Advances in Language and Literary Studies
ISSN: 2203-4714
Vol. 6 No. 4; August 2015
Flourishing Creativity & Literacy
Australian International Academic Centre, Australia
Syllable Structure in Rumthawi Arabic
Naser N. AlBzour
Department of English Language and Literature, Al AlBayt University (AABU), Jordan
E-mail:
[email protected]
Doi:10.7575/aiac.alls.v.6n.4p.185
Received: 08/04/2015
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.7575/aiac.alls.v.6n.4p.185
Accepted: 16/06/2015
Abstract
This paper aims at investigating some phonological aspects of syllable structure in Rumthawi Arabic, a Levantine
variety spoken in the northern region of Jordan. It basically sheds light on the OT constraint interaction that determines
the surfacing onsets and codas of syllables in this dialect. The scope of this paper is more specifically confined to
examining the optimal candidates that surface when the definite article morpheme is prefixed. It thus proves that OT
constraints in RA interact in an interestingly distinctive way that triggers divergence and sometimes convergence with
other dialects due to the parametrical ranking of these constraints in this dialect unlike some other dialects. It is hoped
that this humble endeavor will give insight to many interested researchers to deeply investigate various phonological
aspects of this dialect.
Keywords: optimality, syllable structure, onset, coda, epenthesis, constraints, faithfulness, markedness
1. Introduction
As controversial as the way its name is in multiple variation when articulated as [ər.rəm.thə], [Ir.rəm.thə], [Ir.rIm.thə]
and [Ir.rʊm.thə], Rumthawi dialect is distinctively peculiar. Al-Rumtha, the researchers’ beautiful serene hometown, is
a major Jordanian city lying in the utmost northern boarder-line region at the corner point of Huran Plains between
Jordan and Syria. Approximately, ninety thousand people constitute the population of this agricultural area. Many
integrated historical, economic, social and linguistic aspects pour in one crucible that may contribute to marking this
Rumthawi dialect as such among other regional dialects. This markedness can be noticeably recognized at various
levels: lexical, syntactic, semantic and for sure phonological.
The significance of this paper emanates from the fact that most recent phonological studies tend to label Arabic dialects
into broad categories such as Arabic, Egyptian Arabic, Levantine Arabic, Jordanian Arabic ,etc. Broad categories will
definitely engender broad generalizations that might be inaccurately construed and construed. Few studies have tackled
particularly exclusive dialects in their regional and socio-demographic contexts. Many studies have been conducted in
the over-generic mood examining Levantine Arabic, and less generically Abu-Salim’s (1982) Palestinian Arabic, let
alone what appears to be incognizance of the relationship between such dialects as it is evident in Kager’s erroneous
remark (1997, 496), “Into the second class (‘categorial deletion’) fall syncope patterns found in various Arabic dialects
(Levantine and Palestinian Arabic syncope and metrical structure are analyzed in an OT framework in Kager 1995)”.
The claim that ‘Levantine and Palestinian Arabic…’ ignores the very axiom that Palestinian Arabic is one of the
Levantine Arabic varieties in addition to Jordanian, Lebanese and Syrian. This unfortunately reflects that there is a spirit
of ad hoc analysis of these dialects either because the authors investigate ad hoc data or because the data have not been
authentically attested. However, Irsheid-Kenstowicz’s (1984) Bani- Hassan Arabic is one of the first works in
phonology that seriously endeavors to explore a specific Jordanian dialect as such, yet it is carried out and implemented
in terms of segmental and metrical phonology.
2.
Significance and Scope of the study
The present study attempts to study the syllable structure and its role in determining syllabification patterns in
Rumthawi Arabic (henceforth, RA), with respect to some universal constraints. Theoretically this paper is expected to
present tangible evidence in support of the major assumptions whether the syllable is a universally phonological
constituent in a phonological theory or not. It is an attempt to elucidate some aspects of the syllable structure as
elaborately established by various phonologists who have elicited a plethora of issues pertaining to syllabification
constraints such as Maximal Onset Principle and Sonority Hierarchy; phonological changes such as syncope, geminates,
epenthesis; in addition to manifesting syllable internal structure and syllable types. However, the scope of this research
is technically confined to examining the onset and the coda of the syllable in RA. The focus of the analysis will be,
therefore, within a non-linear framework of generative phonology, in general, with particular emphasis on basic
relevant assumptions of optimality theory to fathom the rudimentary structure of RA syllables which might
consequently diverge or converge at certain levels with previous analyses implemented to other vernacular or Standard
Arabic varieties.
186
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
3.
Results and Analysis
Speech sounds do not normally occur in isolation. Rather, they are put together according to certain constraints (some
are language–specific and some are universal) to build up larger units like syllables and words. Words can, therefore, be
subdivided into smaller units called syllables. The syllable has for long been taken as a phonological unit (Selkirk 1982,
Katamba1989, Roca and Johnson 1999, et al). Such studies have shown that in many languages of the world many
phonological features can be properly accounted for by recognizing the syllable as a unit rather than combinations of
segments. The syllable is therefore a linguistically significant unit which must have its place in a phonological theory.
There is evidence that the syllable is a universal phonological constituent. First, the most general and explanatory
statement of phonotactic constraints in language can be made only by reference to the syllable structure. Selkirk (1982)
and Katamba (1989) emphasize that the syllable is the essence of phonological representation, and that it is purely a
phonological entity. Second, many phonological generalizations and rules are best stated in terms of the syllable
structure, or, put differently, cannot be sufficiently expressed without reference to it . By the same token, the syllable
structure plays a pivotal role in the placement of word stress. When stress falls on a syllable, that syllable is referred as
being stressed.
Non-linear phonology, on the other hand, looks at the syllable as a hierarchical unit consisting of onset and rhyme
constituents. By adopting this approach, it has become more possible for linguists to formulate substantial rules for
word stress placement in different languages without resorting to very complicated sequences of segments, even though
those languages may have diverse syllable structures. The distinction between heavy and light syllables (i.e. syllable
weight) is a fundamental property of syllable constituency in non-linear phonology. In a nutshell, the study in hand
investigates the syllable structure of Rumthawi Arabic adopting the non-linear phonology, mainly OT which considers
the syllable (rather than segments) as the basic phonological unit.
Traditionally, O’Connor (1973, 200) defines the syllable as “a unit containing one and only one vowel either alone or
surrounded by consonants in certain numbers and certain arrangements.” It is apparent that O’Connor focuses on the
distributional, phonotactics description of the syllable. This functional definition is language–specific; that is, it’s
peculiar to one language but may not apply to another. After the segmental/linear approach (see McCarthy 1979,
Kiparsky 1979, Selkirk 1980, et al), the hierarchical analysis of the syllable structure rejected the linear representation
of syllables and adopted a hierarchical structure represented by means of a binary tree diagram. The syllable, according
to this hypothesis, is divided into two constituents: onset and rhyme. The onset occupies the pre-nuclear position, while
the rhyme divides into nucleus and coda.
Abu Salim (1982) examined syllable structure and patterns of Palestinian Arabic within the framework of metrical
phonology, yet his analysis seems somehow misleading in some instances because Palestinian Arabic encompasses
various dialects that Abu Salim apparently handled as one without specifically delineating the drastic differences that
might distinguish these dialects according to differences of their syllable structure. On the other hand, Irsheid and
Kenstowitcs (1984) studied Bani Hassan Arabic (BHA), a dialect spoken by a tribe inhabiting the north-eastern regions
of Jordan at vicinity with Rumtha, where RA is, nonetheless, distinctively spoken in a manner that undoubtedly marks
these two dialects significantly different in terms of their syllable structure and syllabification. Their study, of course, is
within the framework of generative phonology not OT, which had not been yet born. The following data from RA and
BHA reflect the fact that there is some divergence between these two adjacent dialects. Such divergence is due to
prioritizing various violable constraints that each dialect differently stratifies in its hierarchy. Such variation rationalizes
different constraint interactions among Arabic varieties as it can be seen in the following data where BHA tends to
epenthesize /?I/ syllable initially unlike RA.
(1)
RA
səħ.bət
TIl.ʕət
ʃIr.bət
Sər.χət
Thər.bət
Næ.dət
ʕIrf.ət
sək.bət
nId.mət
TIl.ʕət
BHA
?Is. ħə.bət
? IT.lə.ʕət
? Iʃ.rʊ.bət
? I S.rʊ.χət
? IT.hrʊ.bət
? I n.dI.hət
? Iʕ.rʊ.fət
? I s.ku.bət
? In.dI.mət
? IT.lə.ʕət
Gloss
‘she pulled’
‘she went out’
‘she drank’
‘she screamed’
‘she hit’
‘she called’
‘she knew’
‘she poured’
‘she regretted’
‘she went out’
The structure of the syllable in Arabic is similar to English Syllable structure in the way that it consists of a nucleus (an
obligatory segment either short or long) and rhyme: onset and coda (only the coda is an optional segment). Arabic onset
differs from English onset in that it is obligatory and always consists of a single consonant; the coda consists of zero,
one, or two consonants . Every vowel in an utterance in Standard Arabic (SA) represents a syllable nucleus. Moreover,
syllabic consonants are not found in SA. Thus, the number of syllables in an utterance is identical to the number of
vowels (Al-Ani and May 1978: 120).
187
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
3.1 Standard Arabic Syllable Types
Phonologists seem to agree that the following five types of syllable occur in Arabic:
1. CV as in / kə.tə.bə / ‘he wrote’
2. CVV as in / kaa. tə. bə / ‘he corresponded’
3. CVC as in / ʕəm / ‘paternal uncle’
4. CVVC as in / sææd / ‘prevailed’
5. CVCC as in / ʕərD / ‘honor’
With respect to length, the syllable in SA can be divided into the following:
(a) Short syllable : CV
(b) Medium syllables : CVV, CVC
(c) Long syllables : CVVCC, CVVC, CVCC
In terms of open/closed distinction, the syllable in SA can be classified into :
a. Open syllables: CV, CVV
b. Closed syllables: CVC, CVVC, CVCC, CVVCC
Regarding frequency of occurrence, the first three types (CV, CVV, CVC) occur much more often than do the last three
(CVVC, CVCC & CVVCC); the short open syllable (CV) being the most frequent of all , and CVVCC being the least
frequent (Al-Ani and May 1978, 118). As for distribution of syllable types, Al-Ani and May (ibid) argue that the first
three patterns (CV, CVV, CVC) occur without restriction, i.e., initially, medially, and finally in an utterance. The last
three types, nevertheless, are not freely distributed. CVCC and CVVC, for instance, occur mainly in final position of
words and utterances in pause. The CVVCC type occurs exclusively syllable-finally in an utterance in pause form. The
six syllable types in SA clearly refers to the fact that SA syllable structure never begins with a vowel, and if it starts
with a vowel, the glottal stop /ʔ/ is consequently epenthesized before that vowel.
3.2 Syllable Structure in RA
At the phonetic level, there are five basic syllable types in RA. These types are exemplified below. The first five types
are used freely in any position within the word, that is, initially medially and finally. The other three types are somehow
frequent only in initial position as exemplified below:
(2)
Distribution of RA Syllable Types: Word-Initially, Medially and Finally
Syllable
Type
CV
Initially
Medially
Finally
/sə .mə/ ‘sky’
CVV
/bei.tu/ ‘his house ’
CVC
/Təm.mən/
‘she reassured ’
/zʊmm.nI/
‘carry me
/ beit.kʊ /
‘your house’
/stə.wa/ ‘he became ripe’/
/stəw.ħəʃ/ ‘he felt lonely/like a beast ’
/ʧlaab.hum/ ‘ their dogs’
/Slaa.xI/‘jerk.(m)’
/bin.Tə.rId/
‘to be expelled’
/ðə.rei.beh/
‘tax’
/ Təm.mən.nə /
‘he reassured us’
/ðə.bəħt.ta/
‘I killed her’
/xab.beit.hə/
‘I hid her’
/Tə.rət.tʊ/
‘I eplelled him’
/səm.maaU/
‘he named him’
/ ðə.ma:n/
‘insurance’
/In.ħə.bəst/
‘I was jailed’
/səb.beit/
‘I cursed’
CVCC
CVVC
CCV
CCVC
CCVVC
CCVV
Optimality Theory is supposedly the mainstream trend in phonological theories that may best explain differences among
dialects. OT, therefore, accounts for such differences in light of setting pertinent violable constraints. Language-specific
rules, within this model, are “attained through the language-specific ranking of the crucially violable constraints, the
substance of which is ideally conceived of as universal”, (Roca and Johnson 1999, 584-585). The Optimal nominee
‘winner’ is thus decided primarily in accordance with satisfying the top ranked constraints unlike ‘losers’, which are
accordingly excluded as a result of fatal or more serious violations. Then, the OT basic assumptions can be best
understood in a continuous competing relationship between two categories of constraints, namely, Faithfulness and
markedness Constraints (Prince and Smolensky,1993). Accordingly, McCarthy (2007b, 266) sums up:
Faithfulness constraints are inherently conservative, requiring the output of the grammar to resemble its
input. Because markedness constraints favor some linguistic structures over others, they are often in
tension with faithfulness constraints, which resist changes to input structures. This tension is called
constraint conflict, and it is resolved in OT by ranking.
McCarthy (Ibid) argues that every syllable in Arabic must have an onset, therefore, [?] is epenthesized whenever ‘it is
needed to ensure that outcome’. So he correctly, yet in less accurate terms concludes that if Onset dominates the
188
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
antiepenthesis faithfulness constraint (and certain other ranking requirements are met), then candidates that resolve
onsetless syllables by epenthesis will be preferred to candidates that preserve them, so /al-wələd/ ‘The boy’-> [?alwələd], *[al-wələd]. He, Furthermore, reiterates the following:
The mapping from underlying /ktub/ to surface [uktub] ‘write!’ in Arabic involves a two-step derivation
in rule-based phonology, with vowel epenthesis creating the context that necessitates [?] epenthesis. In
OT, on the other hand, the grammar compares candidates that may show the simultaneous effects of two
or more epenthesis operations, and [?uktub] is among them. (ibid, 282).
(3)
/ktub/
☞ a. ?uktub
b. ktub
c. uktub
ONSET
*#CC
DEP
**
L
*L
*W
*W
(McCarthy:282)
McCarthy’s analysis in (3) is undubiously neat and elegant, yet it sometimes lacks accuracy to be so generalized to
‘Arabic’, so which Arabic is in question in this given case. If the answer is Standard Arabic, then that would be
incorrect because there are different Standard varieties of Arabic that contradict MacCarthy’s winner /?uktub/- at least
some different readings of the ‘most standard’ versions of almost unanimously agreed upon standard Arabics exist in
the Holy Qur’an (Muslims’ Holy Scripture), where the non-epenthesized version /ktub/ in Warsh Recitation of
Standard Arabic is the optimal choice along with the other Qurashi SA/?uktub/.
More intriguingly, RA and Lebanese Arabic which belong to the same Levantine Arabic, lend themselves rather
differently to this very constraint- though McCarthy (2007 b) treats almost all Levantine Arabic varieties under the
same broad label ‘Levantine’. For instance, the Lebanese optimal choice is /ktoub/ i.e neither initial vowel nor glottal
epentheses, but rather with medial diphthonganization. RA, on the contrary, prefers to have alliance with the ‘standard’
version with epenthesized elements, so the winner is /?ktub/. More interestingly, Lebanese prefers the ‘standard’
mechanism of SA choice /?alwələd/, although the epenthesized vowel is almost the high front vowel /ɪ/, so the optimal
choice is /?ɪlwələd/. However, RA shows sheer divergence with the ‘standard’ choice, so we have the optimal choice
/lwələd/ which disprefers epentheses, yet it realizes the sonority profile. These two examples clearly reflect how each
dialect can be differently deemed susceptible to these constraints in a disparate manner which, therefore, exemplifies a
parametric choice of these dialects based on different hierarchical interaction between these constraints. Examine the
following two rules and consequently the accompanying tableaus that can show such an interaction in RA which is
similar to SA in (4) and discrepant in (5).
RA /ktʊb/
UR
V-epenthesis
[?] -epenthesis
SR
[?ʊk.tʊb]
/ktʊb/
ʊk.tʊb
? ʊk.tʊb
[?ʊk.tʊb]
(4) Tableau
/ktub/
☞ a. ?ʊktʊb
b. ktʊb
c. ʊktub
ONSET
*#CC
DEP
**
L
*L
*W
*W
Now compare this:
RA / el-wə.ləd /
UR
V-epenthesis
[?] -epenthesis
V-deletion
SR
[lwə.ləd]
/ el-wə.ləd /
--------------------lwə.ləd
[lwə.ləd]
(5)Tableau
/el-walad/
☞ a.lwə.ləd
b. elwalad
c. ?alwalad
*ONSET
*W
CC
*
*W
DEP
*
*L
189
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
Syllables universally prefer C- onsets. It is evident, thus far, that RA is no exception, and it lends itself in the same vein
to such a universal constraint, so it solves the problem of onsetless syllables by either a process of epenthesis or
resyllabification. These two processes anticipate the unique interaction between two constraints; the first bans
epenthesis and the second motivates it as it can be formulated below:
a.
b.
DEP-IO: Every segment of the output has a correspondent in the input. ( McCarthy and Prince, 1995)
ONS:Every syllable has an onset. (Prince and Smolensky, 1993)
To substantiate the validity of this argument, sufficient and authentic data should be herein provided. One of the best
examples that may manifest such constraint race is the definite article prefix /el/ in RA, which is equivalent to /al/ in
SA. This prefix surfaces in two different forms when attached to nouns, thus showing a kind of interaction different
from SA and many other vernacular Arabic varieties like Bani Hasan’s. The first case represents the so-called the Moon
Definite Article, where the consonant /l/ of the definite morpheme prefix /el/ or /al/ is retained and surfaces in the
output.
(6) The Moon Definite Article
/el/al+stem/
Ħma:r
ɣra:b
Ktaab
Kra:r
Ħsaab
Ħwa:r
fra:q
qma:r
ɣlaal
ɣba:r
djʕa:r
SA
?əl.ħI.ma:r
?əl.ɣʊ.ra:b
?əl. I.taab
?əl.KI.ra:r
?əl.ĦI.saab
?əl. ĦI.wa:r
?əl.fI.ra:q
?əl.qI.ma:r
?əl.ɣI.laal
?əl.ɣu.ba:r
?əl.djI.ʕa:r
RA
lIħ.ma:r
lIɣ.ra:b
lIk.taab
lIk.ra:r
lIĦ.saab
lIĦ.wa:r
lIf.ra:g
lIg.ma:r
lIɣ.laal
lIɣ.ba:r
lIdj.ʕa:r
BHA
?ə.ləħ.ma:r
?ə.əɣ.ra:b
?ə.lək.taab
?ə.laK.ra:r
?ələĦ.saab
?ə.ləĦ.wa:r
? ə.ləf.ra:g
?ə.ləq.ma:r
?ə.ləɣ.laal
?ə.ləɣ.ba:r
?ə.lədj.ʕa:r
Glosses
The donkey
The raven
The book
The young donkeys
The account/ Maths/ judgment
The young camel
The departure
The gambling
The harvest
The dust
The donkey’s bray
The data given in this table (6) can delimit the basic phonological features that distinguish the onset in RA from the
other two dialects. Examining these words, one can notice the following:
a) All these stems are monosyllabic.
b) All these stems have a CCVVC or CCV:C templates. This means that they are extra-heavy syllables.
c) When the definite prefix is added they all resyllabify.
d) The suffix morpheme has a VC template, and this creates the problem because V-onset syllables are
impermissible.
e) The resulting words in SA and BHA consist of three syllables while in RA they consist of two syllables.
f) The structure of the first syllable in SA is subject to glottal stop insertion to avoid V-onset, so VC+CCVC
àCVC.CV.CVVC. BHA almost operates in the same manner except that the coda consonant re-syllabifies to
serve as an onset to the second epenthetic vowel, so VC+ CCVC àCV.CVC.CVVC
g) Interestingly, RA syllables behave differently, so the initial vowel elides, thus leaving the consonant /l/ to
resyllabify with the next syllable which originally has a consonant onset. To avoid another problem ,and as a
repairing strategy of undesirable output, a vowel is epenthesized medially as a nucleus; the prefix C serves as
an onset; and the first consonant of the first syllable of the stem serves as a coda of the first syllable, so the
output is CVC. CVVC or CVC. CV:C thus realizing MAX-ONS constraint as well:
*COPLEX: Syllables have at most one consonant at edge.
(Archangel and Langendeon.1997)
Hence, we may assume the following constraint hierarchy in RA as represented in the following tableau where the
winner candidate is the one that incurs the least fatal violations although it does violate the faithfulness constraint DEPIO. In addition, it seems that ALIGN (R) plays a significant role in determining the optimal candidate in RA; that is
why candidate (c) wins over candidate (d).
ALIGN (R): Align root morpheme boundaries with syllable boundaries at both edges; hence, *ONSET>>
MAX.ONS-CC >> ALIGN (R) >>DEP-IO.
In RA, furthermore, the markedness constraint SON (Clements, 1990) which stresses the scale of sonority is realized in
(c) unlike (a) and (b). Compare the following three tableaus to realize the difference between constraint interaction in
RA, on the one hand, and the other two varieties, on the other hand.
190
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
(6.a.) Tableau RA:
/el-ktaab/
a. el.ktaab
b. lk.taab
☞c. lIk.taab
d. II.ktaab
(6.b.) Tableau SA:
*C-ONSET
*!
/al-ktaab/
a. al.ktaab
b. lk.taab
c. lIk.taab
d. . lI.ktaab
☞e.?al.kI.taab
(6.c.) Tableau BHA:
*C-ONSET
*!
/al-ktaab/
a. al.ktaab
b. lk.taab
c. lIk.taab
d. . lI.ktaab
☞e.?a.lIk.taab
*C-ONSET
*!
MAX.ONS-CC
**
*!
ALIGN (R)
DEP-IO
*
*
*
*
*
*
MAX.ONS-CC
**
*!
*!
MAX.ONS-CC
*!
*!
*!
ALIGN (R)
DEP-IO
**
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
ALIGN (R)
DEP-IO
**
*
*
*
**
*
*
The second case is manifested in the so-called the Sun Definite Article, where the consonant /l/ of the definite
morpheme prefix /el/ or /al/ is entirely deletes in the surface output.
(7) The Sun Definite Article
/el/al+stem/
Sa.məĦ
sə.ra:b
Tə.laaq
sə.ma:r
zə.raq
sə.waad
də.ma:r
ʃə.ra:b
ʃə.ra:r
ʃu.djaʕ
Sə.fa:r
Sə.da:q
nə.ha:r
Sə.diq
SA
əs.sə.maĦ
?əs. sə.rab
? ə T.Tə.laaq
?as.sa.ma:r
?ə z.zə.raq
?əs. sə.wad
?əd.da.ma:r
?əʃ.ʃə.ra:b
?əʃ.ʃə.ra:r
?əʃ.ʃu.djaʕ
?əS.Sə.fa:r
? s.Sə.da:q
?ən. nə.ha:r
?ə S.Sə.diq
RA
? Is.sə.maĦ
? Is. sə.rab
? IT.Tə.laag
? Is.sə.ma:r
? Iz.zə.raq
? Is. sə.wad
? Id.də.ma:r
? Iʃ.ʃə.ra:b
? Iʃ.ʃə.ra:r
? Iʃ.ʃu.djaʕ
? IS.Sə.fa:r
? Is.Sə.da:q
? In. nə.ha:r
? IS.Sə.diq
BHA
?əs.sə.maĦ
?əs. sə.rab
?əT.Tə.laag
?əs.sə.ma:r
?əz.zə.raq
?əs. sə.wad
?əd.də.ma:r
?əʃ.ʃə.ra:b
?əʃ.ʃə.ra:r
?əʃ.ʃI.djaʕ
?əS.Sə.fa:r
?əS.Sə.da:q
?ən. nə.ha:r
?ə S.Sə.diq
Glosses
The forgiveness
The mirage
The divorce
The bronwishness
The blueishness
The blackishness
The destruction
The juice
The sparkles
The brave
The yellowishness
The dowry
The day/daylight
The friend
Examining these data, one can notice the differences between the behavior of the data in the case of the Moon Definite
Article and the Sun Definite Article, which van be summarized in the following issues:
a) All these stems are disyllabic.
b) All these stems have a CV.CVVC or CV.CV:C templates. This means that the first syllable in each is light
while the second of each is extra-heavy syllables.
c) When the definite prefix is added they all resyllabify.
d) The suffix morpheme has a VC template, and this creates the problem because V-onset syllables are
impermissible.
e) The resulting words in all SA, RA and BHA consist of three syllables with a unified template CV.CV.CVVC
or CV.CV.CV:C , so the last syllable of each word is bimoraic and thus it is the accented one.
f) The structure of the first syllable in all these dialects alike is subject to glottal stop insertion to avoid V-onset,
so VC+CCVC àCVC.CV.CVVC/ CV:C.
(7.a.) Tableau RA:
/el+Sadiq/
a. el.Sə.dig
b ?eS. ə.dig
c. ?e.Sə.dig
☞d.?eS.Sə.diqg
ONSET
*
*!
MAX.ONS-CC
ALIGN (R)
DEP-IO
**
*
*
*
*
191
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
(7.b.) Tableau SA:
/al+Sadiq/
a. al.Sə.diq
b ?aS.ə.diq
c. ?a.Sə.diq
☞d.?əS.Sa.diq
(7.c) Tableau BHA:
ONSET
*
*!
/al+Sadiq/
a. al.Sə.dig
b ?əS.ə.dig
c. ?ə.Sə.dig
☞d.?əS.Sa.dig
ONSET
*
*!
MAX.ONS-CC
MAX.ONS-CC
ALIGN (R)
DEP-IO
**
*
*
*
*
ALIGN (R)
DEP-IO
**
*
*
*
*
The coda in Arabic at large is not prohibited; on the contrary, it is optionally or rather preferably realized. Examining
the data given in (6), one can discern the following interesting phonological processes: in all these dialects the
consonant /l/ deletes; hence, the first syllable would surface without coda. Because Arabic prefers syllables with codas,
gemination is triggered, so the onset consonant of the second syllable is replicated into two: one to serve as an onset and
another to serve as a coda of the preceding syllable. Thus the following tableau shows the interaction between basic
onset and simple coda constraints, where the candidate (e)[.?eʃ.ʃa .ra:b] wins although it violates some other
constraints, but of course less seriously than the other candidates: it violates – COD twice in the second syllable. On the
other hand, (c) does violate – COD once, yet it does not win it violates ALIGN which over-ranks – COD.
COD : A syllable must not have a coda. (Prince and Smolensky.1993)
(8.a.) Tableau RA:
/el+ʃə.rə:b /
a. el.ʃə.ra:b
b.? Iʃ.ə .ra:b
c. ? I.ʃə.ra:b
d. ? Iʃ.ʃər.a:b
☞e.? Iʃ.ʃə.ra:b
*ONSET
*!
*!
ALIGN (R)
*
*
*!
-COD
**
**
*
***
**
DEP-IO
-COD
**
**
*
***
**
DEP-IO
*
*
*
*
(8.b.) Tableau SA:
/al+ʃə.ra:b /
a. əl.ʃə.ra:b
b.?əʃ.ə .ra:b
c. ?ə.ʃə.ra:b
d. ?əʃ.ʃər.a:b
☞e.?əʃ.ʃə.ra:b
*ONSET
*!
*!
ALIGN (R)
*
*
*!
*
*
*
*
(8.c.) Tableau BHA:
/al+ʃə.ra:b /
a. al.ʃə.ra:b
b.?əʃ.ə .ra:b
c. ?ə.ʃə.ra:b
d. ?əʃ.ʃər.a:b
☞e.?əʃ.ʃə.ra:b
*ONSET
*!
*!
ALIGN (R)
*
*
*!
-COD
**
**
*
***
**
DEP-IO
*
*
*
*
We have, thus far examined the data on The Moon Definite Article in (6) and The Sun Definite Article in (7) in isolation
i.e. the definite article prefixed to the stem. Now, what will happen if we study these data in more interacting
environments i.e. when another morpheme precedes the definite article morpheme with the original stem? Would we
have a similar constraint interaction in RA as well as the other two varieties, or would we expect different interaction to
emerge, and thus new forms to surface?
(9) Morpheme+ The Moon Definite Article+ Morpheme
/stem+el/al+stem/
Sawt+el+Ħma:r
Sout+el+ɣra:b
loun+el+Ktaab
SA
Saw.tul.ħI.ma:r
Sou.tul.ɣu.ra:b
lou.nul.kI.taab
RA
Sawt.lIħ.ma:r
Sawt.lIɣ.ra:b
lawn.lIk.taab
BHA
Saw.ta.laħ.ma:r
Saw.ta.laɣ.ra:b
law.na.lak.taab
Glosses
sound of the donkey
Sound of the raven
color of the book
192
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
sbaaq+el+Kra:r
sI.baa.qul.KI.ra:r
sI.ba:g.lIk.ra:r
sI.baa.ga.laK.ra:r
yawm+el+Ħsaab
laĦm+el+Ħwa:r
yaw.mul.ĦI.saab
laĦ.mul. ĦI.wa:r
youm.lIĦ.saab
la.ĦIm. lIĦ.wa:r
you.ma.laĦ.saab
laĦ.ma.laĦ.wa:r
yawm+el+fra:q
laʕb+el+qma:r
sInin+el+ɣlaal
Ħab.bat+el+ɣba:r
Sawt+el+djʕa:r
yaw.mul.fI.ra:q
la.ʕI.bul.qI.ma:r
sI.ni.nul.ɣI.laal
Ħab.ba.tul.ɣu.ba:r
Saw.tul.djI.ʕa:r
yaum.lIf.ra:g
lI.ʕIb. lIg.ma:r
sI.nin.lIɣ.laal
Ħab.bat.lIɣ.ba:r
Sawt.lIdj.ʕa:r
yaw.ma.laf.ra:g
lIʕ.ba.lIq.ma:r
sI.ni.n a.lIɣ.laal
Ħab.ba.ta.laɣ.ba:r
Saw.ta.ladj.ʕa:r
Race of the young
donkeys
The day of judgment
Meat of the young
camel
The day of departure
The gambling game
The harvest years
The dust grains
The donkey’s bray
a)
All these stems preceding the definite article morpheme are monosyllabic or disyllabic. What is significant
about them is the fact that the adjacent syllables preceding the article are all closed syllables.
b) These adjacent syllables, therefore, have a final C or CC. This creates the difference between RA and other
dialects.
c) Hence, when the these stems precede the definite prefix in SA and BHA , they all resyllabify, so the coda of
the preceding syllable serves as an onset to the definite article syllable which has required an epenthetic glottal
stop in (10) to satisfy the ONS constraint.
d) This issue in RA is solved in the same manner it has been solved in (10), so the definite article /el/ deletes the
/e/ and the /l/ serves as a new onset to the following syllable. However, the preceding syllable in the stem
preceding the definite article does not interact with it unlike SA and BHA, so the resulting syllable structure is
CVC+CVC+CVVC/CV:C
e) The resulting phrase in SA and BHA consists of the preceding stem ( whether it is one or two syllables)
followed by three syllables: (stem CV) +CVC+CV.CVVC/ CV:C so the preceding closed syllable becomes an
open syllable.
(9.a.) Tableau RA:
/yawm+el+fra:g/
a. yau.mlIf.ra:g
b. yaum.lI.fra:g
c. yau.mlIf.ra:g
☞d. yaum.lIf.ra:g
*ONSET
MAX.ONS-CC
*!
*!
*!
ALIGN (R)
*
*
*
*
DEP-IO
*
*
*
*
(9.b.) Tableau BHA:
/el-ktaab/
a. yau.mal.If.ra:g
b. yau.mlIf.ra:g
☞c. yaw.ma.laf.ra:g
d. yaum.lIf.ra:g
*ONSET
*!
MAX.ONS-CC
**
*!
(10) Morpheme+The Sun Definite Article + Morpheme
/stem+el/al+stem/
SA
RA
Talab+el+sa.maĦ
Ta.la.bus.sa.maĦ
Ta.la.bIs.sa.maĦ
miɵl+el+sa.ra:b
miɵ.lus.sa.rab
mi.ɵI.lIs.sa.ra:b
waraq+el+Ta.laaq
wa.ra.quT.Ta.laaq wa.ra.qIT.Ta.laag
lawn+el+sa.ma:r
law.nus.sa.ma:r
law.nIs.sa.ma:r
lawn+el+za.ra:q
law.nuz.za.ra:q
law.nIz.za.ra:g
lawn+el+sa.waad
law.nus. sa.wad
law.nIs. sa.wad
sInin+el+da.ma:r
sI.ni.nud.da.ma:r
sI.ni.nId.da.ma:r
taʕm+el+ ʃa.ra:b
Taʕ.muʃ.ʃa.ra:b
Ta.ʕI.mIʃ.ʃa.ra:b
mIɵl+el+ʃa.ra:r
mIɵ.luʃ.ʃa.ra:r
mI.ɵI.lIʃ.ʃa.ra:r
umm+el+ʃu.djaʕ
um.muʃ.ʃu.djaʕ
Um.mIʃ.ʃu.djaʕ
lawn+el+Sa.fa:r
law.nuS.Sa.fa:r
law.nIS.Sa.fa:r
waraq+el+Sa.da:q
wa.ra.qus.Sa.da:q
wa.ra.gIs.Sa.da:q
waqt+el+na.ha:r
waq.tun. na.ha:r
wa.gI.tI n. na.ha:r
raaĦ +el+Sa.diq
raa.ĦaS.Sa.diq
raa.ĦIS.Sa.diq
a)
ALIGN (R)
*
*
*
BHA
Ta.la.bas.sa.maĦ
miɵ.las.sa.ra:b
wa.ra.qaT.Ta.laag
law.nas.sa.ma:r
Law.naz.za.ra:g
law.nas. sa.wad
sI.ni.nad.da.ma:r
Taʕ .maʃ.ʃa.ra:b
mIɵ.laʃ.ʃa.ra:r
um.m aʃ.ʃI .djaʕ
law.n aS.Sa.fa:r
wa.ra.gas.Sa.da:q
wag.tan. na.ha:r
raa.ĦaS.Sa.diq
DEP-IO
*
*
*
*
Glosses
Ask for the forgiveness
Like the mirage
Documents of the divorce
The bronwishness color
The blueishness color
The blackishness color
Years of the destruction
The flavor of juice
Like the sparkles
Mother of the brave
The yellowishness color
Documents of the dowry
Time of the day/daylight
The friend went away
Again, all these stems preceding the definite article morpheme are monosyllabic or disyllabic and what matters
is the fact that the adjacent syllables preceding the article are all closed syllables with a final C or CC.
b) Hence, when the these stems precede the definite prefix in RA,SA and BHA , they all resyllabify, so the coda
of the preceding syllable serves as an onset to the definite article syllable which has required an epenthetic
193
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
c)
glottal stop /?/in (10) to satisfy the ONS constraint; the resulting geminate still behaves in the same was it does
in(16) i.e. branching into an onset of the original syllable and a coda of the previous syllable
The resulting phrase in SA and BHA consists of the preceding stem (whether it is one or two syllables)
followed by three syllables: (stem CV) +CVC+CV.CVVC/ CV:C so the preceding closed syllable becomes an
open syllable.
(10.a.) Tableau RA:
/mIɵl+el+sə.ra:b/
a. mI.ɵIl. Is.sə.ra:b
b.. mIɵ.l Is. ə.ra:b
☞c. mI.ɵI. IIs.sə.ra:b
d.mIɵ. Il. Is.sə.ra:b
e.mI.ɵIl.? Is.sə.ra:b
*ONSET
*!
*!
ALIGN (R)
*
*
*!*!
*
-COD
***
***
**
***
***
DEP-IO
*
*
*
*
***
-COD
***
***
***
***
***
DEP-IO
*
*
*
*
**
(10.b.) Tableau RA:
/mIɵI+el+sə.ra:b/
a. mI.ɵIl.Iə.sa.ra:b
b.. mIɵ.l Is. ə.ra:b
☞c. mIɵ. las.sə.ra:b
d.mIɵ. Il. Is.sə.ra:b
e.mI.ɵIl. ? Is.sə.ra:b
*ONSET
*!
*!
ALIGN (R)
*
**
*!*!
*
4. Conclusion
In conclusion, Arabic varieties show considerable phonological differences among them at various levels. These
differences can be best described and interpreted in terms of what each variety may optimally opt for while preferring a
particular candidate over others in accordance with the different ranking of such violable constraints that each variety
has in its constraint hierarchy. RA is not an exception since its syllable structure proves to behave sometimes in a
similar way, and oftentimes in a different way to syllables in SA and other dialects. These differences logically result in
deletion, insertion and assimilation in various forms. This study is the first research that handles the phonological
structure within any generative approach, let alone OT. Therefore, it is evident that much research is still recommended
in this rich and virgin area of variation and language varieties. This is a modest attempt that might trigger many other
research to follow.
References
Abu-Salim, I. M. (1982). A Reanalysis of some aspects of Arabic phonology: A metrical approach. Doctoral
Dissertation. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois.
AL-Ani, Salman & Darlene R. MAY. (1973): The phonological structure of the syllable in Arabic. American Journal of
Arabic Studies, 1. 113-125.
Archangeli, D. and D. Terence Langendoen. (1997). Optimality theory: An overview. Blackwell Publishing.
Clements, G. N & Elizabeth V. Humes. (1995). The internal organization of speech sounds. In Goldsmith, John A.
(Ed.), The hand Book of phonology theory. Blackwell Publishing. 245-306.
Hayes, Bruce. (1995). Metrical stress theory: Principles and case studies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Irshid, O. (1984). The phonology of Bani Hasan Arabic. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Illinois at
Urbana-Champaign.
Irshied, Omar, & Michael Kenstowicz. (1984). Some phonological rules of Bani-Hassan Arabic. Studies in the
Linguistic Sciences.14 (1),109-48.
Kager, René (1997). Rhythmic vowel deletion in optimality theory, In Roca, Iggy (Ed.), Derivations and constraints in
phonology (463-499). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kager, René. (1999). Optimality theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Katamba, F. (1989). An introduction to phonology. London: Longman
Kenstowicz, M. (1999). Phonology in generative grammar. Blackwell Cambridge MA & Oxford UK.
Kiprasky, Paul (2003). Syllables and moras in Arabic. in Caroline Fery & Ruben Van De Vijver (Eds.). The Syllable in
Optimality Theory (147-83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McCarthy, J. (1979). On stress and syllabification. Linguistic Inquiry. 17. 207-63.
McCarthy, J. & A. Prince (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18: Papers
in Optimality Theory. [ROA #60]. University of Massachusetts.
ALLS 6(4):185-194, 2015
194
McCarthy, J. (2007.a). What is Optimality Theory? Language and Linguistics Compass 1/4: 260–291.
McCarthy, J. (2007. b). Hidden generalizations, phonological opacity in optimality theory. Equinox Publishing.
O'Conor J. D.(1973). Phonetic drill reader. CUP.
Prince, A. & P. Smolensky. (1993). Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. RUCCS-TR-2.
ROA-537.
Roca, Iggy (Ed.), (1997). Derivations and constraints in phonology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Roca, Iggy. and W. Johnson. (1999). Course in phonology. Oxford: Blackwell.