International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
ISSN 2250-3153
1
Teaching English as a Second Language = Theory +
Methods + Creativity
Rohini Chandrica Widyalankara
English Language Teaching Unit, University of Kelaniya, Sri Lanka
Abstract- This study proposes that the pedagogical
procedures within the genre of Teaching English as a
Second Language should not only gain from the rich
theoretical resources and the vast repertoire of
methodological approaches available it should also tap the
creative powers of the teaching practitioners. Merging tenets
of Behaviorism with Innatism in the Interaction Theory and
adapting theories on Second Language Acquisition the
second language teaching/learning environment discussed
aims to maximize acquisition and lower the affective filters
of the learners. Principled Eclecticism provides the
methodological foundation for cooperative learning and
formative peer assessment with focused corrective feedback
provide remedial support and an opportunity for the learners
to reevaluate their work and engage in timely adjustments.
The session aims at interpsychological cognitive
development
with
peers/teacher
and
individual
intrapsychological cognitive enhancement in language skills.
Index Terms- Interaction Theory, principled eclecticism,
cooperative peer assessment, focused corrective feedback
I. INTRODUCTION
T
1.1 The interactionist hypothesis
he Interaction Theory of language development by
Vygotsky (1978) is a compromise between the Innatist and
the Behaviorist theories. While Skinner's theory of
Behaviorism[2], states that children learn a language mainly
through repetition, imitation and habit formation. Chomsky
(1959)3
deviating from the tenets of Behaviorism
introduces the Innate Hypothesis. Chomsky (1993: 519) 4
states that ‘Language learning is not really something that
the child does; it is something that happens to the child
placed in an appropriate environment, much as the child’s
body grows and matures in a predetermined way when
provided with appropriate nutrition and environmental
stimulation’. He raises the argument that if children learn
language by imitation ‘why do they say things they have
never heard before’? Introducing the parameters Language
Acquisition Device (LAD) or Universal Grammar (UG)
Chomsky states that the LAD is the ability to discover the
underlying rules of a language system. This device contains
the main rules for all possible human languages and he
called this set of common rules UG which is innate to a
child’s biological endowment. Concurring Lightbown and
Spada (2006: 16)[5] state that the LAD is like an imaginary
‘black box’ existing somewhere in the brain and thought to
contain only the principles which are universal to all natural
languages. Merging tenets of Behaviorism and Innatism the
Interactionist theory recognizes that both environmental and
biological factors are important in language development.
For example interactionists believe that language is a
byproduct of the children's social interactions with more
knowledgeable people in their lives and the innate ability to
acquire language as illustrated below.
Figure 1: Combining Behaviorist and Innate hypotheses to illustrate how a language is acquired
www.ijsrp.org
International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
2
ISSN 2250-3153
Thus interactionism postulates that children acquire
language through their innate language abilities to extract
the rules of the language from their environment and
construct the phonology, semantics, and syntax of their
native language. This innate language ability is the ability to
identify patterns in language, formulate rules about those
patterns, and then apply them to new utterances (Rowe &
Levine, 2006: 235)[6].
1.2 Major themes in the Interactionist hypothesis
i.
According to Vygotsky (1978)[7] social interaction
plays a fundamental role in the process of cognitive
development. ‘Every function in the child’s cultural
development appears twice: first, on the social level,
and later, on the individual level; first, between
people (interpsychological) and then inside the child
(intrapsychological).’
ii.
the adult enabled them to refine their thinking or
their performance to make it more effective.
Agreement comes from Shannon (2005)[9] who states
that the basic concept in interactionism is that children have
some innate knowledge of the structure of language, but also
require meaningful interaction with others.
1.3 From first language acquisition to second language
acquisition
Applying the tenets of interactionism to second
language learning Atherton (2013)[10] states that skill
construction is based on three premises 1. Can do alone 2.
Can do with help 3. Cannot do yet. The ZPD centers around
can do with help. This is not a state where learners stagnate
but a stage prior to Can do alone. The Interactionist theory
was resourceful in revolutionizing the TESL classroom
moving it from a locale where the teacher disseminated
knowledge to a reciprocal knowledge constructing
experience between the teacher and the learners. The
learning contexts considered students to be equal
stakeholders who perform an active part in the process of
learning and take responsibility for achieving the objectives.
The teacher and the learners collaborate in order to help
facilitate meaning construction. The entry of Krashen’s
(1981)[11] five hypotheses shed more light on how second
language learners acquire a new language.
At the interpsychological level the child interacts
with a More Knowledgeable Other (MKO). The
MKO refers to anyone who has a better
understanding or a higher ability level than the
learner, with respect to a particular task, process, or
concept. The MKO could be a teacher or older adult,
but the MKO could also be peers, a younger person.
In the modern world technology especially computers
too can become a MKO.
1.3.1 The Acquisition-Learning distinction
Krashen (1982: 10) [12] states that adults have two
iii. The Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) stretches distinct and independent ways of developing competence in
the child’s ability at solving a problem independently a second language.
and moves the child to perform a task under adult
a) Language acquisition
guidance and/or with peer collaboration. According
i.
This process is similar, if not identical, to the
to Vygotsky, learning occurred in this zone. Within
way children develop first language ability.
the ZPD a child’s linguistic development progresses
ii.
It is a subconscious process as the language
from the current to the unknown. For optimal
acquirers are not usually aware of the fact that
outcome the child should work within the ZPD in
they are acquiring language but are only aware
collaboration with a MKO. Thus much important
of the fact that they are using the language for
learning by the child occurs through social interaction
communication.
with a skillful tutor. Vygotsky refers to this as
iii.
Correct grammar feels right, and errors feel
cooperative or collaborative dialogue. According to
wrong, even if the learners do not consciously
McLeod (2007)[8] ZPD is where the most sensitive
know what rule was violated.
instruction or guidance should be given allowing the
iv.
As it is subconscious acquisition error
child to develop skills they will then use on their own
correction has little or no effect.
in the process of developing higher mental functions.
v.
The acquisition-learning hypothesis claims that
Thus Vygotsky proposes that ZPD is where learners
not only children but adults too can acquire
construct the new language through socially mediated
language by accessing the same natural LAD
interaction.
that children use.
Thus Vygotsky’s observations proved that
i.
Children doing tasks on their own rarely did as well
as when they were working in collaboration with a
More Knowledgeable Other (MKO). The MKO can
be an adult, a teacher or the child's peers.
ii.
Even when the MKO was not teaching them how to
perform the task, the process of engagement with
b) Language Learning
i.
Conscious.
ii.
Error correction supposedly helps the learners
to alter their conscious mental representation of
a rule and learn the right form of the rule.
www.ijsrp.org
International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
3
ISSN 2250-3153
1.3.2 The Input Hypothesis
Krashen (1982: 21)[13] makes the following claim on the
input hypothesis:
Acquisition occurs when one is exposed to language
that is comprehensible and that contains i + 1. i stands for
the acquirer's current level of proficiency. He is able to
move to a higher stage by understanding language
containing i + 1 (where "+1" stands for language which is
Figure 2: Operation of the Affective Filter Krashen
slightly beyond the acquirer's current level of competence).
(1982: 32)
A necessary condition to move from stage i to stage i + 1 is
that the acquirer understands input that contains i + 1, where
Identifying affective variables Krashen (1982: 32)[14]
"understand" means that the acquirer is focused on the states ‘The Affective Filter hypothesis implies that our
meaning and not the form of the message.
pedagogical goals should not only comprise of supplying
comprehensible input, but also creating a situation that
i.
The input hypothesis relates to acquisition, not encourages a low filter.’ He further states that three factors
learning.
regulate the Affective Filter during second language
ii.
We acquire by understanding language that acquisition.
contains structure a beyond our current level of
i.
Motivation: Performers with high motivation
competence (i + 1). This is done with the help of
generally do better.
context or extra-linguistic information.
ii.
Self-confidence: Performers with self-confidence
iii.
When communication is successful, when the input
and a good self-image tend to do better
is understood and there is enough of it, i + 1 will be
iii.
Anxiety: Low anxiety appears to be conducive,
provided automatically.
whether measured as personal or classroom anxiety.
iv.
Production ability emerges. It is not taught directly.
Merging Krashen’s (1982)[15] Input Hypothesis with the
1.3.3 The Affective Filter Hypothesis
Interactionist Theory this study identifies stage i as the Can
The Affective Filter hypothesis states how affective do alone or current knowledge. The move from stage i to
factors relate to the second language acquisition process.
stage i + 1 is to enter the ZPD. At the ZPD the acquirer
cognizes input that contains i + 1 under the guidance of a
MKO. The following figure is an adaption from Atherton
(2013)[16] and illustrates the progression of learning.
Learning
Figure 3: The progression of learning
Combining the progression of learning above with the
second language acquisition model by Krashen (1982) [17]
this study visualizes a second language learner classroom
which aims to teach the present continuous tense.
Comprehensible input in this instance is a lesson plan
to elaborate the present continuous tense. It anticipates
creating a classroom environment based on the Affective
Filter Hypothesis. Thus for optimal function at the ZPD a
low filter is emphasized generating high motivation,
enhancing self-confidence and lowering the anxiety level.
At the ZPD the learners work with the guidance of
More Knowledgeable Others. This guidance indoctrinates
www.ijsrp.org
International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
4
ISSN 2250-3153
Krashen’s (1982: 10)[18] Acquisition-Learning distinction and
the interaction between the MKO/s and in the language
acquirers is based on maximizing acquisition. Thus
prioritizing Acquisition over Learning the classroom
environment endeavors to make production ability emerge
as ‘a subconscious process’. The language acquirers ‘are not
usually aware of the fact that they are acquiring language,
but are only aware of the fact that they are using the
language for communication’ (ibid). But experience denotes
that all learner populations in a language teaching
environment consist of the dichotomy: Good and Weak
learners. As the classroom gives priority to acquisition it is
assumed that the Good learners would ideally be more on a
knowledge acquisition mode and learnt knowledge will be
less. On the other hand the Weak learners are expected to be
more on a conscious learnt knowledge mode and will engage
in extensive monitoring prior to producing output. This
study constructs the following Figure to illustrate the
suggested language teaching/learning environment where
input is in the form of a lesson on present continuous tense
and is diversely processed by Good and Weak learners.
Figure 4: An adaptation of second language acquisition model by Krashen (1982) to illustrate a second language
teaching/learning environment.
Within this language teaching/learning environment to
maximize acquisition the teaching methodology is
Eclecticism but it utilizes methodological approaches
available strictly in a principled manner.
II. THE METHOD: PRINCIPLED ECLECTICISM
Teaching English as a Second Language (TESL) at
present is based on an amalgamation of many diverse
methods. Larsen-Freeman (2000)[19] recognizes that there is
no single acceptable way to go about teaching language
today. Introducing the term Principled Eclecticism she
defines it as a desirable, coherent, pluralistic approach to
language teaching. Eclecticism makes the lesson planner
deviate from reliance upon a single approach to teaching
where the planner is constricted within its limited number of
techniques. Furthermore the students’ performance can
become mechanical and as a result they cannot reap
maximum benefits from the learning. By the inclusion of the
term Principled Larsen-Freeman (ibid) cautions against the
haphazard use of a random mix of multitude methods
available within the genre of TESL.
Within the field of TESL methodology traditional
structural approaches such as Grammar Translation, and
Audio-lingual Method; Communicative Methods as the
Communicative Approach, Total Physical Response, Natural
Approach and modern innovative approaches as the Silent
Way and Suggestopedia all carry strengths as well as
weaknesses (Larsen-Freeman, 2000[20]; Richards and
Rodgers 2001[21]; Wesche and Skehan, 2002[22]) provide
detailed descriptions of these methods and approaches).
Thus principled eclecticism ideally is a mix of structural
approaches with communicative use of language appropriate
to the learner population. Guided by the tenets of Principled
Eclecticism and scaffolding the relevant theory discussed
this study creates a lesson plan where intuition and
innovative ideas aim at developing an instrument
appropriate to the target student population of this study.
III. CREATIVITY: THE LESSON PLAN
Armed with the theory and methodology as a TESL
practitioner I seek to include creativity into a lesson plan
with the aim of recreating real-life social and functional
situations in the classroom to guide students toward
communicative competence.
3.1 Aim and Objectives
Graves (2000)[23] states that goals are general intentions
of teaching and objectives are specific, aims at cognitive,
www.ijsrp.org
International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
5
ISSN 2250-3153
psychomotor and affective domains of learning and link
observable behavior to teaching and assessment. Thus the
3.2 The learners
aim of this session is to make learners use the present
The target population consists of 20 low/intermediate
continuous tense with minimum/no errors in tense or tense proficiency first year undergraduates. Each year
contradiction.
undergraduates place high priority on learning grammar in
the pre-entry needs analysis.
Objectives: At the end of this session students will be
able to:
3.3 The lesson plan (Time frame: one hour)
o
Outline the form of the present continuous
tense
3.3.1 Stage I: Recapping the structures of the present
o
Assemble sentence structures to describe continuous tense (05 minutes)
an ongoing process and construct a peer
The material: The form of present continuous as a grid
composition using the present continuous and a short exercise.
tense
Exposition: The present continuous tense is sometimes
o
Analyze and evaluate peer performance in called the present progressive and is generally used to
the present continuous tense
explain an event that is in progress at the current moment.
o
Justify decisions on peer correction
Here is how to form the present continuous tense in English.
o
Create a story line for an ongoing process
Table 1: Grid depicting the structures of the present continuous tense
Structure of present continuous
Negative
Positive
I'm
not
thinking
of
her.
I'm thinking of her.
You're thinking of her. You're not thinking of her.
not
watching
them.
We're watching them. We're
They're
watching They're not watching them.
them.
not
talking
to
her.
He's talking to her. He's
She's listening to him. She's not listening to him.
It's
interesting/ It's not / it isn't interesting/ irritating.
irritating.
Question
Am I thinking of her?
Are you thinking of her?
Are we watching them?
Are they watching them?
Is he talking to her?
Is she listening to him?
Is it interesting/ irritating?
Activity I:
3.3.1.1 Methodological approach: Stage I
The session aims at utilizing the Principled Eclectic
Method. Key concepts in the Audiolingual Method are
employed to make students recap the rules of the present
continuous tense formation. Drills help students to
differentiate between structures and they are reinforced
inductively. Then expressing the rule, questions are asked to
guide the learners through a dialogue which introduces the
lesson's sentence patterns. In this instance the MKO can be
the teacher or might be cherry picked from among the
learners. This is in
www.ijsrp.org
International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
6
ISSN 2250-3153
agreement with a fundamental methodological principle in
Communicative Language Teaching (CLT) which favors of
a focus on form approach. This approach to explicit
grammar teaching emphasizes a form-meaning connection
followed by elaborating the grammar form taught within
contexts and through communicative tasks. Based on
Grammar Translation Method difficult vocabulary (for
example irritate) is presented with a translation equivalent
as it accelerates comprehension.
3.3.2 Stage II (40 minutes)
The material: This section uses a cartoon created to suit the target population for the purpose of exploration and
elaboration. The instrument, Figure 5 below has 5 frames.
Frame 1
Frame 2
Frame 4
Frame 3
Frame 5
Figure 5: Instrument for communicative activities
Procedure: The cartoon frames are placed outside the
classroom in five different spots which are easy the access.
This creates an information gap. The learners form 4
heterogeneous groups of 5. Seating plan allows for easy to
and fro movement and good eye contact between listeners
and speakers during group work. Additionally the
arrangement provides feasibility for eye contact with the
teacher by slight alteration of the positioning of the chairs
while remaining within the group. The procedure is based on
even participation at the right language level. Thus the task
is designed so that students can complete it successfully with
the language that they have + one step higher with the help
of a MKO who is a group member. Member domination in
discussions is minimized by strategic task allocation. When
necessary, the teacher may interfere to guarantee equal
opportunities for students of different levels.
Instructions: Each member of the group is given a number 15 which allots the respective cartoon frame. They are
allotted roles. For example when group member 1 is the
Runner cum Communicator of Information others (2-5) take
the role of Interrogator, Writer, Editor and Presenter. The
teacher introduces each of the command cards in Figure 6 to
keep the time frame intact during the five cycles of activity
and explains the physical response each card demands. At
the end of each cycle a minimum of 5 sentences should be
constructed using the present continuous tense for the
respective frame. Learners could make other sentence
structures which would help cohesion but they will not
undergo assessment.
The following command cards are explained and used
as non-verbal forms of communication.
www.ijsrp.org
International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
7
ISSN 2250-3153
Figure 6: Command cards
There are 5 cycles for the 5 cartoon frames. The first 4
cycles are of 5 minutes duration while the last cycle for
frame 5 is allotted 10 minutes (total = 30 minutes). The first
Command card states that the Runners cannot talk while
running or observing. During the first cycle for example as
the command states START RUNNING member 1 of each
group becomes the Runner runs to Frame 1 observes the
ongoing action gets back to the group and communicates
information. The Runners try to find answers to questions
such as ‘Who are the people / characters? What are they
doing? What are they thinking? What are they saying to each
other? What happens next?’ Second members of the groups
become Interrogators. After/during discussion the third
member who is the Writer writes the five sentences using the
present continuous tense for the respective frame. Other
sentence structures which would help cohesion too can be
added but will not undergo assessment. The Editors of each
group run to the Frame I and check whether editing/
additional information is required. Each of these actions is
controlled by the Command cards which go up at one minute
durations. But this stage is time flexible. For example the
Editors can run and check the frames while the discussion is
ongoing to clarify information if the Runner is in doubt.
Awareness is created among the learners that time
management, dynamism and soft skills are needed for
optimal output as the activities within each of the first 4
frames end in 5 minutes. The last frame is given 10 minutes
as the content is more complex. The learners are advised to
use the time judiciously to reevaluate the sentences produced
for the first 4 frames.
At the end of the each cycle the presenter of each group
reads the sentences to the class (0.5 minutes x 4 groups x 5
presenters = 10 minutes). Through the presenter the teacher
gets an idea of global errors. During the 5 cycles where the 5
frames are covered the roles of Runner, Interrogator, Writer,
Editor and Presenter move clockwise. At the end of this
activity each group will have constructed 5 frame cards each
with a minimum of 5 sentences containing a present
continuous tense form. Furthermore each member of the
group would have played the roles of Runner, Interrogator,
Writer, Editor and Presenter.
the first language the need for new words is kept minimal.
Though emphasis is on learning to communicate through the
target language the use of the first language is not prohibited
if a student is struggling for a word. For accessing the
English equivalent MKO aid can be sought. A key feature in
the Total Physical Response approach: nonverbal aid for
communication is used during Command card usage where
students react to language in the present continuous
subconsciously. This reduces student anxiety and stress.
Furthermore they interact with props and learn to associate
them with motor activity in the learning environment.
The methodological approach in Stage II adheres to the
principles of CLT too. One of the instructional practices
promoted by CLT is that material should reflect real-life
situations and demands. Thus inventiveness in material
preparation is pedagogically necessary to create meaningful,
comprehensible input. In communicative language
classrooms the focus shifts from teacher-led to studentcentered language application and using a cooperative and
collaborative learning mode is recognized as a strong
facilitator of learning. Information gap creation in the
activities result in each runner communicating information
to the rest of the group who do not know the contents of the
cartoon frame while the others listen, interrogate, discuss
and take turns to write, edit and present. Thus during group
activities the language focus encompasses all four skills:
Listening, Speaking, Reading and Writing. Furthermore
during cooperative activities Weimer (2002) [24] identifies the
following as crucial: Positive interdependence among
learners in respect to resources and task accomplishment;
Face-to-face interaction in small groups; Individual
accountability for participation or internalization of the
relevant knowledge or skills. The methodological approach
used during group activities in Stage II satisfies all three
criteria. Cooperative problem-solving behavior is enhanced
as peer performance is analyzed during intergroup work. If a
problem surfaces during the course of communication
solutions are found as a team.
3.3.3 Stage III: Cooperative peer assessment and focused
corrective feedback
The function of assessment is to check whether the aim
and objectives of the session have been met. According to
3.3.2.1 Methodological approach: Stage II
Stage II of this session too aims at utilizing a principled Biggs (2003)[25] assessment tasks tell us how well students
eclectic method which facilitates the language learning have attained the intended learning outcomes as illustrated
process. As too many unknown vocabulary items will raise below.
the affective filter of the students and will tempt them to use
www.ijsrp.org
International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
8
ISSN 2250-3153
Figure 7: Assessment of intended learning outcomes
Based on the above this session selects Formative
Assessment procedures from within the repertoire of
pedagogical evaluation practices. It offers the information
needed and provides remedial support to the learners so that
timely adjustments can be made. To assess learning
outcomes of a session which aims to teach a grammar form
Sheen (2007)[26] endorses the use of Focused corrective
feedback where errors in the specific grammatical structure
are targeted for identification and correction. Thus
Cooperative peer assessment for this session is a Formative
Assessment where corrective feedback is given by peers.
3.3.3.1 Procedure
Activity (10 minutes): amongst the 4 groups two groups
exchange the constructed 5 frame card sets. Each member
gets one frame. The words Reject, Accept and Track
changes are introduced. The markers assess the sentences
mark Reject/Accept, suggest changes and return the card set
to the owner group. The owner groups Track changes
suggested for the Rejected sentences and Reject/Accept the
changes.
Table 2: A sample frame card after assessment and the grading scheme
The Reject- Reject sentences are written on the board and
each marker justifies the suggested change while the owners of
the frames justify their decisions to Reject the Rejections. The
teacher acts as the MKO and finalizes the correct version and
reconsiders the smiley allotment.
3.3.4 Stage IV: Recapping session (05 minutes)
3.3.5 Stage V: Take-home individual task: Write the story in
the cartoon. Give it a title. (10 marks)
During this stage the learners create a storyline for the
ongoing process in the cartoon frames as an individual Takehome task. This hones intrapsychological cognitive skills as the
final output is a self-creation. As it is a mark allotting task the
creativity in each learner will endeavor to couch the sentences
constructed for each frame within a stylistic whole. This
encourages learners to assess their understanding of the taught
grammar form and fine tune abilities of elaboration. Furthermore
an individual task provides the teacher an opportunity to evaluate
each student’s understanding of the grammar form taught.
IV. SUMMARY STATEMENT
The global aim of the session is to make learners self
discover that the process of learning is not only accumulating
knowledge and skills. Solving communicative problems and
monitoring their own work guide the students along a path of
inquiry and generates new patterns of thinking where the final
outcome is evolving knowledge through self-discovery. Thus the
cognitive development is interpsychological as well as
intrapsychological.
One limitation of the session is the tightness of the time
frame. This can be overcome by dividing the lesson plan into two
sessions by moving the session from Stage III: Cooperative peer
assessment and focused corrective feedback onwards to a followup second session. Such a follow-up lesson could include a song
which is supposedly sung at frame 5. My target is ‘A Whole New
World’ with lyrics given as a handout. The Aladdin or the
Peabo Bryson and Regina Belle version provide opportunity for a
listening task where the learners fill in the blank with continuous
tenses. The Sing-along karaoke version can be downloaded from
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=glLZIya_h_4 and used in the
www.ijsrp.org
International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, Volume 4, Issue 10, October 2014
ISSN 2250-3153
classroom. This makes the learners acquire additional vocabulary
items: Soaring, tumbling, freewheeling, dreaming, thrilling etc.
Furthermore though the lesson plan targets a tertiary level learner
population it could be creatively adapted to any audience.
AUTHORS
First Author – Rohini Chandrica Widyalankara, B. A. (English),
University of Colombo, Sri Lanka; M. Ed. (TESL), University of
9
Manchester, UK; M. Phil. (Linguistics), University of Kelaniya,
Sri Lanka; Diploma in TESP University of Manchester, UK;
Certificate in TESOL Methods, University of Oregon, USA.
Correspondence Author – Rohini Chandrica Widyalankara,
Senior Lecturer, English Language Teaching Unit, University of
Kelaniya, Sri Lanka,
[email protected], 00940776615069/
00940112697820
[7]
[2]
L. S. Vygotsky, Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1978.
B. F. Skinner, Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.
3
N. Chomsky, Review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior, Language, vol. 35, pp. 26–58, 1959.
N. Chomsky, A minimalist program for linguistic theory, in K. Hale & S. J. Keyser, eds, `The view from Building 20', MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 1-52, 1993.
[5]
P. M. Lightbown and N. Spada, How Languages Are Learned. 3rd ed., New York: Oxford University Press, 2006.
[6]
B. M. Rowe, and D. P. Levine, A Concise Introduction to Linguistics. USA: Pearson Education, 2006.
4
[8]
S. A. McLeod, Simply Psychology, 2007. Retrieved August 28, 2014 from: http://www.simplypsychology.org/vygotsky.html
F. Shannon, Interactionist Theory in Second Langauge Acquisition, 2005. Retrieved on September 6, 2014 from:
http://fredshannon.blogspot.com/ 2005/11/interactionist-theory-in-second-language-acquisition.html.
[10] [16]
J. S. Atherton, Learning and Teaching; Constructivism in learning, 2013. Retrieved 15 September 2014
from http://www.learningandteaching.info/learning/constructivism.htm
[11] [12] [13] [14] [15] [17] [18]
S. Krashen, Principles and Practice in Second Language Acquisition. Pergamon Press Inc., 1982.
[19] [20]
D. Larson-Freeman, Techniques and Principles in Language Teaching Oxford University Press, 1986.
[21]
J. C. Richards and T. S. Rodgers, Approaches and Methods in Language Teaching, 2nd ed., Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press, 2001.
[22]
M. B. Wesche, and P. Skehan, Communicative, task-based, and content-based language instruction. In The Oxford handbook of Applied Linguistics, R. B. Kaplan,
ed., Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002, pp. 207-228.
[23]
K. Graves, Designing Language Courses: A Guide for Teachers. Boston: Heinle & Heinle. 2000, pp. 75-79.
[24]
M. Weimer, Learner-Centered Teaching: Five Key Changes to Practice, Jossey-Bass, 2002.
[25]
J. B. Biggs, Teaching for quality learning at university, Buckingham: Open University Press/Society for Research into Higher Education, 2nd ed., 2003.
[26]
Y. Sheen, The effect of focused written corrective feedback and language aptitude on ESL learners’ acquisition of articles, TESOL Quarterly, vol. 41, pp. 255-283,
2007).
[9]
www.ijsrp.org