Abstract
Though there is an expanding focus on the beneficial role of pets in the fields of
nursing and veterinary medicine, the social sciences have been behind in paying attention
to the significant role that pets play in human lives. Much has been made of findings that
pet dogs may have a significant impact on physiological measures of health. However,
dogs have also been associated with psychological measures of well-being, both through
animal-assisted therapy and in the general population of dog owners. Whether the
mechanism is touch, exercise, attachment, nonevaluative social support, or some
combination of these, the human connection to the non-human animal world merits
further investigation. Previous results have been mixed, and studies suffer from a lack of
large sample sizes or sufficient control conditions, among other weaknesses.
The current study attempts to address some of the gaps in the literature by
assessing the impact of the presence of pet dogs on their owners‟ responses to a negative
mood induction procedure. Controlling for dog ownership as well as for the presence of
the dog, and collecting demographic information from each participant in addition to
measures of self-esteem, depression, social support, attitudes towards pets, and
attachment to pets, this study found that among single female dog owners, positive
attitudes towards animals were associated with positive mood prior to the mood induction.
In addition, dog owners accompanied by their dogs experienced significantly lower
despondency scores compared to non-owners prior to the mood induction. However, the
presence of a pet dog was associated with increases in anxiety and apprehension
subsequent to the mood induction, suggesting the importance of considering contextual
factors when evaluating the emotional benefits of dog ownership.
Emotional Benefits of Dog Ownership:
Impact of the Presence of a Pet Dog on Owners‟ Responses to Negative Mood
Induction
by
Eve E. Beals
May 2009
Submitted to the New School for Social Research of the New School in partial
fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.
Dissertation Committee
Dr. Joan Miller
Dr. Marcel Kinsbourne
Dr. Michael Schober
Dr. Jim Miller
© Eve E. Beals 2009
iv
Acknowledgments
I would like to thank Dr. Eric Jones at Mary Baldwin College and Dr. Laura
Massa at Loyola Marymount University for their help with the nonparametric statistical
analyses of my data.
I would like to thank the Security department of the New School for allowing
dogs into the building for my study, and the staff of Central United Methodist Church for
allowing me to use their meeting rooms and also permitting dogs in their building.
Special thanks are due to my committee members, particularly my committee
chair Dr. Joan Miller, and to Dr. Marcel Kinsbourne, Dr. Michael Schober, and Dr. Jim
Miller as the Dean‟s Representative.
Thanks to my family, to my parents and especially to John for patience and
support throughout the research, data analysis, and writing process.
This dissertation is dedicated to all my participants and their dogs, and to the
companion animals in my life, both past and present.
for Pooh, Pookie, Sassy, Krishna, and Missy
v
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv
INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Review of the Literature . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
Physiological Impact of Dogs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
Benefits of Dogs for Marginalized Populations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12
Animal-Assisted Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
Pets Across the Life Course: The Family Pet . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
Why Do People Love Their Pets? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
Gaps in the Literature . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40
How Does Mood Change? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42
The Current Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46
Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50
Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59
Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
Tables
1. Demographic Variables by Owner Status and Experimental Group . . . 92
2. Main Differences Between Experimental Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94
Appendices
A. Beck Depression Inventory – II . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96
B. Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98
C. Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99
D. Pet Attitude Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100
E. Comfort from Companion Animals Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101
F. Visual Analogue Mood Scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102
G. Reworded Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
1
"There's just something about dogs that makes you feel good. You come home, they're
thrilled to see you. They're good for the ego."
-Janet Schnellman
Introduction
As popular wisdom has it, a dog is “man‟s best friend.” Dogs have certainly
played a role writ large in the history of human domestication of nonhuman animals, with
their presence as human companions going back not merely centuries, but millennia. As
Peter Messent and James Serpell (1981) point out, archeological evidence of
domestication only suggests when humans began to feed and care for their canine
companions, taking on a more active role in the selective breeding process that has
brought us the many specialized breeds and the characteristics of childlike playfulness
and loyalty that many dog owners so value in their pets today. Skeletal remains suggest
that the wolflike ancestors of Canis familiaris lived alongside our own early human
ancestors possibly as long as 500,000 years ago, a much longer history than any have
suggested for other domesticated animal companions such as the cat or the horse. So the
question to be asked is what characteristics of dogs and the people who have shared their
lives since prehistoric times have contributed to such a long-lasting bond.
Socially, domestic dogs have a tendency to “acknowledge the dominant status of
humans” (Messent & Serpell, 1981, p. 14), and dogs also use a variety of means to
communicate nonverbally that their owners can respond to and attribute emotional
meaning to. Dogs also tend to be playful, and are relatively high in intelligence, as well
as being active during the day and generally responding well to house-training. Over
2
time, dog breeders have also tended to selectively breed for features such as tameness and
docility as well as for physical characteristics that appeal to their particular wishes.
While some breeders may be seeking dogs that will be good hunting companions or
guard dogs, generally the trend has been to favor more infantile or anthropomorphic
attributes among dogs being kept primarily as pets or companion animals.
Recently, there has been an increasing interest in understanding the importance of
the “human-animal bond” among researchers in a variety of disciplines. Social workers,
nurses, veterinarians, anthropologists, occupational therapists, and psychologists (to name
just a few) have all begun to seek answers to the question of why humans seek an affinity
with their animal companions and what are the potential benefits to creating and
sustaining these bonds, as well as considering the possible costs and drawbacks. There
have been calls for more centralized research on the topic to draw together sources from
disparate but interrelated fields, and the creation of groups such as Delta Society and the
field of anthrozoology, with articles being published in the journal Anthrozoös (Hines,
2003). Beginning with a review of literature in the field to date, and then looking more
closely at particular areas in which the benefits of dog ownership have been studied, this
paper will then conclude with the description of an original study designed to test the
hypothesis that pet dogs may serve as a buffer against negative mood. This hypothesis is
based on the notion that the human-animal bond has many characteristics in common
with attachment between humans, and that pets dogs are a source of social and emotional
support. Dogs help their owners maintain a connection with the natural world, and are
perceived as nonevaluative and empathic.
3
Review of the Literature
Viewing the human-animal bond as a form of attachment and a source of social
support, such a bond may be seen as conducive to therapeutic work, with domestic
animals facilitating the building of a therapeutic alliance as well as providing
opportunities for individuals to develop caring and nurturing roles in relation to
companion animals. Companion animals have been used in mental health settings as
long ago as the York Retreat in 1792 (Netting, Wilson, & New, 1987), and there is a
growing body of evidence suggesting that they may offer physical health benefits as well
(Jennings, 1997). With some studies emphasizing the benefits of pet ownership, while
others focus on animal-assisted interventions such as animal-assisted activities (AAA) or
animal-assisted therapy (AAT), there are a variety of approaches in the literature to
investigating the human-animal bond. Even focusing specifically on the potential
benefits of dogs, studies range from anecdotal reports of dog visitation programs to
epidemiological studies of the health benefits of dog walking, so this review will limit
itself largely to research describing how people may benefit emotionally from
interactions with dogs.
The stress-reducing value of the human-animal bond may be based on the
nonjudgmental stance of dogs, and Judith Siegel (1993) argues that dogs may be more
beneficial at times of stress, and that the amount of support they provide may be
dependent on the strength of the person‟s attachment to the dog. Attachment theory may
be applied to interspecies as well as intraspecies relationships (Melson, 2002), and Alan
Beck and Aaron Katcher (2003) draw on the biophilia hypothesis and social support
4
theory in their work, suggesting that the human brain is “hardwired with a predisposition
to pay attention to animals and the stimulus properties of the surrounding environment”
(p. 80). All these authors provide support for studying the human-animal bond within a
larger contextual or ecological approach, one that is often overlooked in the literature by
a failure to control for the environmental factors (single-family homes, back yards, etc.)
that often accompany dog ownership.
As the domestic dog is the most commonly owned pet in today‟s society (Wells,
2007), understanding what motivates dog owners to acquire dogs – as well as
understanding why some people do not want dogs or choose to give up their dogs after
acquiring them – seems an important goal for both human and animal welfare. As
sources of social support, dogs may act as buffers against the stress of traumatic events
and contribute to reductions in loneliness, anxiety, and depression as well as increases in
self-esteem. They may also act as “social lubricants” by promoting interactions between
people. Understanding the ways in which dogs may impact human psychological wellbeing, and identifying which humans are most likely to benefit, are among the primary
goals in research on the human-companion animal bond.
"A dog is one of the remaining reasons why some people can be persuaded to go for a
walk."
-O. A.
Physiological impact of dogs. Though focusing primarily on cardiovascular
health measures such as blood pressure and heart rate, considering that there is a strong
5
connection between physical and psychological well-being, a brief summary of research
on the physiological benefits of human-canine interactions is relevant. The role of touch
is important in the establishment of attachment, and several authors suggest that petting
and playing with dogs has overall health benefits as well. Erika Friedmann and Aaron
Katcher are at the forefront of research in this field, finding a survival advantage for pet
owners one year after being diagnosed with coronary heart disease (Friedmann, Katcher,
Lynch & Thomas, 1980), though the effect was fairly small after controlling for initial
illness severity, employment, age, mood, and place of birth (Wright & Moore, 1982).
They followed up their initial study, finding a survival advantage for dog owners
specifically, independently of other factors, though human social support also tended to
predict survival (Friedmann & Thomas, 1985, 1995).
The first authors to compare how people respond to familiar versus unfamiliar
dogs (Baun, Bergstrom, Langston & Thoma, 1984) found the largest decreases in blood
pressure when participants pet their own dogs rather than unfamiliar dogs, suggesting a
role of attachment in determining the physiological effect of a dog‟s presence. Aaron
Katcher (1985) found that the presence of a dog could lower blood pressure in children
completing a reading task, even if the children did not interact with the dog; Jenkins
(1986) found that dog owners exhibited lower blood pressure while petting their dogs
compared to reading aloud, and Wilson (1987) found the same effect for an unfamiliar
dog with college students. Some authors did not find the same results (Gaydos &
Farnham, 1988), observing that some participants were excited by the novelty of petting
an unfamiliar dog and that their excitement may have canceled out any effect of the
6
presence of the dog on their blood pressure readings; also, dog owners who pet their own
dogs were sometimes anxious about their dogs‟ behaviors, or had high-energy dogs that
they were used to playing with actively outdoors. Therefore, many variables need to be
considered when considering the possible effects of human-dog interactions.
Other authors have moved beyond basic measures of physiological change to
investigate concomitant changes in participants‟ levels of anxiety (Wilson, 1991), but it is
the work of Karen Allen and her colleagues that marked a shift to the specific
examination of the impact of a dog‟s presence in stressful situations (Allen, Blascovich,
Tomaka, & Kelsey, 1991). While reading a book out loud may be considered moderately
stressful, Allen et al.‟s female participants had to perform mental arithmetic tasks by
counting out loud backwards by 13s or 17s from a 4-digit number while in the presence
of a friend, a dog, or only the experimenter (the experimenter was present in all three
conditions). These authors found that physiological reactivity was significantly higher in
the friend present condition than in the control condition, whereas it was significantly
lower in the dog present condition than in the control condition, suggesting that it is not
just physical contact or social support but also a nonevaluative stance that may contribute
to the buffering effect of pet dogs in a stressful situation.
Allen, Shykoff and Izzo (2001) made another unique contribution to the literature
by assigning pet ownership status randomly to a group of hypertensive stockbrokers who
had all expressed a willingness to acquire a pet; though all the participants took blood
pressure medication, resulting in overall lower resting levels of blood pressure, the dog
owners in the presence of their dogs exhibited significantly lower responses to mental
7
stress. The mental arithmetic stress task was used again (Allen, Blascovich, & Mendes,
2002) to demonstrate that pet owners had significantly lower heart rate and blood
pressure levels during a resting baseline, reacted less to the mental arithmetic as well as
to a cold pressor stimuli, and also recovered more rapidly, with the lowest reactivity and
the quickest recovery in the pet-present conditions relative to a friend-present or a
spouse-present condition. Among pet owners, the presence of the pet and spouse resulted
in significantly more reactivity than with just the pet present, but significantly less
reactivity than with just the spouse present. Also, participants tended to make fewer
errors on the mental arithmetic tasks in the presence of their pets.
Some authors have also suggested an applied use for the finding that pet dogs may
reduce blood pressure in stressful situations, with Nagengast, Baun, Megel, and
Liebowitz (1997) finding reductions in children‟s blood pressure, heart rate, and
behavioral distress in the presence of a dog during a physical examination. Havener,
Gentes, Thaler, Megal, Baun, Driscoll, Beiraghi, and Agrawal (2001) did not find the
same effects for children visiting a dentist‟s office, though for children who expressed
verbal distress when they first arrived at the clinic, the presence of the dog decreased
physiological arousal while the child was waiting for the dentist to arrive. The amount of
distress may be related to the potential for benefiting from the presence of a dog. Finally,
attitudes towards pets in general as well as attachment to a specific pet may be related to
how participants respond to the presence of a dog (Friedmann, Locker, & Lockwood,
1993), with college students who had more positive attitudes towards animals
demonstrating lower cardiovascular responses to verbalization with a dog present.
8
Within the past decade, a greater ability to measure neurochemical changes has
contributed a new area of research to the study of human-animal interactions. J. S. J.
Odendaal has done most of this work, with his first study (Odendaal, 2000)
demonstrating that the neurochemicals involved in attention-seeking behaviors increase
during positive interactions between humans and dogs. He also found that dog owners
interacting with their own dogs had higher oxytocin levels than dog owners interacting
with strange dogs; oxytocin is commonly regarded as a measure of social attachment.
Odendaal and Lehmann (2000) found similar neurochemical changes in both dogs and
their owners after positive interactions, indicating a high degree of interspecies social
symbiosis. Some of the neurochemicals involved include beta-endorphin (associated
with analgesia and euphoric states), oxytocin (associated with intimate bonding),
prolactin (associated with nurturing and parenting behavior), phenylethylamine
(associated with attachment), and dopamine (associated with pleasure) (Odendaal &
Meintjes, 2003). Nagasawa, Kikusui, Onaka, and Mitsuaki (2009) lend further support to
this research, finding that a dog‟s gazing at its owner is sufficient to increase the owner‟s
levels of urinary oxytocin during social interaction. In addition, they found that owners
who reported a higher degree of attachment to their dogs also received a greater duration
of gaze from their dogs and had higher levels of urinary oxytocin after social interaction.
Other authors have sought out evidence of more general health benefits of dog
ownership. Anderson, Reid, and Jennings (1992) found that pet owners had significantly
lower blood pressure and cholesterol levels than non-owners, but Robb and Stegman
(1983) did not find any differences between pet and non-pet groups on health-related
9
variables. Friedmann and Thomas (1985) suggest an indirect benefit of pets on health; if
loneliness causes increased morbidity and mortality, pets may decrease loneliness.
Judith Siegel (1990) used a prospective design of 1,036 Medicare recipients in
California, finding that respondents with pets had fewer total doctor contacts over a oneyear period than those without pets, even when controlling for other demographic
variables; additionally, while stressful life events tended to precipitate more doctor visits
among respondents without a pet, stressful life events were unrelated to doctor visits
among respondents with a dog. Stallones, Marx, Garrity, and Johnson (1990) did not find
a significant association between pet ownership, attachment to pets, and illness among a
group of 1,300 respondents; however, younger adult (21-34 years of age) and older adult
(45-65 years of age) pet owners who had stronger human social support networks tended
to be less attached to their pets, and among 35-44 year olds, pet attachment was
positively associated with higher depression scores; age clearly needs to be taken into
account when evaluating the potential impact of pet ownership.
Dog owners reported a highly significant reduction in minor health problems and
had elevated scores on a health questionnaire 10 months after the initial acquisition of a
pet (Serpell, 1991). Garrity and Stallones (1998) believe that the health impact of pets is
due to their role as sources of social support, where “pets are viewed as a type of
nonhuman social support capable of conferring health and behavioral benefits on their
human partners” (p. 5). Budge, Spicer, Jones, and St. George (1998) suggest that it is
when owners are more compatible with their pets that they are the most likely to
experience physical and psychological benefits from pet ownership; they also
10
hypothesize that the mixed findings relating pet attachment to health may be due to some
owners with more health problems becoming more attached to their pets because they are
more limited in their social interactions outside the home.
In the past decade, Australian epidemiological research has focused a great deal
on the association of pets with well-being, with Bruce Headey (1999) supporting Siegel‟s
earlier (1990) findings that pet owners make fewer annual doctors‟ visits. He also found
that both dog and cat owners were less likely to be on medications for heart problems and
sleeping difficulties than non-owners. The three gender-age groups who tended to utilize
physician services the most (young women, older women and older men) also showed the
greatest health benefit from owning pets. Dog owners in particular reported feeling less
lonely than non-owners, and a majority of pet owners felt they had met other people
through their pets, and that having a pet facilitated conversations. In contrast, Parslow
and Jorm (2003) found that among Australians aged 40-44 measures of physical and
mental health were not significantly associated with pet ownership, and pet owners
tended to use pain relief medication more frequently than non-owners; they suggest that
the age group involved is less likely to benefit from pets, as they were also more likely to
have dependent children and to be in the workforce; there are circumstances in which
caring for a pet may become more of a chore than a pleasure.
Overall, pet owners who continuously own pets are in the best health, whereas
people who either stop owning pets or never owned pets are less healthy (Headey &
Grabka, 2007). Headey, Na and Zheng (2008) had the unique opportunity to conduct a
“natural experiment” on pet ownership, since pets had been banned from urban areas in
11
China until 1992. Among 3,031 women, half of whom owned dogs, the dog owners
reported better health, more exercise, better sleep, fewer sick days off from work, and
fewer visits to doctors.
Most recently, there has been a surge in research specifically investigating the
relationship between dog ownership and dog walking. Bauman, Russell, Furber, and
Dobson (2001) found that while dog owners walked on average 18 minutes per week
more than non-owners, more than half of dog owners did not walk their dogs at all. In
addition, dog owners in their study were actually less likely than non-owners to meet
recommended levels of physical activity, suggesting that exercise is not likely to be the
means by which dogs convey health benefits to their owners. Brown and Rhodes (2006)
did find that dog owners spent more time in mild and moderate physical activities and
walked an average of 300 minutes per week compared to non-owners who walked an
average of 168 minutes per week, but there was not a difference between dog owners and
non-owners for more strenuous physical activity; also, once dog walking was removed,
dog walkers walked less and were less physically active than non-owners. They found
that dog owners who felt more of an obligation to care for their dog were more likely to
walk their dogs more often.
The primary source of information on dog walking is the Australian RESIDE
study, a 5-year longitudinal study evaluating the impact of a state government
subdivision code in Perth, Western Australia. Hayley Cutt, Billie Giles-Corti, Matthew
Kniuman, and Valerie Burke (2006) use an ecological model to explore factors that may
obstruct or facilitate owner‟s decisions to walk their dogs in public. They observe
seasonal variations in dog walking patterns, as well as an association between owners‟
12
levels of attachment to their dogs and the size of the dogs with frequency of dog walking
behaviors. They propose that it is not only the exercise but also the contact with other
people in the community and the time spent in a natural environment that contribute to
the health-enhancing properties of walking dogs. Cutt, Giles-Corti, and Knuimann (2007)
found that while 23% of dog owners did not walk their dogs, among dog walkers, more
dog walkers achieved 150 minutes of physical activity per week than dog owners who did
not walk their dogs. Cutt, Giles-Corti, Knuiman, Timperio, and Bull (2008) also found
that dog owners saw their neighborhoods as more attractive than did non-owners,
reported higher neighborhood cohesion than non-owners, and had higher confidence in
their ability to adhere to a daily walking schedule irrespective of barriers such as work,
family, or social commitments. While dog ownership does not automatically result in an
increase in physical activity, dog owners who do walk their dogs are more likely to
achieve more minutes of moderate to vigorous physical activity than non-owners or dog
owners who do not walk their dogs (Coleman, Rosenberg, Conway, Sallis, Saelens, Frank,
& Cain, 2008).
"No animal I know of can consistently be more of a friend and companion than a dog."
-Stanley Leinwoll
Benefits of dogs for marginalized populations. Aside from the obviously
utilitarian benefits of service dogs, ranging from guide dogs helping their blind owners
navigate to assistance dogs performing tasks for wheelchair-bound owners, many authors
13
have suggested that service dogs provide social support for their owners as well. These
dogs help to facilitate interactions in the community, as many individuals without
disabilities have a tendency to exhibit social discomfort in the presence of individuals
with disabilities, demonstrating gaze avoidance, greater personal distance, and briefer
social interactions (Eddy, Hart, & Boltz, 1988). Eddy et al. found that passersby tended
to smile more often and engage in more conversations with participants in wheelchairs
when a service dog was present. On the down side, some participants felt that their
service dogs received more acknowledgments than they did. Children with disabilities
have also been found to benefit socially from the presence of a service dog (Mader and
Hart, 1989), and individuals with hearing ear dogs reported feeling less lonely, safer, and
more independent, as well as experiencing more friendliness from strangers (Valentine,
Kiddoo, and LaFleur, 1993).
Owners of assistance dogs reported an increased sense of social integration and a
supportive relationship with their dog (Lane, McNicholas, & Collis, 1998), blind guide
dog owners felt the support provided by their dogs surpassed their expectations (Steffens
& Bergler, 1998). There were stronger emotional ties between blind people and their
dogs than in an average population of dog owners. Guide dogs brought them into more
contact with other people, reduced tension and strain, and promoted more relaxed
walking.
Another instance of dogs contributing to well-being in marginalized populations
is among prisoners, with Aaron Katcher, Alan Beck, and Deniel Levine (1989) describing
the People, Animals and Love (PAL) program at the District of Columbia Department of
14
Corrections Lorton facility. Though they found a small decrease in the number of
offenses by the pet group compared to a control group during the first year after pet
acquisition, it was not statistically significant. Other authors found decreased depression
among women prisoners training dogs to become companions for the elderly and
individuals with disabilities (Walsh & Mertin, 1996) but their follow-up was done when
the women were about to be released from prison, creating a possible confound in the
interpretation of positive effects. Earl Strimple (2003) found one prison associating
participation in a dog training program with reduced rates of recidivism, though Gennifer
Furst (2006) notes that inmates are generally selected for these programs based on good
behavior. Fournier, Geller, and Fortney (2007) did find an increase in social sensitivity
scores among prisoners participating in a human-animal interaction program with dogs,
as well as increased treatment progress in the therapeutic community and decreased
institutional infractions. While some might argue that prisons are intended to punish
rather than rehabilitate, it is possible that inmate-animal interaction programs may have a
larger benefit on society as a whole if they contribute to decreased recidivism; in addition,
prisoners are often training animals for adoption or to prepare them for future roles as
guide dogs or therapy dogs.
Among the most socially isolated and marginalized members of our society are
the homeless. While homelessness clearly comes with a decreased ability to financially
care for a pet, there are nonetheless a large number of homeless people with pets. Aline
and Robert Kidd (1994) found that homeless pet owners were extremely attached to their
pets and had owned significantly more pets in childhood and adolescence than non-
15
owners. They also observed that the presence of a pet made other people friendlier.
Taylor, Williams, and Gray (2004) found that homeless dog owners were less likely to be
taking drugs than non-owners, though they were also more likely to have health problems
since they often gave food to their pets before themselves, and avoided visits to the
doctor partly because they were worried they might be separated from their pets.
Finally, though some health professionals raise concerns regarding the safety of
housing pets with immunocompromised individuals, in other cases the emotional benefits
of pet ownership among patients with HIV and AIDS may outweigh the risks, so long as
proper hygiene and reasonable precautions are maintained. Betty Carmack (1991) found
that gay men with HIV or AIDS perceived their pets as providing affection, support,
nurturance, and acceptance. They also said that the presence of their pets made it easier
to talk to other people. Siegel, Angulo, Detels, Wesch, and Mullen (1999) found that
participants in the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study with AIDS who owned pets reported
less depression than participants with AIDS who did not own pets; this pattern was
strongest among those who additionally reported having few confidants.
"There is no psychiatrist in the world like a puppy licking your face."
-Ben Williams
Animal-assisted therapy. The bulk of the literature on the benefits of pets is in the
field of animal-assisted therapy (AAT). Early writers in this field were already proposing
that humans have an innate need to affiliate with animals, prior to the introduction of the
biophilia hypothesis (Brickel, 1986), and as of 1995, there were approximately 2,000
16
distinct AAT programs in the United States alone (Voelker, 1995). The most commonly
used animal is the dog, though not all dogs are equally suited to becoming therapy
animals. In the 1990s, The Delta Society, an international and interdisciplinary group
with research, educational, and public service activities in the area of interaction between
people and animals developed the first comprehensive standards of practice,
differentiating between programs that “incorporate animals for the purposes of
entertainment or generalized population benefits” as animal-assisted activities (AAA),
and “those that seek to cause a prescribed effect on specific patients” as animal-assisted
therapy (AAT) (Hines & Fredrickson, 1998).
One meta-analysis of empirical research to date on AAT (Nimer & Lundahl, 2007)
found that 49 out of 250 studies reported on AAT rather than AAA or pet ownership,
included five or more participants in a treatment group, and included data sufficient for
the calculation of an effect size. The authors identified moderate effect sizes in
improving outcomes in four areas: autism spectrum disorders, medical difficulties,
behavioral problems, and emotional well-being; the use of dogs was most consistently
associated with moderately high effect sizes. Individuals with disabilities tended to
benefit more on medical outcomes, whereas non-disabled individuals tended to benefit
more on well-being and behavioral outcomes. Young children consistently benefited
across all outcome variables, while other age groups were less consistent in the degree to
which they benefited from AAT. The authors also noted that it might be possible to
“overdo” AAT, as there were negative associations between the number of sessions and
17
medical outcomes. They speculate that the use of animals in therapy may be beneficial
because “animals seem to have a natural tendency to create a bond with people” (p. 226).
"The dog was created specially for children. He is the god of frolic."
-Henry Ward
Animal-assisted interventions with children. Boris Levinson, widely recognized
as the founder of animal-assisted psychotherapy, stumbled upon the therapeutic benefits
of his pet dachshund, Jingles, when the unplanned presence of his dog in the office
facilitated a positive reaction from a previously withdrawn child. Levinson (1961)
promotes the belief that the importance of pets “is psychological rather than practical. In
many ways, the relationship between man and dog, especially between child and dog, can
be more salutary than one between two human beings” (p. 59). He surmises that the
provision of acceptance, companionship, and emotional expression are more crucial to
therapy than any prescribed theory or technique, and that pets can be a source of these
benefits. Since children are rarely the ones to initiate therapy, a dog can be a potential
icebreaker for children who do not feel comfortable interacting with a therapist.
Levinson also expresses optimism that pets can be used to encourage children to become
more socially oriented and empathic.
Though it took more than twenty years for research on AAT with children to
begin in earnest, Yvonne Gonski (1985) describes the use of dogs as therapy in a child
welfare setting, observing that the presence of her dogs elicited conversations from the
children. While children in the foster care system often have difficulties in forming
18
attachments and are frequently distrustful of adults due to histories of inconsistent
parenting and abuse or neglect, dogs may serve as non-threatening sources of comfort
and affection that can act as a bridge between child and caseworker. According to
Gonski, dog therapy exploits “the natural affinity that most children have for animals and
the spontaneous and ingenuous bond that exists between them . . . A child‟s involvement
with an animal can teach him/her responsibility, trust, empathy, and affection” (p. 101).
Emotionally disturbed children have demonstrated improved social relations with
other children after AAT (Kogan, Granger, Fitchett, Helmer, & Young, 1999), and AAT
has also been used in residential treatment facilities (Mallon, 1994) and with abused
children (Reichert, 1994, 1998; Parish-Plass, 2008), with therapists observing that the
presence of a dog may help the child to disclose abuse and to express feelings. Because a
dog is seen as nonjudgmental, children may feel more confident when sharing feelings
that they may be ashamed of in the presence of a dog. Parish-Plass additionally surmises
that a therapy pet may serve as an attachment figure, enabling the abused child to develop
more adaptive social strategies and more empathic abilities. Patients may also see the
clinicians that accompany pets as friendlier and less threatening (Bardill & Hutchinson,
1997).
In hospital settings, medically ill children displayed more positive affect
(Kaminski, Pellino, & Wish, 2002; Wu, Neidra, Pendergast, & McCrindle, 2002) in the
presence of a therapy dog. Dog visitation programs have also been used in hospitals to
decrease children‟s perception of pain (Sobo, Eng, & Kassity-Krich, 2006). AAT has
also been used with autistic children to increase socially appropriate behaviors and to
19
increase the overall frequency of social interactions with an adult (Redefer & Goodman,
1989; Martin & Farnum, 2002). Therapy dogs have also been shown to increase
performance on psychomotor tasks among preschoolers (Gee, Harris, & Johnson, 2007)
and to increase social responsiveness among children with developmental disabilities
(Esteves & Stokes, 2008).
AAT has been incorporated into school-based social training programs (Tissen,
Hergovich, & Spiel, 2007), with teachers observing a significant improvement in social
behavior in classrooms with a therapy dog even when social training was not part of the
curriculum; the effect was similar in magnitude to social training without the dog present
and social training with the dog present. Social training with the dogs present had a
significantly stronger impact in a positive direction on the students‟ levels of open and
relational aggression compared to social training without the dogs present or the presence
of dogs without the social training curriculum.
Animal-assisted interventions with older adults. Samuel and Elizabeth Corson
(1978, 1981) were among the first researchers to propose therapeutic benefits of pets
among the elderly. They suggest that the loss of economic and social roles that
accompany retirement and children leaving the home may contribute to decreases in selfesteem, independent functioning, socialization, and goal-directed activity. When an older
adult is additionally confined to a nursing home, the effect is only compounded, as the
psychosocial environment of a nursing home may be detrimental to the physical and
psychological well-being of already impaired clients. The positive nonverbal
communication that occurs between dogs and nursing home residents is intended to
20
counteract or alleviate frequently negative nonverbal communication that may occur
between nursing home residents and staff. The Corsons speculate that people develop an
attachment to pet dogs in particular due to their nonjudgmental nature and maintenance
of “a sort of infantile dependence” throughout their lives. They observe that even among
nursing home residents who initially relate almost exclusively to therapy pets, the pets
eventually begin to serve as social catalysts, encouraging interpersonal interactions.
Clark Brickel (1980-81) also notes that AAT increases opportunities for social
interaction among the elderly. Robb, Boyd, & Pristash (1980) found that social behaviors
among nursing home residents were highest in the presence of a caged puppy, compared
to a flowering plant or a wine bottle. Francis, Turner, & Johnson (1985), while
investigating animal visitation (AAA) rather than AAT, found that residents of an adult
home receiving visits from six puppies and their handlers when compared to residents of
an adult home receiving only visits from people without dogs demonstrated
improvements in social interaction, psychosocial function, life satisfaction, mental
function, social competence, and psychological well-being at a statistically significant
level. Other authors have not found a significant difference between pet visits and human
visits (Hendy, 1987), or have found that initial benefits demonstrate rapid attrition after
the dog visitation period is over (Winkler, Fairnie, Gericevich, & Long, 1989). Winkler
et al. additionally found a greater benefit for the nursing home staff than for the nursing
home patients.
Evidence of the social facilitation effect of pet visitation or pet therapy programs
continues to be reported fairly consistently, with Fick (1993) observing that the presence
21
of a dog during a group therapy session was associated with nearly twice the number of
verbal and nonverbal interactions among nursing home residents during a socialization
group. Kaiser, Spence, McGavin, Struble, & Keilman (2002) found that a dog elicited
more pro-social behaviors from nursing home residents than did a “happy person,” but
that this was primarily due to patting the dog; when patting the dog was removed, prosocial behaviors in response to the dog were similar in number to those in response to the
happy person. Marian and William Banks (2002) found that AAT was associated with
decreased reports of loneliness among nursing home residents, and Prosser, Townsend,
and Staiger (2008) observed increases in social interactions between nursing home
residents who participated in AAT.
Alzheimer‟s and other dementias. Kongable, Buckwalter, and Stolley (1989)
found that the presence of a pet dog increased the total number of social behaviors of
Alzheimer‟s clients in a veteran‟s home. They observe that for Alzheimer‟s patients,
interacting with pets provides companionship and social stimulation without the
requirement of high intellectual, cognitive or social skills. The nonjudgmental nature of
pets means that they provide affection and companionship regardless of the patient‟s
cognitive or physical abilities, and this unconditional acceptance may have a restorative
effect on the Alzheimer‟s patient‟s self-esteem. Walsh, Mertin, Verlander, and Pollard
(1995) suggest that such effects may be palliative rather than therapeutic, modifying
extreme behaviors and generating pro-social responses in the presence of the dog, but not
producing lasting measurable changes in adaptive functioning.
22
The overall frequency of smiles, touching, gaze, warmth and praise as well as
duration scores for smiles, touching, gaze and physical proximity were significantly
higher in the presence of a pet dog and human visitor compared to a human visitor alone
(Batson, McCabe, Baun, & Wilson). Churchill, Safaoui, McCabe, and Baun (1999)
reported similar findings, and Richeson (2003) also found a significant increase in social
interactions among patients with dementia while they were participating in AAT, though
the effects did not persist for long beyond the intervention period. Sellers (2005) found a
significant increase in social behavior of dementia patients receiving AAT from baseline
to treatment, though her study lacked a comparison group for control.
Other authors have found emotional benefits for Alzheimer‟s and other dementia
patients receiving AAT, with Kawamura, Niiyama, & Niiyama (2007) finding
improvements in stability and appropriateness of emotional expression over a 12-month
period. The same authors (Kawamura, Niiyama, & Niiyama, 2009) additionally found
that Alzheimer‟s and dementia patients who were likely to benefit from AAA or AAT
were more likely to have owned a dog in the past; the therapy dogs triggered memories of
their own dogs. They felt that the dogs provided a break from their daily routines, and
gave them the opportunity to interact with other residents and staff. They also enjoyed
communicating with the volunteers who accompanied the dogs.
AAT and mental illness. Arlene Siegel‟s (1962) writing on the use of AAT with
mentally ill patients predates most of the work of Boris Levinson, and it was actually
veterinarians who were among the first to notice “the therapeutic effect of pets on people
with physical or mental illnesses who are depressed or even suicidal” (p. 1046). Among
the mentally ill, “communication may be established more easily with a pet than with a
23
parent or relative” (p. 1046). She speculates that severely withdrawn patients,
particularly schizophrenics, may be reachable through pets.
The Corsons were among the first to empirically validate such observations
(Corson, Corson, Gwynne, & Arnold; 1975, 1977), finding that psychiatrically
hospitalized patients that did not demonstrate any interest in watching television would
intensely watch videotapes of their own social interactions with therapy dogs. They also
observed that in addition to individual benefits, the AAT program contributed to an
overall positive increase in social interactions on the ward as a whole. While in the initial
stages some of the patients interacted with the dogs to the exclusion of human
interactions, the dogs ended up serving as a catalyzing social link on the patient ward.
AAT has been successfully used in the rehabilitation of psychiatric inpatients,
with group therapy conducted in the presence of caged finches associated with
significantly greater improvements than therapy conducted in the absence of the finches
(Beck, Seraydarian, & Hunter, 1986). Though using birds instead of dogs, what is
noteworthy about this study is that it ended after just ten weeks because half the patients
in the treatment group were successfully discharged, while all the patients in the control
group were still hospitalized. Barker and Dawson (1998) also found statistically
significant reductions in anxiety scores after AAT for patients with psychotic disorders;
the reduction was twice as great after AAT as after therapeutic recreation. Barak, Savorai,
Mavashev, and Beni (2001) found that a one-year controlled trial of AAT was associated
24
with significant improvement in social adaptive functioning among elderly schizophrenic
patients.
Other authors found a schizophrenic AAT group demonstrating significant
improvements in anhedonia, improved use of leisure time, and improved motivation in
relation to a control group (Nathans-Barel, Feldman, Berger, Modai & Silver, 2005).
Nathans-Barel et al. especially noted that the patients reported feeling attached to the
therapy dog, said they missed the dog between sessions, and looked forward to the AAT
sessions with anticipation. During sessions, patients called the dog by name and used
terms of endearment as well as nicknames; they also expressed concern for the dog‟s
health and general well-being. After the study ended, patients expressed feelings of
sadness and longing for the dog and also asked how it was doing.
AAT has also been suggested for use with patients diagnosed with posttraumatic
stress disorder (PTSD) (Altschuler, 1999; Lefkowitz, Pharia, Prout, Debiak, & Bleiberg,
2005), though only one case study describes the actual implementation of such a program
(Sockalingam, Li, Krishnadev, Hanson, Balaban, Pacione, & Bhalerao, 2008). The
patient displayed improvements in mood, outlook on life, and increased spontaneous
speech, as well as demonstrating decreased anxiety and agitation and exhibiting improved
quality of sleep and concentration abilities. The patient also reported improvements in
physical health and reduced social isolation as the result of walking the dog. AAT has
also been incorporated into Reality Therapy (Minatrea & Wesley, 2008) with inpatients
having a history of substance abuse, with patients in the AAT group reporting a more
positive opinion of the therapeutic alliance.
25
Other health conditions. While not used to treat health conditions per se, AAT
has been used to alleviate the stressful impact of illness. Irene Muschel (1984) believes
that the effectiveness of AAT with terminal cancer patients stems from the “empathic
presence of a caring creature who can be emotionally and physically close” (p. 453).
Though the physical deterioration in many patients precluded an increase in socialization
in relation to AAT, patients did report decreases in loneliness, fear, despair, and isolation.
Not surprisingly, patients who were able to emotionally engage with the pets were more
likely to report benefits of AAT.
Florence Nightingale, who observed that pets provide companionship to the sick,
first promoted AAT among the medically ill, particularly chronically ill patients
(Jorgenson, 1997). Animals were used at the Army Air Corps Convalescent Hospital in
Pawling, New York from 1944-45, where patients worked on the hospital farm. Johnson,
Meadows, Haubner, and Sevedge (2003, 2008) found that among cancer patients waiting
for radiation treatments, both dog visits and human visits were scored more positively
than reading magazines. Participants in the dog group gave more positive responses than
did those in the human group about whether their session made radiation therapy easier
and made them feel better. They also rated the dog as a confidante and a friend to whom
they felt attached more often than did those in the human visitor group. More
participants in the dog group reported that the dog comforted them, made them feel
happy, and gave them energy than did participants in the human visitor group; they were
also more likely to tell others about the dog, to look forward to seeing the dog, and to
want to have the dog visit their home. Also, participants in the human visitor and reading
26
groups believed that their emotional health had declined during the study, whereas
participants in the dog group believed it improved.
Finally, two studies have found a positive impact of AAT for patients with
aphasia (Macauley, 2006; LaFrance, Garcia, & Labreche, 2007). Aphasic clients felt
more motivated, less stressed, and enjoyed the therapy sessions more during AAT
compared with traditional therapy. The patients also demonstrated more emotion during
AAT sessions, initiating communication about their own pets or loss of pets. The
patients tended to speak with more effort when responding to the clinician than when
asked to direct their responses toward the dog, and initiated more speech during AAT
sessions than traditional sessions; most of these initiations were directed towards the dog.
Impact of AAA/AAT on caregivers. If the presence of a therapy dog facilitates
social interactions, it is likely to do so for the staff and therapists who work with patients
as well. The emotional support provided by a dog may also buffer against “burn out”
among therapists who use AAT, though this has not been studied systematically. In
addition, the presence of a therapy dog may change the behaviors of the therapists as well
as the patients; Kahn and Jacobs (2003) found that physical therapists using AAT
provided directions that resulted in fewer revisions in contrast to standard treatment; in
addition, the physical therapist using AAT was less likely to interrupt the patient.
Volunteers or handlers who bring dogs for visitation programs find that the presence of
their dogs eases interactions with nursing home residents or hospital patients, situations
they might normally find socially awkward or stressful (Savishinsky, 1992). Despite
27
their relationship with their dogs, however, some volunteers still drop out due to the
unexpected emotional strain of coping with depressed or ill patients.
“Acquiring a dog may be the only time a person gets to choose a relative.”
-Unknown
Pets across the life course: the family pet
In a special issue of Marriage and Family Review on “Pets and the Family,”
several authors offer a variety of perspectives on the role of pets in family life. Cain
(1985) describes how many pet owners see their pets as family members, with pets
sometimes being brought into conflicts between family members. In addition, the vast
majority of pet owners report experiencing an important to extreme loss when they lose a
pet. Soares (1985) believes the role of a pet varies depending on how functional or
dysfunctional the family unit is, with problematic attachment patterns leading to
compulsive care giving and pathological mourning on the death of a pet. Pets may serve
as protective factors for abused children, but the pets of abused children are unfortunately
likely to be targets of abuse themselves. Attachment to pets tends to be highest among
never-married, divorced, widowed and remarried people, childless couples, newlyweds,
and empty-nesters (Albert & Bulcroft, 1988).
The beneficial role of pets in families also includes social catalysis, as couples
who have pets but no children – whether by choice or due to infertility – may use their
pets to establish involvement with other pet owners (Blenner, 1991). Family pet
attachment is also strongly related to family adaptability and family cohesion (Cox, 1993);
28
family cohesion is the emotional connectedness experienced between family members. A
majority of people with pets refer to themselves as their pets‟ parent (Cohen, 2002), and
nearly three quarters of married respondents greet their pets before their spouses.
Pets and children. Some authors have found an association between childhood
experience with pets and liking for pets in adulthood (Serpell, 1981), while others believe
that pets play a large role in children‟s emotional development (VanLeeuwen, 1981).
VanLeeuwen promotes the notion that children who never have the opportunity to
become involved with an animal are “deprived of an opportunity that usually gives depth
to a wide range of emotional experience” (p. 175). While this does not necessarily have
to be accomplished through pet ownership, learning to care for animals and feel warmth
towards them are important aspects of the human-animal bond in childhood development.
In a survey of 10-year-old children (MacDonald, 1981), most of the children surveyed
saw their dog as a companion, and additionally reported social contact with other children
while playing with or walking their dog. A majority of the children talked to their dog
and believed that their dog could understand their speech, respond to their moods, and
understand their feelings.
Children‟s attachment to pets may start quite early, as even infants between the
ages of 6 and 12 months will respond to a family dog or cat entering the room by smiling,
and may even try to follow the pet (Kidd & Kidd, 1985). Clark Brickel (1985) suggests
that children are “taught” to love animals by their families within a framework of social
learning theory, while Robin and ten Besel (1985) describe the role of pets in the
socialization of children. Pets may satisfy a child‟s need for physical contact and touch,
29
as well as empathic listening and association with others. Pets may also elicit nurturing
behaviors in children as young as 3 years old.
The age at which children have their first experience with pets may be related to
their self-concepts as adults (Poresky, Hendrix, Mosier, & Samuelson, 1987, 1988), with
college students‟ total positive self-concept scores being higher if they were under 6 or
over 10 years old when they had their first pet. Though relying on retrospective reporting,
which tends to be problematic, the authors also found that the strength of the adolescent
pet-owner bond was associated with the highest self-concept scores. Ratings of students‟
relationships to their current pets were also higher for respondents who had stronger
child-companion animal bonding scores. Poresky (1990; Poresky & Hendrix, 1990) also
found a significant correlation between empathy scores for children and empathy scores
for pets; although children who had pets did not have significantly higher empathy scores
than those that did not have pets, children who had a strong bond with a pet had higher
empathy scores than children without a pet.
Self-care children, or “latchkey kids,” may be especially likely to benefit from
having pets (Heath & McKenry, 1989; Guerney, 1991), as playing with pets was second
only to watching television for pet owners as a way of coping with boredom or loneliness.
Other authors have found a positive relationship between childhood pet ownership and
humane attitudes in adulthood (Paul & Serpell, 1993), with higher levels of childhood pet
keeping related to more positive attitude towards pet animals and greater concerns about
the welfare of non-pet animals as well as humans. Vegetarianism and involvement with
animal welfare and environmental organizations were also associated with having pets in
childhood. Ascione and Weber (1996; Ascione, 1992) also found that children‟s
30
involvement with pets was positively related to their humane attitude toward animals, and
that the enhancement of humane attitudes was generalized to human-directed empathy.
Preadolescent pet owners have been shown to have higher levels of autonomy,
more positive self-concept, and higher self-esteem relative to their non-pet-owning
counterparts (van Houte & Jarvis, 1995). A longitudinal study designed to investigate the
impact of obtaining a new dog found that dog-owning children received more visits from
their friends than their non-dog-owning counterparts, and that dog-owning families
engaged in more leisure activities at home together; however, these effects only lasted for
one month (Paul & Serpell, 1996). Robert Poresky (1996, 1997) found statistically
significant links between the strength of children‟s companion animal bond and their
empathy for other children, as well as finding that boys with dogs and girls with cats
tended to have relatively higher self-concept scores as adults compared to girls with dogs
and boys with cats.
Drawing on attachment theory, Sandra Trieibenbacher (1998) surmises that
human-pet attachments bear a good deal of similarity to human-human attachments, with
“[t]he enduring emotional bond and the reciprocal nature of the attachment
relationship . . . central components defining attachment theory” (p. 136). Adolescent
girls in particular were likely to have higher self-esteem in relation to the strength of
attachment to their pets. Adolescent pet owners also report a higher level of well-being
and more family resources than non-owners (Bodmer, 1998), though among adolescents
with few family resources, pet ownership did not serve as a buffer with relation to well-
31
being (i.e. if they had fewer family resources, well-being was lower regardless of the
presence or absence of a pet).
Pets may serve as a buffer against traumatic experiences, as Arambasic, Kerestes,
Kuterovac-Jagodic, and Vizek-Vidovic (2000) found that among Croatian children, girls
with pets other than a dog or cat had the highest levels of posttraumatic stress reactions
(PTSR), while boys without pets and girls with a dog or cat had the least PTSR. Children
with a dog or cat were more likely to exhibit coping skills such as expressing emotions,
seeking support, and problem solving relative to children without pets. Gail Melson
(2003) posits that animals are powerful motivators for children because children are
emotionally invested in them, and this emotional investment involves factors such as
social support and nurturing.
“The greatest pleasure of a dog is that you may make a fool of yourself with him and not
only will he not scold you, but he will make a fool of himself too.”
-Samuel Butler
Pet ownership and the older adult. Though primarily interested in the use of pets
in psychotherapy with children, Boris Levinson (1969) was also one of the first to
suggest the potential benefits of pet ownership for older adults. As people age, and begin
to lose relatives, friends, and work associates, they may become more socially isolated;
Levinson suggests that the non-human environment begins to play an increasingly
significant role in their lives. Pets may become sources of security and social support
again, and pets do not respond to signs of age and infirmity in the same way that other
32
people do. Levinson notes that widows and widowers may respond differently to a pet,
with widows having a greater tendency to maintain the pet as a link to their deceased
husbands, while widowers are more likely to want the pet removed.
Among older adults, pet owners tend to see themselves more favorably (Kidd &
Feldman, 1981), and particularly see themselves as more nurturing than non-owners.
Non-owners were more likely to perceive themselves as less self-accepting, more selfcentered, more pessimistic, and more dependent relative to pet owners. Among older
women, a larger percentage of women who were not attached to their pets were unhappy,
relative to women who were very attached to their pets or who did not have pets (Ory &
Goldberg, 1983). Also, pet ownership tended to be associated with greater happiness
among women with higher socioeconomic status, but with less happiness among women
with lower socioeconomic status. Other authors have found that pet ownership does not
make a difference for owners living with others but does make a difference for those
living alone (Goldmeier, 1986); while loneliness was alleviated by pet ownership in
women living alone, women living with other people were significantly less lonely
regardless of whether or not they owned a pet.
Among the bereaved, pet ownership and strong attachment were significantly
associated with less depression only when the number of available human confidants was
minimal (Stallones, Marx, Garrity, & Johnson, 1988). For older adults with less human
social support, greater attachment to a pet is associated with less reported illness, whereas
for older adults with higher levels of human social support, attachment to a pet is
associated with slightly greater reported illness. Lago, Delaney, Miller, and Grill (1989)
33
conducted a longitudinal study of pet owners, finding lower mortality rates among
current pet owners and higher mortality rates among former owners who no longer
owned pets. Miller, Staats, and Partlo (1992) similarly found pet owners to have greater
perceived physical health than non-owners, as well as greater overall satisfaction with life
and a significant increase in time spent doing things with friends; rather than substituting
for human interactions, pet interactions were positively associated with more human
interactions.
In another longitudinal study of the influence of companion animals on the
physical and psychological health of older people (Raina, Waltner-Toews, Bonnett,
Woodward, & Abernathy, 1999), while no statistically significant direct association was
observed between pet ownership and a change in psychological well-being, pet
ownership did significantly modify the relationship between social support and change in
psychological well-being over a 1-year period. Specifically, the relationship between
psychological well-being and the availability of support in a crisis situation was modified
by the presence or absence of a pet. “That is, the respondents who owned pets and had
lower social support in a crisis situation were, on average, less likely to decline in
psychological well-being compared with respondents who did not own pets and had
lower social support” (p. 327).
“If I have any beliefs about immortality, it is that certain dogs I have known will go to
34
heaven, and very, very few persons.”
-James Thurber
Pets and bereavement. Boris Levinson (1967) introduces the connection between
pets and bereavement from two perspectives, one being a child‟s bereavement upon the
loss of a cherished pet, the other the use of a pet in helping a child cope with the loss of a
parent or other family member. Cowles (1985) describes responses of pet owners to the
deaths of their pets, noting that there is little recognition that the intensity of grief
experienced at the loss of a pet may be as strong as that associated with the loss of a
human family member. Pet owners have the additional challenge of not feeling
understood by friends and colleagues who seem to be insensitive to the degree of
bereavement they experience (Carmack, 1985); sometimes it is even family members
who appear insensitive to the depth of the owner‟s grief, leaving the pet owner unable to
share his or her own feelings within the family.
Higher emotional attachment to the pet, sudden death, and living alone all predict
higher reported grief, with attachment to the pet showing the highest correlation with
grief (Archer & Winchester, 1994). Morley and Fook (2005) also discuss the importance
of types of pet loss that do not necessarily involve the death of the animal, such as losing
a pet in a divorce, or having to give up a pet due to financial circumstances or new living
arrangements, as well as no longer being able to care for a pet due to changes in health.
They suggest that, “Relationships with companion animals need to be understood and
valued as a significant part of normal mainstream human experience, and that similarly
35
loss of these friends is an experience likely to be encountered by a major proportion of
the population” (p. 140). Hunt, Al-Awadi, and Johnson (2008) investigated the impact of
Hurricane Katrina, as many animals were lost or abandoned as a result of this natural
disaster; forced abandonment of a pet was found to add considerably to acute trauma,
increasing the risk of long-term posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Both the acute loss
of the pet and the continued absence of the pet contributed to depressive symptom
severity.
Finally, turning to a consideration of how pets impact the experience of
bereavement in response to human loss (Akiyama, Holtzman, & Britz, 1986-87), widows
without pets reported higher frequencies of symptoms of physical illness compared to pet
owners. Non-owners also reported significantly increased use of medication, particularly
psychotropic drugs. Clearly, pet owners in a variety of circumstances derive great
emotional benefits from their pets, especially when they are under stress or do not have
adequate human social support networks.
“When most of us talk to our dogs, we tend to forget they‟re not people.”
-Julia Glass
Why do people love their pets? Consistent themes in the literature reviewed so
far suggest that the primary potential benefits of pets – as well as the subjective reasons
for acquiring a pet provided by pet owners – tend to be related to the factors of
attachment and social support. While greater attachment to a pet has often been
associated with greater benefits, and pet owners with lower overall social support
36
networks are often among those most likely to benefit from contact with a pet, there is
less understanding of how these variables play a role in the general population of pet
owners. There is an unfortunate tendency for people to see inter-species relationships as
somehow “less” than human-to-human relationships (Ryder, 1973), and reluctance
among many pet owners to admit to strong attachment to their pets.
Ryder posits that pet animals provide love and freedom to a greater degree than
many fellow human beings do, and that attachment to pets is established through tactile
contact. Pets are also sensitive to human emotional states, and can engage in nonverbal
emotional communication, a major component of empathy. Pets can also be loved
without much risk to the owner of ever being rejected. Rynearson (1978) adds that the
bond between humans and their pets depends on “their commonality as animals and their
mutual need for attachment” (p. 550), though Fox (1981) notes that not all pet owners
necessarily develop the same degree of attachment to their pets, with some acquiring pets
to be their children‟s playthings or accessories. Other people obtain animals for purely
utilitarian purposes, though in many instances these still develop into emotionally
attached relationships. While neurotic overattachment is a possibility in some cases,
overall emotional attachment and seeing the pet as a companion is a positive experience
that may assist in the development of empathy and compassion.
Katcher (1981) observes that the development of attachment theory is largely
responsible for a renewed interest in examining the nature of affection. Katcher suggests
that pets may not be mere substitutes for human contact, but may offer a type of
relationship that other human beings do not provide. One major reason for this is the
37
non-evaluative nature of a pet. Also, pets are perceived as empathic, and provide a
socially acceptable form of affectionate touch; the tactile relationship between humans
and pets is a major source of the human-animal bond. Pets are perceived as their owners‟
children (Berryman, Howells, & Lloyd-Evans, 1985), and become highly charged with
emotional meaning, serving as “supernormal stimuli for attachment” (Case, 1987, p. 251)
by their provision of affection and unconditional acceptance.
Dog owners come to see their dogs as “unique individuals who are minded,
empathetic, reciprocating, and well aware of basic rules that govern the relationship”
(Sanders, 1993, p. 207). Dogs are seen as having an emotional life and as being
responsive to human emotional experiences. Their owners tend to see them as
reciprocating in social interactions with the additional advantage that they do not make
the same demands as fellow humans do. Mutual play is a primary mode of interaction
between people and their pets (Sanders, 2003), and human-animal play has the advantage
of not having winners or losers since maintaining the play interaction is itself the primary
goal. Play requires the presence of intersubjectivity and the ability to make decisions in
concert, with gaze and mutual direction of attention assuming central roles in the
establishment of interspecies intersubjectivity. Pet owners often express speaking “for”
their animals, and attribute the quality of mindedness to their pets. Though most pet
owners acknowledge that dogs and humans have different cognitive abilities, they tend to
express that there are qualitative similarities between the mental processes of dogs and
humans, particularly in terms of emotional experience.
38
As social lubricants (Veevers, 1985), pets attract attention, have interpersonal
appeal, provide a topic of conversation, and cause their owners to appear more
approachable. Robins, Sanders, and Cahill (1991) observed dog owners at a public park,
noticing that the presence of dogs elicited responses from strangers and aided in the
establishment of interpersonal trust. McNicholas and Collis (2000) found the presence of
a pet dog to be associated with significantly more interactions with strangers. Pets in the
workplace have also been associated with facilitated social interaction (Wells and Perrine,
2001), and Wells (2004) found that a Labrador puppy elicited significantly longer
conversations from passersby than a teddy bear or a potted plant. Other authors have
suggested that pets not only act as social lubricants, but may also serve as catalysts for
social networks (Wood & Giles-Corti, 2005; Wood, Giles-Corti, Bulsara, & Bosch, 2007).
Roth (2005) offers a new psychodynamic perspective on pet ownership designed
to counter the previous tendency of analytic literature to diminish or even pathologize the
role of human-animal relationships. Roth considers the adaptive function of humans
sharing their psychic environment with animals as they engage in nonverbal
communication with their pets. Pets are able to share in our sensory and emotional
experiences, and to convey meaning through behaviors, touch and play. They appear to
understand and respond to gestures, tone of voice, and emotional expressions and respond
to separation and loss. Pets bond to their owners just as intensely as their owners bond to
them.
“The more people I meet the more I like my dog”
39
-Unknown
Personality characteristics of pet owners. Early authors investigating personality
differences between pet owners and non-owners tended to associate pet ownership with
poor psychological health, lending support to the stereotype that pet owners prefer their
pets to people and are often isolated or even anti-social. Cameron and Mattson (1972)
believed that the idealization of a pet “seems to function as a detriment to effective social
relationships and consequently to the person‟s mental health” (p. 286). Boris Levinson
(1978) had a more positive view of the role pet ownership may play in personality
development, believing that pet ownership in childhood is related to the development of
empathy, self-esteem, self-control, and autonomy; he perceived human attitudes towards
and treatment of animals as closely paralleling their attitudes towards and relationships
with people.
Kidd & Kidd (1980) found that pet ownership was associated with lower levels of
aggression, and Guttman (1981) found a greater tendency for pet owners to avoid
loneliness and being alone relative to non-owners. Guttman also observed that nonowners show a greater tendency to be independent and to avoid lasting obligations,
whereas the idea that a pet may restrict personal freedom rarely even occurred to pet
owners. Hyde, Kurdek, and Larson (1983) found that pet owners were marginally higher
on empathy and significantly higher on interpersonal trust relative to non-owners, and
Paden-Levy (1985) found negative correlations of neuroticism and alienation with pet
ownership. Marks, Koepke, and Bradley (1994) found a positive relationship between
40
pet owners‟ attachment scores and their scores on a measure of generativity, considered
to be a measure of how concerned individuals are for the next generation and also seen as
related to nurturance. On the other hand, Brown and Katcher (2001) found a significant
association between high attachment scores and clinical levels of dissociation among a
group of female veterinary technician students. It may be that moderate levels of
attachment are most associated with the psychological health of pet owners, with both
low and high attachment scores – especially at their most extreme – associated with
negative outcomes related to pet ownership.
Gaps in the literature. While this review included some empirical studies with
longitudinal designs, control groups, and large sample sizes (particularly epidemiological
studies), there is regrettably a continuing reliance on small sample sizes, anecdotal
findings, and retrospective reports. Also, though most experimental research designs
include some type of control group, not all studies of pet ownership have included nonowner groups for comparison. Just as importantly, other demographic variables have not
consistently been controlled for in many studies. There are clearly factors such as
socioeconomic status, living arrangements, and household composition (to name just a
few) that are associated to varying degrees with pet ownership as well as with varying
levels of attachment to pets and thus also with the potential benefits of pets.
Hines and Frederickson (1998) cite the National Institutes of Health regarding the
best way to proceed in research in a field that has primarily relied on anecdotal or
qualitative research and case studies, which bears repeating here:
Methodologies for future research can begin without explicit hypothesis
and proceed from descriptive studies of representative and, hopefully,
41
random samples. There is nothing intrinsically wrong with extrapolating
from attitudinal information as long as the sample is representative of the
target population. It should be remembered that samples of convenience
are prone to bias, and interpretation must be limited and made with great
care. If the hypothesis is supported, research cold proceed to crosssectional and retrospective studies and then to long-range prospective
investigations. A causal association between animal contact and human
health can be demonstrated only by prospective studies. (National
Institutes of Health, 1998, p. 5) (In Hines & Fredrickson, 1998, p. 33)
Wilson and Barker (2003) also note the lack of evidence-based practice in the field of
animal-assisted therapy and a prevalence of hypothesis-generating rather than hypothesistesting studies. There is especially a need for researchers to exercise more caution in
attributing causality to correlational findings, and to avoid making inappropriate
generalizations based on results from homogenous samples. Future studies should also
control for interactive demographic variables such as gender, age, and socioeconomic
status. It is also common to rely on “convenience samples” such as participants already
known to have dogs, or residents of a particular nursing home. Another common
problem is nonresponse bias, as there may be different characteristics associated with dog
owners participating in a study and those who elect not to participate. Previous studies
have also demonstrated little description of the content and process of interventions, and
there is an overall need for more follow-up measurements and long-term studies.
There continue to be problems in terms of small sample size, lack of
randomization, lack of a control or “usual care” group, inadequate control groups,
selection bias, poor generalizability, minimal reporting of the reliability and validity of
tools used to measure outcomes, and attrition rates (Morrison, 2007). There is also a lack
of agreement in terminology and lack of consistency in the instruments being used (Steed
42
& Smith, 2003), and a need to distinguish between transient emotional responses and
lasting change (Mallon, 1992). Finally, the hypotheses frequently cited as proposed
underlying mechanisms of the benefits of pet ownership or AAA/AAT have rarely been
directly tested. One hypothesis of the current study is that pet dogs may serve as a buffer
against the impact of negative mood through the mechanisms of attachment and
nonevaluative social support; therefore, the final section of the literature review is a
consideration of the theoretical bases for changes in mood and the factors that may have
an impact on mood.
How does mood change? Before determining how the presence of a pet dog can
impact mood or serve as a buffer against negative mood, it will be helpful to define the
construct of mood as it is being used in this context. Some authors use the terms mood,
emotion, and affect interchangeably (Neumann, Seibt, & Strack, 2001; Westermann,
Spies, Stahl, & Hesse, 1996); however, many authors differentiate between mood and
emotion, with both terms falling under the larger umbrella of affect. Affect is generally
described as whatever a person is experiencing or feeling, whether pleasant or unpleasant;
it has varying degrees of intensity and duration and can be triggered or activated in a
variety of ways (Humrichouse, Chmielewski, McDade-Montez, & Watson, 2007).
Emotions consist of a subjective experience, a physiological reaction, an expressive
component, and a behavioral response, whereas moods are similar to the subjective
components of emotions. However, moods additionally include an array of low-intensity
states and mixed states that are not traditionally considered to represent emotions (such as
fatigue, confusion, etc.).
43
While emotions are short-term reactions to events or stimuli in the environment,
moods are “rather diffuse affective states that subtly affect our experience, cognitions,
and behavior” (Wilhelm & Schoebi, 2007, p. 258). Moods can be experienced passively,
tend to last for some time, and are difficult to “shake off” (Parrott, 1991, p. 50). Also,
while emotions presuppose that one knows the origin of the affective feeling, moods do
not necessarily hinge on that knowledge (Neumann et al., 2001). Most previous studies
of mood have addressed the consequences or concomitants of mood rather than what
brings about a mood state (Pignatiello, Camp, Elder, & Rasar, 1989), but understanding
how mood changes is important when carrying out research that relies on the
effectiveness of a mood induction procedure.
Since moods tend to be viewed as subjective affective states, Mood Induction
Procedures (MIPs) are generally evaluated in terms of their ability to elicit a change in
self-reported mood in the desired direction. Mood is typically measured either on a
bipolar continuum between positive and negative affect states or with respondents rating
their degree of agreement or disagreement with regard to how much they are
experiencing specific mood states; a growing body of literature suggests that positive and
negative affect states are actually independent constructs rather than opposite ends of a
continuum (Parrott, 1991), though regrettably there is still a good deal of variability and
relatively little consensus regarding how to define and measure specific mood states.
Depression is postulated to result from thoughts involving unfulfilled expectations, loss,
and failure, whereas anxiety results from thoughts that anticipate danger or
unpleasantness. Sadness occurs when there is a failure of a major plan or loss of an
44
active goal, whereas anxiety occurs when self-preservation is threatened (Martin, 1990, p.
679). Overall, negative mood inductions tend to produce larger effect sizes than positive
mood inductions (Westermann et al., 1996).
Cognitive theories of mood change tend to dominate the literature, with Martin
(1990) relating mood states to thought processes. Many MIPs rely on the theory that
changes in thought patterns elicit mood shifts, with the most commonly used MIP
requiring participants to read a set of 60 self-referent statements designed to trigger a
particular mood state (elated, depressed, anxious, or neutral) (Velten, 1968). However,
other authors argue that the explicitness of the directions to maintain a particular mood
state may create demand effects among participants in MIPs (Larsen & Sinnett, 1991),
and that cognitive MIPs are likely to induce multiple moods rather than discrete mood
states (Polivy, 1981). Other cognitive methods of mood induction include remembering
past unpleasant events, listening to a taped depressing story, or being given failure
feedback on a task (Goodwin & Williams, 1982).
Depressed mood has been associated with learned helplessness and selfperceptions of defeat and/or entrapment (Gatchel, Paulus, & Maples, 1975; Pittman &
Pittman, 1979, 1980; Goldstein & Willner, 2002), and most authors support the notion of
a reciprocal relationship between cognition and mood. In cases of clinical depression,
this becomes part of a vicious circle with negative cognitions triggering more depressed
mood, and depressed mood leading in turn to further negative cognitions (Teasdale &
Fogarty, 1979). Memories of unpleasant experiences induce depressed moods (Brewer,
Doughtie, & Lubin, 1980), and negative memories are also more accessible when
45
participants are in a depressed mood (Rholes, Riskind, & Lane, 1987). Depressed mood
has also been associated with failure to meet one‟s own standards as well as failure to
meet externally imposed duties or standards (Goldstein & Willner, 2002). Self-focused
attention has been hypothesized to initiate, maintain, and exacerbate depression (Scheier
& Carver, 1977; Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1987; Green, Sedikides, Saltzberg, Wood,
and Forzano, 2003); “the more self-referencing and/or self-evaluating a mood induction
system is, the more likely an intense and meaningful emotional mood state will be
produced” (Seibert & Ellis, 1991, p. 121).
Physiological factors may also underlie changes in mood, from seasonal changes
in light (Park, Kripke, & Cole, 2007) to the direct physical manipulation of facial
expressions in the laboratory (Laird, 1974). High negative affect is correlated with
physical problems, and low positive affect is additionally associated with health
complaints (Clark & Watson, 1988), though causal direction cannot necessarily be
inferred in studies observing a relationship between mood states and physical health; as
with cognition, it seems likely to be a bidirectional relationship, as depression is often
associated with poor physical health. Depressed mood has been associated with saturated
fat intake (Anton & Miller, 2005), lack of physical activity, not eating breakfast, irregular
sleep hours, not using a seat belt (Allgöwer, Wardle, & Steptoe, 2001), and alcohol use
(Parker, Parker, Harford, & Farmer, 1987; Armeli, Tennen, Affleck, & Kranzler, 2000;
Steptoe & Wardle, 1999), though there tends to be a great deal of variability between
individual responses and even within individuals, suggesting that personality factors and
individual differences also need to be considered when considering influences on mood.
46
Monoamine depletion is indirectly related to depressed mood, but is likely to represent a
vulnerability trait rather than a causal factor (Ruhé, Mason, & Schene, 2007), and
hypoglycemia has been shown to induce negative mood, tension, and fatigue (Gold,
MacLeod, Frier, & Deary, 1995).
Personality variables that interact with changes in mood include sociability (with
sociable rather than impulsive extraverts tending to demonstrate more positive affect),
while more emotionally reactive and interpersonally sensitive individuals and those with
lower self-esteem are more likely to report negative mood (Emmons & Diener, 1985). In
one mood induction study, neurotic subjects tended to demonstrate heightened emotional
reactivity to a negative mood induction, whereas extraverts tended to demonstrate
heightened emotional reactivity to a positive mood induction (Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991).
Individuals with high discrepancies between their actual and ideal selves tend to express
more sadness and dejection (Higgins, Bond, Klein, & Strauman, 1986), and individuals
with lower self-esteem are more likely to lower their self-evaluations after a negative
mood induction (Brown & Mankowski, 1993). Therefore, individual differences in
personality also need to be considered when evaluating the impact of MIPs.
The Current Study
Considering the preponderance of models in the field promoting attachment and
social support as likely mechanisms for the beneficial effects of dog ownership, these
constructs were selected as the theoretical framework for the current study as well.
However, even within the construct of social support, different means of enhancing social
47
support have been suggested. Some authors posit a direct positive effect of social support
provided by dogs through the provision of companionship and comfort (Rosenkoetter,
1991; Siegel, 1993; Beck & Katcher, 2003), while others suggest an indirect role with
dogs either acting as “social lubricants” to facilitate social interactions with other humans
or acting as buffers against the adverse impact of stressful life events (Wells, 2007). It is
quite possible, of course, that dogs play multiple beneficial roles for their owners, and
that interactions between characteristics of individual dogs and individual owners
contribute in determining the potential benefits for both members of the dyad. However,
for the sake of attempting to clarify the particular relationship between social support and
attachment among dog owners, and more specifically the potential role of the pet dog in
buffering against stress, a more limited framework must be used.
First of all, very few previous studies have actually involved a direct assessment
of the presence of a pet dog on individual owners‟ emotional states or moods, at least not
in a neutral setting. Usually such studies, when conducted at all, involve assessing the
owners in their homes with their pets or rely on retrospective reports. There is a general
problem with conducting studies in owners‟ homes, as there is much more variability
introduced through differences between their homes and the control of extraneous
variables is also more difficult.
In contrast, in a neutral setting such as a laboratory or similar meeting room, the
environment will be more standard. Most laboratory studies that have asked owners to
bring in their own dogs have examined primarily physiological measures in relation to
the presence of the pet dog. Also, they tend to employ within-subjects repeated measures
designs, having participants serving as their own “controls” by being assessed with and
48
without their dog.
If previous studies have generally found that pet dogs may serve to buffer against
the physiological effects of stress and anxiety, and there have been inconsistent findings
with regard to the impact of pet ownership on self-esteem and depression, perhaps the
presence of a pet dog will have a favorable effect on mood. The central question being
asked is whether or not pet dogs can serve as a buffer against the effects of a negative
mood induction procedure administered in a neutral, laboratory-like setting. Rather than
using within-subjects comparisons, participants were recruited from the general
community of single female dog owners and single females without dogs, with dog
owners randomly assigned to a dog-present or dog-absent condition.
The presence of a non-owner group not only serves as an additional source of
control, but also allows for comparisons between characteristics of dog owners and nonowners at baseline. Attachment to pets and social support were assessed, in addition to
depression, self-esteem, and general attitudes towards pets. Demographic information
was collected regarding pet ownership, employment, education, relationship status (all
women were not co-habiting with partners or married; however, some were dating),
living situation (some had roommates; though some had adult children, none currently
lived with children), age, and geographical location. All demographic variables were
assessed in relation to pet ownership to determine whether there were any demographic
differences between pet owners and non-owners that might have an effect on the
interpretation of the results.
49
The author‟s primary hypothesis was that among dog owners, the presence of the
pet dog would result in a significantly smaller magnitude response to the administration
of a negative mood induction. The negative mood induction was expected to result in
decreases in self-reported happiness and self-esteem, and increases in self-reported
sadness and despondency. Other moods (irritability, fatigue, anxiety, and apprehension)
were included to test for the specificity of the mood induction procedure, with the
hypothesis that these measures of mood would not change significantly from before to
after the mood induction procedure. The mood induction was also expected to contribute
to slower performance on a writing speed task. No specific hypotheses were made
regarding the non-owners‟ response to the negative mood induction, though it was
suspected that they might exhibit an intermediate degree of responsiveness somewhat
greater than that of dog owners with their dogs present, but less than that of dog owners
without their dogs (who could be perceived of as being deprived of the benefit of their
dogs‟ presence).
Secondary hypotheses included that an association would be found between dog
owners‟ attachment levels to their dogs and their responses to the negative mood
induction, so that buffering effects would be more evident among more highly attached
owners with their dogs present. It was also hypothesized that dog owners with their dog
present at the follow-up might demonstrate higher positive mood and self-esteem and
lower negative mood prior to the mood induction procedure, as well as demonstrating
more rapid and accurate performance on a writing speed task, and that this effect might
50
be enhanced among dog owners with stronger attachments or more positive attitudes
towards their dogs.
Though past studies of the relationship between attachment and social support
have been mixed, it was also hypothesized that dog owners with higher attachment scores
might have higher self-perceived social support, as well as lower depression scores and
higher self-esteem scores. It was also hypothesized that non-owners with more positive
attitudes towards pets might have similar advantages on measures of self-perceived social
support, self-esteem, and depression. Finally, though again previous results have been
mixed, it was considered that there might be a significant difference between dog owners
and non-owners at baseline on measures of self-esteem, social support, and depression.
Method
Participants
Sixty-six women ranging in age from 19 to 76 years (mean age 43 years; women
over the age of 65 were only included if they were still in the work force at least part-time)
were recruited for participation in three different geographic areas, using flyers posted on
university campuses, at dog runs, veterinary offices, post offices, dog day care and
grooming facilities, libraries, and on internet websites such as craigslist and mailing lists
for dog owners. Additional participants were also recruited by word of mouth.
Participants were living in the New York City metropolitan area, the Hudson Valley in
New York State, or the Shenandoah Valley in Virginia. Separate flyers were designed
for dog owners and for volunteers without pets, though aside from the initial heading
51
(either “Dog Owner?” or “No Pets?”) the information given to potential participants was
identical. The flyers asked that respondents be female, single (not married or living with
a partner; dating was fine), not living with children, and between the ages of 18 and 65,
though as mentioned older adults who wanted to participate were included provided that
they were still active in the work force on at least a part-time basis. Forty-four women
owned at least one dog; some owned other pets as well. Twenty-two women did not own
any pets. All participants were offered the opportunity to enter in a raffle prize drawing
for cash prizes (1 $100 prize, 2 $50 prizes, and 4 $25 prizes); students could also receive
course credit.
Materials
Prescreening measures. Trait measures of depression (The Beck Depression
Inventory – II [BDI-II] [Appendix A] [Beck, 1996]) and self-esteem (The Rosenberg
Self-Esteem Scale [Appendix B] [Rosenberg, 1965, 1989]), as well as the
Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) (Appendix C) (Zimet,
Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988), were administered to all participants. The Beck
Depression Inventory – II is a 21-item self-report rating inventory presented in multiple
choice format which measures the presence and severity of depression in adolescents and
adults (Beck, 1996). Though the original inventory is over forty years old (Beck, Ward,
Mendelson, Mock & Erbaugh, 1961), the Beck Depression Inventory – II (BDI-II) has
been revised multiple times and continues to demonstrate high internal consistency, splithalf reliability, and content validity (Beck, 1996).
52
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965, 1989) (Appendix B) is one
of the most frequently used measures of global self-esteem, consisting of ten statements
related to overall feelings of self-worth or self-acceptance. The items are answered on a
four-point scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree. Test-retest correlations
are typically in the range of .82 to .88, and Cronbach‟s alpha for various samples are in
the range of .77 to .88 (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1993). Self-esteem is strongly negatively
correlated with depression, and is a good indicator of psychological well-being.
The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) (Appendix C)
(Zimet et al., 1988) measures perceived social support using a 7-point Likert-type scale
ranging from 1 (very strongly disagree) to 7 (very strongly agree) and includes factor
groups relating to the source of social support (family, friends, or significant other).
Since previous studies have reported mixed and even inconsistent results regarding the
relationship between pet ownership and perceived social support, this measure is included
to assess for relationships between pet ownership, pet attachment and social support.
While some authors have attempted to add on to this measure with pet-related items, in
this study separate measures were used for pet attachment and attitudes towards pets.
All participants also completed a scale to assess their attitudes towards animals.
The 18-item Pet Attitude Scale (PAS) (Templer, Salter, Dickey, Baldwin, & Veleber,
1981) (Appendix D) has been shown to have an inter-item reliability of .93 and a testretest reliability of .92. It has been compared to scores on the Marlowe-Crowne Social
Desirability Scale and an acquiescent response measure, with nonsignificant correlations
less than .20, and has been found to differentiate successfully between kennel workers
53
and social work students. Items are scored on a 7-point Likert-type scale, with some
items reverse scored to assess negative as well as positive attitudes towards pets. It was
expected that the pet owners would display more positive attitudes towards pets, and that
a higher score on the PAS might also be associated with decreased responsiveness to the
mood induction procedure for participants in the dog-present group.
In addition, dog owners completed a brief scale that measures the quality of
attachment to their pets. The Comfort from Companion Animals Scale (Zasloff, 1996)
(Appendix E) is an 11-item measure for assessing the perceived level of emotional
comfort that owners receive from their pets. Responses are based on a Likert scale from
1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Inter-item reliability for the pilot
administration was .85 (p < .01). This measure was included to assess the possibility that
dog owners who are more attached to their pets are also more likely to benefit from their
presence.
Experimental measures. The primary experimental manipulation was the use of a
musical mood induction procedure to induce negative mood in the laboratory setting.
Martin (1990) describes the use of music as an instrument of mood induction, with
participants being asked to use music to aid in their own efforts to get into a depressed,
elated, or neutral mood. This type of induction has been shown to affect both self-report
measures of mood, particularly Visual Analogue Mood Scales (VAMS) (Appendix G),
and psychomotor measures known to be affected by depression. Psychomotor measures
of mood are less susceptible to demand effects than self-report measures of mood. In
addition, music induction techniques have been shown to increase depression without
54
increasing anxiety, whereas other techniques often increase depression and anxiety. Also,
music induction is usually successful in inducing the relevant mood over 75% of the time,
in contrast to another commonly used procedure called the Velten Mood Induction
Procedure that has only a 50% success rate. Finally, mood induction procedures are less
time-consuming than other procedures, typically lasting less than ten minutes.
The most common musical mood induction for depressed mood is a recording of
“Russia Under the Mongolian Yoke” from the film Alexander Nevsky composed by
Prokofiev and played at half speed (Clark & Teasdale, 1985; Parrott, 1991; Slyker &
McNally, 1991). Visual Analogue Mood Scales (VAMS) were used as the self-report
measure of mood before and after the mood induction procedure. Though there are
several different formats used for visual analogue scales (Clark & Teasdale, 1985; Slyker
& McNally, 1991; Goldstein & Willner, 2002; Green, Sedikides, Saltzberg, Wood &
Forzano, 2003), the basic premise is that subjects are asked to place a mark on a
horizontal line in relation to how they currently perceive their mood, with endpoints on
the line either being two different mood states at opposite ends of a bipolar continuum
(e.g., happy on one end and sad on the other), or each line being used to rate the
experience of a particular mood state on a continuum with one end of the line being “not
at all” and the other end of the line being “extremely.” The latter method was used in the
current study, with the moods “sad,” “irritated,” “happy,” “anxious,” “tired,”
“despondent,” and “apprehensive.” The mood induction was expected to result in a
decrease in happy mood and an increase in sad and despondent mood; based on the
55
theory that the mood induction should only induce depressed mood rather than anxious
mood, it was not expected to have an impact on anxiety or apprehension.
The psychomotor impact of depressed mood was measured in the present study
using a writing speed task (Parrott, 1991). For this task, participants were asked to write
a column of numbers counting backwards from 100, being stopped after sixty seconds;
slower performance on the task is associated with depression, while more anxious
participants make more mistakes. Finally, a state measure of self-esteem was included to
assess the impact of the mood manipulation on self-esteem. Previous researchers have
found positive correlations between high self-esteem and positive affect (Watson, Suls &
Haig, 2002), as well as finding that subjects with high self-esteem are less susceptible to
the effects of negative mood on self-evaluation (Brown & Mankowski, 1993).
Crocker & Wolfe (2001) suggest that global state self-esteem can be assessed
with the same items used on a measure of global trait self-esteem such as the Rosenberg
Self-Esteem Scale, with the items reworded to assess how the subject feels in the moment
(Appendix F). According to Crocker & Wolfe, self-esteem is a crucial element in
determining quality of life, as it is also closely related to positive affect and fewer
symptoms of depression. They argue that rather than being a relatively stable trait, selfesteem varies in stability from person to person depending on what domains, or
contingencies of self-worth, self-esteem is centered on. While most people do have an
average level of self-esteem that can be measured as a personality trait, judgments of selfesteem may fluctuate around this level based on how an individual feels at the moment
the judgment is made. Therefore, it was expected that the mood induction procedure
56
might impact self-report measures of self-esteem, and that the presence of a pet dog
might additionally serve as a buffer against this effect.
Procedures
Upon contacting the researcher either by phone or by e-mail, participants were
informed of the basic requirements of the study. They were told that two meetings would
be scheduled, approximately one week apart (in some cases it was necessarily to wait
more than a week, but at least one week passed between the baseline and follow-up
measures in all cases), lasting approximately 15-20 minutes each. They were also
informed that they would be filling out self-report measures at the first meeting, and that
they would fill out some more self-report measures and “listen and respond to music”
during the second session; they were told that the experimenter was interested in general
differences between dog owners and non-owners on a variety of standard laboratory tasks.
Dog owners were additionally questioned to find whether or not they would be
able to transport their dog to the study location (a university laboratory in New York,
library meeting rooms or similar settings in the Hudson Valley, a church basement
meeting room in Virginia), in order to ensure that the assignment of participants to the
dog-present condition could be done completely randomly; they were additionally
assured that their dog would not have to do anything as part of the study besides merely
be present. Non-owners were screened to make sure they had not owned a dog within the
past few years; it was not practical to eliminate previous ownership entirely, as the vast
majority of non-owners had owned a pet at some point in their lives. At the first meeting,
all participants were given informed consent forms to sign, after which they completed
57
the BDI-II, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived
Social Support, and the Pet Attitude Scale. Dog owners additionally completed the
Comfort from Companion Animals Scale. Due to requiring two meetings, some
participant attrition did occur; any results from participants only completing the first
meeting were dropped from the study, as there were not enough cases to investigate
differences between participants who returned and participants who did not, though there
were geographic differences (five in New York City, three in the Hudson Valley, none in
the Shenandoah Valley).
At the end of the first meeting, dog owners were randomly selected and informed
whether or not they would need to bring their dog with them to the second meeting. At
the follow-up, all participants completed the modified version of the Rosenberg SelfEsteem Scale, the writing speed task, and the Visual Analogue Mood Scale. Then
participants wore headphones while listening to Prokofiev‟s “Russians Under the
Mongolian Yoke” played at half speed for seven minutes. They were told simply to “try
to get into the mood of the music,” without the specific mood being named, in order to
avoid demand characteristics. Finally, the participants completed the modified
Rosenberg, the writing speed task, and the Visual Analogue Mood Scale a second time,
followed by a debriefing and an opportunity to ask the researcher questions or to provide
any observations or feedback to the researcher regarding their own perceptions of the
mood induction procedure and, when relevant, the effect of their dog‟s presence.
It should be noted that the researcher used scoring methods on all measures using
a Likert-type scale so that points were given for greater degrees of agreement on items
58
associated with positive measures of the construct being assessed, and points were
subtracted for greater degrees of disagreement with these items; likewise, points were
given for greater degrees of disagreement on items negatively associated with the
construct being assessed, and points were subtracted for greater degrees of agreement
with these items. For example, if someone endorsed an item positively associated with
self-esteem, they would get one point for agreeing and two points for strongly agreeing.
If someone endorsed an item negatively associated with positive attitudes towards pets,
they would lose one point for agreeing, two points for agreeing strongly, and three points
for agreeing very strongly. While the researchers‟ scoring methods were not always
identical to standard scoring methods employed by the measures‟ authors, they were
utilized consistently across all measures and were used to allow more direct comparisons
between the different prescreening and experimental measures.
Statistical procedures for data analysis were conducted using SPSS version 17.0
and included multivariate analysis of variance with the independent factors of
experimental condition and outcome measures of self-report and behavioral mood.
Nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis tests were used rather than univariate ANOVA due to a
high degree of non-normality of data. Nonparametric Mann-Whitney U tests were
performed for pairwise comparisons. Chi-square was used to assess for any interaction
between experimental group or dog owner status with the demographic variables of age,
education, occupation, geography, living situation, and relationship status. Though
interpreted with caution due to the nonlinear nature of many of the dependent variables,
regression analyses were also used to test for interactions of any demographic variables
59
with the experimental group or dog owner status variables on the outcome measures.
Finally, nonparametric correlations (Spearman‟s ρ) were examined to identify any
associations between dependent variables, with a particular interest in measures of
attitude towards pets (Pet Attitude Scale [PAS]), pet attachment (Comfort from
Companion Animals Scale [CCAS]), and social support (Multidimensional Scale of
Perceived Social Support [MSPSS]).
Results
Demographics
The overall demographics are summarized in Table 1. Since there was no
statistically significant difference between groups for age (mean age for dog owners with
their dogs was 42.68 years, for dog owners without their dogs 44.9 years, and for nonowners 41.64 years), age was recoded as a categorical variable since all the other
demographic variables were categorical. Overall there were not any statistically
significant differences between the experimental groups in terms of demographic
variables. The random assignment of dog owners to the dog-present and dog-absent
conditions was successful. Going forward, then, the results related to experimental group
condition can be interpreted without concern for demographic factors acting as
confounding variables.
The only demographic differences between dog owners and non-owners were
occupation and household composition, with an association between dog ownership and
occupation found primarily in relation to more working students not having dogs, and
more dog owners working full-time (Chi square = 7.603, p < .05). In addition there was
60
an association between dog ownership and household composition, with dog owners
being relatively more likely to live alone, and non-owners having more roommates (Chi
square = 3.902, p < .05). Again, there were no reliable differences between the three
experimental groups, so these demographic differences only need to be considered when
interpreting overall differences between dog owners and non-owners.
[Insert Table 1 about here]
Experimental measures
The overall effect of the mood induction across all groups was as expected, with
decreases in happiness (t = 1.477, p < .05) and increases in sadness (t = -1.319, p < .05)
and despondency (t = -1.603, p < .05) after listening to the music. Self-esteem scores did
not change significantly, however. The specificity of the mood induction procedure was
also supported by a lack of significant change in measures of anxiety or apprehension;
anxiety actually decreased slightly after the mood induction procedure. One unexpected
finding was that the mood induction procedure was not successful in significantly altering
participants‟ performances on the writing speed task, though of course it is quite possible
that practice effects overwhelmed any impact of the mood induction procedure.
The first outcome measure that was significantly associated with dog owner status
using the Kruskal-Wallis test was the change in apprehension ratings from before to after
the mood induction procedure (Chi square = 3.902, p < .05). The result was supported by
the Mann-Whitney U test (U = 336.500, p < .05). For dog owners, there was an average
increase in apprehension scores of 2.92 (SD = 13.130) in response to the mood induction
procedure; for non-owners, there was an average decrease in apprehension scores of
–4.93 (SD = 13.900) in response to the mood induction procedure. There was not a
61
statistically significant difference between dog owners without their dogs and non-owners
on this measure, so this effect only occurred for dog owners in the presence of their dogs
(U = 157.000, p < .05). When other demographic variables were controlled for, owner
status was still significant (β = -.255, t = -1.842, p < .05).
Among dog owners with their dogs present, changes in scores on the anxiety
measure of the VAMS in response to the musical mood induction procedure differed
significantly from those of non-owners (U = 139.500, p < .05), with the dog owners
exhibiting increased anxiety in the presence of their dogs (M = 1.93, SD = 15.132), and
non-owners exhibiting decreased anxiety (M = -6.66, SD = 16.234). Again, there was no
significant effect of other demographic variables on this outcome measure. Dog owners
with their dog present exhibited a similar pattern with changes in irritability, again with
dog owners displaying increased irritability in response to the musical mood induction
(M = -4.27, SD = 15.055) (U = 156.000, p < .05). This effect diminished when other
demographic variables were entered into the equation (β = -.227, t = -1.842, p = .070),
though it still approached statistical significance.
There were no statistically significant experimental group differences on the
effect of the negative mood induction in relation to happiness, sadness, tiredness,
despondency, self-esteem, or writing speed task performance. Overall, the main
difference between non-owners and dog owners was increased apprehension, anxiety, and
irritability scores among dog owners with their dog present relative to non-owners. The
62
scores of dog owners without their dogs present tended to fall in between the scores of
dog owners with their dogs present and non-owners.
Due to the large number of outcome measures, a Bonferroni correction was used,
so that only highly significant correlations between pet attitude and pet attachment and
the outcome measures (p < .001) were considered; this was to limit the likelihood of
spurious correlations and Type 1 errors. With this stringent criterion, however, no
significant associations were found between measures of pet attitude or attachment and
the outcome measures. However, some significant associations were found between
measures of pet attitude and baseline measures of happiness.
Prescreening and baseline measures
Since this was not a clinical sample, the BDI-II was not administered with the
intention of measuring clinical levels of depression (if participants were found to have
clinical levels, they were offered a referral for mental health services), but rather to
determine if there were any differences between dog owners and non-owners on lowlevel symptoms associated with depression and also to screen participants for depression
prior to the administration of a negative mood induction procedure. The average BDI-II
score across all groups was quite low (M = 5.52, SD = 5.254) and was not significantly
different between dog owners and non-owners. However, the Mann-Whitney U test
revealed a statistically significant difference between dog owners who were assigned to
bring their dog and non-owners, with dog owners assigned to bring their dog scoring
significantly lower on the BDI-II (i.e., having fewer depressive symptoms;
63
U = 153.000, p < .05). Since the BDI-II scores were also significantly different between
the Hudson Valley and Shenandoah Valley (U = 47.000, p < .05), with higher scores
associated with the Hudson Valley, and BDI-II scores were also significantly different
between participants without a college degree and participants with a doctoral degree (U
= 16.000, p < .05) with higher education associated with lower scores, geography and
education were entered along with experimental group status into a regression model.
With geography and education included in the model, experimental group was the only
statistically significant predictor of the BDI-II score (β = .287, t = 2.351, p < .05).
The average score on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE) across all groups
was 12.91 (SD = 5.462; maximum possible score of 20 based on strong endorsements of
positive statements and strong rejections of negative statements). There was not a
statistically significant difference between experimental groups on this measure, though
there was a trend for dog owners (M = 13.43, SD = 5.258) to have slightly higher scores
than non-owners (M = 11.86, SD = 5.833). Not surprisingly, the prescreening measure of
self-esteem was significantly positively correlated with the reworded experimental
measures of self-esteem (RSE and self-esteem before the mood induction,
ρ = .634, p < .001; RSE and self-esteem after the mood induction, ρ = .501, p < .001).
The average score on the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS)
across all groups was 18.55 (SD = 13.296; maximum possible score of 36). There was
not a statistically significant difference between experimental groups on this measure,
though there was a trend for non-owners (M = 21.64, SD = 10.399) to have slightly
higher scores than dog owners (M = 17.00, SD = 14.390).
64
Not surprisingly, dog owners exhibited a significantly higher score on the Pet
Attitude Scale (PAS) relative to non-owners, with the mean PAS score for non-owners
being 21.64 (SD = 18.186; maximum possible score of 56), and the mean PAS score for
dog owners being 41.45 (SD = 9.872) (U = 163.000, p < .001). There was no significant
difference in PAS scores between dog owners who were assigned to the dog-present
condition for the follow-up and those who did not bring their dogs to the follow-up. The
Comfort from Companion Animals Scale (CCA) was only administered to dog owners,
with an average score of 18.75 (SD = 4.947; maximum possible score of 22). Again,
there was not a significant difference in scores between dog owners assigned to bring
their dog with them to the follow-up and dog owners who did not bring their dogs with
them to the follow-up. Among dog owners, PAS scores were highly associated with
scores on the Comfort from Companion Animal Scale (CCA) (ρ = .711, p < .001).
Overall, higher PAS scores were positively associated with measures of happiness
prior to the negative mood induction (ρ = .402, p < .001). There was also a
nonsignificant trend for dog owners with their dogs to be somewhat less sad (M = 10.41,
SD = 18.78) than dog owners without their dogs (M = 12.52, SD = 21.118), and for dog
owners without their dogs to be somewhat less sad than non-owners (M = 18.23, SD =
20.908) prior to the mood induction procedure. However, this difference was not
statistically significant, and there was not a difference in how they responded to the
music.
Among dog owners with their dogs present, scores on the reworded self-esteem
measure were significantly higher than for non-owners prior to the negative mood
65
induction (U = 158.000, p < .05), with the average score for dog owners with their dog
present being 17.55 (SD = 2.738), and for non-owners 14.50 (SD = 5.280). This
association was also maintained when controlling for other demographic variables.
Despondency ratings on the VAMS prior to the negative mood induction procedure were
significantly lower for dog owners with their dogs present (M = 4.32, SD = 4.031)
relative to non-owners (M = 15.48, SD = 18.635) (Chi square = 3.532, p < .01).
Despondency ratings on the VAMS prior to the negative mood induction procedure were
also significantly lower for dog owners without their dogs (M = 7.61, SD = 8.396)
relative to non-owners (Chi square = 3.544, p < .05). Only experimental condition was a
significant predictor of this measure (β = .360, t = 3.172, p < .01).
Finally, apprehension ratings on the VAMS prior to the negative mood induction
procedure were significantly lower for dog owners with their dogs (M = 10.02, SD =
16.650) and without their dogs (M = 6.77, SD = 6.254) relative to non-owners (M =
19.95, SD = 24.706) (F = -8.800, p < .05). Both household composition and relationship
status affected the association between dog ownership and apprehension ratings. The
effect of owner status was only significant for participants who were living alone (F =
19.430, p < .001), and was not significant for participants with roommates. Similarly, the
effect of owner status was only significant for participants in a relationship (F = 7.544, p
< .05), and not for participants who were single. When experimental group, relationship
status, and household composition were entered in a regression equation, only
relationship status significantly predicted apprehension ratings prior to the negative mood
induction (β = .328, t = 3.027, p < .01). When owner status and relationship status were
66
entered into a regression equation, they accounted for approximately equal proportions of
variance in one model, but it was not a very strong predictor (Eigenvalue = .113, variance
proportions for owner status = .49, for relationship status = .48).
[Insert Table 2 about here]
"A good dog deserves a good home."
-Proverb
Discussion
Though this study may still have failed to address the commonly-occurring
problem of limited generalizability of findings due to its reliance on a somewhat small
sample size and use of a fairly homogenous population (though drawing from different
geographic areas, sampling from a wide range of ages, and using a community sample
rather than a clinical sample), its strengths lie in its consideration of mood variables as
outcome measures, its careful consideration of demographic factors, its presence of
control groups both for testing the effect of dog ownership in general and for the effect of
the presence of a pet dog in particular, and its use of standardized measurements.
In addition, while both within-participants and between-participants models have
their place in research on the effects of dog ownership and the characteristics of dog
owners, the tendency has been to rely primarily on within-participants designs.
Additionally, measures of attitudes towards pets were included for both dog owners and
non-owners, and the effects of levels of attachment to one‟s pet were assessed. Of course,
as tends to be the case when studying the human-companion animal bond, random
67
assignment to dog owner and non-owner conditions was impractical, though random
assignment to the dog present and dog absent conditions was done.
It was not surprising to find that working students were less likely to have dogs,
considering the limitations on their time and the financial responsibilities involved in
caring for a dog, whereas dog owners were more likely to be working full-time but not in
school. Dog owners were also more likely to live alone than with roommates; previous
studies have suggested that pet ownership is more beneficial for owners who have fewer
other sources of social support. Though the general trend is for dog owners to live with
families, the present study was only looking at single women, so it may not be that
surprising that among single women, pet ownership is more likely when they live alone.
What was surprising is that the mood induction main effects did not differ
significantly between the experimental groups, as the primary hypothesis suggested.
While the mood induction overall had the expected effect on happiness, sadness, and
despondency, it did not impact self-esteem or performance on the writing speed task. In
addition, dog owners with their dogs became significantly more anxious and
apprehensive after the negative mood induction relative to non-owners; while it was
initially expected that non-owners‟ scores might fall between those of dog owners with
their dogs and dog owners without their dogs, it turned out that the scores of dog owners
without their dogs fell between those of dog owners with their dogs and non-owners.
It appears that the demands of this particular experimental paradigm were such
that the presence of a dog distracted from the task and exacerbated anxious and
apprehensive responses to the negative mood induction. The owners may have had
68
difficulty focusing on the music in the presence of their dogs; they may also have been
concerned for how their dogs were adjusting to a novel environment. There were also
few relationships between levels of pet attachment and attitude towards pets and the
outcome measures, with none reaching stringent criteria for statistical significance.
However, the dog owners all tended to have very high PAS and Comfort from
Companion Animals Scale scores, limiting the possibility of finding differences in
outcome measures in relation to variations on these measures.
Interestingly, participants who had more positive attitudes towards pets in general
tended to report feeling happier prior to the negative mood induction. This occurred
regardless of the presence of a pet dog, suggesting that there may be mood-enhancing
qualities associated with positive views of pets in general, or perhaps that people who see
pets more positively tend to feel more positively about other things as well. Dog owners
had initially lower levels of despondency and apprehension prior to the negative mood
induction, regardless of the presence of their dogs, though the difference in apprehension
only occurred for dog owners who were living alone or dating.
It is not surprising that dog ownership might confer a greater advantage among
single women living alone; while not discussed in the results section as it did not have a
direct bearing on the current research question, participants who were dating reported
higher initial apprehension scores overall, but the scores were significantly lower for dog
owners. Dog owners with their dogs also reported significantly higher self-esteem prior
to the mood induction, suggesting that the dog owners felt initially more positive in the
presence of their dogs, even if their dogs‟ presence was associated with increases in
69
apprehension and anxiety subsequent to the negative mood induction procedure.
It is hard to know what to make of the lower incidence of depressive symptoms
among only the group of dog owners that brought their dog with them to the follow-up,
as assignment to the dog condition was random, and the BDI-II was administered prior to
this assignment. Most likely, considering the relatively large number of variables and the
relatively small number of participants, this correlation was spurious. No demographic
variables could account for it, but the correlation would disappear if outlying scores on
the BDI-II among non-owners were eliminated.
Also, although there was not an overall association between BDI-II scores and
age, the one outlying score in the group of dog owners without their dogs was also the
oldest participant (76 years old, though still active in the work force), and she even stated
to the researcher that she felt most of her positive responses to items on the BDI-II were
related to her age. If her score was eliminated, there may have been an overall effect of
dog owner status on the BDI-II, not merely for dog owners who brought their dogs with
them to the follow-up.
In an attachment framework, the benefits of being attached to a pet may appear
even in the absence of the pet, as suggested by the lower initial despondency and
apprehension scores among dog owners prior to the negative mood induction procedure.
Participants with positive attitudes towards pets overall also reported higher levels of
happiness prior to the negative mood induction procedure, and dog owners with their
dogs reported higher self-esteem prior to the negative mood induction procedure. Rather
70
than serving to buffer against the effects of the negative mood induction, however, the
presence of pet dogs exacerbated feelings of anxiety and apprehension. Understanding
the specific circumstances in which pets can be positive distractions and sources of
support, rather than negative distractions and sources of anxiety, would be one additional
goal for future research.
Another factor that has not been addressed adequately is how the behaviors of
pets can impact the degree of attachment that their owners experience. James Serpell
(1996) found that less-attached owners tended to be dissatisfied with various aspects of
their pets‟ behaviors. Since pet owners express varying expectations with regard to
expressions of affection from their pets, assessing the degree to which pets meet their
owners‟ expectations may be a significant factor in predicting attachment and the
successful formation of a lasting human-animal bond (Kidd, Kidd, & George, 1992).
This is particularly important considering the frequency of pet relinquishment and the
number of pets that are in shelters or on the streets (Salman, New, Scarlett, Kass, RuchGallie, & Hetts, 1998; New, Salman, Scarlett, Kass, Vaughn, Scherr, & Kelch, 1999;
Scarlett & Salman, 1999; New, Salman, King, Scarlett, Kass, & Hutchison, 2000; Kass,
New, Scarlett, & Salman, 2001).
The relationships between pet ownership, attachment, and social support are also
connected to the biophilia hypothesis and empathy. The biophilia hypothesis suggests
that human beings not only benefit from contact with the natural world but are also
deprived by an absence of contact with the natural world. Developing attachments to
pets as well as to other human beings is likely to provide emotional as well as physical
71
benefits, and pets provide a special quality of social support due to their nonevaluative
nature. The relationship between empathy and the human-animal bond is also deserving
of further consideration, as relatively few studies have touched on this construct
(McPhedran, 2009). Since humans communicate with their pets without language, and
the ability to communicate emotionally is at the core of empathic responses, it is quite
possible that our relationships with companion animals and our connection to the natural
world are important in the development of empathy. Empathic responding is also at the
core of effective social support, and may be related to stronger attachments as well; a
greater understanding of empathy in human-animal interactions is desirable.
The establishment of a bond is clearly important to the welfare of companion
animals as well as to their owners (Friedmann & Son, 2009). Understanding the factors
that contribute to this bond is an ongoing project in the field of human-animal
interactions. While this study suggests that dog owners who have more positive attitudes
toward their pets and are more attached to their pets may derive some benefits from this
attachment, more longitudinal studies are needed to understand how the bonds are formed
and what factors contribute to predicting the strength of the bonds, as well as what risk
factors may predict the failure of a bond to form or the breaking of a bond. Again, people
seek different qualities in their pets as sources of companionship and comfort, so a
greater understanding of how both human and animal qualities contribute to the bond is
needed as well. Some dog owners prefer a more active and energetic dog, others prefer a
calmer dog; some dog owners prefer large dogs, and others prefer small dogs.
72
"Greater cooperation is necessary between psychologists and applied ethologists
to develop methods for scoring and assessing the interactions between humans and
animals, and the effects of poor relationships" (Appleby & Hughes, 1993, p. 394). In
order for the field to progress in answering questions regarding the qualities necessary in
the establishment of a successful human-animal bond, there is a need for greater
standardization of instruments used to assess factors such as attachment, social support,
and other qualities perceived as benefits or correlates of pet ownership. While the current
study used instruments that had been used previously in the context of human-animal
bond research (with the exception of the mood induction and the VAMS), there is still a
general lack of consistency, with some authors making up their own measures of pet
attachment and attitudes towards pets, and others selecting from a veritable hodgepodge
of instruments. Appleby & Hughes additionally suggest a greater use of developmental
and longitudinal studies.
Finally, to conclude where we began, Boris Levinson‟s (1982) suggestions for
future research are just as germane today as they were twenty-seven years ago. While a
huge body of research has been published, there is still a lack of clear terminology in
defining the field (anthrozoology, a study of human-animal interactions, an investigation
of the human-companion animal bond . . .). Though clearer theoretical frameworks are
developing related to attachment theory, social support, and the biophilia hypothesis,
there is still no unifying theoretical model. While some qualitative research is helpful in
generating new hypotheses, there is still insufficient empirically sound work being done.
There is more attention now to cultural factors, though this area of study is rather slim,
73
and the scarcity of longitudinal research makes it difficult to draw conclusions relating
companion animal ownership to the development of human personality. How we
communicate with animals has scarcely been studied, though the suggestion that
nonverbal communication is primarily emotional in nature and related to empathy is an
important one. The major goal that has been met since Levinson‟s article is that a huge
body of research has been built around the concept of animal-assisted therapy, but while
this research is very rich in its observations regarding specific clinical populations, the
typically small and homogenous samples make it virtually impossible to generalize
beyond specific groups and settings.
While this study was able to provide some more evidence supporting the
importance of dog ownership in the general community, with some associations between
dog ownership and self-esteem, and additional evidence supporting the association of
positive attitudes towards pets with happiness, it also indicated that there are some
situations where the presence of a pet dog may increase apprehension and anxiety. Also,
generalizability is an ongoing problem in the field, with self-selected participants, the
inability to randomly assign participants to a dog ownership condition, reliance on
samples that are either too homogenous (so that results may not apply to samples with
different demographic compositions) or too heterogeneous (so that demographic factors
are not sufficiently controlled), and small sample sizes. Still, it appeared that the dog
owners who participated in this study almost universally expressed high degrees of
attachment to their pets, so that the role of attachment and social support should continue
74
to be considered as important variables in the establishment and maintenance of the
human-animal bond.
Future research assessing the impact of the presence of companion animals may
want to more carefully control for the presence of the experimenter as well, by at the very
least having research protocols that involve an experimenter who is blind to the
hypothesis, and ideally having the researcher outside the room altogether for as much of
the time as possible. Considering that dogs impact caregivers and nursing staff in
addition to patients, it is not unlikely that the presence of a dog will have an effect on
experimenters, which cannot help but have some influence on the participants as well.
Another possible explanation of the dog owners‟ anxiety and apprehension in this study
may have been that they were worried how their dog was reacting in the presence of a
stranger, or were concerned what the experimenter thought of their dog‟s behavior. Also,
conducting a mood induction study using neutral music that has not been shown to have a
significant impact on participants‟ moods could be used as a control condition to see if
dog owners with their dogs present become more anxious regardless of the type of music
they are listening to. Finally, a “happy” music condition would help determine if the
anxiety and apprehension occur regardless of the affective content of the music, and
would also assess for the possibility that rather than buffering against negative mood, the
presence of a pet dog may enhance positive mood.
75
References
Akiyama, H., Holtzman, J., & Britz, W. (1987). Pet ownership and health status during
bereavement. Omega, 17, 187-192.
Albert, A., & Bulcroft, K. (1988). Pets, families, and the life course. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 50(2), 543-552.
Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2002). Cardiovascular reactivity and the
presence of pets, friends and spouses: The truth about cats and dogs.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 727-739.
Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., Tomaka, J., & Kelsey, R. M. (1991). Presence of human
friends and pet dogs as moderators of autonomic responses to stress in women.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(4), 582-589.
Allen, K. M., Shykoff, B. E., & Izzo Jr., J. L. (2001). Pet ownership, but not ACE
inhibitor therapy, blunts home blood pressure responses to mental stress.
Hypertension, 38, 815-820.
Allgöwer, A., Wardle, J., & Steptoe, A. (2001). Depressive symptoms, social support,
and personal health behaviors in young men and women. Health Psychology, 20(3),
223-227.
Altschuler, E. L. (1999). Pet-facilitated therapy for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder.
Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 11(1), 29-30.
Anderson, W. P., Reid, C. M., & Jennings, G. L. (1992). Pet ownership and risk factors
for cardiovascular disease. Medical Journal of Australia, 157, 298-301.
76
Anton, S. D., & Miller, P. M. (2005). Do negative emotions predict alcohol
consumption, saturated fat intake, and physical activity in older adults?
Behavior Modification, 29(4), 677-688.
Appleby, M. C., & Hughes, B. O. (1993). The future of applied ethology. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science, 35(4), 389-395.
Arambasic, L., Kerestes, G., Kuterovac-Jagodic, G., & Vizek-Vidovic, V. (2000). The
role of pet ownership as a possible buffer variable in traumatic experiences. Studia
Psychologica, 42(1-2), 135-145.
Archer, J., & Winchester, G. (1994). Bereavement following death of a pet. British
Journal of Psychology, 85(2), 259-272.
Armeli, S., Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & Kranzler, H. R. (2000). Does affect mediate the
association between daily events and alcohol use? Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
61(6), 862-871.
Ascione, F. R. (1992). Enhancing children‟s attitudes about the humane treatment of
animals: Generalization to human-directed empathy. Anthrozoös, 5(3), 176-191.
Ascione, F. R., & Weber, C. V. (1996). Children‟s attitudes about the humane treatment
of animals and empathy: One year follow-up of a school-based intervention.
Anthrozoös, 9(4), 188–195.
Banks, M., & Banks, W. (2002). The effects of animal-assisted therapy on loneliness in
an elderly population in long-term care facilities. Journals of Gerontology: Series A,
Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 57(7), M428-432.
77
Barak, Y, Savorai, O., Mavashev, S., & Beni, A. (2001). Animal assisted therapy for
elderly schizophrenic patients: a one year controlled trial. American Journal of
Geriatric Psychiatry, 9(4), 439-432.
Bardill, N., & Hutchinson, S. (1997). Animal-assisted therapy with hospitalized
adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 70(1), 17-24.
Barker, S. B., & Dawson, K. S. (1998). The effects of animal-assisted therapy on
anxiety ratings of hospitalized psychiatric patients. Psychiatric Services, 49(6), 797801.
Batson, K., McCabe, B., Baun, M. M., & Wilson, C. (1998). The effect of a therapy dog
on socialization and physiological indicators of stress in persons diagnosed with
Alzheimer‟s disease. In C. C. Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds.), Companion animals in
human health (pp. 203-215). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Bauman, A. E., Russell, S. J., Furber, S. E., Dobson, A. J. (2001). The epidemiology of
dog walking: An unmet need for human and canine health. Medical Journal of
Australia, 175, 632-634.
Baun, M., Bergstrom, N., Langston, N., & Thoma, L. (1984). Physiological effects of
human/companion animal bonding. Nursing Research, 15,126-129.
Beck, A., Seraydarian, L., & Hunter, G. F. (1986). The use of animals in the
rehabilitation of psychiatric inpatients. Psychological Reports, 58, 63-66.
Beck, A. M., & Katcher, A. H. (2003). Future directions in human–animal bond
research. American Behavioral Scientist, 47, 79–93.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Hock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory
for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
78
Beck, A. T. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio: Harcourt Brace &
Company.
Berryman, J., Howells, K., & Lloyd-Evans, M. (1985). Pet owner attitudes to pets and
people: A psychological study. Veterinary Record, 17, 659-661.
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1993). Measures of self-esteem. In J. P. Robinson, P. R.
Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (eds.), Measures of Personality and Social
Psychological Attitudes, 3rd edition. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research.
Blenner, J. L. (1991). The therapeutic functions of animals in infertility. Holistic
Nursing Practice, 5, 6-10.
Bodmer, N. M. (1998). Impact of pet ownership on the well-being of adolescents with
few familial resources. In C. C. Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds.), Companion animals
in human health (pp. 237-247). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Brewer, D., & Doughtie, E. B. (1980). Induction of mood and mood shift. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 36(1), 215-226.
Brickel, C. M. (1980-1981). A review of the role of pet animals in psychotherapy and
with the elderly. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 12(2),
119-128.
Brickel, C. M. (1985). Initiation and maintenance of the human-animal bond: Familial
roles from a learning perspective. Marriage and Family Review, 8(3-4), 31-48.
Brickel, C. M. (1986). Pet-facilitated therapies: A review of the literature and clinical
implementation considerations. Clinical Gerontologist, 5(3-4), 309-332.
79
Brown, J. D., & Mankowski, T. A. (1993). Self-esteem, mood, and self-evaluation:
Changes in mood and the way you see you. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 64(3), 421-430.
Brown, S., & Katcher, A. (2001). Pet attachment and dissociation. Society and Animals,
9, 25-42.
Brown, S. (2002). Ethnic variations in pet attachment among students at an American
school of veterinary medicine. Society and Animals, 10, 455–456.
Brown, S. G., & Rhodes, R. E. (2006). Relationship among dog ownership and leisuretime walking in western Canadian adults. American Journal of Preventive Medicine,
30, 131-136.
Budge, R. C., Spicer, J., Jones, B. & St. George, R. (1998). Health correlates of
compatibility and attachment in human-companion animal relationships. Society and
Animals, 6(3), 219-234.
Cain, A. O. (1985). Pets as family members. Marriage and Family Review, 8(3-4), 5-10.
Cameron, P., & Mattson, M. (1972). Psychological correlates of pet ownership.
Psychological Reports, 30, 286.
Carmack, B. J. (1985). The effects on family members and functioning after the death of
a pet. Marriage and Family Review, 8(3-4), 149-161.
Carmack, B. J. (1991). The role of companion animals for persons with AIDS/HIV.
Holistic Nursing Practice, 5, 24-31.
Case, D. B. (1987). Dog ownership: A complex web? Psychological Reports, 60(1),
247-257.
80
Churchill, M., Safaoui, J., McCabe, B. W., & Baun, M. M. (1999). Using a therapy dog
to alleviate the agitation and desocialization of people with Alzheimer's disease.
Journal of Psychosocial Nursing, 37(4), 16-22.
Clark, D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1985). Constraints on the effects of mood on memory.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(6), 1595-1608.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and the mundane: relations between daily life
events and self-reported mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2),
296-308.
Cohen, S. P. (2002). Can pets function as family members? Western Journal of Nursing
Research, 24(6), 621-638.
Coleman, K. J., Rosenberg, D. E., Conway, T. L., Sallis, J. F., Saelens, B. E., Frank, L.
D., & Cain, K. (2008). Physical activity, weight status, and neighborhood
characteristics of dog walkers. Preventive Medicine, 47(3), 309-312.
Corson, S. A., Corson, E. O., Gwynne, P. H., & Arnold, E. A. (1975). Pet facilitated
psychotherapy in a hospital setting. Current Psychiatric Therapies, 15, 277-286.
Corson, S. A., Corson, E. O., Gwynne, P. H., & Arnold, E. A. (1977). Pet dogs as
nonverbal communication links in hospital psychiatry. Comprehensive Psychiatry,
18(1), 61-72.
Corson, S. A., & Corson, E. O. (1978). Pets as mediators for therapy. Current
Psychiatric Therapies, 18,195-205.
Corson, S. A. & Corson, E. O. (1981). Companion animals as bonding catalysts in
geriatric institutions. In B. Fogle (Ed.), Interrelations between people and pets
(pp. 146-174). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
81
Cowles, K. V. (1985). The death of a pet: Human responses to the breaking of the bond.
Marriage and Family Review, 8(3-4), 135-148.
Cox, R. P. (1993). The human/animal bond as a correlate of family functioning.
Clinical Nursing Research, 2(2), 224-231.
Crocker, J. & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth. Psychological Review,
108(3), 593-623.
Cutt, H., Giles-Corti, B., & Knuiman, M. (2007). Encouraging physical activity
through dog walking: Why don't some dog owners walk with their dog? Preventive
Medicine, 46(2), 120-126.
Cutt, H., Giles-Corti, B., Knuiman, M., & Burke, V. (2006). Dog ownership, health and
physical activity: A critical review of the literature. Health and Place, 13(1), 261272.
Cutt, H., Giles-Corti, B., Knuiman, M., Timperio, A., & Bull, F. (2008). Understanding
dog owners' increased levels of physical activity: Results from RESIDE. American
Journal of Public Health, 98, 66-69.
Davis, J. H. (1988). Animal-facilitated therapy in stress meditation. Holistic Nursing
Practice, 2, 75–83.
Eddy, J., Hart, L. A., & Boltz, R. P. (1988). The effects of service dogs on social
acknowledgments of people in wheelchairs. The Journal of Psychology, 122(1), 39
45.
Emmons, R. A., & Diener, E. (1985). Personality correlates and subjective well-being.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11(1), 89-97.
82
Esteves, S. W. & Stokes, T. (2009). Social effect of a dog‟s presence on children with
disabilities (Report). Anthrozoös. 21(2), 5-15.
Fick, K. (1993). The influence of an animal on social interactions of nursing home
residents in a group setting. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 47(6), 529534.
Fournier, A. K., Geller, E. S., & Fortney, E. V. (2007). Human-animal interaction in a
prison setting: Impact on criminal behavior, treatment progress, and social skills.
Behavior and Social Issues, 16(1), 89-105.
Fox, M. (1981). Relationships between the human and nonhuman animals. In B. Fogle
(Ed.), Interrelations between people and pets (pp. 23-40). Springfield, IL: Charles
C. Thomas.
Francis, G., Turner, J. T., & Johnson, S. B. (1985). Domestic animal visitation as
therapy with adult home residents. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 22,
201-206.
Friedmann, E., Katcher, A. H., Lynch, J., & Thomas, S. (1980). Animal companions and
one-year survival of patients after discharge from a coronary care unit. Public
Health Reports, 95, 307-312.
Friedmann, E., Locker, B. Z., & Lockwood, R. (1993). Perceptions of animals and
cardiovascular responses during verbalizations with an animal present. Anthrozoös,
6, 115-134.
Friedmann, E. & Son, H. (2009). The human-companion animal bond: How humans
benefit. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice, 39(2), 293326.
83
Friedmann, E. & Thomas, S. (1985). Health benefits of pets for families. Marriage and
Family Review, 8(3-4), 191-203.
Friedmann, E., & Thomas, S. (1995). Pet ownership, social support and one year
survival after acute myocardial infarction in the Cardiac Arrhythmia Suppression
Trial (CAST). American Journal of Cardiology, 76, 1213-1217.
Furst, G. (2006). Prison-based animal programs: A national survey. The Prison Journal,
86(4), 407-430.
Garrity, T. F., & Stallones, L. (1998). Effects of pet contact on human well-being:
Review of recent research. In C. C. Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds.), Companion
animals in human health (pp. 3-22). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Gatchel, R. J., Paulus, P. B., & Maples, C. W. (1975). Learned helplessness and selfreported affect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84(6), 732-734.
Gaydos, L. S., & Farnham, R. (1988). Human-animal relationships within the context
of Rogers‟s principle of integrality. Advances in Nursing Science, 10(4), 72–80.
Gee, N. R., Harris, S. L., & Johson, K. L. (2007). The role of therapy dogs in speed and
accuracy to complete motor skills tasks for preschool children (Report).
Anthrozoös, 20(4), 375-386.
Gold, A. E., MacLeod, K. M., Frier, B. M., & Deary, I. J. (1995). Changes in mood
during acute hypoglycemia in healthy participants. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 68(3), 498-504.
Goldmeier, J. (1986). Pets or people: Another research note. Gerontologist, 26(2), 203206.
84
Goldstein, R. C. & Willner, P. (2002). Self-report measures of defeat and entrapment
during a brief depressive mood induction. Cognition and Emotion, 16(5), 629-642.
Gonski, Y. (1985). The therapeutic utilization of canines in a child welfare setting.
Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal, 2(2), 93-105.
Goodwin, A. M., & Williams, M. G. (1982). Mood-induction research: Its
implications for clinical depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20(4), 373382.
Green, J. D., Sedikides, C., Saltzberg, J. A., Wood, J. V. & Forzano, L. B. (2003).
Happy mood decreases self-focused attention. British Journal of Social Psychology,
42(1), 147-158.
Guerney, L. F. (1991). A survey of self-supports and social supports for self-care
children. Elementary School Guidance and Counseling, 25(4), 243-254.
Guttman, G. (1981). The psychological determinants of keeping pets. In B. Fogle (Ed.),
Interrelations between people and pets (pp. 89-98). Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Thomas.
Havener, L., Gentes, L., Thaler, B., Megel, M. E., Baun, M. M, Driscoll, F., Beiraghi, S.,
& Agrawal, S. (2001). The effects of a companion animal on distress in children
undergoing dental procedures. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 24(2),
137-152.
Headey, B. (1999). Health benefits and health cost savings due to pets: Preliminary
estimates from an Australian survey. Social Indicators Research, 47, 233–243.
Headey, B., Fu, N., & Zheng, R. (2008). Pet dogs benefit owners‟ health: A „natural
experiment‟ in China. Social Indicators Research, 87(3), 481-493.
85
Headey, B., & Grabka, M. M. (2007). Pets and human health in Germany and Australia:
National longitudinal results. Social Indicators Research, 80, 297-311.
Heath, D. T., & McKenry, P. C. (1989). Potential benefits of companion animals for
self-care children. Childhood Education, 65(5), 311-314.
Hendy, H. M. (1987). Effects of pet and/or people visits on nursing home residents.
International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 25(4), 279-291.
Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1986). Self-discrepancies and
emotional vulnerability: How magnitude, accessibility, and type of discrepancy
influence affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(1), 5-15.
Hines, L. M. (2003). Historical perspectives on the human-animal bond. American
Behavioral Scientist, 47(1), 7-16.
Hines, L. M. & Frederickson, M. (1998). Perspectives on animal-assisted activities
and therapy. In C. C. Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds.), Companion animals in
human health (pp. 23-40). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Humrichouse, J., Chmielewski, M., McDade-Montez, E. A., & Watson, D. (2007).
Affect assessment through self-report methods. In J. Rottenberg & S. L. Johnson
(Eds.), Emotion and psychopathology: Bridging affective and clinical science
(pp. 13-34). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Hunt, M., Al-Awadi, H., & Johnson, M. (2008). Psychological sequelae of pet loss
following Hurricane Katrina (Report). Anthrozoös, 21(2), 109-121.
Hyde, K. R., Kurdek, L., & Larson, P. (1983). Relationships between pet ownership and
self esteem, social sensitivity, and interpersonal trust. Psychological Reports,
52,110.
86
Jenkins, J. L. (1986). Physiological effects of petting a companion animal.
Psychological Reports, 58, 21-22.
Jennings, L. B. (1997). Potential benefits of pet ownership in health promotion. Journal
of Holistic Nursing, 15(4), 358-372.
Johnson, R., Meadows, R., Haubner, J., & Sevedge, K. (2003). Human-animal
interaction: A complementary/alternative medical (CAM) intervention for cancer
patients. American Behavioral Scientist, 47(1), 55-69.
Johnson, R. A., Meadows, R. L., Haubner, J. S. & Sevedge, K. (2008). Animal-assisted
activity among patients with cancer: Effects on mood, fatigue, self-perceived health,
and sense of coherence. Oncology Nursing Forum, 35(2), 225-232.
Jorgenson, J. (1997) Therapeutic use of companion animals in health care. Image:
Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 29(3), 249-254.
Kahn, H. & Jacobs, E. (2003). The effects of animal-assisted therapy on communication
between health care provider and patient. The Gerontologist, 43(1), 20.
Kaiser, L., Spence, L. J., McGavin, L., Struble, L., & Keilman, L. (2002). A dog and a
"happy person" visit nursing home residents. Western Journal of Nursing Research,
24(6), 671-683.
Kaminski, M., Pellino, T., & Wish, J. (2002). Play and pets: The physical and
emotional impact of child-life and pet therapy on hospitalized children. Children's
Health Care, 31(4), 321-335.
87
Kass, P. H., New Jr., J. C., Scarlett, J. M., & Salman, M. D. (2001). Understanding
companion animal surplus in the United States: Relinquishment of nonadoptables to
animal shelters for euthanasia. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 4(4),
237-248.
Katcher, A. H. (1981). Interactions between people and their pets: Form and function.
In B. Fogle (Ed.), Interrelations between people and pets (pp. 41-67). Springfield,
IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Katcher, A. H. (1985). Physiologic and behavioral responses to companion animals.
Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice, 15(2), 403-410.
Katcher, A., Beck, A. M., & Levine, D. (1989). Evaluation of a pet program in prison:
The PAL project at Lorton. Anthrozoös, 2(3), 175-180.
Kawamura, N., Niiyama, M. & Niiyama, H. (2007). ORIGINAL ARTICLE: Long-term
evaluation of animal-assisted therapy for institutionalized elderly people: A
preliminary result. Psychogeriatrics, 17(1), 8-13.
Kawamura, N., Niiyama, M., & Niiyama, H. (2009). Animal-assisted activity:
Experiences of institutionalized Japanese older adults. Journal of Psychosocial
Nursing & Mental Health Services, 47(1), 41-47.
Kidd, A. H. & Feldman, B. M. (1981). Pet ownership and self-perceptions of older
people. Psychological Reports, 48, 867 – 875.
Kidd, A. H., & Kidd, R. M. (1980). Personality characteristics and preferences
in pet ownership. Psychological Reports, 46, 939-949.
Kidd, A. H. & Kidd, R. M. (1985). Children‟s attitudes towards their pets.
Psychological Reports, 57, 15 – 31.
88
Kidd, A. H. & Kidd, R. M. (1994). Benefits and liabilities of pets for the homeless.
Psychological Reports, 74(3, pt 1), 715-722.
Kidd, A. H., Kidd, R. M., & George, C. C. (1992). Successful and unsuccessful pet
adoptions. Psychological Reports, 70, 547–561.
Kogan, L. R., Granger, B. P., Fitchett, J. A., Helmer, K. A., & Young, K. J. (1999). The
human-animal team approach for children with emotional disorders: Two case
studies. Child and Youth Care Forum, 28(2), 105-121.
Kongable, L., Buckwalter, K., & Stolley, J. (1989). The effects of pet therapy on the
social behavior of institutionalized Alzheimer‟s clients. Archives of Psychiatric
Nursing, 3(4), 191-198.
LaFrance, C., Garcia, L. J., & Labreche, J. (2007). The effect of a therapy dog on the
communication skills of an adult with aphasia. Journal of Communication Disorders,
40(3), 215-224.
Lago, D., Delaney, M., Miller, M. & Grill, C. (1989). Companion animals, attitudes
towards pets, and health outcomes among the elderly: A long-term follow-up.
Anthrozoös, 3, 25-34.
Laird, J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotion: The effects of expressive behavior on
the quality of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
29(4), 475-486.
Lane, D. R., McNicholas, J., & Collis, G. M. (1998). Dogs for the disabled: Benefits to
recipients and welfare of the dog. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 59, 49-60.
89
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and
negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 132140.
Larsen, R. J., & Sinnett, L. M. (1991). Meta-analysis of experimental manipulations:
Some factors affecting the Velten mood induction procedure. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(3), 323-334.
Lefkowitz, C., Prout, M., Bleiberg, J., Paharia, I., & Debiak, D. (2005). Animal-assisted
prolonged exposure: A treatment for survivors of sexual assault suffering
posttraumatic stress disorder. Society and Animals, 13(4), 275-295.
Levinson, B. M. (1961). The dog as co-therapist. Mental Hygiene, 46, 59-65.
Levinson, B. M. (1967). The pet and the child‟s bereavement. Mental Hygiene, 51(2),
197-200.
Levinson, B. M. (1969). Pets and old age. Mental Hygiene, 53(3), 364-368.
Levinson, B. M. (1978). Pets and Personality Development. Psychological Reports,
42(3), 1031-1038.
Levinson, B. M. (1982). The future of research into relationships between people and
their animal companions. International Journal for the Study of Animal Problems,
3(4), 283-294.
Macauley, B. L. (2006). Animal-assisted therapy for persons with aphasia: A pilot
study. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 43(3), 357-366.
MacDonald, A. (1981). The pet dog in the home: A study of interactions. In B. Fogle
(Ed.), Interrelations between people and pets (pp. 195-206). Springfield, IL: Charles
C. Thomas.
90
Mader, B., Hart, L. & Bergin, B. (1989). Social acknowledgments for children with
disabilities: Effects of service dogs. Child Development, 60, 1529-1534.
Mallon, G. P. (1992). Utilization of animals as therapeutic adjuncts with children and
youth. Child and Youth Care Forum, 21, 53-67.
Mallon, G. P. (1994). Some of our best therapists are dogs. Child and Youth Care
Forum, 23(2), 89-101.
Marks, S. G., Koepke, J. E., & Bradley, C. L. (1994). Pet attachment and generativity
among young adults. The Journal of Psychology, 128(6), 641-650.
Martin, F. & Farnum, J. (2002). Animal-assisted therapy for children with pervasive
developmental disorders. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 24(6), 657-670.
Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 669697.
McNicholas, J. & Collis, G. M. (2000). Dogs as catalysts for social interactions:
Robustness of the effect. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 61-70
McPhedran, S. (2009). A review of the evidence for associations between empathy,
violence, and animal cruelty. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14(1), 1-4.
Melson, G. F. (2002). Psychology and the study of human-animal relationships. Society
and Animals, 10(4), 347-352.
Melson, G. F. (2003). Child development and the human-companion animal bond.
American Behavioral Scientist, 47, 31-39.
Messent, P. R., & Serpell, J. A. (1981). An historical and biological view of the petowner bond. In B. Fogle (Ed.), Interrelations between people and pets (pp. 5-22).
Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
91
Miller, D., Staats, S., & Partlo, C. (1992). Discriminating positive and negative aspects
of pet interaction: Sex differences in the older population. Social Indicators
Research, 27, 363-374.
Minatrea, N. B. & Wesley, M. C. (2008). Reality therapy goes to the dogs. International
Journal of Reality Therapy, 28(1) 69-77.
Morley, C., & Fook, J. (2005). The importance of pet loss and some implications for
services. Mortality, 10, 127-143.
Morrison, M. (2007) Health benefits of animal assisted interventions. Complementary
Health Practice Review, 12(1), 51-62.
Muschel, I. (1984). Pet therapy with terminal patients with cancer. Social Casework,
65(8), 451-458.
Nagasawa, M., Kikusui, T., Onaka, T., & Ohta, M. (2008). Dog's gaze at its owner
increases owner's urinary oxytocin during social interaction. Hormones and
Behavior, In Press, Corrected Proof, Available online 14 December 2008.
Nagengast, S. L., Baun, M. M., Megel, M., & Leibowitz, J. M. (1997). The effects of
the presence of a companion animal on physiological arousal and behavioral distress
in children during a physical examination. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 12, 323
330.
Nathans-Barel, I., Feldman, P., Berger, B., Modai, I. & Silver, H. (2005). Animalassisted therapy ameliorates anhedonia in schizophrenia patients. Psychotherapy
and Psychosomatics, 74, 31-35.
Netting, F. E., Wilson, C. C., & New, J. C. (1987). The human-animal bond:
Implications for practice. Social Work, 32, 60-64.
92
Neumann, R., Seibt, B., & Strack, F. (2001). The influence of mood on the intensity
of emotional responses: Disentangling feeling and knowing. Cognition and
Emotion, 15(6), 725-747.
New Jr., J. C., Salman, M. D., Scarlett, J. M., Kass, P. H., Vaughn, J. A., Scherr, S, &
Kelch, W.J. (1999). Moving: Characteristics of dogs and cats and those
relinquishing them to 12 U.S. animal shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare
Science, 2(2), 83-97.
New Jr., J. C., Salman, M. D., King, M., Scarlett, J. M., Kass, P. H., & Hutchison, J. M.
(2000). Characteristics of shelter-relinquished animals and their owners compared
with animals and their owners in U.S. pet-owning households. Journal of Applied
Animal Welfare Science, 3(3), 179-201.
Nimer, J. & Lundahl, B. (2007). Animal-assisted therapy: A meta-analysis. Anthrozoös,
20(3), 225-238.
Odendaal, J. S. J., & Lehmann, S. M. C. (2000). The role of phenylethylamine during
positive human-dog interaction. ACTA Veterinaria Brno, 69, 183-188.
Odendaal, J. S. J. (2000). Animal-assisted therapy -- Magic or medicine? Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 49, 275-280.
Odendaal, J. S. J., & Meintjes, R. A. (2003). Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative
behaviour between humans and dogs. The Veterinary Journal, 165, 296-301.
Ory, M. G., & Goldberg, E. L. (1983). Pet possession and well-being in elderly women.
Research in Aging, 5(3), 389-409.
Paden-Levy, D. (1985). Relationship of extraversion, neuroticism, alienation, and
divorce incidence with pet-ownership. Psychological Reports, 57, 868–870.
93
Parish-Plass, N. (2008). Animal-assisted therapy with children suffering from insecure
attachment due to abuse and neglect: A method to lower the risk of
intergenerational transmission of abuse? Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
13(1), 7-30.
Park, D., Kripke, D. F., & Cole, R. J. (2007). More prominent reactivity in mood
than activity and sleep induced by differential light exposure due to seasonal and
local differences. Chronobiology International, 24(5), 905-920.
Parker, D. A., Parker, E. S., Harford, T. C., & Farmer, G. C. (1987). Alcohol use and
depression symptoms among employed men and women. American Journal of
Public Health, 77(6), 704-707.
Parrott, W. G. (1991). Mood induction and instructions to sustain moods: A test of the
subject compliance hypothesis of mood congruent memory. Cognition and Emotion,
5(1), 41-52.
Parslow, R. A. & Jorm, A. F. (2003). The impact of pet ownership on health and health
service use: Results from a community sample of Australians aged 40 to 44 years.
Anthrozoös, 16, 43-56.
Paul, E. S., & Serpell, J. A. (1993). Childhood pet keeping and humane attitudes in
young adulthood. Animal Welfare, 2, 321-337.
Paul, E. S. & Serpell, J. A. (1996). Obtaining a new pet dog: Effects on middle
childhood children and their families. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 47, 1729.
94
Pignatiello, M., Camp, C. J., Elder, S. T., & Rasar, L. A. (1989). A
psychophysiological comparison of the Velten and Musical mood induction
techniques. Journal of Music Therapy, 26(3), 140-154.
Pittman, N. L., & Pittman, T. S. (1979). Effects of amount of helplessness training and
internal-external locus of control on mood and performance. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 37(1), 39-47.
Pittman, T. S., & Pittman, N. L. (1980). Deprivation of control and the attribution
process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(3), 377-389.
Polivy, J. (1981). On the induction of emotion in the laboratory: Discrete moods or
multiple affect states? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(4), 803-817.
Poresky, R. H. (1990). The young children‟s empathy measure: Reliability, validity and
effects of companion animal bonding. Psychological Reports, 66, 931–936.
Poresky, R. H. (1996). Companion animals and other factors affecting young children‟s
development. Anthrozoös, 9(4), 159-168.
Poresky, R. H. (1997). Sex, childhood pets and young adults‟ self-concept scores.
Psychological Reports, 80(2), 371-377.
Poresky, R. H. & Hendrix, C. (1990). Differential effects of pet presence and petbonding on young children. Psychological Reports, 67(1), 51-54.
Poresky, R. H., Hendrix, C., Mosier, J. E., & Samuelson, M. L. (1987). Children's pets
and adults' self-concepts. Journal of Psychology, 122(5), 463-469.
Poresky, R. H., Hendrix, C., Mosier, J. E. & Samuelson, M. L. (1988). Young children‟s
companion animal bonding and adults‟ pet attitudes: A retrospective study.
Psychological Reports, 62, 419-425.
95
Prosser, L., Townsend, M., Staiger, P. (2008) Older people‟s relationship with
companion animals: A pilot study. Nursing Older People, 20(3), 29-32.
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the
depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression.
Psychological Bulletin, 102(1), 122-138.
Raina, P., Waltner-Toews, D., Bonnett, B., Woodward, C., & Abernathy, T. (1999).
Influence of companion animals on the physical and psychological health of older
people: An analysis of a one-year longitudinal study. Journal of the American
Geriatrics Society, 47, 323-9.
Redefer, L. A., & Goodman, J. F. (1989). Brief report: Pet-facilitated therapy with
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 461-467.
Reichert, E. (1994). Play and animal-assisted therapy: A group treatment model for
sexually abused girls ages 9-13. Family Therapy, 21(1), 55-62.
Reichert, E. (1998). Individual counseling for sexually abused children: A role for
animals and storytelling. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 15(3), 177-185.
Rholes, W. S., Riskind, J. H., & Lane, J. W. (1987). Emotional states and memory
biases: Effects of cognitive priming and mood. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 52(1), 91-99.
Richeson, N. E. (2003) Effects of animal assisted therapy on agitated behaviours
and social interactions of older adults with dementia. American Journal of
Alzheimer‟s Disease and Other Dementias, 18, 6, 353-358.
Robb, S. S., Boyd, M., & Pristash, C. L. (1980). A wine bottle, plant, and puppy:
Catalysts for social behavior. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 6, 721-728.
96
Robb, S. S. & Stegman, C. E. (1983). Companion animals and elderly people: A
challenge for evaluators of social support. Gerontologist, 23(3), 277-282.
Robin, M., & ten Bensel, R. (1985). Pets and the socialization of children. Marriage and
Family Review, 8(3-4), 63-78.
Robins, D., Sanders, C., & Cahill, S. (1991). Dogs and their people: Pet-facilitated
interaction in a public setting. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 20, 3-25.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, New Jersey:
Princeton University Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image (Revised edition).
Middletown, Connecticut: Wesleyan University Press.
Rosenkoetter, M. M. (1991). Health promotion: The influence of pets on life patterns in
the home. Holistic Nursing Practice, 5, 42–51.
Roth, B. (2005). Pets and psychoanalysis: A clinical contribution. Psychoanalytic
Review, 92(3), 453-468.
Ruhé, H. G., Mason, N. S., & Schene, A. H. (2007). Mood is indirectly related to
serotonin, norepinephrine and dopamine levels in humans: a meta-analysis of
monoamine depletion studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 331-359.
Ryder, R. D. (1973). Pets in man‟s search for sanity. Journal of Small Animal Practice,
14, 657–668.
Rynearson, E. K. (1978). Humans and pets and attachment. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 133, 550–555.
97
Salman, M. D., New Jr., J. G., Scarlett, J. M., Kass, P. H., Ruch-Gallie, R., & Hetts, S.
(1998). Human and animal factors related to the relinquishment of dogs and cats in
12 selected animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare
Science, 1(3), 207-226.
Sanders, C. (1993). Understanding dogs: Caretakers‟ attributions of mindedness in
canine-human relationships. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 22, 205-227.
Sanders, C. R. (2003). Actions speak louder than words: Close relationships between
humans and nonhuman animals. Symbolic Interaction, 26, 405-425.
Savishinsky, J. S. (1992). Intimacy, domesticity and pet therapy with the elderly:
Expectation and experience among nursing home volunteers. Social Science and
Medicine, 34, 1325-1334.
Scarlett, J. M. & Salman, M. D. (1999). Reasons for relinquishment of companion
animals in U.S. animal shelters: Selected health and personal issues. Journal of
Applied Animal Welfare Science, 2(1), 41-58.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1977). Self-focused attention and the experience of
emotion: Attraction, repulsion, elation, and depression. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 35(9), 625-636.
Seibert, P. S., & Ellis, H. C. (1991). A convenient self-referencing mood induction
procedure. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29(2), 121-124.
Sellers, D. M. (2005). The evaluation of an animal assisted therapy intervention for
elders with dementia in long-term care. Activities, Adaptation and Aging, 30(1), 6177.
98
Serpell, J. A. (1981). Childhood pets and their influence on adults‟ attitudes.
Psychological Reports, 49, 651-654.
Serpell, J. A. (1991). Beneficial effects of pet ownership on some aspects of human
health and behaviour. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 84, 717-720.
Serpell, J. A. (1996). Evidence for an association between pet behaviour and owner
attachment levels. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 47, 49-60.
Siegel, A. (1962). Reaching the severely withdrawn through pet therapy. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 118, 1045-1046.
Siegel, J. M. (1990). Stressful life events and use of physician services among the
elderly: The moderating role of pet ownership. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 58(6), 1081-1086.
Siegel, J. M. (1993). Companion animals: In sickness and in health. Journal of Social
Issues, 49(1), 157-167.
Siegel, J. M., Angulo, F. J., Detels, R., Wesch, J., & Mullen, A. (1999). AIDS diagnosis
and depression in the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study: The ameliorating impact of
pet ownership. AIDS Care, 11(2), 157-170.
Slyker, J. P. & McNally, R. J. (1991). Experimental induction of anxious and depressed
moods: Are Velten and musical procedures necessary? Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 15(1), 33-45.
Soares, C. J. (1985). The companion animal in the context of the family system.
Marriage and Family Review, 8(3-4), 49-62.
Sobo, E. J., Eng, B., & Kassity-Krich, N. (2006). Canine visitation (pet) therapy: pilot
data on decreases in child pain perception. Journal of Holistic Nursing, 24(1), 51-57.
99
Sockalingam, S., Li, M., Krishnadev, U., Hanson, K., Balaban, K., Pacione, L. R., &
Bhalerao, S. (2008). Use of animal-assisted therapy in the rehabilitation of an
assault victim with a concurrent mood disorder. Issues in Mental Health Nursing,
29, 73-84.
Stallones, L., Marx, M. B., Garrity, T. F., & Johnson, T. P. (1988). Attachment to
companion animals among older pet owners. Anthrozoös, 2, 118-124.
Stallones, L., Marx, M., Garrity, T, & Johnson, T. (1990). Pet ownership and attachment
in relation to the health of U.S. adults, 21 to 64 years of age. Anthrozoös, 4,100-112.
Steed, H. N., & Smith, B. S. (2003). Animal assisted activities for geriatric patients.
Activities, Adaptation and Aging, 27(1), 49-61.
Steffens, M. C. & Bergler, R. (1998). Blind people and their dogs: An empirical study
on changes in everyday life, in self-experience, and in communication. In C. C.
Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds.), Companion animals in human health (pp. 149-157).
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (1999). Mood and drinking: A naturalistic study of alcohol,
coffee and tea. Psychopharmacology, 141, 315-321.
Strimple, E. O. (2003). A history of prison inmate-animal interaction programs. The
American Behavioral Scientist, 47(1), 10-18.
Taylor, H., Williams, P., & Gray, D. (2004). Homelessness and dog ownership: An
investigation into animal empathy, attachment, crime, drug use, health and public
opinion. Anthrozoös, 17(4), 353-368.
100
Teasdale, J. D., & Fogarty, S. J. (1979). Differential effects of induced mood on
retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 88(3), 248-257.
Templer, D. I., Salter, C. A., Dickey, S., Baldwin, R., & Veleber, D. M. (1981). The
construction of a pet attitude scale. The Psychological Record, 31, 343-348.
Tissen, I., Hergovich, A., & Spiel, C. (2007). School-based social training with and
without dogs: Evaluation of their effectiveness (Report). Anthrozoös, 20(4), 365373.
Triebenbacher, S. L. (1998). The relationship between attachment to companion animals
and self-esteem. In C. C. Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds.), Companion animals in
human health (pp. 135-148). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Valentine, D. P., Kiddoo, M. & LaFleur, B. (1993). Psychosocial implications of service
dog ownership for people who have mobility or hearing impairments. Social Work
in Health, 19(1), 109–125.
Van Houte, B. A. and Jarvis, P. A. (1995). The role of pets in preadolescent
psychosocial development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16,
463–479.
Van Leeuwen, J. (1981). A child psychiatrist‟s perspective on children and their
companion animals. In B. Fogle (Ed.), Interrelations between people and pets (pp.
175-194). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Veevers, J. E. (1985). The social meanings of pets: Alternative roles for companion
animals. Marriage and Family Review, 8(3-4), 11-30.
101
Velten, E., Jr. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 6(4), 473-482.
Voelker, R. (1995). Puppy love can be therapeutic, too. Journal of the American
Medical Association, 274(24), 1897-1899.
Walsh, P. G., Mertin, P. G., Verlander, D. F., & Pollard, C. F. (1995). The effect of a
"pets as therapy" dog on persons with dementia in a psychiatric ward. Australian
Occupational Therapy Journal, 52, 161-166.
Walsh, P G., & Mertin, P G. (1996). The training of pets as therapy dogs in a women's
prison: A pilot study. Anthrozoös, 7(2), 124-128.
Watson, D., Suls, J. & Haig, J. (2002). Global self-esteem in relation to structural
models of personality and affectivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
83(1), 185-197.
Wells, M. & Perrine, R. (2001). Critters in the cube farm: Perceived psychological and
organizational effects of pets in the workplace. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 6(1), 81-87.
Wells, D.L. (2004). The facilitation of social interactions by domestic dogs. Anthrozoös,
17, 340-352.
Wells, D. L. (2007). Domestic dogs and human health: An overview. British Journal of
Health Psychology, 12, 145–156.
Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness of
mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 26, 557-580.
102
Wilhelm, P., & Schoebi, D. (2007). Assessing mood in daily life: Structural validity,
sensitivity to change, and reliability of a short-scale to measure three basic
dimensions of mood. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(4), 258267.
Wilson, C. C. (1987). Physiological responses of college students to a pet. The Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(10), 606-612.
Wilson, C. C. (1991). The pet as an anxiolytic intervention. The Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 179(8), 482-489.
Wilson, C. C. & Barker, S. B. (2003). Challenges in designing human-animal
interaction research. American Behavioral Scientist, 47(1), 16-28.
Winkler, A., Fairnie, H., Gericevich, F., & Long, M. (1989). The impact of a resident
dog on an institution for the elderly: Effects on perceptions and social interactions.
Gerontologist, 29(2), 216-233.
Wood, L., & Giles-Corti, B. (2005). The pet connection: Pets as a conduit for social
capital? Social Science and Medicine, 61, 1159-1173.
Wood, L. J., Giles-Corti, B., Bulsara, M. K. & Bosch, D. A. (2007). More than a furry
companion: The ripple effect of companion animals on neighborhood interactions
and sense of community. Society and Animals, 15, 43-56.
Wright, J. C., & Moore, D. (1982). Comments on animal companions and one-year
survival of patients after discharge. Public Health Reports, 97, 380–381.
Wu, A. S., Niedra, R., Pendergast, L. & McCrindle, B. W. (2002). Acceptability and
impact of pet visitation on a pediatric cardiology inpatient unit. Journal of Pediatric
Nursing, 17, 354–362.
103
Zasloff, R. L. (1996). Measuring attachment to companion animals: A dog is not a cat
is not a bird. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 47, 43-48.
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G. & Farley, G. K. (1988). The
Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 52, 30-41.
92
Table 1
Demographic Variables by Owner Status and Experimental Group
Group
Geography
------------New York City
Hudson Valley
Shenandoah Valley
Dog owner
21
8
15
With dog
9
3
10
Without dog
12
5
5
Non-owner
13
1
8
Group
<26
26-35
Age
------36-45
Dog owner
8
10
4
8
11
3
With dog
3
7
2
3
7
0
Without dog 5
3
2
5
4
3
Non-owner
3
3
3
5
2
PhD
6
46-55
56-65
>65
Group
> H.S.
BA/BS
Education
---------------> BA
MA/MS/MFA
Dog owner
8
15
4
14
3
With dog
3
8
4
6
1
Without dog 5
7
0
8
2
Non-owner
5
4
5
4
4
93
Group
Student
Occupation
------------------Working student
Part Time
Dog owner
4
3
2
30
5
With dog
3
2
1
14
2
Without dog 1
1
1
16
3
Non-owner
7
0
11
2
2
Full Time
None
Group
Household
------------Alone
Roommate(s)
Relationship Status
-------------------------Single
Dating
Dog owner
36
8
31
13
With dog
16
6
16
6
Without dog
20
2
15
7
Non-owner
13
9
16
6
94
Table 2
Main Differences Between Experimental Groups
Group
Beck Depression Inventory - II
-----------------------------------------Mean
Standard Deviation
With dog
3.64
3.43
Without dog
5.95
5.385
Non-owner
6.95
6.214
Pet Attitude Scale
---------------------------------Standard Deviation
Group
Mean
With dog
42.45
7.645
Without dog
40.45
11.791
Non-owner
21.64
18.186
Group
Reworded Self-Esteem Scale (Prior to Mood Induction)
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------Mean
Standard Deviation
With dog
17.55
2.738
Without dog
15.86
3.342
Non-owner
14.50
5.280
95
Group
Despondency (Prior to Mood Induction)
---------------------------------------------------------------Mean
Standard Deviation
With dog
4.32
4.031
Without dog
7.61
8.396
Non-owner
15.58
18.635
Group
Apprehension (Prior to Mood Induction)
-----------------------------------------------------Mean
Standard Deviation
With dog
10.02
16.650
Without dog
6.77
6.254
Non-owner
19.95
24.706
Change in Anxiety
--------------------------------Standard Deviation
Group
Mean
Dog owner
+1.93
15.132
Non-owner
-6.66
16.234
Change in Apprehension
---------------------------------------Group
Mean
Standard Deviation
Dog owner
+2.92
13.130
Non-owner
-4.93
13.900