accordingly, we considered
vigor and focus as two relatively independent components of checking behavior, where a change in the vigor of checking was indexed
by concurrent changes in the ‘frequency of checking’ and ‘length of
check’, and similarly where a change in the focus on checking was
indexed by concurrent changes in the ‘time to return to check’ and
‘number of stops before returning to check’.
In addition to the above criteria measures, the ‘time to next
checking bout’ was also evaluated (Dvorkin et al., 2006b). This
measure is greatly reduced in quinpirole-sensitised rats showing
compulsive checking, and has been proposed to index the third constitutive component of compulsive checking behavior, i.e. ‘satiety’
or rest after checking (Dvorkin et al., 2010). It was reasoned that, in
the animal model, the reduced ‘satiety’ or ‘rest’ after a bout of
checking corresponded to notions that OCD reflects failure to
achieve a ‘sense of task completion’ (Pitman, 1989) or ‘feeling of
knowing’ (Szechtman & Woody, 2004). The computation of checking bouts is detailed in Dvorkin et al. (2006b). Briefly, the method
followed the logic used to identify the clustering of a bout of eating
behavior into a ‘meal’ and the time between meals into a period of
post-ingestion satiety (Tolkamp et al., 1998; Tolkamp & Kyriazakis,
1999). A bout of behavior, according to those authors, is defined on
the basis of the distribution of time intervals between behavioral
events (interevent intervals). This distribution is examined to locate
and extract a time-point that will produce a natural split between
clusters of interevent intervals. Specifically, the identified time-point
will separate the time intervals into a class of (relatively long) intervals that are between the bouts of behavior (interbout intervals) and
a class of (relatively shorter) intervals that belong within a bout of
behavior (intrabout intervals; Tolkamp et al., 1998; Tolkamp & Kyriazakis, 1999). This principle was employed in an algorithm developed to identify bouts of checking behavior and extract the ‘time to
next checking bout’ (Dvorkin et al., 2006b). A rat may complete a
bout of checking but not start the next bout during the session and
hence the number of rats used for the ‘time to next checking bout’
was generally smaller than for the four criteria measures for compulsive checking. Generally, saline-treated rats had 1–2 bouts of checking behavior in a session, whereas quinpirole-treated rats usually
performed two or more bouts (Dvorkin et al., 2006b, 2010). Following the procedure modified in Tucci et al. (2013), if more than one
bout of checking was performed, only the first ‘time to next checking bout’ was used for statistical analysis.
Design and procedure
The study consisted of a 2 9 2 fully crossed factorial design where
one of the between-group factors was Lesion (sham lesion vs. NAc
lesion) and the other was Drug (saline vs. DPAT). Animals were
assigned to treatment groups at 2–3 days before the start of behavioral testing based on body weight.
Following recovery from surgery, testing on the open field began.
Rats were weighed in the colony room and transported to the experiment room containing the open field. Rats were administered their
assigned treatment appropriately, and immediately gently placed
onto the center of the open field. Each trial lasted 55 min. Rats
received a total of four trials, each separated by 2–3 days. After the
final trial, rats were killed and brains were collected as described
above.
Statistical analysis
The research question of the present study was whether two treatments had an additive effect and more specifically whether those
treatments, which on their own did not yield compulsive checking,
did so when combined. Hence, the experiment tested the hypothesis
that the combination of an NAc lesion and a serotonergic agonist
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 2971–2979
2974 M. C. Tucci et al.
(DPAT) together will yield compulsive checking but each treatment
separately will not. A Lesion (sham lesion vs. NAc lesion) by Drug
(saline vs. DPAT) ANOVA was used to evaluate each criterion measure of compulsive checking and the measure of post-checking satiety. Compulsive checking was defined by a significant difference
from saline-treated controls on all criteria measures. It was expected
that NAc lesion would increase vigor, demonstrated by a significant
main effect of lesion on the two vigor-related measures (frequency
of checking and length of check) and no significant effects of lesion
on the two focus-related measures (recurrence time of checking and
stops before returning to check). Similarly, it was expected that
DPAT would increase focus, demonstrated by a significant main
effect of drug for the measures of focus and no significant effects of
drug on the measures of vigor. Accordingly, it was expected that
compulsive checking behavior would be evident only in the NAc
lesion group injected with DPAT because only in this group would
there be an effect on frequency of checking and length of check
(due to a main effect of lesion), and on the recurrence time of
checking and stops before returning to check (due to a main effect
of drug). In addition, it was expected that the NAc lesion group
injected with DPAT would show a reduced time to the next checking bout due to a main effect of lesion. The significance level was
set at P < 0.05. Analysis was computed using SPSS 20 for Windows.
Values presented in graphs are the mean and SEM.
We analysed and present data for the fourth trial only. It was reasoned that this test would be free of any non-specific effects of surgery and should reflect the most stable effects of the drug treatment,
as previous observations from our laboratory suggested robust
effects around this period (Alkhatib et al., 2013). A total of six tests
across four groups had missing data for the fourth trial and for these
rats trial 3 was used instead.
Table 1. Number of rats in each group with proper lesion in the target
ROI, NAc, and the percentage of the NAc area that had a lesion
(mean SEM and the minimum size of a lesion in the group)
Type of lesion
Sham lesion
NAc lesion
Drug
N
Mean (%)
N
Mean SEM (%)
Minimum (%)
Saline
DPAT
37
18
0
0
25
14
73.6 0.0
75.6 0.0
55
66
Results
Histology
Animals that met the criterion of at least 55% lesion to the NAc
were included in the behavioral analysis (Dvorkin et al., 2010). The
number of animals for each treatment condition is shown in Table 1.
The average size of the cell-body lesion across the NAc lesion
groups was approximately 74% of the total ROI (Table 1). Figure 1
shows a representative NAc lesion animal with size of cell-body
destruction similar to the mean across NAc lesion groups. Across
atlas plates 12, 14 and 16 (Paxinos & Watson, 1998), lesions were
well localised within the accumbens core subregion, with minimal
encroachment to the accumbens shell subregion and ventral pallidum
at more posterior sections. Sham lesion rats had no detectable
damage to the NAc and this is indicated by 0% lesion in Table 1.
Routes of travel
An overview of the key finding can be gained from a visual inspection of the routes of travel shown in Fig. 2. The left column displays paths of locomotion of a typical sham lesion rat treated with
saline or DPAT, whereas the right column shows these paths for a
corresponding NAc lesion rat. A visual comparison of the routes
shown by the different groups makes it evident that an injection of
DPAT to the NAc lesion rat produced routes of travel extremely
reminiscent of the paths of travel shown by rats treated with quinpirole (see, e.g. Fig. 5 in Dvorkin et al., 2010). The quantitative attributes of the paths of locomotion shown by quinpirole rats were:
(i) the amount of locomotion was elevated, as measured by the distance traveled; (ii) the spatial extent of the explored space was constricted, as measured by two standard deviational ellipse; and (iii)
the paths of locomotion were restricted to traveling along a few
routes only, as measured by path stereotypy (Eilam et al., 1989;
Szechtman et al., 1994; Dvorkin et al., 2006a,b, 2010). The values
for these measures in the present study are shown in Fig. 3, and
their statistical analysis is presented below.
As shown in Fig. 3a, the amount of locomotion was elevated by
NAc lesion (main effect of lesion – F1,90 = 28.39, P < 0.001,
g2p = 0.240) but not by treatment with DPAT (main effect of drug –
F1,90 = 0.002, P = 0.961, g2p = 0; lesion by drug interaction –
F1,90 = 2.72, P = 0.102, g2p = 0.029). In contrast, as shown in
Fig. 3b, the spatial extent of exploration was reduced by injection of
DPAT (main effect of drug – F1,90 = 27.56, P < 0.001, g2p = 0.234)
and was not affected by NAc lesion (main effect of
lesion – F1,90 = 0.13, P = 0.723, g2p = 0.001; lesion by drug
Fig. 1. A representative neuronal nuclei-stained section for NAc lesion in a rat representative of the average cell-body damage across lesion groups. The left,
middle and right panels represent atlas plates 12, 14 and 16 across the NAc, respectively (Paxinos & Watson, 1998). These plates are located 2.76, 2.28, and
2.04 mm from bregma, respectively. The dashed line demarcates the area of cell damage.
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 2971–2979
Synthesis of compulsive checking 2975
Thus, the results showed that NAc lesions had a predominant
effect on the amount of motor activity, whereas the predominant
effects of DPAT were on a process related to the distribution or the
focus of this activity. Together, these effects yielded a pattern of
locomotor activity that was similar to the locomotor profile of quinpirole-treated rats, suggesting that the quinpirole-like appearance of
the routes of travel in the NAc lesion rat injected with DPAT was
produced by the additive effects of NAc lesion and DPAT.
Additive effects of nucleus accumbens core lesion and
8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino) tetralin hydrochloride on
compulsive checking
Fig. 2. Effects of DPAT on the routes of travel in sham and NAc lesion
rats. The routes of travel are shown as path plots for a representative rat with
a sham lesion (left column) and NAc lesion (right column) that was treated
with saline (Sal) (top row) or DPAT (bottom row); the selected rat has a distance of travel value closest to the group mean. Locomotor trajectories during the entire 55 min session are shown, and each line represents a trajectory
of locomotion; the density of trajectory lines corresponds to the amount of
locomotion. Gray squares indicate locations of the four objects in the
open field.
interaction – F1,90 = 1.15, P = 0.286, g2p = 0.013). Finally, as
shown in Fig. 3c, the restriction of hyper-locomotion to a few
routes, as demonstrated by increased path stereotypy, was produced
by the additive effects of NAc lesion and DPAT (main effect of
lesion – F1,90 = 19.83, P < 0.001, g2p = 0.181; main effect of drug
– F1,90 = 17.89, P < 0.001, g2p = 0.166; lesion by drug interaction
– F1,90 = 0.65, P = 0.421, g2p = 0.007).
Figure 4 displays criteria measures for compulsive checking behavior in groups of rats treated with a lesion of the NAc, an injection
of DPAT, or the combination of the two treatments. As shown, there
was a main effect of lesion for the two measures of vigor (frequency
of checking – F1,90 = 35.80, P < 0.001, g2p = 0.285; length of
check – F1,90 = 21.18, P < 0.001, g2p = 0.190) and for the measure
of rest (time to next checking bout – F1,50 = 9.12, P = 0.004,
g2p = 0.154). For the two measures of focus, there was a significant
but small main effect of lesion for one of the measures (recurrence
time of checking – F1,90 = 8.12, P = 0.005, g2p = 0.083) but no significant main effect of lesion for the other measure of focus (stops
before returning to check – F1,90 = 0.40, P = 0.841, g2p = 0).
As expected, the main effect of drug was significant for the two
measures of focus (recurrence time of checking – F1,90 = 17.11,
P < 0.001, g2p = 0.160; stops before returning to check –
F1,90 = 33.58, P < 0.001, g2p = 0.272). For the two measures of
vigor, there was a significant but small main effect of drug for one
of the measures (frequency of checking – F1,90 = 4.94, P = 0.029,
g2p = 0.052) but no significant main effect of drug for the other
measure of vigor (length of check – F1,90 = 0.92, P = 0.341,
g2p = 0.010). Moreover, as expected, there was no main effect of
drug for the measure of rest (time to next checking bout –
F1,50 = 1.11, P = 0.296, g2p = 0.022).
a
b
c
Fig. 3. Effects of DPAT on distance traveled (a), two standard deviational ellipse (b) and path stereotypy (c) in sham and NAc lesion rats. Bar graphs show
mean performance (+ SEM) in 55 min by each group. Open bars, sham controls injected with saline; hatched bars, sham controls injected with DPAT; gray
bars, NAc lesion rats injected with saline; gray hatched bars, NAc lesion rats injected with DPAT. *Main effect of lesion; #main effect of drug.
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 2971–2979
2976 M. C. Tucci et al.
a
b
c
d
e
Fig. 4. Performance on criteria measures for compulsive checking behavior (a–d) and post-checking rest (e) shown by groups of sham controls and NAc lesion
rats treated with saline or DPAT. Open bars, sham controls injected with saline; hatched bars, sham controls injected with DPAT; gray bars, NAc lesion rats
injected with saline; gray hatched bars, NAc lesion rats injected with DPAT. *Main effect of lesion; #main effect of drug.
There was no significant interaction between lesion and drug for
any of the measures in Fig. 4.
Thus, administration of a low dose of DPAT (0.0625 mg/kg) to
NAc lesion rats produced a statistically significant difference
between this group and sham + saline controls on all criteria measures for compulsive checking (frequency of checking, length of
check, recurrence time of checking, and stops before returning to
check) and for the measure of post-checking satiety (Fig. 4). Hence,
an injection of DPAT to rats with NAc lesion induced full-blown
compulsive checking, and only the combination of these two treatments produced this phenomenon.
Correlations amongst measures of vigor and focus
In a previous study, vigor and focus were identified as two relatively independent components of checking behavior, where a
change in the vigor of checking was evident by concurrent effects
on both ‘frequency of checking’ and ‘length of check’, and similarly where a change in the focus on checking was manifested by
concurrent effects on both the ‘time to return to check’ and ‘number of stops before returning to check’ (Dvorkin et al., 2010). However, in the present study, NAc lesion and DPAT showed not only
the expected impact on the domains of vigor and focus, respectively, but also produced a small secondary change in one of the
measures from the other domain (‘recurrence time for checking’ for
NAc lesion and ‘frequency of checking’ for DPAT; Fig. 4c and a,
respectively).
To shed some light on these findings, Table 2 presents the Pearson correlation coefficients between pairs of the four criteria measures. The shaded cells show the correlation for measures within a
domain and the remaining cells show correlations between vigor
and focus measures. The correlation coefficient for frequency of
checking with length of check was very strong and higher than any
correlation of these measures with a measure of focus, consistent
with the notion that frequency of checking and length of check
reflect vigor. Similarly, for the measures of focus (recurrence time
of checking and number of stops before returning to check), their
correlation coefficient was strong and higher than any correlation
with a measure of vigor, except for one correlation, i.e. recurrence
time of checking with frequency of checking, which was somewhat
stronger than the correlation of the within-focus measures (0.488 vs.
0.410). This latter finding complicated a straightforward interpretation of the pattern of correlations in Table 2. If measures of vigor
and focus indexed independent components, then the two measures
of one domain (e.g. vigor) should correlate with each other more
strongly than with the two measures of the other domain (here, with
measures of focus). Hence, the observation that the magnitude of
the correlation for the two measures of focus did not exceed one of
the four between-domain correlations (recurrence time of checking
with frequency of checking) cast doubt on vigor and focus being
necessarily independent. It suggested instead ‘cross-talk’ between
aspects of the focus and vigor components of compulsive checking.
Table 2. Pearson correlations between pairs of criteria measures of compulsive checking
Length
of check
Frequency of checking
Length of check
Recurrence time of checking
0.752**
Recurrence
time of
checking
0.488**
0.284**
No. of stops
before
returning to
check
0.091
0.105
0.410**
Cells with dark gray color refer to measures within the domain of vigor and
cells with light gray color refer to measures within the domain of focus; nonshaded cells show correlations between these domains. The Pearson correlation coefficient was computed for the four groups combined (N = 94).
**P ≤ 0.01 (two-tailed).
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 2971–2979
Synthesis of compulsive checking 2977
Discussion
Animal models of psychopathology can be useful preparations to
discover the psychological components of a psychiatric disorder and
their underlying neurobiology (Szechtman & Eilam, 2005). An ever
richer armament of neuroscience tools can be employed to fractionate the nervous system and thereby split the apparent unity of a phenomenon into its constitutive behavioral components (Teitelbaum &
Pellis, 1992; Teitelbaum, 2012). However, the soundness of the
findings from such analysis must be confirmed by ‘resynthesis’ (Teitelbaum & Pellis, 1992), i.e. the behavioral components identified in
analysis must be ‘put together’ or synthesised into the original phenomenon to demonstrate the correctness of the analysis. This
approach of ‘analysis followed by synthesis’ underlies the rationale
for the current study where we aimed to synthesise compulsive
checking behavior from the components identified in a previous
analysis (Dvorkin et al., 2010). That analysis of the quinpirole sensitisation rat model of OCD reported that the model compulsive
behavior consisted of at least three partially independent processes,
all greatly exaggerated by quinpirole – vigor of checking performance, focus on the task of checking, and satiety following a bout
of checking (Dvorkin et al., 2010). Here, we showed that, without
quinpirole, compulsive checking behavior is reconstituted from the
simultaneous effects of bilateral lesions to the NAc (which exaggerated predominantly the vigor of checking performance and satiety)
and treatment with DPAT (which increased predominantly the focus
on the task of checking). This successful synthesis of quinpirole-like
compulsive checking behavior by a non-quinpirole method suggests
relatively strongly that the previously identified three constitutive
components do indeed underlie OCD behavior in the quinpirole
sensitisation rat model.
Fig. 4c and a, respectively). There are three possible explanations
for the observed partial ‘spill-over’ into a second domain: (i) the
NAc lesion and DPAT treatments, rather than having effects exclusively on one domain, may also partially impact the other domain;
(ii) ‘recurrence time for checking’ and ‘frequency of checking’,
rather than being exclusive indices of vigor and focus respectively,
may also be open to some modulation by the other domain; or (iii)
some combination of both the foregoing alternatives. Although the
present study does not truly discriminate amongst these alternatives,
there is merit in the second possibility given the relatively high correlation between ‘recurrence time for checking’ and ‘frequency of
checking’, and evidence that another measure, path stereotypy, is
impacted by both vigor and focus. Nevertheless, regardless of which
alternative is correct, none of them discounts the main finding of the
present study, which is that to reconstitute compulsive checking two
distinct concurrent treatments were necessary – a lesion to the NAc
and an injection of DPAT.
Contribution of dopamine and serotonin receptor subtypes to
focus on the task of checking
There are at least three possible neurochemical explanations for the
similarity in effects on focus produced by quinpirole in intact rats
and DPAT in NAc lesion animals.
Relationship between vigor and focus
1. The focus component is controlled by both DA and 5-HT neurotransmission and involves D2/D3 and 5-HT1A receptors, respectively. Accordingly, focus may be heightened either by D2/D3
stimulation by quinpirole or 5-HT1A activation with DPAT.
2. The focus component is controlled by one or both of the above
neurotransmitter systems but quinpirole and DPAT exert their pharmacological activity on the same receptor (D2/D3 or 5-HT1A).
3. The focus component has another unidentified target receptor
activated equally well by quinpirole and DPAT.
The combined effects of NAc lesion and DPAT reconstituted not
only the quinpirole profile on compulsive checking but also on
locomotion; NAc lesion rats injected with DPAT showed more
locomotion compared with saline controls but their routes of travel
were constricted to a smaller portion of the environment and constrained to relatively few pathways (see Figs 2 and 3), like quinpirole-treated rats (Eilam et al., 1989; Szechtman et al., 1994;
Dvorkin et al., 2006a,b, 2010). This locomotor pattern (as measured by distance traveled, two standard deviational ellipse, and
path stereotypy) is presumably another view on the product of
vigor and focus constituting compulsive checking. Accordingly, the
increase in the amount of locomotion produced by NAc lesion is
consistent with this measure reflecting vigor, whereas the shrinkage
in two standard deviational ellipse produced by DPAT is consistent
with this measure being related to focus. Interestingly, path stereotypy was increased by NAc lesion and by DPAT and consequently
this measure was highest in NAc lesion rats injected with DPAT,
the group that showed compulsive checking and quinpirole-like
path plots. These findings provide another type of evidence that
increases in both vigor and focus are required to produce the quinpirole-like pattern of routes associated with compulsive checking.
However, they also suggest that path stereotypy is impacted by
both vigor and focus and that this measure is not an exclusive
index of either domain.
A similar interpretation may apply to the observation that, in the
present study, NAc lesion and DPAT produced a small change in
one of the measures from the other domain (‘recurrence time for
checking’ for NAc lesion and ‘frequency of checking’ for DPAT;
The present study does not discriminate amongst those alternatives
and the literature does not discount any of them either. A similar pharmacological profile for quinpirole and DPAT at D2/D3 receptors
(Smith & Cutts, 1990; van Wijngaarden et al., 1990; Matuszewich
et al., 1999; Newman-Tancredi et al., 1999; Rinken et al., 1999) or
at 5-HT1A receptors (Ahlenius & Larsson, 1997) had been noted in
the literature as a possible explanation for the observed similarity in
functional effects, especially as both drugs are ergot derivatives (Ahlenius & Larsson, 1997) although non-ergot compounds that act as an
agonist at both D2/D3 and 5-HT1A receptors have been recently synthesised (Glennon et al., 2006). However, other studies build on the
rich network of neuroanatomical and neurochemical interconnections
between 5-HT and DA systems (Barnes & Sharp, 1999; Leger et al.,
2001; Fink & G€
othert, 2007; Filip & Bader, 2009; Albert & Le
Francßois, 2010; Hayes & Greenshaw, 2011; Navailles & De Deurwaerdere, 2011) and propose instead that the similarity in function is
the outcome of separate regulations via pre-synaptic and/or post-synaptic D2/D3 and 5-HT1A receptors of the interconnected systems (Ahlenius & Larsson, 1997; Matuszewich et al., 1999; Clement et al.,
2006; Shin et al., 2012), a schema also proposed for the regulation of
the OCD network (Goodman et al., 1990; Zohar et al., 2000; Szechtman & Woody, 2004; Westenberg et al., 2007; Nikolaus et al., 2010;
Alkhatib et al., 2013). Nevertheless, specific experiments are needed
to identify whether the focus on the checking component is truly mediated by both D2/D3 and 5-HT1A receptors or only one of them, or
conceivably yet another target of quinpirole and DPAT.
Regardless of their identity, it is likely that the pertinent receptors
are outside the NAc, given that enhanced focus was produced by
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 2971–2979
2978 M. C. Tucci et al.
DPAT in rats with a lesion of the NAc and the lesion by itself did
not enhance focus. Interestingly, a similar finding was observed in
the spontaneous alternation paradigm, where the effects of DPAT
were enhanced in rats with an electrolytic lesion of the nucleus accumbens, but on its own, the lesion did not affect spontaneous alternations (van Kuyck et al., 2003).
Dopamine and serotonin regulating behavioral components of
obsessive-compulsive disorder behavior
Much research implicates a role for both DA and 5-HT in OCD
(Goodman et al., 1990; Zohar et al., 2000; Westenberg et al.,
2007; Nikolaus et al., 2010). In the quinpirole sensitisation rat
model of OCD, the model compulsive behavior is induced by
repeated injections of quinpirole that result in sensitisation of DA
receptors (Szechtman et al., 1998, 1999). Hence, the model OCD
behavior is driven by sensitised DA activity. However, the present
synthesis suggests that the model OCD phenotype also includes
activity in parts of the 5-HT systems. Specifically, the enhancement of focus with low-dose DPAT suggests that stimulation of 5HT1A receptors outside the NAc may serve to enhance compulsive
behavior. Other studies in the quinpirole model show normalisation
of vigor and post-checking rest with 1-(3-chlorophenyl)-piperazine
hydrochloride (Tucci et al., 2013, 2014). The authors interpret
those studies as suggesting that stimulation of 5-HT2 receptors
may have a dampening effect on DA activity and promote a
reduction of compulsive behavior. Considering the complexity of
5-HT receptors, with pre-synaptic and post-synaptic receptors often
having opposite functional effects on 5-HT activity (Barnes &
Sharp, 1999; Hoyer et al., 2002; Fink & G€othert, 2007; Navailles
& De Deurwaerdere, 2011), it is likely that, depending on the
behavioral component, either an increase or a decrease in 5-HT
activity may promote or ameliorate compulsive behavior. By the
same token, the present findings strongly support the current clinical attempts of targeting simultaneously DA and 5-HT activity as
a viable pharmacotherapeutic strategy for OCD (Greist et al.,
2003; Dougherty et al., 2004; Koran, 2004; Marazziti et al., 2004;
Bloch et al., 2006).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Sean Amodeo, Vanessa Aversa, Justina Ellery, Husein
Gulamhusein, Ashley Kirk, Emma Mauti, Kester Ng, John Peel, and Renee
Sharma for excellent help in running the experiment, and Erik Woody for
advice on statistical analysis. This study was supported by operating grants
to H.S. from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR MOP64424), Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada
(RGPIN A0544) and Ontario Mental Health Foundation, and by an Ontario
Graduate Scholarship award to M.C.T.
Abbreviations
5-HT, serotonin; DA, dopamine; DPAT, 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)
tetralin hydrochloride; NAc, nucleus accumbens core; OCD, obsessivecompulsive disorder; ROI, region of interest.
References
Ahlenius, S. & Larsson, K. (1997) Specific involvement of central 5-HT1A
receptors in the mediation of male rat ejaculatory behavior. Neurochem.
Res., 22, 1065–1070.
Albert, P.R. & Le Francßois, B. (2010) Modifying 5-HT1A receptor gene
expression as a new target for antidepressant therapy. Front. Neurosci., 4, 35.
Alkhatib, A.H., Dvorkin-Gheva, A. & Szechtman, H. (2013) Quinpirole and
8-OH-DPAT induce compulsive checking behavior in male rats by acting
on different functional parts of an OCD neurocircuit. Behav. Pharmacol.,
24, 65–73.
Aouizerate, B., Guehl, D., Cuny, E., Rougier, A., Bioulac, B., Tignol, J. &
Burbaud, P. (2004) Pathophysiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a
necessary link between phenomenology, neuropsychology, imagery and
physiology. Prog. Neurobiol., 72, 195–221.
Aouizerate, B., Guehl, D., Cuny, E., Rougier, A., Burbaud, P., Tignol, J. &
Bioulac, B. (2005) Updated overview of the putative role of the serotoninergic system in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychiatr. Dis.
Treat., 1, 231–243.
Barnes, N.M. & Sharp, T. (1999) A review of central 5-HT receptors and
their function. Neuropharmacology, 38, 1083–1152.
Barr, L.C., Goodman, W.K. & Price, L.H. (1993) The serotonin hypothesis
of obsessive compulsive disorder. Int. Clin. Psychopharm., 8(Suppl 2),
79–82.
Ben Pazi, A., Szechtman, H. & Eilam, D. (2001) The morphogenesis of
motor rituals in rats treated chronically with the dopamine agonist quinpirole. Behav. Neurosci., 115, 1301–1317.
Bloch, M.H., Landeros-Weisenberger, A., Kelmendi, B., Coric, V., Bracken,
M.B. & Leckman, J.F. (2006) A systematic review: antipsychotic augmentation with treatment refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder. Mol. Psychiatr., 11, 622–632.
Carli, M., Baviera, M., Invernizzi, R.W. & Balducci, C. (2005) Dissociable
contribution of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors in the medial prefrontal
cortex to different aspects of executive control such as impulsivity and
compulsive perseveration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 757–767.
Clement, P., Bernabe, J., Kia, H.K., Alexandre, L. & Giuliano, F. (2006)
D2-like receptors mediate the expulsion phase of ejaculation elicited by 8hydroxy-2-(di-N-propylamino)tetralin in rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther.,
316, 830–834.
Dougherty, D.D., Rauch, S.L. & Jenike, M.A. (2004) Pharmacotherapy for
obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Clin. Psychol., 60, 1195–1202.
Drai, D. & Golani, I. (2001) SEE: a tool for the visualization and analysis of
rodent exploratory behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. R., 25, 409–426.
Drai, D., Benjamini, Y. & Golani, I. (2000) Statistical discrimination of natural modes of motion in rat exploratory behavior. J. Neurosci. Meth., 96,
119–131.
Dvorkin, A., Culver, K.E. & Szechtman, H. (2006a) Differential effects of
clorgyline on sensitization to quinpirole in rats tested in small and large
environments. Psychopharmacology, 186, 534–543.
Dvorkin, A., Perreault, M.L. & Szechtman, H. (2006b) Development and
temporal organization of compulsive checking induced by repeated injections of the dopamine agonist quinpirole in an animal model of obsessivecompulsive disorder. Behav. Brain Res., 169, 303–311.
Dvorkin, A., Silva, C., McMurran, T., Bisnaire, L., Foster, J. & Szechtman,
H. (2010) Features of compulsive checking behavior mediated by nucleus
accumbens and orbital frontal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci., 32, 1552–1563.
Eilam, D. & Golani, I. (1989) Home base behavior of rats (Rattus norvegicus) exploring a novel environment. Behav. Brain Res., 34, 199–211.
Eilam, D. & Szechtman, H. (2005) Psychostimulant-induced behavior as an
animal model of obsessive-compulsive disorder: an ethologlocal approach
to the form of compulsive rituals. CNS Spectr., 10, 191–202.
Eilam, D., Golani, I. & Szechtman, H. (1989) D2-agonist quinpirole induces
perseveration of routes and hyperactivity but no perseveration of movements. Brain Res., 490, 255–267.
Filip, M. & Bader, M. (2009) Overview on 5-HT receptors and their role in
physiology and pathology of the central nervous system. Pharmacol. Rep.,
61, 761–777.
Fink, K.B. & G€
othert, M. (2007) 5-HT receptor regulation of neurotransmitter release. Pharmacol. Rev., 59, 360–417.
Glennon, J.C., Van Scharrenburg, G., Ronken, E., Hesselink, M.B., Reinders,
J.-H., Van Der Neut, M., Long, S.K., Feenstra, R.W. & McCreary, A.C.
(2006) In vitro characterization of SLV308 (7-[4-methyl-1-piperazinyl]-2
(3H)-benzoxazolone, monohydrochloride): a novel partial dopamine D2
and D3 receptor agonist and serotonin 5-HT1A receptor agonist. Synapse,
60, 599–608.
Golani, I., Benjamini, Y. & Eilam, D. (1993) Stopping behavior: constraints on exploration in rats (Rattus norvegicus). Behav. Brain Res.,
53, 21–33.
Goodman, W.K., McDougle, C.J., Price, L.H., Riddle, M.A., Pauls, D.L. &
Leckman, J.F. (1990) Beyond the serotonin hypothesis: a role for dopa-
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 2971–2979
Synthesis of compulsive checking 2979
mine in some forms of obsessive compulsive disorder? J. Clin. Psychiat.,
51(Suppl), 36–43; discussion 55–58.
Graybiel, A.M. & Rauch, S.L. (2000) Toward a neurobiology of obsessivecompulsive disorder. Neuron, 28, 343–347.
Greist, J.H., Bandelow, B., Hollander, E., Marazziti, D., Montgomery, S.A.,
Nutt, D.J., Okasha, A., Swinson, R.P. & Zohar, J. (2003) WCA recommendations for the long-term treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder
in adults. CNS Spectr., 8, 7–16.
Hayes, D.J. & Greenshaw, A.J. (2011) 5-HT receptors and reward-related
behaviour: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. R., 35, 1419–1449.
Hen, I., Sakov, A., Kafkafi, N., Golani, I. & Benjamini, Y. (2004) The
dynamics of spatial behavior: how can robust smoothing techniques help?
J. Neurosci. Meth., 133, 161–172.
Hinds, A.L., Woody, E.Z., Van Ameringen, M., Schmidt, L.A. & Szechtman,
H. (2012) When too much is not enough: obsessive-compulsive disorder
as a pathology of stopping, rather than starting. PLoS One, 7, e30586.
Hoyer, D., Hannon, J.P. & Martin, G.R. (2002) Molecular, pharmacological
and functional diversity of 5-HT receptors. Pharmacol. Biochem. Be., 71,
533–554.
Huey, E.D., Zahn, R., Krueger, F., Moll, J., Kapogiannis, D., Wassermann,
E.M. & Grafman, J. (2008) A psychological and neuroanatomical model
of obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Neuropsych. Clin. N., 20, 390–408.
Ichimaru, Y., Egawa, T. & Sawa, A. (1995) 5-HT1A-receptor subtype mediates the effect of fluvoxamine, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, on
marble-burying behavior in mice. Jpn. J. Pharmacol., 68, 65–70.
Jongen-Relo, A.L. & Feldon, J. (2002) Specific neuronal protein: a new tool
for histological evaluation of excitotoxic lesions. Physiol. Behav., 76, 449–
456.
Koran, L.M. (2004) Augmentation strategies for treatment resistant obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin. Neuropsychiatry, 1, 65–71.
van Kuyck, K., Demeulemeester, H., Feys, H., De Weerdt, W., Dewil, M.,
Tousseyn, T., De Sutter, P., Gybels, J., Bogaerts, K., Dom, R. & Nuttin,
B. (2003) Effects of electrical stimulation or lesion in nucleus accumbens
on the behaviour of rats in a T-maze after administration of 8-OH-DPAT
or vehicle. Behav. Brain Res., 140, 165–173.
Leckman, J.F., Grice, D.E., Barr, L.C., de Vries, A.L., Martin, C., Cohen, D.J.,
McDougle, C.J., Goodman, W.K. & Rasmussen, S.A. (1994) Tic-related vs.
non-tic-related obsessive compulsive disorder. Anxiety, 1, 208–215.
Leger, L., Charnay, Y., Hof, P.R., Bouras, C. & Cespuglio, R. (2001) Anatomical distribution of serotonin-containing neurons and axons in the central nervous system of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol., 433, 157–182.
Marazziti, D., Mungai, F., Vivarelli, L. & Dell’Osso, B. (2004) Critical
issues in the pharmacological treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Clin. Neuropsy. J. Treat. Eval., 1, 59–64.
Matuszewich, L., Lorrain, D.S., Trujillo, R., Dominguez, J., Putnam, S.K. &
Hull, E.M. (1999) Partial antagonism of 8-OH-DPAT’S effects on male rat
sexual behavior with a D2, but not a 5-HT1A, antagonist. Brain Res., 820,
55–62.
Navailles, S. & De Deurwaerdere, P. (2011) Presynaptic control of serotonin
on striatal dopamine function. Psychopharmacology, 213, 213–242.
Newman-Tancredi, A., Cussac, D., Audinot, V. & Millan, M.J. (1999)
Actions of roxindole at recombinant human dopamine D2, D3 and D4 and
serotonin 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B and 5-HT1D receptors. N.-S. Arch. Pharmacol., 359, 447–453.
Nikolaus, S., Antke, C., Beu, M. & Muller, H.W. (2010) Cortical GABA,
striatal dopamine and midbrain serotonin as the key players in compulsive
and anxiety disorders – results from in vivo imaging studies. Rev. Neuroscience, 21, 119–139.
Njung’e, K. & Handley, S.L. (1991) Evaluation of marble-burying behavior
as a model of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Be., 38, 63–67.
Noldus, L.P., Spink, A.J. & Tegelenbosch, R.A. (2001) EthoVision: a versatile video tracking system for automation of behavioral experiments.
Behav. Res. Meth. Ins. C., 33, 398–414.
Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. (1998) The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates.
Academic Press, San Diego, CA.
Pitman, R.K. (1989) Animal models of compulsive behavior. Biol. Psychiat.,
26, 189–198.
Rasmussen, S.A. & Eisen, J.L. (1992) The epidemiology and differentialdiagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Clin. Psychiat., 53, 4–10.
Rinken, A., Ferre, S., Terasmaa, A., Owman, C. & Fuxe, K. (1999) Serotonergic agonists behave as partial agonists at the dopamine D2 receptor.
NeuroReport, 10, 493–495.
Saxena, S., Brody, A.L., Schwartz, J.M. & Baxter, L.R. (1998) Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brit.
J. Psychiat. Suppl., 35, 26–37.
Shin, E., Garcia, J., Winkler, C., Bj€
orklund, A. & Carta, M. (2012) Serotonergic and dopaminergic mechanisms in graft-induced dyskinesia in a rat
model of Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol. Dis., 47, 393–406.
Smith, C.F. & Cutts, S. (1990) Dopamine agonist activity of 8-OH-DPAT.
Arch. Int. Pharmacod. T., 306, 106–113.
Spink, A.J., Tegelenbosch, R.A., Buma, M.O. & Noldus, L.P. (2001) The
EthoVision video tracking system – a tool for behavioral phenotyping of
transgenic mice. Physiol. Behav., 73, 731–744.
Stein, D.J. (2002) Obsessive-compulsive disorder. Lancet, 360, 397–405.
Summerfeldt, L.J. (2004) Understanding and treating incompleteness in
obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Clin. Psychol., 60, 1155–1168.
Szechtman, H. & Eilam, D. (2005) Psychiatric models. In Whishaw, I.Q. &
Kolb, B. (Eds), The Behavior of the Laboratory Rat: A Handbook with
Tests. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 462–474.
Szechtman, H. & Woody, E. (2004) Obsessive-compulsive disorder as a disturbance of security motivation. Psychol. Rev., 111, 111–127.
Szechtman, H., Talangbayan, H., Canaran, G., Dai, H. & Eilam, D. (1994)
Dynamics of behavioral sensitization induced by the dopamine agonist
quinpirole and a proposed central energy control mechanism [published
erratum appears in Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 1994 Sep; 116(1), 124].
Psychopharmacology, 115, 95–104.
Szechtman, H., Sulis, W. & Eilam, D. (1998) Quinpirole induces compulsive
checking behavior in rats: a potential animal model of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Behav. Neurosci., 112, 1475–1485.
Szechtman, H., Culver, K. & Eilam, D. (1999) Role of dopamine systems in
obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): implications from a novel psychostimulant-induced animal model. Pol. J. Pharmacol., 51, 55–61.
Szechtman, H., Shivji, S. & Woody, E.Z. (2014) Pathophysiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: insights from normal function and neurotoxic
effects of drugs, infection and brain injury. In Kostrzewa, R.M. (Ed.),
Handbook of Neurotoxicity. Springer, London, pp. 2231–2253.
Teitelbaum, P. (2012) Some useful insights for graduate students beginning
their research in physiological psychology: anecdotes and attitudes. Behav.
Brain Res., 231, 234–249.
Teitelbaum, P. & Pellis, S.M. (1992) Toward a synthetic physiological psychology. Psychol. Sci., 3, 4–20.
Tolkamp, B.J. & Kyriazakis, I. (1999) To split behaviour into bouts, logtransform the intervals. Anim. Behav., 57, 807–817.
Tolkamp, B.J., Allcroft, D.J., Austin, E.J., Nielsen, B.L. & Kyriazakis, I.
(1998) Satiety splits feeding behaviour into bouts. J. Theor. Biol., 194,
235–250.
Tucci, M.C., Dvorkin-Gheva, A., Graham, D., Amodeo, S., Cheon, P., Kirk,
A., Peel, J., Taji, L. & Szechtman, H. (2013) Effects of the serotonergic agonist mCPP on male rats in the quinpirole sensitization model of obsessivecompulsive disorder (OCD). Psychopharmacology, 227, 277–285.
Tucci, M.C., Dvorkin-Gheva, A., Sharma, R., Taji, L., Cheon, P., Peel, J.,
Kirk, A. & Szechtman, H. (2014) Separate mechanisms for development
and performance of compulsive checking in the quinpirole sensitization rat
model of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Psychopharmacology,
doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3505-6. [Epub ahead of print].
Wahl, K., Salkovskis, P.M. & Cotter, I. (2008) ‘I wash until it feels right’ –
the phenomenology of stopping criteria in obsessive-compulsive washing.
J. Anxiety Disord., 22, 143–161.
Westenberg, H.G., Fineberg, N.A. & Denys, D. (2007) Neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: serotonin and beyond. CNS Spectr., 12, 14–27.
van Wijngaarden, I., Tulp, M.T. & Soudijn, W. (1990) The concept of selectivity in 5-HT receptor research. Eur. J. Pharmacol., 188, 301–312.
Winstanley, C.A., Chudasama, Y., Dalley, J.W., Theobald, D.E., Glennon,
J.C. & Robbins, T.W. (2003) Intra-prefrontal 8-OH-DPAT and M100907
improve visuospatial attention and decrease impulsivity on the five-choice
serial reaction time task in rats. Psychopharmacology, 167, 304–314.
Wise, S. & Rapoport, J.L. (1989) Obsessive compulsive disorder – is it a
basal ganglia dysfunction? In Rapoport, J. (Ed.), Obsessive Compulsive
Disorder in Children and Adolescence. American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC, pp. 327–344.
Yadin, E., Friedman, E. & Bridger, W.H. (1991) Spontaneous alternation
behavior: an animal model for obsessive-compulsive disorder? Pharmacol.
Biochem. Be., 40, 311–315.
Zohar, J., Chopra, M., Sasson, Y., Amiaz, R. & Amital, D. (2000) Obsessive
compulsive disorder: serotonin and beyond. World J. Biol. Psychia., 1,
92–100.
Zor, R., Szechtman, H., Hermesh, H., Fineberg, N.A. & Eilam, D. (2011)
Manifestation of incompleteness in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD)
as reduced functionality and extended activity beyond task completion.
PLoS One, 6, e25217.
© 2014 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 2971–2979