Improving students’ fluency through voice recording*
Aldy Rizky Nesapiradana & Rahmila Murtiana
LBPP LIA Banjarmasin
Abstract
Fluency as an important goal in learning English is the ability to produce the
language smoothly and naturally. The problem is, learners often fail to achieve the
expected fluency despite the time spent in learning English. In an attempt to help
learners improve their fluency, we conducted this classroom action research. Two
classes of Intermediate 4 at LIA Banjarmasin were selected to be the object of this
research. The students in both classes were asked to read a monologue and their
voice was recorded. The result was then played, and the teacher pointed out which
parts needed improvement. At this stage, the teacher modeled the correct chunks,
intonation, and stress. The next step, the students were asked to re-read the
monologue, re-record their voice, and play the result to see the improvement. The
result is, through this procedure most of the students were able to improve their
reading fluency, as evidenced by their correct use of chunking, phrasing, intonation,
and word stress.
Background
One important goal in learning English is fluency, that is, the ability to produce the
language smoothly and naturally. Fluency involves a degree of automaticity and the
ability to retrieve ready-made chunks of language (McCarthy, 2010). The term
fluency is more often associated with speaking skills, while actually it can also be
applied to reading skills. In reading, fluency is defined by the National Assessment
of Educational Progress (NAEP) as "the ease or 'naturalness' of reading, including
how a reader groups or phrases words as revealed through intonation, stress, and
pauses, and expresses oneself in feeling, anticipation, and characterization during
oral reading” (http://www.learningrx.com/reading-fluency.htm).
Reading fluency is a challenge for students who learn English as a foreign language,
not only they have to deal with the pronunciation but also with the appropriate chunk
and word stress to make the text meaningful. Ford (2012) stated that fluent readers
are able to read words accurately and automatically with expression, and they can
focus their attention on the text's meaning because they do not have to slow down in
order to concentrate on decoding the individual words in a text. In this way, fluency
acts as a bridge between word recognition and comprehension.
Unfortunately, teachers often overlook the power of reading aloud, and emphasize
more on silent reading activity. There has been a belief that reading aloud is an
inauthentic language activity, while as a matter a fact, reading aloud can contribute
to spoken fluency (Thornbury, 2005; Ford, 2012). Among the advantages of reading
*
Paper presented at the 6th LIA Research Colloquium, 22 October 2013. Published in LIA CAR Journal vol.6.
aloud is it provides learners a framework to focus on lower level features of talk such
as pronunciation, word stress, and intonation. It is also a form of scaffolding for
learners before they are assigned to do a spoken activity. For example, prior to
acting out a dialog, a role play, or giving an oral presentation, learners can rehearse
the script first by reading it aloud (Thornbury, 2005, p. 70). In LIA promotion test,
reading aloud is even a part of oral test which assesses the student spoken ability. In
real life, reading fluency is needed in certain occasion such as giving an
announcement, giving a presentation, or reporting news.
Based on the consideration that improving learners’ fluency in reading is equally
important as fluency in speaking, we decided to conduct this classroom action
research. Our main purpose is to tap the students’ awareness of the importance of
fluency and help them improve their ability in producing smooth and natural
language. The technique we used in improving students’ fluency is through voice
recording. Voice recording is believed to be an effective way because when the
voice is recorded, it can be replayed and students can listen again to it and notice
the mistakes or which parts need improvement. The questions of this research are:
1. How will the use of voice recording improve the students’ fluency?
2. Which component of fluency is most improved and which component is least
improved?
Theoretical Framework
More fluent vs. less fluent reader
Several sources acknowledge that fluent readers can read aloud effortlessly and with
expression, because they recognize the word automatically, and their reading
sounds natural as if they are speaking. Hook and Jones (2004), for example, wrote
that “Good readers read fluently with adequate speed and when they read aloud,
they use appropriate phrasing, intonation, and their oral reading mirrors their spoken
language” (p.18). In contrast, less fluent readers are marked by their halting and
inconsistent rate, poor phrasing, and inadequate intonation pattern; they read slowly,
word by word and their oral reading is often choppy, and without natural expression
(sources:http://www.reading-skills-pyramid.org/reading-fluency.htm;
http://www.learningrx.com/reading-fluency.htm).
A study on reading fluency conducted by the NAEP revealed that there is a
significant correlation between reading fluency and reading comprehension. Another
study conducted by Researchers at the National Institute for Literacy investigated
two major instructional approaches related to reading fluency. The first is repeated
and monitored oral reading, where students read passages aloud several times and
receive guidance and feedback from the instructor. The second is independent silent
reading, where students are encouraged to read extensively on their own. The key
finding was "repeated and monitored oral reading improves reading fluency and
overall reading achievement" (National Institute for Literacy website, 2006).
In the case of reading fluency of L2 learners, Robertson (2009) argued that “hearing
the text out loud may not necessarily provide a step towards comprehension as it is
likely to do for native speakers”. Many L2 learners can read fast and accurate
because they are good readers in their native language and have strong decoding
skills but it does not mean they automatically understand the text (Robertson, 2009).
A more important factor to be fluent and at the same time understand the text is by
reading in meaningful chunks. In a study which investigated the relationship between
chunking and L2 reading fluency, Yamashita and Ichikawa (2010) found that the
difficulty of chunking a text negatively affected comprehension and smoothness for
the intermediate learners. Therefore, they suggested that reading in meaningful
chunks or grouping words into grammatical and meaningful units is one of the
fundamental processes necessary for fluent reading (Yamashita & Ichikawa, 2010).
Students often fail to put chunks when reading because the prosodic features are not
present explicitly in a text. In a spoken language, the features are easily recognized
by gestures, facial expression, intonation, and stress. In a text, prosodic features are
sometimes signaled through punctuation, but it is often not enough to guide the
students to read in a meaningful chunks. Therefore, the students should be trained
to map or to connect the prosodic features of spoken language onto a text (Hook &
Jones, 2004), in order to enhance their fluency.
Instruction technique to improve fluency
To help students become more fluent readers, teachers can provide models of fluent
reading, then have students reread the text on their own. By listening to good models
of fluent reading, students learn how a reader's voice can help written text make
sense. Usually, having students repeatedly read a text four times is sufficient to
improve fluency (source: Reading Pyramid). Teacher can have the student
immediately read it back to the teacher, or allow students to practice reading along
with the taped text, which is an excellent way for them to learn appropriate
pronunciation and phrasing (Robertson, 2009).
Several other ways of reading activities to improve fluency can also be done such
as:
1). choral reading, that is, a group of students read the same material aloud;
2). paired reading, where students work with one competent reader and one that is
not as strong in reading;
3). recorded reading , that is, students follow the reading by listening to the book
from a tape or CD;
4). echo reading , that is, when the teacher reads a sentence or phrase and the
student echoes back;
5). buddy reading, that is, students at about the same reading level are paired up
and read together (source: http://www.learningrx.com/reading-fluency.htm)
To develop student awareness of the prosodic features such as intonation, stress,
and phrasing, Thornbury (2005) suggested that before reading aloud activity,
students are asked to mark on the stressed words and divided parts of the text into
meaningful chunks. A more structured and systematic instruction to facilitate
students to recognize prosodic features in a text was offered by Hook & Jones
(2004). The first focus is by practicing on intonation. Teachers can introduce a short
sentence and model how to read it with different word stress. When the students
can practice it until they are fluent, teachers can modify and expand the sentence
and vary the rate, intensity, and pitch. Teachers can also vary the sentence by
alternating punctuation marks at the end of the sentence, so the students can see
the different intonation and meaning between sentence with full stop, with question
mark, or with exclamation point. The second focus is practicing phrasing. Teachers
can assign the students to recite a group of alphabet in chunks, then when they
understand the basic concept of chunking, teachers can move on the next step, that
is, assign the students to chunk a text into syntactic or meaningful units such as
noun phrase, verb phrase, or prepositional phrase. Teachers can give example by
marking the phrases with slashes, then ask the students to continue putting the
slashes. An alternative way is by asking the student to read a text while scooping
under the phrases with their finger or a pencil. Below is the example of slashing and
scooping:
a) slashes: Meg told Jim / her kite was stuck / in a tree
b) scooping: Meg told Jim her kite was stuck in a tree
Measuring students’ fluency
Ford (2012) proposed three components of measuring reading fluency, that is,
accuracy, automaticity, and prosody. Accuracy refers to the percentage of words a
reader can read correctly in a given text. Reading accuracy can be divided into three
levels: independent level, instructional level, and frustrational level. A student is
at independent reading level if he/she can read with 98-100% accuracy, or with no
more than approximately 1 in 20 words difficult for him/her. The accuracy range for
instructional-level text is 90-97%, or with no more than approximately 1 in 10 words
difficult for the student. Whereas frustrational reading level is if the student can read
with less than 90% accuracy, or with more than 1 in 10 words difficult for the student.
At this level, the student would have difficulty reading and comprehending the text,
even with the support of the teacher (Ford, 2012).
The second component, automaticity is usually measured as reading rate, or the
number of words a student reads per minute (WPM). Teacher can use a stopwatch
to count the minute, or in the voice recording, it is automatically shown on the
screen. To count the reading rate, multiply the total number of words in the passage
by 60, and divide by the student's reading time in seconds. For example, if a student
read a 200-word passage in 2 minutes and 32 seconds, which is equal to 152
seconds, the reading rate is (200 x 60) ÷ 152 seconds = 79 words per minute (wpm).
The third component, prosody refers to the appropriate use of intonation and
phrasing in reading, which involves paying attention to punctuation signs, assigning
appropriate stress to individual words within a sentence, and raising or lowering
voice intonation to match the meaning of the text. Chunking is included here. In
assessing prosody, teacher can use rubrics to assess whether students are reading
with appropriate pitch variation, intonation, phrasing, and expression. One such
rubric is the Oral Reading Fluency Scale created for the National Assessment of
Educational Progress (NAEP). To use this rubric, simply have a student read a short
passage of text, and assign a score of 1-4 (see appendix 2).
As a comparison to this scale, Fountas and Pinnell (2001) also developed a fluency
scale which consists of 6 components: rate, phrasing, intonation, pausing, stress,
and integration. The first component, rate refers to pace in reading, which is not too
fast or not too slow; in other words, the reader should moves along the reading text
appropriately with few slow downs, stops or pauses. The second component,
phrasing refers to the way the reader puts words together in group to represent the
meaningful units of language, to make it sound like an oral language. The third
component, intonation is sometimes called expression, or the variation of the voice in
tone, pitch, and volume to reflect the meaning of the text. The fourth component,
pausing refers to the way the reader pauses at punctuation such as commas,
dashes or full stops. The fifth component, stress, refers to the emphasis put on
particular word, which is marked with a louder tone. These four components:
intonation, phrasing, pausing, and stress are part of prosody. Finally, integration as
the sixth component is to assess the overall fluency which involves the consistent
use of the five aforementioned dimensions (Fountas & Pinnell, 2001). The score for
each component ranges from 1 to 4 (see appendix 3 for more details).
Data Analysis
In conducting this research, we followed an action research cycle: plan, act, observe,
and reflect, as suggested by Burns (2010).
1. Plan: It is the first stage in which we started to identify the problem, develop
research questions, and consult relevant literature. The identified problem
was students of Intermediate levels cannot yet be categorized as fluent
readers. Most of them still struggled to read smoothly, naturally, accurately,
and automatically. Thus, we developed an action plan, that is, by using voice
recording, and analyzed whether it could help improve student fluency. In
addition, we consulted literature related to fluency including the theories and
previous research on reading fluency
2. Act: It is the second stage in which we started to collect data by recording the
students’ voice. The data were collected during term 2/2013. Two classes of
Intermediate 4 at LBPP LIA Banjarmasin were selected as the object of this
research. We chose Intermediate 4 students because they have been
learning about language chunk and word stress. The first recording was
conducted after covering lesson 3, where at the end of the unit there is a
listening practice on language chunk. The students read a monologue (see
appendix 1), and their voice was recorded using a cellular phone. In the next
meeting, the result was played, and the teacher pointed out which parts
needed improvement. At this stage, the teacher modeled the correct chunks,
intonation, and stress. Then, the students were asked to re-read the
monologue, and re-record their voice.
The total number of the students who were involved in this research was 16,
consisting of 5 males and 11 females. Actually there were more students in
the class but we could not involve all of them because a few students did not
come either during the first recording or the second recording, so we decided
to eliminate them from the data presentation and analysis.
3. Observe: It is the third stage in which the results of the plan are analyzed. As
Burns (2010) points out, analyzing data in an action research cycle is a
continuing process of reducing information to find explanations and patterns,
which involve some steps such as: assembling the data, coding the data,
comparing the data, and building meanings and interpretation. To implement
the data analysis, we played both recordings and compared the results to see
whether there was improvement in student fluency. This stage also included
scoring the student fluency using a fluency scale rubric.
In assessing the students’ fluency, we decided to use Fountas and Pinnell
Fluency Scale because it includes the components of automaticity (reading
rate), and prosody (phrasing, chunking, intonation, and word stress).
However, we deliberately eliminated the sixth component (integration), as the
sixth component is meant for overall performance, while we intended to focus
on each component and see which component improved most and which one
improved least. For accuracy, we assumed that the students were already
between independent and instructional reading level.
4. Reflect: The last stage is making reflection and planning for further action.
Reflecting includes reflecting on practice, reflecting on the research process,
reflecting on beliefs and values, and reflecting on feelings and experiences
(Burns, 2010). It is the point where we as the researchers question our action
and the results whether it has made significant improvement to the students
or not, and whether there is other alternative plan or further action which is
more effective.
Result and Discussion
In analyzing the result we used both quantitative and qualitative analysis.
Quantitative analysis was used to measure students’ fluency in terms of nominal
scale from 1 to 4, whereas qualitative analysis is used in describing the fluency
components produced by the students while reading the text.
As previously explained, the first recording was conducted after the student finished
learning Lesson 3 of Intermediate 4 book. At the end of the lesson there was a
listening exercise where students had to identify the correct chunks in a monologue
they heard by putting slashes on the text. As a follow up of this activity, the students
were asked to read the text and their voice was recorded. Although the students
have heard the text being read to them from the cassette, on the first recording,
some of the students were unable to reproduce the text as smooth as the example.
The result is presented in the following table:
Table 1. Result of the first recording of Mon-Wed class
No
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Name
Iyet
Josh
Michael
Nabila
Nazmi
Ridho
Sean
Rate
3
3
3
3
3
2
3
Phrasing
2
3
3
2
2
2
2
Intonation
2
2
3
2
3
2
3
Pausing
2
3
3
3
3
2
2
Stress
2
3
3
3
3
2
3
Generally, students of Monday - Wednesday class were able to read the monologue
at appropriate rate, so the score for the first component (rate) was quite good;
although it still needs some improvement to make them achieve a higher score. The
students were able to start reading the text quite smoothly, however, they stumbled
over the words in the middle of the text, especially in the part : “We didn’t check
everything twice the way we normally do and instead decided to start immediately. It
turned out that the wire at the other end was not properly secured and it came
loose.” The sentence was quite long, so students had to figure out themselves
where to pause or group phrases.
It was evident that most of the students still had problem in placing the appropriate
phrasing and chunking in the text. For example, Iyet read: “twice/ the /the /way/ we
normally” and “at/ the/ the/ other end /was/ not properly/ se/cured” instead of putting
them into one phrasing. Beside repeating the word when she stumbled over some
words, she also missed some words such as “that” after “it turned out” , and she
misread some words such as “was” became “ways”, “the other” become “the
others”, and “this business” became “the business”. Thus, Iyet needed to improve
her reading ability not only in terms of fluency but also in terms of accuracy.
The other student, Josh was quite fluent but he read too fast, so he slipped in words
such as “decided” became “deciding”. His intonation was also rather flat, little
variation in tone and voice. Similarly, Ridho had a flat intonation; he seemed to read
in a hurry that he mispronounced some words such as: “secured” became “square”,
“the way we normally do” became “the way our normal do”, “came” became “come”,
and “loose” became “lost”.
Other examples of inappropriate phrasing and chunking are by Michael in reading
“the way we / normally / do”, Nazmi in reading “the other end wasn’t/ properly
secured”, or Sean in reading “everything/ twice the way/ we normally do”, and “the
wire at the other/ end was not properly”. Sean, Nazmi, and Nabila had been trying to
put stressing on certain words although it still did not sound natural. Particularly for
Nabila, not only that she used wrong phrasing, but she also had some problems with
pronunciation, so she needed more practice to improve her fluency.
Meanwhile, the result of the first recording of Tuesday- Thursday class is presented
in the following table.
Table 2. Result of the first recording of Tue-Thu class
No
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
Name
Ferina
Naufal
Rezka
Rima
Rina
Rizwan
Roshi
Yulita
Rate
3
3
4
3
2
3
1
1
Phrasing Intonation Pausing
2
3
2
3
2
3
3
3
4
3
3
3
3
2
3
3
2
3
1
1
1
1
1
2
Stress
3
2
3
3
2
2
1
1
9 Windy
3
3
3
3
3
As shown in the table, the fluency of students of Tuesday-Thursday class was quite
varied. Rezka was the best in terms of rate, as well as other components compared
to her classmates. She was able to read smoothly with appropriate pace and
pausing. The only part she hesitated was when she read: “at the other /end”. Rima
and Windy were also quite fluent, but Rima used inappropriate phrasing in reading
“check everthing/ twice/ the way we normally”, whereas Windy made some
pronunciation errors when she read immediately, lesson and she stumbled on
reading ”concentration”
Other students mostly have problems in phrasing, stress, and intonation. For
example, Ferina made inappropriate phrasing in reading “It wasn’t/ because I lost my
concentration”, “We didn’t check/ everything/ twice the way/ we normally/ (do) and
instead decided”, “the wire at the other end/ wasn’t/ properly /secured”. Rizwan
stumbled over the part “check everything twice the way/ we normally do/ and instead
decided to/ start immediately”. Similarly, Naufal and Rina used inappropriate
phrasing in reading “everything twice/ the way we normally do”. In terms of
intonation and stress, Naufal was less expressive, while in terms of rate, Rina read
rather fast so she made some pronunciation errors in reading immediately, twice,
came, and lesson.
The worst fluency was shown by Roshi and Yulita. Referring to three components of
fluency: accuracy, automaticity, and prosody as suggested by Ford (2012), these two
students were the least fluent. It actually surprised us that students at Intermediate 4
level could be at frustrational reading level (less than 90% accuracy). Roshi read
poorly and awkwardly as she tended to read word per word with lots of hesitation,
and her reading rate was 58 seconds, much longer compared to her peers who took
only between 30 – 35 seconds to read the monologue. She misread words such as it
wasn’t became I wasn’t, and she also had difficulty in pronouncing some words such
as concentration, immediately, wire, secured, came, business, disastrous, crucial. All
this affected other component of fluency, that is, prosody, which includes phrasing,
intonation, pausing, and stress.
Similarly, Yulita still lacked in accuracy and automaticity. She read slowly with lots of
hesitation, repeated some words when she felt unsure about the way to say it,
misread some words such as everything became anything, and paused too long at
certain parts such as a... mistake, rush…ing. She used inappropriate chunks such
as “Yes, we made a/mistake that day”, “we normally / do and/ instead/ decided”, “It
turned /out that / the wire /at the other / end was not properly/ secured”. As for
reading rate, it took her 59 seconds to finish the monologue. It means she had
problem in recognizing and decoding words automatically, not to mention reading
them smoothly with appropriate phrasing and intonation.
Finding out from the first recording that the students had not yet been able to read
fluently, we decided to re-teach the students about the importance of
phrasing/chunking as part of reading fluency, and modeled how to read the text with
appropriate chunking, intonation, and stressing. Then we asked the students to
reread the monologue, and their voice was again recorded. The results of the
second recording are presented in table 3 and 4.
Table 3. Result of the second recording of Mon-Wed class
No
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Name
Iyet
Josh
Michael
Nabila
Nazmi
Ridho
Sean
Rate
4
4
4
4
4
3
4
Phrasing Intonation Pausing
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
3
4
4
4
4
Stress
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
Table 4. Result of the second recording of Tue-Thu class
No
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
Name
Ferina
Naufal
Rezka
Rima
Rina
Rizwan
Roshi
Yulita
Windy
Rate
4
4
4
4
4
4
1
4
4
Phrasing Intonation
4
4
3
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
3
2
2
3
4
4
3
Pausing
4
4
4
4
4
4
3
4
4
Stress
4
4
4
4
3
4
2
4
3
These tables show that most of the students from the two classes have been able to
make much improvement in all components of fluency. Rate improved as this time
most of the students no longer read with hesitation, too slow, or too fast. They were
able to read with intonation and stress, and make the text more meaningful. What is
more important was in terms of phrasing and pausing. The correct chunking made
the students sound more fluent and the monologue more meaningful.
Iyet, for example, who previously had problem in pausing at certain part, finally were
able to improve, not only in terms of chunking but also in terms of stress and
intonation. Ridho, although still sounded in a hurry at the beginning part of the text,
also could improve in terms of pausing and stress. Rina also improved a lot, despite
her struggles with correct pronunciation. To some students, pronunciation problem
had made them hesitant in saying certain words and pause at inappropriate place,
thus hampered their fluency.
The most striking change was shown by Yulita. On the first recording she read
poorly, but on the second recording she made significant improvement in almost all
components. Her automaticity improved as it took her 33 seconds to finish the
monologue, and she was able to recognize prosodic features of fluency by using
appropriate phrasing, pausing and intonation. She only stumbled over “we
were/late”.
The only student who seemed difficult to improve was Roshi. She struggled to use
correct phrasing, and in terms of automaticity she still read too slowly (57 seconds),
and pronounced most of the words with difficulty. The teacher (Aldy) acknowledged
that among her classmates Roshi is a weak student, but she is eager to learn. Her
difficulty in producing an automatic and smooth language might be due to some
psychomotor factors.
Conclusion and Suggestion
Based on the findings above, it can be concluded that teachers could not just take
for granted that students at intermediate levels are already able to read fluently, as
there are various components of fluency that should be paid attention to and be
practiced on. We as teachers also need to ensure that students are aware of the
importance of producing the language smoothly and naturally, and should facilitate
them to achieve the expected reading fluency because it will lead to spoken fluency.
In this classroom action research, students are able to improve their fluency in
reading through voice recording, as from their recorded voice students could identify
the components of fluency which they were still weak at. After re-reading the text
and re-recording the voice, it was found that the component of fluency that most
improved was rate, phrasing, pausing, and stress. It was partly because the students
had been shown the way to use the appropriate chunking and phrasing by marking
parts in the text with slashes and then read them accordingly. The voice recording
also helped them learn where to put the appropriate stress on certain words. While
automaticity and prosody improved, it was found that the least improved component
was intonation, or where to raise or lower voice to emphasize on meaning. It was
found as well that pronunciation is another area that least improved. Pronunciation
error made the students fail to read the text smoothly, and this hampered their
fluency.
Voice recording and chunking practice conducted in this action research is only one
of many ways to help students produce the language smoothly and naturally. As our
reflection we acknowledged that there was a limitation of this research as we did not
do further action such as checking students’ comprehension to prove whether their
fluency significantly related to their comprehension of the text, neither did we
conduct a follow-up activity such as speaking tasks to prove that reading fluency
contributes to spoken ability. In regards to students’ weakness in pronunciation,
students would need a lot of repeated exposure on correct pronunciation as well as
repetition drills, and this was not done in our action research. Therefore, further
research on these aspects is strongly encouraged.
References
Burns, Anne. Doing Action Research in English Language Teaching. New York:
Routledge, 2010.
Explore 4 Student Book. Jakarta: Pusat Penerbitan Lia, 2006.
Fluency Instruction (article online), accessed on 22 July 2013, available from
http://www.reading-skills-pyramid.org/reading-fluency.htm
Ford, Karen. ELLs and Reading Fluency in English. 2012, accessed 28 July 2013,
available from http://www.colorincolorado.org/article/54200/
Fountas, Irene C., and Gay Su Pinnell. Teaching for Comprehending and Fluency.
Heinemann,
2001,
accessed
27
July
2013,
available
from
http://books.heinemann.com/comprehending/formsResources.html
Hook, Pamela E., and Sandra D. Jones "The Importance of Automaticity and
Fluency for Efficient reading Comprehension." Perspective (Spring 2004): 1621.
McCarthy, Michael. "Spoken Fluency Revisited." English Profile Journal 1, No.
1.(2010): 1-15.
Reading Fluency (article online), accessed on 21 July 2013, available from
http://www.learningrx.com/reading-fluency.htm
Robertson, Kristina. Reading 101 for English Language Learners. 2009, accessed
22 July 2013, available from http://www.colorincolorado.org/article/33830/
Thornbury, Scott. How to Teach Speaking. Pearson Longman, 2005
Yamashita, Junko, and Shingo Ichikawa. "Examining reading fluency in a foreign
language: Effects of text segmentation on L2 readers." Reading in a Foreign
Language 22, No. 2 (October 2010): 263-283.
Appendix 1: Monologue to be read by students
Yes, we made a mistake that day. It wasn’t because I lost my concentration or
anything like that. On that day we were late and people had been waiting to see the
show. We didn’t check everything twice the way we normally do and instead
decided to start immediately. It turned out that the wire at the other end was not
properly secured and it came loose. But we learned our lesson. In this business
rushing things can be disastrous; being careful is crucial. (84 words)
(Source: Explore 4 Lesson 3 p. 19)
Appendix 2: NAEP Oral Reading Fluency Scale
NAEP Oral Reading Fluency Scale
Fluent
Reads primarily in larger, meaningful phrase groups. Although
some regressions, repetitions, and deviations from text may be
Level
present, these do not appear to detract from the overall structure
4
of the story. Preservation of the author's syntax is consistent.
Some or most of the story is read with expressive interpretation.
Level
3
Reads primarily in three- or four-word phrase groups. Some
small groupings may be present. However, the majority of
phrasing seems appropriate and preserves the syntax of the
author. Little or no expressive interpretation is present.
Level
2
Reads primarily in two-word phrases with some three- or fourword groupings. Some word-by-word reading may be present.
Word groupings may seem awkward and unrelated to larger
context of sentence or passage.
Level
1
Reads primarily word-by-word. Occasional two-word or threeword phrases may occur – but these are infrequent and/or they
do not preserve meaningful syntax.
Nonfluent
SOURCE: U.D. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for
Education Statistics, National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), 2002 Oral Reading Study
Appendix 3: Scale for assessing fluency by Fountas & Pinnel