DOI: 10.1002/mar.21310
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Intrasexual female competition and female trust in gay male
sales associates’ recommendations
Eric M. Russell1
|
Hannah K. Bradshaw2
Rebekah Russell‐Bennett
|
Mark S. Rosenbaum3
|
Sarah E. Hill2
|
4
1
Department of Psychology, University of
Texas at Arlington, Arlington, Texas
2
Department of Psychology, Texas Christian
University, Fort Worth, Texas
3
Department of Retailing, University of South
Carolina, Columbia, South Carolina
4
Advertising, Marketing, and Public Relations
Department, Queensland University of
Technology, Brisbane, Queensland,
Australia
Correspondence
Mark S. Rosenbaum, Graham School of
Management, Saint Xavier University, 3825 W
103rd St, Chicago, IL 60655.
Email:
[email protected]
Abstract
These studies draw on evolutionary psychology and intrasexual female competition
to examine why female shoppers often prefer working with gay male (GM) sales
associates over heterosexual female (HF) sales associates. Study 1 finds that female
shoppers often attribute trustworthiness to GM sales associates. Study 2 draws on
theories of intrasexual competition and shows that female shoppers are more likely
to trust product recommendations from a GM sales associate than an HF sales
associate when they feel a sense of competitiveness with the female associate. Study
3 reveals that female shoppers’ trust in GM sales associates is limited to situations in
which they are intending to purchase products that are meant to enhance their
physical appearance. Study 4 extends these findings by showing that women are more
likely to trust GM sales associates (vs. HF sales associates) when the objective of their
purchase is to attract a desirable mate. The findings suggest that retailers should hire
a diverse workforce and consider the role of e‐commerce in helping some female
shoppers avoid potentially uncomfortable situations.
KEYWORDS
evolutionary psychology, gay employees, gay–straight relationships, intrasexual female
competition, retailing, social influence
1 | INTRODUCTION
require assistance with merchandise selections and are uncomfortable working with more physically attractive or younger female
Popular culture and television often depict the presence of gay men
associates (Peretz, 1995).
in the fashion industry and as sales associates in retail settings.
Although research showing positive interactions between GM sales
Indeed, recent retailing investigations reveal that gay male (GM)
associates and female customers in some retail contexts contributes to
sales associates tend to work well with female customers, particu-
the well‐established relationship marketing paradigm (Gummesson,
larly in high‐end retail or service establishments, such as boutiques
2002), the field lacks a clear theoretical understanding of why these
and hair salons (Lawson, 1999; Rosenbaum, Russell‐Bennett, &
relationships transpire in some retailing locales. Evolutionary theory
Drennan, 2015). Research further suggests that female shoppers
may help shed some light on the causes and contexts that influence
show a preference for working with GM sales associates over female
female shoppers to prefer working with GM sales associates over their
sales associates when they believe they are less attractive or are
female counterparts in some retail settings. Emerging research
considerably older than the female sales associates (Rosenbaum,
suggests that human friendships, the way human beings build and
Russell, & Russell‐Bennett, 2017). These findings imply that in certain
maintain alliances with others, and the extent to which they enhance
consumption situations, female customers may put more credibility in
personal happiness through social interactions with others are all
the advice from GM sales associates—most notably when they
innately driven (Lewis, Al‐Shawaf, Russell, & Buss, 2015).
Psychology & Marketing. 2019;1–17.
wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/mar
© 2019 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
|
1
2
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
Evolutionary theory on female intrasexual competition suggests
direct competition for romantic partners (Fisher & Cox, 2011),
that women are innately driven to perceive highly attractive and
research suggests that intrasexual rivalry stemming from physical
feminine women as competition in their efforts to attract or retain a
attractiveness can spill over into other life domains. For example,
male mate (Buss, 1988; Fink, Klappauf, Brewer, & Shackelford, 2014).
women are harsher on attractive (vs. unattractive) female offenders
As a result, women often perceive mating advice from gay men as
(Phillips & Hranek, 2012) and are less likely to hire more attractive
more trustworthy than that from either heterosexual men or women
women (Luxen & Van De Vijver, 2006). This research suggests that
(Russell, DelPriore, Butterfield, & Hill, 2013; Russell, Ta, Lewis,
women’s social interactions with other women are often laden with
Babcock, & Ickes, 2017). Intrasexual competition in service‐intensive
underlying competitive rivalry, leading women to often distrust
retail environments may encourage some female shoppers to refrain
same‐sex others (Fisher, 2017).
from obtaining, to discount, or to reject advice from female sales
Given that women’s interactions with attractive same‐sex others
associates, particularly when they are considering purchasing specific
are often characterized by an underlying thread of competitiveness,
merchandise to attract or retain a heterosexual man (Prendergast, Li,
how might this affect women’s interactions with female sales
& Li, 2014).
associates? Research examining this topic has found that female
This investigation draws on evolutionary models of intrasexual
consumers often feel uncomfortable interacting with some female
competition to understand why female shoppers often demonstrate a
sales associates in retail settings (Wan & Wyer, 2015). For example,
preference for GM sales associates in retail settings. The article
research shows that female cosmetic shoppers, in Hong Kong,
consists of four studies. Study 1 provides descriptive insights into
express a stronger purchase intention when working with male sales
how heterosexual female (HF) shoppers discern GM sales associates
associates over their female counterparts because they believe that
in retail settings and uncovers their feelings of comfort and
male sales associates will value their opinions and work harder
trustworthiness toward working with them.
than female sales associates to enhance their facial appearance
Study 2 expands on the first study by exploring the extent to
(Prendergast et al., 2014). That research suggests that in a cosmetic
which intra‐female competition, in consumption settings, encourages
selling context, female customers may perceive a male sales
HF shoppers to view advice from GM sales associates as more
associate’s advice as being more credible than a female counterpart
trustworthy compared to the same advice provided by HF sales
because from a “Darwinian view of reproduction” males are better
associates. Study 3 builds upon the second study by showing that HF
attuned than females to provide useful judgments about female
shoppers’ preference for a GM sales associate manifests when
beauty (Prendergast et al., 2014).
selecting merchandise meant to enhance their physical appearance.
Extant research supports the idea that women may feel most
Last, Study 4 builds upon the previous by clarifying the types of
comfortable with GM sales associates when purchasing certain
consumption situations and interpersonal characteristics that en-
product classifications, including cosmetics, accessories, apparel, and
courage women to seek the advice of GM sales associates. That is,
shoes. Rosenbaum et al. (2017) show that women prefer working
this study reveals that women perceive more positive intent from
with GM sales associates over female sales associates whom they
and are more likely to trust GM sales associates (vs. HF sales
perceive as being more attractive than themselves when they need
associates) when purchasing apparel to attract or to retain a
assistance with purchasing apparel and accessories; however, this
romantic partner in contrast to attending a family (i.e., nonromantic)
preference diminishes for assistance with other products, such as
event. Overall, by drawing on the tenets of evolution theory, these
housewares.
four studies provide academics and practitioners with a clearer
While this retailing research is insightful, research chasms still
understanding regarding the contexts and conditions that foster
exist in the understanding of the GM sales associate–HF shopper
“commercial friendships” between GM sales associates and their HF
relationship. First, although research shows that heterosexual
customers.
women may prefer working with GM sales associates in certain
situations (Peretz, 1995) or for specific products, such as apparel and
accessories (Rosenbaum et al., 2017), how do female shoppers
2 | B ACK GR O UN D L IT E RA T UR E
identify GM associates in retail settings? That is, to date, marketing
researchers exploring commercial friendships and relationships
Because men put high importance on physical attractiveness in their
between heterosexual women and GM sales associates have failed
mates (Smith, Waldorf, & Trembath, 1990), mating competition
to discern how these relationships often begin or how female
among women tends to occur in the domain of physical attractive-
customers discern GM associates on a retail sales floor.
ness. Women tend to fixate on potential female rivals who possess
Although psychological studies in experimental settings show
physical qualities desired by men, such as facial attractiveness and an
that women discern gay men from available facial, verbal, and
ideal waist‐to‐hip ratio (Fink et al., 2014). When women are in the
nonverbal behavioral cues (Tabak & Zayas, 2012; Valentova &
presence of more attractive same‐sex others (Massar & Buunk,
Havlíček, 2013), knowledge of how this transpires in retail settings is
2010), they may feel competitiveness, distress (Buss, Shackelford,
somewhat lacking. Peretz (1995) discussed how GM sales associates
Choe, Buunk, & Dijkstra, 2000), and jealousy. Although evolutionary
working in a Paris‐based boutique may be discernable because they
theorists generally focus on intrasexual competition as it pertains to
tend to wear cosmetics or to accessorize with feminine scarves.
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
3
Along these lines, Lawson (1999) noted how female beauty salon
Australian city that has a large gay population. The shopping district
customers often discern gay men by the way they walk. Thus, a
is an open‐air, upscale lifestyle center, and contains several high‐end
comprehensive, in‐depth analysis of how heterosexual women
retailers and luxury boutiques (e.g., Louis Vuitton, Chanel, Gucci) and
discern GM sales associates in retail and service settings instead of
popular eateries. Interviews were conducted with a mall‐intercept,
laboratory settings is well warranted. Second, research lacks an
convenience sample (Bush & Hair, 1985) of 23 self‐identified
understanding of the causal conditions and consequences that
heterosexual women who recalled interacting with at least one
support working relationships between GM sales associates and
presumably GM sales associate in a retail setting within the past 90
their HF customers in retail settings.
days. Informants ranged in age from 19 to 56 years (Table 1). Note
that the informants’ recall of working with a GM sales associate was
not corroborated further.
3 | ST UD Y 1
This study required the collection of information, that is, not
normally available or regularly discussed and that may be greatly
Study 1 achieves three objectives. First, although descriptive studies
influenced by political and societal sensitivities, which were
allude to how women attempt to deduce a male sales associate’s
heightened in the sample site from the pending gay marriage
sexual orientation by analyzing physical characteristics, voice tone, or
legislation in the country. Given these environmental conditions,
clothing in retail settings (Lawson, 1999; Peretz, 1995), no study, to
two of the authors trained a female graduate student on how to
date, has explored this phenomenon in depth. Given that prior
conduct semi‐structured interviews with female consumers. This data
research suggests that judgments of sexual orientation can derive
collection strategy has two advantages. First, informants may feel
from relatively limited cues, Study 1 aims to uncover the precise
more comfortable speaking truthfully to a younger female graduate
signals that enable heterosexual women to guess a male sales
student than with university‐affiliated, older researchers, who may
associate’s homosexuality, thus, buttressing the existence of “mar-
trigger a social desirability bias. Second, because the graduate
kers of queer identities” (Clarke & Turner, 2007, p.271), albeit, in
student identified herself as a heterosexual woman who regularly
retail settings.
shops in the community, insider knowledge facilitates expressions of
Second, the humanistic data reveal specific attributes that female
shared experiences, yielding rich, descriptive insights.
customers assign to GM sales associates. On the one hand, many of
these attributes, such as honesty, friendliness, and helpfulness,
represent the bedrock of friendships and thus provide GM sales
T A B L E 1 Demographic profile
Age
Country of birth
Ethnicity
Citizenship
19
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
20
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
20
UK
Caucasian
Britain
subgroup stereotypes into which gay men are often classified into by
20
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
heterosexuals (McCutecheon & Morrson, 2019), suggesting that
20
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
certain stereotypes about gay men remain pervasive in society.
associates with a foundation to build meaningful “commercial
friendships” (Garzaniti, Pearce, & Stanton, 2011; Price & Arnould,
1999) with their female customers. On the other, some of the
attributes, such as feminine and flamboyant, represent two common
20
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
Research shows that customer comfort positively affects the
20
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
success of service delivery as people disclose more information about
21
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
themselves when they feel comfortable, and a high level of comfort
21
Australia
Ingenious/Black
Australia
22
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
22
Australia
Asian
Australia
23
New Zealand
Caucasian
New Zealand
24
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
little empirical or humanistic research examining this in retail and
27
Australia
Caucasian
Australian
service contexts. As such, the third objective of this study is to
33
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
explicate the attributes that female shoppers assign to GM associates
39
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
and examine how these attributes influence their sense of comfort
41
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
and trust in working with them.
42
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
44
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
46
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
55
England
Caucasian
Australia/UK
56
Scotland
Caucasian
New Zealand
56
Australia
Caucasian
Australia
helps customers reduce anxiety and realize enhanced self‐esteem
during service encounters (Spake, Beatty, Brockman, & Crutchfield,
2003). Although retailing researchers have conceptually proposed
that female shoppers may sense empathy, trust, and comfort when
working with GM sales associates (Rosenbaum et al., 2015), there is
3.1 | Sample and procedures
The data for this study came from female informants who were
present in a shopping district located in a large metropolitan
4
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
The interviews followed McCracken’s (1988, p.34) procedures for
3.2 | Attributes related to GM sales associates
conducting long interviews. The interview questions were sufficiently
broad enough to allow the informants to “tell their own story in their
3.2.1 | Honesty
terms.” To probe the study’s two research objectives, prompts were
also used when necessary to guide the interview. The informants
The data reveal that 15 (65%) of the 23 female informants noted that
were asked to recall one of their experiences with an apparent or
they attributed feelings of honesty to GM sales associates. A theme
confirmed GM sales associate. Informants were then asked to discuss
that arose in many of the interviews is that a GM sales associate is
how they discerned that the sales associate was gay and to describe
more likely than either a female or a heterosexual male sales
the sales associate in their own words. Last, each informant was
associate to “tell it like it is.” Other informants noted the word
asked to discuss how the sales associate made her feel during that
“genuine,” when discussing their perceptions of gay sales associates.
service experience.
A 23‐year‐old informant highlighted the honesty and genuineness
First, the interviews were audiotaped and then transcribed into
that nearly two‐thirds of the informants also attributed to gay sales
documents for exporting into NVivo 11 software. Second, the data
associates as follows: “He was honest. There was one piece that I
were coded by means of Strauss and Corbin (1990) axial coding
selected, and he said, “um, no.” He could understand my insecurities
method, which builds a causal‐consequential pathway around a
and what I wanted to get out of a dress. He gave his honest opinion,
phenomenon of interest. In this study, the main interest was
which I appreciated.”
understanding the drives that encourage positive working relationships between GM sales associates and HF customers. The causal
conditions were the attributes of GM associates that female
3.2.2 | Feminine
customers expressed, and the consequences were sensations related
to how they felt during and after the service experience. The action
Eight (35%) informants used the term “feminine” to describe their
strategies that constitute axial coding were the tactics women
GM sales associates. A 39‐year‐old informant noted that her gay
employ to discern a male sales associate’s sexual orientation in a
sales associate was wearing makeup; thus, she perceived him as being
“a feminine male sort of thing” (a “thing” being a new gender
retail setting.
Figure 1 presents the theoretical framework. This framework
classification); however, she then remarked that “the thing” was “a
illustrates the causes, consequences, and actions surrounding the
lovely person, just yeah, he was gay.” The feminization of the gay
relationship between GM sales associates and HF customers in retail
sales associate is not necessarily a hostile response, but rather an
settings. The following subsections delineate the frameworks’
endearing response that allows a sense of comfort to ensue between
conceptual categories.
the gay associate and his female client. The same customer later
FIGURE 1
Conceptual framework: gay male sales associates and heterosexual female customer relationships in retail settings
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
5
noted, “I was very secure and comfortable, and I did trust him.” The
seems to be a win‐win for the sales associate, as the customer said,
classification of gay men into a unique gender category, separate
“So the guy made me feel really … important. Like it wasn’t busy but
from their heterosexual counterparts, seems to encourage female
often in those kinds of environments, you feel a bit pushed around
customers to feel comfort and trust when working with gay sales
like they don’t really care about you but no he … genuinely wanted to
associates.
make me look nice.” Thus, women may perceive the talkative gay man
as an indication of his nonsexual, genuine interest.
3.2.3 | Flamboyant
3.2.5 | Helpful
Five (22%) informants described their GM sales associate as
flamboyant, and four (17%) used the term “excited” to describe their
Two informants described their gay sales associate as “helpful,” and
sales associate. Although “flamboyant” maybe have a negative
two other informants used the term “articulate.” A 22‐year‐old
connotation, GM flamboyancy, and excitedness again seem to trigger
woman remarked that her sales associate “was incredibly helpful.”
a sensation in female customers that separates gay men from
She went on to say, “I could tell that he was very sales motivated but
heterosexual men in a positive manner. A 20‐year‐old female
I was very, very comfortable with him. I thought … he knew the
described her gay sales associate as follows: “He was very
product very well actually.” Thus, responsiveness yields feelings of
flamboyant, very over the top, and his knowledge in fashion at times
comfort and, ultimately, sales.
exceeded my own. I would expect a heterosexual male to act a lot
Two women described their sales associates as being articulate in
more subdued.” The same informant later noted, “I definitely trusted
terms of physical demeanor or vocabulary. These informants noted
his advice and felt very comfortable in what he was choosing.” Thus,
that heterosexual men lack the same articulation as gay men. A
by acting in a manner that is opposite that of a heterosexual man, the
41‐year‐old informant stated, “He sort of spoke quite articulately and
GM sales associate is able to engender trust, comfort, and credibility
that doesn’t always obviously mean it [that he is gay] …but it’s just
when working with female customers.
different than what I would expect from a heterosexual male.” This
The display of ebullience seems to set the gay man apart
difference seems to serve both the GM sales associate and the
from his heterosexual counterpart. A 55‐year‐old informant noted
customer, as she further said, “I definitely didn’t think he was sales
that her gay sales associate was excited about her dress: “I’d
motivated or pushy. I actually found him quite comfortable to deal
expect a gay male to be excited about fashion and not a
with, he was very down to earth and very … informative and I kind of
[heterosexual] male, who would not be excited.” Again, when
trusted his judgment a lot on the types of things he was showing me.”
heterosexual women are able to surmise that a male sales associate
is homosexual, a sense of trust between them and the employee
emerges. Indeed, the same client remarked, “Yeah, I think he was
3.3 | Discerning GM sales associates
definitely more interested in what I was purchasing rather than um …
just taking your money, which is good.” Thus, female clients tend to
3.3.1 | Gestures
perceive gay sales associates who display “zing” on the sales floor as
trustworthy.
When asked how they recognize or discern GM sales associates, 13
(57%) of the 23 informants noted that they judge the way these men
gesture with their hands. A 56‐year‐old informant discussed how she
3.2.4 | Friendly
honed in on hand gestures to guess homosexuality: “Oh, the way he
moved his hands and the way he stood and the way he laughed and
Three (13%) informants described their GM associate as being
um … yes he was definitely gay. He wasn’t trying to hide it either and
friendly, and two (9%) used the term “chatty” to describe their
he wasn’t trying to flaunt it, he just was [himself].”
associate. Friendliness seems to evoke feelings of service quality in
female customers, which helps the gay associate “close the sale.” A
44‐year‐old woman purchasing cosmetics noted, “The experience
3.3.2 | Physical
was great, so the person [gay associate] was very knowledgeable
about the products; very friendly; knew what he was talking about.”
Eleven (48%) informants noted that they appraise a male sales
The sales associate’s friendly demeanor served to assure the
associate’s physical appearance, apparel selection, and personal
customer, as she said, “I just found it would be odd because I wasn’t
grooming to judge his homosexuality. Another 56‐year‐old informant
expecting a male to be behind that counter.”
said, “His appearance was very neat … yeah very well dressed, very
Another informant described her gay sales associate at a
neat. Not in the manner of a heterosexual male.” Similarly, a 39‐year‐
cosmetic counter as “chatty.” The informant remarked that the sales
old woman noted how she discerned that her sales associate was gay:
associate “was asking me about what fake tan I use, telling me that
“He was quite slim, very neatly dressed, very short sort of manicured
his sister gets that fake tan in Australia rah, rah.” Yet this banter
hair and very clean.” Thus, qualities such as being neatly dressed,
6
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
tidy, manicured, and clean seem to be identifying characteristics that
confidence, the way he walked; the way he presented himself; his
help women feel at ease with GM sales associates.
dress, his tone of voice even, it just fit the fact that he was a gay
person pretty much.”
3.3.3 | Voice
3.3.6 | Conclusion
Eight (35%) informants noted that they determine a male sales
associate’s sexual orientation from listening to his tone of voice. A
On the one hand, the findings suggest that heterosexual women
21‐year‐old informant noted that she uses several signals to
discern GM sales associates primarily by looking for attributes that
determine an associate’s homosexuality: “I don’t know how to
are part and parcel of existing gay stereotypes, which often result in
explain this … but you know how they [gay men] talk sort of like in
gay men being viewed by others as less masculine than heterosexual
a higher pitch and it’s … like just his voice in general and just like the
men (Blashill & Powlishta, 2009). On the other hand, this study
manner in which he was like talking. It was like the language he was
provides a first attempt in the retailing domain to thoroughly classify
using and the tone but also physical characteristics.”
characteristics that heterosexual women use to discern, whether
correctly or incorrectly, GM sale associates in retail contexts, thereby
buttressing key works in the relationship marketing paradigm
3.3.4 | Cosmetics
(Peretz, 1995; Rosenbaum et al., 2015, 2017).
Four (17%) informants deduced their male sales associate’s homosexuality from his use of cosmetics. A 20‐year‐old woman discussed
3.4 | Reactions
how she noted hand gestures, voice, and the presence of cosmetics to
surmise her male sales associate’s homosexuality: “Because of his
3.4.1 | Comfort
hand mannerisms, his voice; he was wearing makeup himself, not
over‐the‐top makeup, but you could see that he had foundation on
Fourteen (61%) of the 23 informants used the term “comfortable” to
and obviously a bit of blush and some kind of lipstick.” Similarly,
describe how they felt when working with a presumably GM
another 20‐year‐old informant paid attention to body language,
associate. Several informants discussed that they felt more comfor-
appearance, voice, and makeup to guess a male sales associate sexual
table working with gay sales associates, especially if they felt
orientation; she said, “[I] suppose just general body language; how he
stigmatized by working with a younger or more attractive female
looked; he already had makeup on.”
associate. A 24‐year‐old informant said, “If I’m trying on clothing, it
makes me feel insecure [if] it looks better on the female sales
associate, it’s that sort of thing…. Gay men are easier to work with.”
3.3.5 | Working with fashion/cosmetics
The results suggest that when HF customers feel intimidated, they
will purposefully seek out GM sales associates, who seem to be more
Two (9%) informants judged the male sales associate’s knowledge
honest with them and, perhaps, more empathetic to understanding
about fashion as an indicator of his possible homosexuality, while one
the personal complexities and insecurities that many women have
(4%) informant surmised that a male sales associate working in a
regarding their physical appearance.
department store’s cosmetic department was gay. A 23‐year‐old
woman discussed how she used several signals to determine a male
sales associate’s sexual orientation: “I guess his over‐the‐top sort of
3.4.2 | Interest
arm gestures, [his] higher pitch voice, you know, you don’t really get
that with heterosexual guys … and just his knowledge of fashion.”
Six (26%) informants remarked that they believed that their GM sales
Similarly, a 20‐year‐old informant said, “It’s hard to really pick how
associate was genuinely interested in helping them or were attentive.
someone is gay but as much as a stereotype as it was … he was very,
A 39‐year‐old woman noted her sense of comfort with working with
very stylishly dressed and he was very flamboyant, very over the top,
a gay sales associate, saying, “I would return to the store because I’d
and his knowledge in fashion at times exceeded my own. So,
feel comfortable with him and knowing if I saw him there again I’d
obviously he didn’t say he was gay, but there were hints, and it was
say, ‘oh, yeah, I had a good experience with that assistant.’” She later
sort of clear.”
noted that her sales associate seemed more interested in the job
A 20‐year‐old informant also remarked that she determined her
than the heterosexual associates: “I think that a lot of sales assistants
male sales associate’s homosexuality by his employment in a
do get into a bit of a rut in that they’re sort of…. A heterosexual sales
cosmetic department. However, she also used other signals to guess
assistant would be working their normal hours, it’s hard to sort of put
the associate’s homosexuality; she said, “First of all, the fact that he
into words, but he [gay male sales associate] just seemed more
was a male working in a cosmetics section of a department store kind
attentive and I find that … I don’t know perhaps gay people are just
[of] fits the stereotype [of being gay]. Second, just his demeanor; his
more attentive in retail stores than others, I don’t know.”
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
7
A common finding is that GM sales associates seem to display
informed research finds that women put greater trust in gay
greater knowledge and more positive attitudes in retail settings than
men’s (vs. heterosexual women’s) advice because of the lack of
their heterosexual counterparts. Still, other informants noted that
intrasexual rivalry (Russell et al., 2013, 2017); thus, trust is the
they often feel more comfortable asking a gay man for assistance
foundation of these close and meaningful relationships between gay
because of their fear of judgment from other women. Thus, the
men and heterosexual women. In a similar vein, trust and commit-
genuineness that heterosexual women attribute to GM sales
ment represent “the twin foundations of [relationship marketing]”
associates may have less to do with the associates’ actions and more
(Sheth, 2017, p.7), and Price and Arnould (1999), p.44) consider trust
to do with their own inferiority complex.
“the central feature of effective business relationships” and the
bedrock of commercial friendships between service providers and
their customers.
3.4.3 | Trustworthiness
Although trust is often the impetus behind customer‐service
provider relationships and friendships, the retailing discipline
Four (17%) informants noted that they viewed their gay sales
lacks empirical understanding of how trust ensues between GM
associate as trustworthy. A 20‐year‐old informant noted the role of
sales associates and HF customers in retail contexts. Study 2
attractiveness: “The more attractive the gay male employee is, the
addresses this research chasm by exploring why women’s trust in
more likely I am to trust his opinion; but it’s the opposite effect with
GM sales associates may emerge in retail settings, with a further
a female; the more attractive means less product knowledge.”
goal of understanding the role of female intrasexual competition.
Similarly, a 39‐year‐old informant based her trustworthiness in
the gay sales associate on the “coming‐out” process. She remarked,
“I’m not gay, but I think obviously being gay, it takes a lot to come
4 | STUDY 2
out and openly be gay and that person is obviously true to
themselves so then they would be living that through every other
As previously discussed, the evolutionary theory posits that
area of their life you know … they wouldn’t sort of hold back
competitive rivalry sometimes present in women’s same‐sex social
anything. They may sugarcoat something but they wouldn’t hold
interactions can lead women to distrust their same‐sex peers (Fisher,
back in giving you an honest answer.” In other words, this customer
2017). As such, the lack of intrasexual rivalry between gay men and
believes that women are wary of other sales associates’ honesty, as
heterosexual women encourages many women to put more trust in
compared with gay men.
gay men’s advice, especially when they perceive increased competi-
One (4%) 20‐year‐old informant noted that her gay sales
tion from same‐sex others, such as in dating contexts (Russell et al.,
associate made her feel important. Although only one informant
2017). The humanistic evidence obtained in Study 1 reveals that
used the term “important,” a theme that emerged in the findings is
female consumers often perceive advice from GM sales associates as
that the informants tended to believe that GM sales associates are
trustworthy; however, empirical evidence for this relationship is
more likely than female associates to spend time with them to help
absent.
them feel good about themselves. Indeed, the informants used terms
The purpose of Study 2 is to empirically investigate the
such as “less judgmental” and “more honest” to compare GM sales
extent to which perceived intrasexual competition influences
associates with HF sales associates. The findings also suggest that
heterosexual women’s perceptions of trust in advice from HF and
many female customers have difficulty accepting advice from a more
GM
attractive female; thus, GM sales associates may have an upper hand
evolutionarily‐informed research (Russell et al., 2017), this study
in retail settings simply because they are not female.
sales
associates
in
retail
contexts.
Drawing
on
past
predicts that HF shoppers who perceive high appearance‐related
competitiveness with HF sales associates will perceive GM sales
associates as more trustworthy.
3.5 | Discussion
Study 1 reveals that HF shoppers experience feelings of comfort,
4.1 | Sample and procedures
genuineness, trustworthiness, and importance during their interactions with GM sales associates. According to prior research
The final sample consisted of 365 undergraduate HF students
(Rosenbaum et al., 2017), these positive feelings arise from the lack
(Mage = 20.45; standard deviation [SD]age = 3.44) from a mid‐sized
of intrasexual female competition and sexual interest between GM
university located in a metropolitan city in the United States who
sales associates and HF customers, especially given the need for
agreed to participate in the study through the university’s
closeness or touch when working with apparel, cosmetics, or
participant pool recruitment system. In terms of ethnicity,
accessories in retailing contexts.
280 (77%) participants reported being Caucasian, 49 (13%)
The descriptive data further reveal that HF shoppers may
Hispanic, 13 (4%) African American, 13 (4%) Asian, and 10 (2%)
perceive the advice from GM sales associates as more trustworthy
“other” ethnicity. All participants received partial course credit for
than advice from their HF counterparts. Indeed, evolutionarily‐
their participation.
8
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
To assess women’s trust in both a HF and a GM sales associate,
Pairwise comparisons revealed that women who scored higher
participants indicated their level of agreement with three items,
(vs. lower) on intrasexual competition were less likely to trust
assessed on 7‐point Likert scales: (a) “I could see myself trusting an
recommendations from HF sales associates (F(1, 363) = 4.47; p = .04;
HF sales associate’s recommendations,” (b) “I could count on an HF
ηp2 = 0.01). In addition, women’s likelihood to trust GM sales
sales associate to give me an unbiased opinion on what looks good on
associates over HF sales associates was evident in the high
my body,” and (c) “I believe that an HF sales associate would have my
intrasexual competition cluster (F(1, 363) = 21.65; p < .001; ηp2 =
best interests in mind.” Participants then indicated their agreement
0.06), which represents a medium effect (Sullivan & Feinn, 2012). The
with the same items about GM sales associates. A reliability analysis
results showed no significant difference in trust between the two
indicated that these three items were valid (Nunnally, 1978) for both
sales associates in the low intrasexual competition cluster (F(1,
types of sales associates (trust in GM sales associates: α = .81; trust in
363) = 3.13; p = .08). Thus, the intrasexual competition encourages
HF sales associates: α = .78).
female shoppers to view advice from GM sales associates as more
To prevent order effects, the HF and GM trust items appeared in
random order. To explore intrasexual female competition, participants
trustworthy than advice from HF sales associates. Figure 2 reports
the mean results.
indicated their level of agreement with four items (7‐point Likert scale)
that measured their concern about appearance‐related competitiveness
with an HF sales associate: (a) feeling “in competition” with the sales
5 | STUDY 3
associate, (b) the sales associate being more attractive than they are,
(c) what their body looked like compared with the sales associates, and
Study 2 found evidence that women’s concerns about being in
(d) feeling a sense of appearance‐related rivalry with the sales associate.
appearance‐related competition with heterosexual women lead them
Another reliability analysis of these four items revealed that together
to trust GM sales associates’ product suggestions more than female
these items were a valid measure of perceived appearance‐related
sales associates’ suggestions. However, if women’s distrust of female
competitiveness (α = .90; Nunnally, 1978).
sales associates is driven by feelings of intrasexual competitiveness,
Participants completed the study online. Before beginning the
the findings should show that women’s trust in GM sales associates’
study, they were told that they would be taking part in a study
suggestions is limited to appearance‐enhancing products (e.g.,
examining how they perceive themselves compared with different
apparel), as these products can play an important role in mate
individuals in consumer settings. Then, participants completed the
competition. For products that do not play such a role; however,
series of measures in the order specified previously.
women should trust GM sales associates no more or less than HF
sales associates.
Study 3 tests this hypothesis using an experimental design. The
4.2 | Results
prediction was that female shoppers’ increased trust in gay men
(vs. heterosexual women) would be evident when they received
The data were first screened for acquiescent response bias (i.e.,
suggestions on products that function to potentially enhance their
participants who consistently provided the same response across
physical appearance, but not when they received suggestions on
both positively and negatively scored items; Watson, 1992). Cases
products that do not serve this function. Although it is plausible
whose data exhibited this pattern (n = 33) were removed from the
that female
dataset, which left data from 365 participants for analyses.
recommendations to the same degree as gay men’s recommendations
buyers may trust heterosexual men’s
product
A two‐step cluster analysis analyzed the data for the four items
assessing concern about being in intrasexual competition with an HF
sales associate. An optimum number of clusters, a log‐likelihood
7
distance measure, and Schwarz’s Bayesian information criterion were
6.5
quality. The first cluster comprised 58.6% of the cases and was
associated with lower levels of intrasexual competition (four‐item
average = 1.97). The second cluster comprised 41.4% of the cases and
was associated with higher levels of intrasexual competition (four‐
item average = 4.67).
Next, a 2 × 2 mixed‐model analysis of variance (ANOVA)
Trust in sales associate
employed. The analysis revealed two clusters with good cluster
Heterosexual female sales associate
5.5
5
4.5
4
examined women’s level of perceived trust in each sales associate
3.5
(within‐subject variable) depending on each participant’s predicted
3
cluster membership (between‐subjects variable). As predicted, the
ANOVA showed a two‐way interaction between the type of sales
associate (HF vs. GM) and the cluster membership (low vs. high
intrasexual competition; F(1, 363) = 5.88; p = .02).
Gay male sales associate
6
Low perceptions
High perceptions
Intra-sexual competition cluster
F I G U R E 2 Women’s trust in heterosexual female versus gay male
sales associates when perceiving intrasexual competition
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
9
(as heterosexual men are not in intrasexual competition with
unattractive, 7 = very attractive). Three paired‐sample t tests then
women), the prediction was that they would not, given previous
compared the ratings of the two targets. The analysis revealed that
research revealing women’s decreased comfort in interacting
the male and female targets did not vary in perceived age (Mmale =
with straight (vs. gay) men (Russell, Ta, & Ickes, 2018b) and their
28.65, Mfemale = 28.27; p = .58), perceived professionalism (Mmale =
reduced preference for working with straight men in such sales
5.81, Mfemale = 5.84; p = .91), or perceived physical attractiveness
interactions
(Mmale = 5.22, Mfemale = 5.05; p = .57), though the mean attractiveness
due
to
potential
sexual
concerns
(Rosenbaum
et al., 2017).
of the sales associates was well above the midpoint of the scale (i.e.,
above‐average attractiveness).
In terms of product stimuli, six different product photographs
5.1 | Sample and procedures
served as the sales associate’s recommended products.2 Three
products were chosen for their potential to positively affect the
Two hundred and forty‐three women (Mage = 24.49; SDage = 4.48)
physical appearance of a female buyer (i.e., a woman’s dress, a pair of
took part in this study, 84 from the participant pool recruitment
women’s high‐heeled shoes, and a tube of lipstick). These items
system at a mid‐sized university located in a metropolitan city in the
served as the “appearance‐enhancing” products. The other three
United States and 160 from Amazon Mechanical Turk. Of the
product stimuli (i.e., kitchen blender, suitcase, and vacuum cleaner)
participants, 141 (58%) reported their ethnicity as Caucasian, 28
were unlikely to positively affect a female shopper's physical
(12%) as Hispanic, 32 (13%) as Asian, 15 (6%) as African American, 16
appearance but were products also easily found in retail department
(7%) as Indian, and 11 (4%) as another ethnicity or missing. The
stores. These items served as the “nonappearance‐enhancing”
participants recruited from the university received course credit, and
(control) items. Finally, regardless of product type, all the photo-
those recruited from Mechanical Turk received $0.50 for their
graphs were converted to black‐and‐white images to minimize the
participation.1
potential role of color in participants’ evaluations.
Three different conditions were generated using two target sales
associate photographs; each photograph was embedded in a
fictitious, computer‐generated LinkedIn profile.2 Participants viewed
5.2 | Design
their assigned target’s LinkedIn profile, which contained (a) a
photographic image of the target; (b) the target’s employment
This experiment was a 3 (sales associate target: HF vs. heterosexual
information (e.g., “works at Macy’s”); and (c) a professional organiza-
male vs. GM) × 2 (product: appearance‐enhancing vs. control)
tion to which the target belonged. However, the target’s sex and
between‐subjects factorial design. Participants completed the study
sexual orientation varied across three conditions: one profile
online and were told that they would be participating in
belonged to an HF sales associate, one to a heterosexual male sales
an experiment examining their expected buyer behavior in retail
associate, and one to a GM sales associate. To manipulate the gender
stores.
of the target, a professional headshot photograph of a man and a
When the participants logged in to complete the experiment,
woman was used, and the professional organization on each profile
they were informed that they would be viewing the LinkedIn profile
provided an indication of the target’s sexual orientation. For example,
of a sales associate who resides in the Dallas/Fort Worth area. To
the gay man’s LinkedIn profile indicated that he was a member of
bolster the believability of this cover story, the participants were
“Dallas Gay Business Professionals,” and the profiles of the
instructed to wait for 10–20 s until a sales associate’s LinkedIn
heterosexual man and woman indicated that he and she were
profile was “pulled at random” from the Internet. Unbeknownst to
members of “Dallas Business Professionals.”
the participants; however, they were randomly assigned to view one
For the two photographs selected, both individuals appeared to
of the two target stimuli conditions. After the participants viewed
be the same age, looked professional, and were equally attractive.
their assigned target’s profile, they were asked to imagine that they
However, to ensure that the male and female stimuli did not vary on
were in a department store to buy the three appearance‐enhancing
these dimensions, an independent sample of judges (n = 37) rated the
items (i.e., a dress, shoes, and lipstick) or the three control items (i.e.,
targets on (a) their perceived age; (b) their perceived professionalism,
a blender, suitcase, and vacuum). Then, they were asked to imagine a
on a Likert‐type scale (1 = very unprofessional, 7 = very professional);
situation in which they were being assisted by their assigned sales
and (c) their attractiveness, on a Likert‐type scale (1 = very
associate.
Participants were then presented with a scenario in which they
were instructed to imagine picking out store items. As part of the
1
While this compensation was well within the norms for Mechanical Turk workers (Dupuis,
Endicott‐Popovsky, & Crossler, 2013), it is nevertheless under the minimum wage standards
in the United States. Low payment for Mechanical Turk workers is a growing ethical concern.
2
In accordance with international copyright law, the present article contains only figures,
tables, and other content that is owned or controlled by the authors, or content for which
permission to reproduce in this article has been sought and obtained from those who legally
own or control such rights. Readers wishing to view referenced figures, tables, or related
content not published herein, are urged to consult the referenced publication.
procedure, participants viewed three different sets of four similar
products from which to choose. For example, participants assigned to
view the appearance‐enhancing products were presented with four
different dresses and instructed to select the dress that they would
like to add to their shopping cart. The participants in this condition
repeated this task by also selecting from four different high‐heeled
10
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
shoes and four different lipstick tubes. Participants assigned to the
enhance their physical appearance; there was no significant effect of
control product condition viewed three sets of control products (i.e.,
the control product type on women's differential likelihood to trust
four kitchen blenders, four suitcases, and four vacuum cleaners) and
the sales associates (F(2, 213) = 2.67, ns). Together, these results
then were instructed to select one item from each set.
show that women are more likely to trust product recommendations
After participants selected an item from each product set, they
made by GM sales associates (vs. their HF or male counterparts)
were presented with another scenario in which they were asked to
when choosing merchandise that serves an appearance‐enhancement
imagine that the sales associate from the LinkedIn profile (“Taylor”)
function.
evaluated their options and provided feedback. For example, the
scenario read: “Imagine that Taylor examines the items that you have
just selected. Then, imagine that Taylor says: ‘Your choices seem
6 | STUDY 4
great, but I have some suggestions that you should consider buying
instead. I have them in the back.’”
The results of Study 3 provide support for the hypothesis that
Participants were then presented with each of the three
women are more likely to trust appearance‐enhancement product
appearance‐enhancing or control products with a recommendation
recommendations from GM sales associates than from either
from Taylor. For example, the participants who viewed the image of
heterosexual male or female sales associates. Moreover, these
the dress also saw Taylor’s endorsement of the product (e.g., “I think
findings are consistent with past research showing that women
that you should buy this dress instead. It has a similar price to the
perceive gay men to provide more trustworthy mating advice than
item that you chose originally, and it would look great on you”).
straight men and women (Russell et al., 2013). However, the
Participants then rated how likely they would be (1 = very unlikely,
reasoning behind women’s increased likelihood to trust recommen-
7 = very likely) to trust Taylor’s recommendation instead of the
dations from GM sales associates can be extended further to a
original dress choice they had selected previously.
nuanced set of predictions about the contexts in which the observed
Last, the participants were asked to report the sexual orientation
effect is most likely to occur on the sales floor.
of the sales associate that they imagined interacting with, which
Previous research indicates that in mating‐relevant contexts,
served as the manipulation check for the study. Data from
women, especially those who are highly attractive and desirable, are
participants who failed to correctly report the sexual orientation of
likely to be competitively targeted by other envious heterosexual
their assigned sales associate were excluded from the analysis
women (Krems, Neuberg, Filip‐Crawford, & Kenrick, 2015; Leenaars,
(n = 25), resulting in a sample of 219 heterosexual women.
Dane, & Marini, 2008; Russell, Babcock, Lewis, Ta, & Ickes, 2018a).
As such, women—particularly those who are more physically
attractive than other women—may be sensitive to the quality of
5.3 | Results
advice provided by an HF sales associate when they are looking to
purchase products to attract a desirable partner. For example, female
Two composite scores for the participants’ likelihood to trust the
customers may experience discomfort consulting with a female sales
sales associates’ control product suggestions (α = .81) and the
associate in such a context because they may perceive themselves to
appearance‐enhancing product suggestions (α = .71) were computed
be in rivalry with the sales associate. By contrast, because GM sales
by averaging their responses to the three items in each category.
associates are not in competition with women for the same mates
Then, a 2 × 3 between‐subjects factorial ANOVA assessed whether
(Russell et al., 2017), attractive female customers should perceive gay
participants’ trust varied as a function of (a) the type of product
(control vs. appearance‐enhancing) and (b) the sales associate
(heterosexual woman vs. heterosexual man vs. gay man). As
7
Figure 3.
To probe this interaction, the simple effect of the sales associate
target within each product type was examined. The results revealed a
significant difference in participants’ likelihood to trust the three
sales associates’ appearance‐enhancing product suggestions (F(2,
213) = 8.36; p < .001; ηp2 = 0.07); the participants were more likely to
trust the GM sales associate’s suggestions (M = 4.74; standard error
[SE] = 0.22) than the heterosexual male (M = 3.50; SE = 0.22) or HF
(M = 4.05; SE = 0.21) sales associate’s suggestions.
By contrast, participants did not differ in their perceived
likelihood to trust a gay man or heterosexual woman when they
received recommendations for products that did not function to
Trust in sales associate's
recommendations
expected, the results of the ANOVA revealed a significant two‐way
interaction (F(2, 213) = 4.43; p = .013; ηp2 = 0.04), as shown in
Heterosexual female sales associate
Heterosexual male sales associate
Gay male sales associate
6
5
4
3
2
1
Control
Apperance-Enhancing
Product Type
F I G U R E 3 Women’s trust in heterosexual female versus
heterosexual male versus gay male sales associates when buying
appearance‐enhancing versus control items
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
11
men as more benevolent in these contexts and therefore be more
two buyer objectives in Study 4, certain appearance‐enhancing
likely to trust their product suggestions. Consistent with this
products (e.g., a dress) may be more appropriate to wear for one
reasoning, past research finds that more attractive women place
occasion than another. For example, female buyers may be less likely
greater value on gay men’s mating‐relevant advice (Russell et al.,
to purchase and wear a “more revealing” outfit when they intend to
2018a).
go to a party with family members than when they intend to go
When female customers consult with sales associates in buying
downtown to attract a potential mate. Thus, the product stimuli
products for nonmating‐related occasions; however, there are no
needed not only to be average in perceived desirability but also to be
apparent reasons they should feel less comfortable working with HF
versatile across different occasions. An independent group of female
than GM sales associates. For example, a female shopper who is
judges (n = 18) rated the appearance‐enhancing products on two
searching for an outfit to wear to a family event—regardless of how
statements as they viewed each product: (a) “This item could be worn
attractive she is—would have equal reason to trust the opinion of a
to go out” and (b) This item could be worn to a family party.” A
gay man and a heterosexual woman because of the absence of mating
paired‐sample t test revealed that the products did not significantly
motives that may elicit feelings of intrasexual rivalry over physical
differ on these measures (t(16) = 0.74; p = .47), indicating that the
appearance.
product stimuli were functionally versatile across contexts. Finally,
The predictions for Study 4 were as follows: women would be
more likely to (a) perceive good intent on the part of the GM sales
Study 4 also used the same target sales associate stimuli as in
Study 3.
associate and (b) trust a GM sales associate’s appearance‐enhancing
product recommendations more than a HF sales associate’s
recommendations, especially when intending to use the product for
6.2 | Design
a mating‐related objective (i.e., to look nice for potential mates) but
not for a nonmating‐related objective (i.e., to look nice for relatives).
The experiment was a 2 (buyer objective: mating vs. nonmating) × 2
In addition, this effect was predicted to be driven by attractive
(target sales associate: straight female vs. GM) between‐subjects
women (who are at risk for facing intrasexual rivalry from other
factorial design. Participants completed the study online. When they
women; Krems et al., 2015; Leenaars et al., 2008; Russell et al.,
logged online to complete the experiment, they read the same cover
2018a), such that more attractive women who were assigned to the
story as in Study 3. After providing their consent to participate,
mating‐related objective condition would be more likely to perceive
participants were randomly assigned to one of the two target stimuli
the GM sales associate as having good intent, which in turn would
conditions that depicted the target’s LinkedIn profile.
lead to increased trust in his product recommendations.
After participants viewed the target’s LinkedIn profile, the
computer randomly assigned them to one of the two buying‐
objective conditions. Participants were instructed to imagine that
6.1 | Sample and procedure
they either (a) needed to buy a new outfit to wear to a party
downtown with single attractive men in attendance (mating‐related
One hundred seventy‐one undergraduate HF students (Mage = 20.39;
objective) or (b) needed a new outfit to wear to a party at their
SDage = 4.22) recruited from a mid‐sized university’s psychology
parents’ house with their family and relatives in attendance
participant pool took part in the study. All participants received
(nonmating‐related objective). To further prime the buyer objective,
partial course credit for taking part in the study. The sample was 28%
participants were asked to write down their thoughts and feelings
Caucasian, 30% Hispanic, 20% African American, 15% Asian, and 7%
about the upcoming party for 3 min.
“other” ethnicity.
Two priming vignettes served as the two buying‐objective
Next, participants were asked to imagine being in a Macy’s
department store with their assigned sales associate (i.e., Taylor) to
conditions for the study. The first buying‐objective vignette
buy the three appearance‐enhancing products to complete their
asked participants to imagine themselves in a situation in which
outfit for the party. Participants then rated three items designed to
they needed to purchase an outfit to wear to a public party
assess their perception of whether or not Taylor had good intentions
downtown where attractive single men would be in attendance. This
in their sales interaction on a 7‐point Likert scale (1 = strongly
priming vignette served as the mating‐related objective. The second
disagree, 7 = strongly agree). The three items were (a) “I think that
buying‐objective vignette, which introduced the nonmating‐related
Taylor would want me to look my best for those at the party,” (b) “I
objective, asked participants to imagine themselves in a situation in
believe Taylor would make me feel good as he/she helps me try
which they needed to purchase an outfit to wear to a party at their
different items,” and (c) “I think that Taylor would have the best
parents’ house where their parents and relatives would be in
intentions for me.”
attendance. The Appendix provides the full texts of the priming
vignettes.
Participants then read a scenario that instructed them to imagine
picking out items they wanted to purchase and adding them to their
The study used the same categories of appearance‐enhancing
shopping cart. Similar to Study 3’s procedure, the participants saw
products (i.e., a dress, high‐heeled shoes, and a tube of lipstick) for
three different sets of four similar products: four dresses, four high‐
the product stimuli as in Study 3. However, given the salience of the
heeled shoes, and four tubes of lipstick. Participants were instructed
12
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
to select the item from each set that they would like to add to their
on (a) participants’ perceived intent of the sales associates and (b)
shopping cart for purchase.
their reported likelihood to trust the sales associate’s product
Next, participants read another scenario that asked them to
recommendations.
imagine that the sales associate from the LinkedIn profile (“Taylor”)
The results revealed a multivariate, two‐way interaction between
had evaluated their options and told them that they should consider
the sales associate and the participant’s buyer objective (Λ = 0.95,
his or her recommendations (“Your options seem great, but I have
mult. F(2, 155) = 4.22; p = .016; ηp2 = 0.05). This two‐way interaction
some suggestions that you should consider buying for the party
was evident for participants’ perceived intent of the sales associate
instead”). On the next screen, participants saw the product
(F(1, 156) = 7.24; p = .008; ηp2 = 0.04) and trust in the sales associate’s
stimuli and read instructions that they should imagine Taylor
recommendations (F(1, 156) = 5.29; p = .023; ηp2 = 0.03). Participants
endorsing the product (e.g., “I think that you should buy this dress
assigned to the mating‐related objective condition perceived higher
instead for the party. It would look great on you”). After reading this
positive intent on the part of the GM sales associate (M = 5.38;
prompt for each of the three products recommended by Taylor,
SE = 0.18) than the HF associate (M = 4.82; SE = 0.17; F(1, 156) = 5.23;
participants indicated how likely they would be to trust Taylor’s
p = .024; ηp2 = 0.03) and were more likely to trust the GM sales
product recommendations on 7‐point Likert‐type scales (1 = very
associate (M = 4.97; SE = 0.21) than the HF associate (M = 3.82;
unlikely, 7 = very likely).
SE = 0.20; F(1, 156) = 16.09; p < .001; ηp2 = 0.09). There were no
At the end of the experiment, participants reported on (a) their
own perceived attractiveness on a 7‐point scale (“Relative to my peer
differences in these two measures for participants assigned to the
nonmating‐related condition (ps > .13; see Figure 4).
group, I consider myself ____.” 1 = Much less attractive, 7 = much more
For the next set of predictions, a moderated mediation analysis
attractive) and (b) the sexual orientation of the sales associate they
(5,000 bootstrap samples, Model 12 of PROCESS Macro; Hayes,
imagined interacting with to serve as the manipulation check.
2018) tested for a three‐way interaction among the sales associate
Participants who failed to correctly report the sexual orientation of
condition (HF vs. GM), the buyer objective condition (mating vs.
their assigned sales associate were excluded from the analysis
nonmating), and the female buyer’s perceived attractiveness to
(n = 11), resulting in a sample of 160 heterosexual women.
predict her perception of the sales associate’s positive intent. Then,
the analysis tested whether the perceived intent of the sales
associate predicted female buyers’ trust in the sales associate’s
6.3 | Results
product recommendations.
Before the analysis, as the composite variable, participants’ trust in
participants’ likelihood to trust the sales associate (R2 = 0.31; F(2,
The model accounted for a significant proportion of variance in
the sales associate’s recommendations for each of the three
157) = 35.24; p < .001). As predicted, the model revealed the
appearance‐enhancing products was averaged (α = .78). Then, a
predicted three‐way interaction (b = 0.78; SE = 0.29; t(152) = 2.68;
2 × 2 between‐subjects factorial multivariate ANOVA tested the
p = .008). To probe this interaction, the authors tested for the
effects of the sexual orientation of the sales associate (straight
presence of the two‐way interaction between the buyer objective
woman vs. gay man) and the buyer objective (nonmating vs. mating)
and sales associate condition at both low (−1 SD) and high (+1 SD)
F I G U R E 4 Women’s perceived intent of the heterosexual female versus gay male sales associate and their likelihood to trust her or his
appearance‐enhancing product recommendations under either a mating‐ or nonmating‐related buyer objective
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
13
Next, as expected, the model showed that the more female
buyers perceived positive intent on the part of the sales associate,
the more they trusted their product recommendations (b = 0.58;
SE = 0.09; t(157) = 6.60; p < .001). Thus, the analysis revealed a
significant index of moderated mediation (b = 0.46; SE = 0.21; 95%
confidence interval [CI] = [0.05, 0.88]) at high levels of attractiveness
in the mating‐related objective condition only. There was also a
significant indirect effect of sales associate condition on trust
through the perceived intent of the sales associate (b = 0.70;
SE = 0.21; 95% CI = [0.28, 1.08]). In other words, female buyers who
perceived themselves as more attractive in the mating‐related
objective condition were more likely to trust the GM than the HF
sales associate’s product recommendations because they perceived
him as having more positive intentions in their sales interaction.
Importantly, women’s increased trust in the GM than the HF
F I G U R E 5 Women’s perceived intent of the heterosexual female
versus gay male sales associate as a function of the women’s
perceived physical attractiveness
sales associate was fully mediated by their perceived positive intent
of the associate. There was no direct effect after controlling for the
effect of this mediator and the buyer objective and attractiveness
moderators in the model (p = .10). The results of the moderated
levels of women’s self‐perceived attractiveness. The results
mediation model are shown in Figure 6.
revealed no significant two‐way interaction at low levels of
women’s perceived attractiveness (p = .87); however, there was a
significant two‐way interaction at higher levels of attractiveness
7 | D I S C U SS I O N
(b = 1.47; F(1, 152) = 13.26; p < .001). Further probing of this
interaction at each buyer objective condition (mating vs. nonmating)
This research offers an evolutionarily‐informed perspective on recent
showed that attractive women in the mating‐related objective
findings that suggest that female shoppers often prefer working with
condition were more likely to perceive positive intent on part of
GM sales associates for assistance with certain merchandise
the GM sales associate than the HF sales associate (b = 1.17;
selections (Rosenbaum et al., 2015, 2017). Study 1 extends
SE = 0.28, t(152) = 4.12; p < .001). There was no such effect among
evolutionary studies that show that women have an innate ability
the attractive women in the nonmating‐related condition (p = .30),
to recognize gay men from their heterosexual counterparts in the
see Figure 5.
marketplace (Tabak & Zayas, 2012; Valentova & Havlíček, 2013).
F I G U R E 6 Moderated mediation model from Study 4 illustrating the indirect effect of the sales associate condition (heterosexual female vs.
gay male) on women’s trust in the product recommendation, as mediated by the perceived intent of the sales associate, which was a significant
function of the interaction among women’s attractiveness, the buyer objective, and the sales associate condition. CI, confidence interval; SD,
standard deviation; SE, standard error
14
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
More specifically, the data reveal that female shoppers discern a
and GM sales associates. Past qualitative research finds that women
male sales associate’s sexual orientation from five different types of
exhibit a heightened preference for working with GM sales associates
physical cues. Several female shoppers also revealed that after
when they are older or heavier than a female sales associate
recognizing and working with GM associates, they sensed feelings of
(Rosenbaum et al., 2017). Consistent with this past work, Study 2
comfort, and many perceived them as being attentive and trust-
demonstrates that women who perceive high appearance‐related
worthy. Study 2 draws on theories of female intrasexual competition
competition with HF sales associates report placing greater trust in
(Buss, 1988; Fink et al., 2014) and demonstrates that female
the advice of GM sales associates. However, Study 4 reveals that
shoppers are more likely to trust recommendations from a GM sales
highly attractive female shoppers may also prefer working with GM
associate when they believe they are in appearance‐related rivalry
associates, albeit for a different reason. Given that highly attractive
with an HF sales associate. This finding extends the work of Russell
women face the risk of being competitively targeted by envious same‐
et al. (2013, 2017), which shows that women perceive dating advice
sex others (Leenaars et al., 2008; Russell et al., 2018b), these women
from gay men as more trustworthy and sincerer than advice from
are more likely to value mating‐relevant advice from gay men and
heterosexual women. That is, when female shoppers feel threatened
prefer GM friends (Russell et al., 2018b). In line with this reasoning,
by a female sales associate’s physical traits, they may view advice
Study 4 demonstrated that, in a mating‐objective context, highly
from gay men on items that serve to attract or retain a mating
attractive women were more likely to trust the product recommenda-
partner (e.g., apparel) more favorably than advice from the female
tions of a GM (vs. HF) sales associate because they perceived him as
sales associate. Study 3 helps buttress the case of the presence of
having more positive intentions in their sales interaction. Taken
intrasexual female competition in retailing. That is, the study clarifies
together, this work illustrates that women’s preference for and trust in
that female shoppers’ preference for GM sales associates’ advice is
GM sales associates (as compared to their female counterparts) is
limited to appearance‐enhancing product selections (e.g., apparel,
likely to be heightened in women who are concerned about negative
cosmetics) versus selections of other products (e.g., vacuum) that do
appearance comparison with female sales associates or likely to
not elicit feelings of appearance‐related competitiveness between
experience intrasexual rivalry from other women.
female sales associates and their less attractive female customers.
This research also suggests that pioneering theoretical and
Consistent with prior research showing that attractive women are
empirical opportunities abound for researchers to investigate
more at risk for intrasexual rivalry and place greater value on gay
women’s feelings when working with metrosexual sales associates
men’s mating‐relevant advice (Russell et al., 2018a), Study 4 shows
in retail settings. Aldrich (2004, p.1733) conceptualizes a metrosex-
that attractive women are more likely to trust GM (vs. HF) sales
ual male as a heterosexual male who “is nevertheless in touch with
associates when purchasing products to attract a romantic partner.
his feminine side”; however, other researchers typically label
This effect is mediated by increased perceptions of the positive
metrosexuals as either gay or bisexual (Coad, 2008). Metrosexuality
intent on the part of GM sales associates.
is often associated with trendiness, a concern with one’s appearance,
the use of grooming practices, and feminine proclivities (Mitchell &
Lodhia, 2017). Therefore, in marketplace situations in which they feel
7.1 | Theoretical implications
uncomfortable working with a more attractive or younger female
sales associate, female shoppers will likely show a preference for
These studies contribute to an evolutionary paradigm in marketing
working with a metrosexual‐appearing male sales associate, if one is
regarding the role of intrasexual competition between heterosexual
available on the sales floor.
men (Otterbring, Ringler, Sirianni, & Gustafsson, 2018) and women
(Prendergast et al., 2014) in retail settings, as well as its influence on
merchandise selection and sales associate preference. More specifi-
7.2 | Managerial implications
cally, this research shows that intrasexual competition may encourage female shoppers to select a GM sales associate when they
Given that gay men living in the United States face discrimination in
discern the presence of one on a sales floor. As such, future research
the hiring process (Tilcsik, 2011), the managerial implications of the
should explore the extent to which intrasexual competition motivates
findings suggest that retail organizations should work to increase
female shoppers to purchase items via e‐commerce, catalogs, or
diversity and not discriminate against gay men in the hiring process.
home shopping to escape feelings of rivalry or to retaliate against
To be clear, the authors are not suggesting recruitment preferences
more attractive female associates with complaints or low customer
based on sexual orientation, as this would be discriminatory; rather,
satisfaction scores. Furthermore, given the existence of intrasexual
the findings reveal that some female shoppers may be inclined to
competition in the business‐to‐consumer marketplace, the rivalry is
avoid retail settings in which they sense rivalry or a disrupted self‐
also likely to arise in business‐to‐business professional and sales
concept. At the same time, heterosexual men may also feel
relationships, which may cause women to react negatively to sales
intimidated when working with a younger and more attractive male
overtures from same‐sex others.
sales associates (Otterbring et al., 2018).
This work helps further clarify the contexts and interpersonal
This research suggests that physical retailers, particularly high‐
factors that might foster commercial friendships among HF shoppers
end specialty stores and luxury boutiques, may benefit from hiring a
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
15
diverse workforce. This is because customers may be intimated by
that our findings from Study 4 point to women trusting GM sales
sales associates for reasons beyond their control, and thus diversity
associates because they perceive greater positive intentionality on
offers customers the option to work with preferable associates. The
their part, it is not highly likely that perceived expertise is influencing
lack of a diverse workforce may be a reason for consumers who feel
this process versus intrasexual female competition. Nonetheless,
stigmatized in retail settings (e.g., overweight, aging) to patronize
future research should explore this possibility.
online retailers (Scaraboto & Fischer, 2013).
The findings also offer provocative insights for GM retail
Third, future research should explore women’s sales transactions
and perceived trust in other gay–straight sales associate combina-
employees. Gay retail employees who learn subcultural “campy”
tions. For example, would HF shoppers trust appearance‐enhancing
signals of flamboyance (Kates, 2002) and display them in retail
product recommendations provided from lesbian female sales
settings may realize monetary benefits by attracting female
associates? We have reason to believe they would not, considering
customers. Yet these same employees may experience backlash
that—like
from other customers who misinterpret these signals or find
women might be perceived as being sexually attracted to their
them too explicit in nature (Oakenfull & Greenlee, 2005). Further-
female shopper clientele, which might cause discomfort and less
more, as an increasing number of countries permit homosexuals to
trusting sales interactions. We, therefore, suspect that the increased
sales
interactions
with
heterosexual
men—lesbian
marry and more people are accepting of homosexuality in
trust that women place in GM sales associates’ appearance‐
general (Brown, 2017), “market‐manifested stereotypes of gayness”
enhancement product recommendations may be unique to the
(Ginder & Byun, 2015, p.835) might prevail or dissipate as gay
commercial relationships between heterosexual women and gay
identity becomes increasingly mainstream and accepted in these
men, which could be an interesting prediction to test in future
countries.
research.
Further, data for these studies came from participants who
resided in metropolitan areas in either Australia or the United States.
7.3 | Research limitations and future directions
In both countries, GM sales associates are commonly employed in
retail settings, and same‐sex marriage is legal; in addition, these
Although the present findings make a case for the role intrasexual
metropolitan areas have large gay populations. Thus, the participants
female competition plays in female shoppers’ trust in GM sales
were likely to have an awareness of gay men and perhaps
associates, more research remains to be done. One limitation
to hold a favorable or tolerant opinion of gay men in general. The
inherent in the current experimental studies is their reliance on a
research findings are likely to generalize to other countries that hold
relatively young, college‐aged sample of women. It will be important
favorable attitudes toward lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and
to determine whether the findings from our experimental studies are
intersex (LGBTI) people and have legal protections (e.g., human
specific to college‐aged heterosexual women or are generalizable to
rights, same‐sex marriage) but not to countries that criminalize same‐
other age groups of women. Given that female intrasexual competi-
sex relations or marginalize LGBTI people from participating in
tion decreases with age (Fernandez, Muñoz‐Reyes, & Dufey, 2014), it
general society (for specific country details, see Human Rights
is possible that older generations of women who are married or
Watch, 2017).
approaching the end of their reproductive years would be less
In addition, in Study 1, participants discussed the experience with
concerned with attracting and competing for a mate (relative to
a confirmed GM sales associate; however, the study did not probe
younger women). Rather these women may have other reasons for
the extent to which an informant had a long‐term “commercial
avoiding younger female sales employees—for example, body image
friendship” (Price & Arnould, 1999) with the associate. Although
self‐consciousness. Indeed, previous qualitative research shows that
researchers have found that feelings of trustworthiness may ensue
women who are older with heavier body types (i.e., correlates of
quickly between customers and employees in retail settings
unattractiveness) are more comfortable consulting with gay men so
(Rosenbaum, Massiah, & Wozniak, 2013), it is possible that some
that they are not directly compared to younger and thinner female
informants had long relationships with GM sales associates. Thus, the
sales associates. (Rosenbaum et al., 2017). Thus, future experimental
findings do not serve to diminish the likelihood that relationship
research should explore whether these implicit social comparisons
duration between any customer and service provider helps facilitate
operate to steer older and less attractive female shoppers away from
feelings of trustworthiness between the parties.
working with younger, thinner saleswomen and toward working with
gay salesmen.
In a similar vein, although intrasexual female competition
generalizes across the globe, its impact in retail settings in which
Second, although we received evidence that women trust GM
GM sales associates are not overtly present (e.g., Persian Gulf, North
sales associates’ appearance‐enhancing product recommendations
Africa) will differ. For example, intrasexual competition may
when they are intending to use the product for a mating‐related
encourage some women to retaliate against attractive female sales
purpose (which we attributed to intrasexual competition), it will be
associates by treating them in an inferior manner or by spending
informative to explore whether perceived expertise is somehow
large sums of money to show superiority.
involved in women’s perceived trust in GM sales associates when
An underlying assumption of the studies presented herein is that
women are trying to use products to attract a mate. However, given
heterosexual women can accurately discern the sexual orientation of
16
|
a GM sales associate from his physical appearance or participation in
a gay professional organization or have knowledge of his sexual
orientation before engaging in marketplace exchanges. Undoubtedly,
heterosexual women could also mistakenly surmise an incorrect
sexual orientation from physical appearance or membership in
certain organizations. Furthermore, in Study 1, heterosexual women
discussed how they discern homosexual men in retail settings, and
many of the clues they mentioned follow long‐standing stereotypes
that attribute feminine characteristics to gay men.
To be clear, the goal of this article is not to promote gay
stereotyping, and not all GM associates display feminine characteristics. Furthermore, in all the studies, heterosexual women assumed,
albeit accurately or incorrectly, that they were interacting with a gay
man, and they were given no reason to doubt their assumption. Thus,
from a theoretical perspective, this article advances an understanding of the marketplace relationship between heterosexual
women and gay men. Despite the limitations, this research highlights
the innate underpinnings that often encourage HF shoppers to
purposefully seek out assistance from GM associates in many retail
settings.
REFERENC ES
Aldrich, R. (2004). Homosexuality and the city: An historical overview.
Urban Studies, 41(9), 1719–1737.
Blashill, A. J., & Powlishta, K. K. (2009). Gay stereotypes: The use of sexual
orientation as a cue for gender‐related attributes. Sex Roles, 61(11/
12), 783–793.
Brown, A. (2017). 5 key findings about LGBT Americans. Retrieved from
https://www.pewresearch.org/fact‐tank/2017/06/13/5‐key‐findings‐
about‐lgbt‐americans/
Bush, A. A., & Hair, J. F., Jr. (1985). An assessment of the mall intercept as
a data collection method. Journal of Marketing Research, 22(2),
158–167.
Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition:
Tactics of mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54(5), 616–626.
Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Choe, J. A. E., Buunk, B. P., & Dijkstra, P.
(2000). Distress about mating rivals. Personal Relationships, 7(3),
235–243.
Clarke, V., & Turner, K. (2007). Clothes maketh the queer? Dress,
appearance, and the construction of lesbian, gay, and bisexual
identities. Feminism & Psychology, 17(2), 267–276.
Coad, D. (2008). The metrosexual: Gender, sexuality, and sport. Albany, NY:
SUNY Press.
Dupuis, M., Endicott‐Popovsky, B., & Crossler, R. (2013). An analysis of the
use of Amazon’s Mechanical Turk for survey research in the cloud. In
B. Endicott‐Popovsky (Ed.), ICCSM2013‐Proceedings of the international
conference on cloud security management (pp. 10–18). Reading, UK:
ICCSM.
Fernandez, A. M., Muñoz‐Reyes, J. A., & Dufey, M. (2014). BMI, age, mate
value, and intrasexual competition in Chilean women. Current
Psychology, 33(4), 435–450.
Fink, B., Klappauf, D., Brewer, G., & Shackelford, T. K. (2014). Female
physical characteristics and intrasexual competition in women.
Personality and Individual Differences, 58, 138–141.
Fisher, M., & Cox, A. (2011). Four strategies used during intrasexual
competition for mates. Personal Relationships, 18(1), 20–38.
RUSSELL ET AL.
Fisher, M. L. (Ed.). (2017). The Oxford handbook of women and competition.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Garzaniti, I., Pearce, G., & Stanton, J. (2011). Building friendships and
relationships. The role of conversation in hairdressing service
encounters. Managing Service Quality, 21(6), 667–687.
Ginder, W., & Byun, S. E. (2015). Past, present, and future of gay and
lesbian consumer research: Critical review of the quest for the queer
dollar. Psychology & Marketing, 32(8), 821–841.
Gummesson, E. (2002). Relationship marketing in the new economy.
Journal of Relationship Marketing, 1(1), 37–57.
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional
process analysis: a regression‐based approach (2nd ed.). New York:
Guilford Press.
Human Rights Watch. (2017). Human Rights Watch country profiles:
Sexual orientation and gender identity. Retrieved from https://www.
hrw.org/video‐photos/interactive/2018/04/16/sexual‐orientation‐
gender‐identity‐country‐profiles
Kates, S. M. (2002). The protean quality of subcultural consumption: An
ethnographic account of gay consumers. Journal of Consumer Research,
29(3), 383–399.
Krems, J. A., Neuberg, S. L., Filip‐Crawford, G., & Kenrick, D. T. (2015). Is
she angry? (Sexually desirable) women “see” anger on female faces.
Psychological Science, 26, 1655–1663.
Lawson, H. M. (1999). Working on hair. Qualitative Sociology, 22(3),
235–257.
Leenaars, L. S., Dane, A. V., & Marini, Z. A. (2008). Evolutionary
perspective on indirect victimization in adolescence: The role of
attractiveness, dating, and sexual behavior. Aggressive Behavior: Official
Journal of the International Society for Research on Aggression, 34(4),
404–415.
Lewis, D. M. G., Al‐Shawaf, L., Russell, E. M., & Buss, D. M. (2015). Friends
and happiness: An evolutionary perspective on friendship. In M.
Demir (Ed.), Friendship and happiness: Across the life‐span and cultures
(pp. 37–57). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.
Luxen, M. F., & Van De Vijver, F. J. R. (2006). Facial attractiveness, sexual
selection, and personnel selection: When evolved preferences matter.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 241–255.
Massar, K., & Buunk, A. P. (2010). Judging a book by its cover: Jealousy
after subliminal priming with attractive and unattractive faces.
Personality and Individual Differences, 49(6), 634–638.
McCracken, G. (1988). Qualitative research methods: The long interview.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
McCutecheon, J. M., & Morrson, M. A. (2019). Beyond the superordinate
categories of “gay men” and “lesbian women”: Identification of gay
and lesbian subgroups. Journal of Homosexuality, 0–26. https://doi.org/
10.1080/00918369.2019.1627129
Mitchell, V. M., & Lodhia, A. (2017). Understanding the metrosexual and
spornosexual as a segment for retailers. International Journal of Retail
& Distribution Management, 45(4), 349–365.
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
McGraw‐Hill.
Oakenfull, G. K., & Greenlee, T. B. (2005). Queer eye for a gay guy: Using
market‐specific symbols in advertising to attract gay consumers
without alienating the mainstream. Psychology & Marketing, 22(5),
421–439.
Otterbring, T., Ringler, C., Sirianni, N. J., & Gustafsson, A. (2018). The
Abercrombie & Fitch effect: The impact of physical dominance on
male customers’ status‐signaling consumption. Journal of Marketing
Research, 55(1), 69–79.
Peretz, H. (1995). Negotiating clothing identities on the sales floor.
Symbolic Interaction, 18(1), 19–37.
Phillips, A. E., & Hranek, C. (2012). Is beauty a gift or a curse? The
influence of an offender’s physical attractiveness on forgiveness.
Personal Relationships, 19, 420–430.
|
RUSSELL ET AL.
Prendergast, G. P., Li, S. S., & Li, C. (2014). Consumer perceptions of
salesperson gender and credibility: An evolutionary explanation.
Journal of Consumer Marketing, 31(3), 200–221.
Price, L. L., & Arnould, E. J. (1999). Commercial friendships: Service
provider–client relationships in context. Journal of Marketing, 63(4),
38–56.
Rosenbaum, M. S., Massiah, C., & Wozniak, R. (2013). An exploratory
analysis of social commonalities and subjective discounts. International
Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 41(9), 671–687.
Rosenbaum, M. S., Russell, E. M., & Russell‐Bennett, R. (2017). “I’ll wait for
him”: Understanding when female shoppers prefer working with gay
male sales associates. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 36,
172–179.
Rosenbaum, M. S., Russell‐Bennett, R., & Drennan, J. (2015). Commercial
friendships between gay sales associates and heterosexual female
customers in luxury settings: A proposed theoretical framework.
Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 27, 179–186.
Russell, E. M., Babcock, M. J., Lewis, D. M. G., Ta, V. P., & Ickes, W. (2018a).
Why attractive women want gay male friends: A previously
undiscovered strategy to prevent mating deception and sexual
exploitation. Personality and Individual Differences, 120, 283–287.
Russell, E. M., Ta, V. P., & Ickes, W. (2018b). Women interact more
comfortably and intimately with gay men—but not straight men—after
learning their sexual orientation. Psychological Science, 29, 288–303.
Russell, E. M., DelPriore, D. J., Butterfield, M. E., & Hill, S. E. (2013).
Friends with benefits, but without the sex: Straight women and gay
men exchange trustworthy mating advice. Evolutionary Psychology, 11,
132–147.
Russell, E. M., Ta, V. P., Lewis, D. M., Babcock, M. J., & Ickes, W. (2017).
Why (and when) heterosexual women trust gay men: Ulterior mating
motives and female competition. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46,
763–773.
Scaraboto, D., & Fischer, E. (2013). Frustrated fatshionistas: An
institutional theory perspective on consumer quests for greater
choice in mainstream markets. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(6),
1234–1257.
Sheth, J. (2017). Revitalizing relationship marketing. Journal of Services
Marketing, 31(1), 6–10.
Smith, J. E., Waldorf, V. A., & Trembath, D. L. (1990). “Single white male
looking for thin, very attractive…”. Sex Roles, 23, 675–685.
Spake, D. F., Beatty, S. E., Brockman, B. K., & Crutchfield, T. N. (2003).
Consumer comfort in service relationships: Measurement and
importance. Journal of Service Research, 5(4), 316–332.
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. M. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded
theory procedures and techniques. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sullivan, G. M., & Feinn, R. (2012). Using effect size—or why the P
value is not enough. Journal of Graduate Medical Education, 4(3),
279–282.
Tabak, J. A., & Zayas, V. (2012). The roles of featural and configural face
processing in snap judgments of sexual orientation. PLoS One, 7(5)
e36671
Tilcsik, A. (2011). Pride and prejudice: Employment discrimination against
openly gay men in the United States. American Journal of Sociology,
117(2), 586–626.
Valentova, J. V., & Havlíček, J. (2013). Perceived sexual orientation based
on vocal and facial stimuli is linked to self‐rated sexual orientation in
Czech men. PLoS One, 8(12) e82417
Wan, L. C., & Wyer, R. S., Jr (2015). Consumer reactions to attractive
service providers: Approach or avoid? Journal of Consumer Research,
42(4), 578–595.
Watson, D. (1992). Correcting for acquiescent response bias in the
absence of a balanced scale. Sociological Methods and Research, 21,
52–88.
17
How to cite this article: Russell EM, Bradshaw HK,
Rosenbaum MS, Hill SE, Russell‐Bennett R. Intrasexual female
competition and female trust in gay male sales associates’
recommendations. Psychology & Marketing. 2019;1–17.
https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21310
Appendix A
Buying Objectives: Study 4
Mating‐Related Objective
Vignette 1:
• Imagine that you are single and you’ve received an invitation to
attend a public party downtown. You received word that a couple
of attractive guys that you know will be attending this party, so
you are looking forward to the evening and hoping to look nice.
• Please take a couple of minutes to write about your ideal
expectations for the evening (e.g., How would you be feeling?
What could you see yourself saying? What could you see yourself
doing?)
Vignette 2:
• Imagine that you let Taylor know that you are looking for a dress, a
pair of shoes, and lipstick. When Taylor asks you what the occasion
is for these items, you tell Taylor that you are attending a party
downtown, and you are hoping to look nice for some good‐looking
single men.
Non‐Mating‐Related Objective
Vignette 1:
• Imagine that you’ve received an invitation to attend a party at your
parents’ house. You know that your parents and a few of your
older relatives will be there, so you are looking forward to the
evening and hoping to look nice for your family.
• Please take a couple of minutes to write about your ideal
expectations for the evening (e.g., How would you be feeling?
What could you see yourself saying? What could you see yourself
doing?)
Vignette 2:
• Imagine that you let Taylor know that you are looking for a dress, a
pair of shoes, and lipstick. When Taylor asks you what the occasion
is for these items, you tell Taylor that you are attending a party at
your parents’ house, and you are hoping to look nice for your
parents and older relatives.