The Description and Classification of Craquelure
Spike Bucklow
Studies in Conservation, Vol. 44, No. 4. (1999), pp. 233-244.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0039-3630%281999%2944%3A4%3C233%3ATDACOC%3E2.0.CO%3B2-%23
Studies in Conservation is currently published by International Institute for Conservation of Historic and Artistic Works.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained
prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in
the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.
Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/journals/iich.html.
Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.
The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic
journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers,
and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take
advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact
[email protected].
http://www.jstor.org
Mon Sep 24 05:44:01 2007
THE DESCRIPTION AND CLASSIFICATION OF CRAQUELURE
Spike Bucklow
Summary--The priper provicies the stutisticul context of research to rfejne sinzple ciichotonzous terms ,for the
ciescription of crrzquelure. The valiciity of the proposed set of eight descr@tors of cruquelure is established by
reference to the results of various clussiJicution tusks. These include the unassisted rissi'qnnr~lentof craqztelure to
spec& categories by experienced subjects, the same task undertaken by ine.xperiencet1 subjects assisted by
rules, the c.la.rsification of repres~ntationsof craquelure by a neurul network, ant1 61- discriminant anulysis.
Tu.o vpe.s qf repres~ntutionare c,or?lpareci;one based upon the Repertory Grid technique and t h other
~
based
upon Brzier c.urves.
Introduction
A recent paper offered a number of simple dichotomous terms for the description of craquelure [I]. It
also presented some 'rules of thumb' which related
particular descriptive terms to particular technical
traditions of painting or art historical categories.
Those rules of thumb were derived from a statistical analysis of numerical representations of crack
patterns generated by 31 subjects judging 40 crack
patterns against a series of scales defined by the
dichotomous terms.
This paper offers comparisons between various
ways of describing craquelure. Two activities, in
which individuals classified photographs of crack
patterns, are compared. The computational classifications of two types of numerical representation of
craquelure are also compared.
The human activities both involved the sorting of
40 photographs into four sets of 10 photographs.
One group of individuals (including experienced
conservators) received no assistance in this task.
The other group of individuals (inexperienced students. for example) were guided in their task by the
rules of thumb. The computational routines (involving neural networks and discriminant analysis) were
applied to two numerical representations of the 40
photographs. One set of numerical representations
was derived heuristically from the data provided by
31 subjects from which the rules of thumb were
also derived. The other set of numerical representations was generated algorithmically from the data
obtained by scanning and digitizing the 40 photographs.
Unassisted sorting
Subjects were presented with a set of 40 photographs. 10 examples each of: fifteenth century
Studies in Conservution 44 (1999) 233-244
Italian panels (It), sixteenth century Flemish panels
(Fl), seventeenth century Dutch canvas (Du) and
eighteenth century French canvas paintings (Fr).
The 4 x 6 inch black and white photographs
showed areas of craquelure of between c. 3 x 5
and c. 13 x 20cm. A scale bar was printed in the
margin of the photograph to indicate the degree of
magnification and orientation with respect to the
wood grain or the maximum stretcher dimension.
Subjects were asked to sort the 40 photographs into
four groups of 10 and identify the groups as
Italian, Flemish. Dutch or French.
One subject with little experience of the variety of
crack patterns had a success rate of little better
than chance when sorting the 40 photographs. Ten
experienced subjects (eight conservators, a conservation scientist and a curator) sorted the 40 photographs. They were given no assistance in this task
and analysis of the groups they created revealed a
success rate in classification of between 25 and 35
out of 40. This represents 62.5 to 87.5% correct
attribution of crack patterns. The average score was
73.0%.
Rule-based sorting
Twenty-one subjects with little o r no experience of
crack patterns undertook the same task of sorting
40 photographs into four groups of 10. They were
assisted in this task by four sets of rules which correlated pattern characteristics and painting categories [I]. Analysis of the groups they created
revealed a success rate in classification of between
27 and 32 out of 40. This represents 67.5 to 80.0%
correct attribution of crack patterns. The average
score was 74.0%.
Although the average scores for both sets of subjects were effectively the same, the range of scores
was significantly different. The range of scores
233
S. Bucklow
among those assisted by rules ( i 6.25%) was half
that of those who undertook the task unassisted
( i 12.5%). This indicates that the rules enable a better level of performance than the least successful
unassisted subject but d o not allow the novice to
emulate the performance of the expert.
The 10 subjects who undertook the task unassisted represented a range of experience: one professed a degree of expertise, two had formally
studied craquelure as part of their training and the
remaining seven had no particular interest in
cracks. The time taken to complete the task
(c. 3 0 4 5 minutes) was similar for the two groups
and the most successful subjects took the least time.
In view of the speed of execution and range of
results, it is most unlikely that the connoisseurship
of craquelure relies upon a mental representation of
patterns such as the one developed in this research.
Its value lies not in the emulation of connoisseurship, but in demonstrating that complex visual phenomena may be described simply, and that simple
descriptions nonetheless possess potential for rigorous analysis in terms of the material structure of
paintings.
Heuristic representation
The rules correlating pattern descriptions with
painting categories were derived from the eight
numerical scores given to each of the 40 photographs by all 31 subjects. The mean value and
standard deviations of these numerical scores were
calculated to determine rules such as: that Italian
panels usually displayed cracks perpendicular to the
grain, cracks in Flemish paintings were parallel to
the grain, and that cracks in French paintings had
no particular direction, etc.
A method known as the Repertory Grid technique was employed to convert perceptual judgments into numerical scores. A critical evaluation
of the acquisition stage that generated the data for
subsequent analysis is given elsewhere [2], but arteTable 1 Neural network classijcation of one subject's heuristic representution of 40 patterns
Neural
net
It
F1
Du
Fr
as if It
as if F1
as if Du
as if Fr
Total
6
1
3
0
10
0
10
0
0
10
0
1
0
0
7
2
10
3
Group
total
1
7
10
6
12
3
9
30140=75.0%
facts which may have influenced the statistical evaluation of the data, such as crack patterns due to
specific localized damages or stresses, were avoided.
Some pre-selection of the data presented to subjects
was therefore undertaken. This did not, however,
influence the statistical evaluation of the crack patterns as the criteria for selection were independent
of the classification categories. The resultant numerical representations of crack patterns were classified
directly using a computer. This classification of the
heuristic representation enabled a quantitative
assessment of the validity of the dichotomous
descriptions underlying the rules.
Each subject's responses were classified individually using a 'neural network'. In a typical example,
the neural network created four groups which can
be identified as mainly Italian, mainly Flemish,
mainly Dutch and mainly French, containing 6, 12,
13 and 9 crack patterns in total, respectively. As it
was not possible to specify to the neural network
that there were 10 examples of each category, this
task is not exactly equivalent to the human sorting
tasks reported above. A summary of the assignments is presented in Table 1.
The neural network classified 30 (6 Italian, 10
Flemish, 7 Dutch and 7 French) out of 40 correctly. This represents a success rate of 75.0%,
effectively the same as the average performance of
the 31 subjects who directly sorted the same 40
photographs. This suggests that the rules given to
the 21 less experienced subjects, if not actually
responsible for their enhanced level of performance,
were at least not inconsistent with their judgments.
The set of eight dichotomous descriptions classified by computer and the heuristic rules available to
novices both resulted in approximately the same
level of discrimination between patterns as the average achieved by experienced conservators. However,
the neural network classification of 40 sets of eight
numbers, the sorting of 40 photographs by novices
using rules and the unassisted sorting of 40 photographs by experts are all equally artificial tasks.
To the extent that craquelure has attributive significance, it is judged in terms of the conformity of
an individual example to a class of examples. The
neural net separately judged representations of individual unclassified photographs against representations of the other, already classified, photographs.
The inexperienced subjects judged individual photographs against the rules representing each class.
The experienced subjects judged individual photographs against their previous experience of
craquelure patterns in general.
These tasks are artificial as, in each case, the
class of the photograph is unknown. To the extent
that craquelure is used in the attribution of a painting, it is used to confirm or refute an existing proStudies in Conservution 44 (1999) 233-244
The description and classification of craquelure
posed attribution. In other words, before we judge
a crack pattern in terms of the technical tradition
which produced it, we already know (or think we
know) the identity of that tradition.
With the computational classification of the
numerical representations of crack patterns, this
prior knowledge of the origin of the pattern can be
emulated by using discriminant analysis instead of a
neural network. Discriminant analysis classified
examples not only on the strength of the eight
numerical scores describing the crack pattern, but
also with reference to the art historic category of the
painting. Using the same data that illustrated the
neural network classification above, Table 2 shows
the assignments made by discriminant analysis.
That discriminant analysis produced four groups
with 10 examples in each was fortuitous and did
not happen with every subject's data. However, the
heuristic representations of the 40 crack patterns
appear to cluster together in a manner which accurately reflects the art historic categories of the
paintings. This suggests two things: first, that crack
patterns are indeed related to technical traditions,
and second, that the eight descriptive terms capture
sufficient information about the pattern to enable
discrimination between categories.
The heuristic representation is, however, provided
by a human being, and by analysing the data provided from only one subject we d o not know
whether or not their description is idiosyncratic. A
heuristic representation is only valid if it is used
consistently [3].
T o check whether the representations were consistent, discriminant analysis was undertaken for
the data provided by all 31 subjects. Analysis of the
groups created revealed a success rate in classification of between 30 and 39 out of 40. This represents 75.0 to 97.5% correct attribution of crack
patterns with an average score of 91.4%.
The range of results ( 2 11.25%) is similar to the
range of results in the direct sorting task undertaken by unassisted experienced subjects. This suggests that the insertion of a (simple) mediating
Table 2 Discriminant analysis of one subject's
heuristic representation of 40 patterns
Discriminant
analysis
It
as if It
as if F1
as if Du
as if Fr
Total
FI
Du
Fr
Group
total
9
0
0 1 0
1
0
0
0
10
10
1
0
8
1
10
0
0
1
9
10
10
10
10
10
36140 = 90%
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
Table 3 Discriminant analysis of the author's
heuristic representution of 386 putterns
Discriminant
analysis
It
FI
Du
Fr
Group
totul
as if It
as if F1
asifDu
as if Fr
Total
2
80
4
0
86
0
1
92
6
99
0
2
3
99
104
86
86
109
105
3551386 = 92%
84
3
10
0
97
description does not exaggerate the variation across
subjects in classifying what are, after all, complex
perceptual stimuli.
Having demonstrated that the representations
were related to the categories and that they were
generated consistently by a significant number of
subjects, it remained to demonstrate consistency
across a larger sample of paintings. The author
judged a set of 528 photographs of craquelure in
terms of the eight dichotomous descriptors to create
heuristic numerical representations. Discriminant
analysis was then applied to a statistically significant number of examples of each of the technical
traditions. The results are presented in Table 3.
The author's representation of 386 examples produced effectively the same level of success as an
inexperienced subject's representation of 40 examples. This indicated that the method of representation was robust; it could be applied equally well by
different individuals and it was applicable to large
numbers of crack patterns with considerable variation across categories.
The 92% correct attribution of 386 examples
results from simultaneously comparing each example with four potential categories. When using attribution to an art historical category as a means of
assessing the validity of a representation of craquelure, it is more realistic to compare pairs of categories. In attribution, the craquelure pattern is
Table 4 Pair-wise discriminant analysis of the
author's heuristic representation of Italian and
Flemish crack patterns
Discriminant
analysis
It
FI
Group
total
as if It
as if F1
Total
84
3
87
2
80
82
86
83
1641169 = 97%
S. Bucklow
considered in concert with numerous other features
of the painting to confirm or refute an impression
derived from, for example, iconographic or stylistic
data. Those features will suggest one particular category and the crack pattern either will, or will not,
be consistent with the patterns typically associated
with that category.
We should therefore consider the level of success
in attributing to individual pairs of categories. This
can be done by reducing the 4 x 4 data in Table 3
into a number of 2 x 2 tables. Table 4 is one
example of the six possible tables (ItalianIFlemish,
ItalianIDutch,
ItalianIFrench,
Flemish/Dutch,
FlemisWFrench and DutcWFrench) which can be
derived from Table 3.
These 2 x 2 tables are summarized in Table 5,
which shows, for example, 97% success in distinguishing between Italian and Flemish panels, 95%
success in distinguishing between Italian panels and
Dutch 'paintings on canvas and 100% success in distinguishing between Italian panels and French
paintings on canvas.
The least accurate discrimination (95%) was
effected between the Italian and Dutch categories
and the Dutch and French categories. In the first
case, discrimination between Italian and Dutch
craquelure would be improved by reference to
global patterns. These could include 'keying out'
cracks on Dutch canvases (see Figure 10 in reference [I]). Global patterns were, however, beyond
the scope of this study. The distinction could also
be improved by reference to local patterns due to
the canvas weave. However, in order to ensure that
this study was statistically verifiable, such patterns
were not included. Also, the effects of impact on
paintings from different categories are significantly
different (see Figure 1). Such local patterns were
avoided in this survey but would obviously aid discrimination between the Italian and Dutch categories.
Improvement in discrimination between the
Dutch and French categories is more complex.
They are both on canvas, so the kinds of features
referred to above are of little help. Differences will
Figure I Impact cracks on ( a ) a French painting on canvas (Francois Boucher, 'Venus asks Vulcan for the
arms of Aeneas', Louvre, Paris) and ( b ) a Dutch painting on canvas (Abraham de Verwer, 'The Battle of
Zuderzee ,' Rijksmuseum, Amsterdam).
236
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
The description and classijication of craquelure
Table 5 Level of success in discriminating between
individual categories using data presented in Table 3
%
It
It
F1
DU
Fr
pair-wise
average
FI
Du
Fr
Averages
-
97
-
95
97
100
97
95
100
97.3%
97.7%
95.7%
98.0%
97
99
-
99
95
95
-
97.2%
exist which are not evident in a study based upon
only approximately 100 examples of each, but there
is an inherent relationship between categories which
must manifest itself in the craquelure pattern.
The general pattern of misattribution throws
light upon the variations within each category and
the ability of the eight descriptive terms to characterize each category. There are two types of possible error: false rejection or false acceptance of the
null hypothesis [4]. The erroneous classifications
reported in Table 3 are summarized in Table 6.
Table 6 shows that most incorrect inclusions
occurred in the Dutch category. This indicates that
Dutch craquelure is the category least well represented by the eight descriptors. It also shows that
the Italian category suffered the most incorrect
exclusions. This indicates that the greatest variation
within any category was found amongst Italian
examples of craquelure.
Craquelure from other categories of painting was
also examined to determine the applicability of the
method beyond the above four categories. For
example, 65 samples of crack patterns from early
German panel paintings made evident the difficulties associated with any attempt at making general
statements about a technically heterogeneous category of painting. In this case, what became apparent from analysis of the data was the influence of
other technical traditions upon individuals and
workshops across a broadly defined region.
That technical traditions do not arise and flourish in isolation is evident in, for example, the
craquelure of early German panels, and in the overlap between Dutch and French craquelure.
Consequently, in this context, attribution to a
Table 6 False inclusions and exclusions in the classijication of 386 examples presented in Table 3
It
Fl
Du
Fr
Error type
2
13
6
6
17
7
6
5
Type I: false inclusions
Type 11: false exclusions
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
Figure 2 Impact pattern in drying cracks in a nineteenth century British painting (unknown artist,
'Portrait of Bishop Moore ', Bishop's Palace, Ely).
broad school or period has limited validity. The
study reported here is presented in terms of such
attributions for the sole reason that they enable
large numbers of examples to be encompassed while
remaining concise. Although incomplete, the framework of attribution nonetheless demonstrates the
validity of a causal connection between the material
structure of a physical object and subtle variations
in the visible image.
The contribution of craquelure to the conservator
is not primarily as an indicator of the probable art
historic category of a painting but as a method of
non destructive testing. As such, craquelure can be
considered independent of any category [5]. For
example, Figure 2 displays some 'drying crack'
characteristics and some 'aging crack' characteristics, but neither of these generally accepted categories is, on its own, sufficient to characterize the
pattern. In this case, the overall configuration of
cracks is determined by brittle failure ('aging
cracks' in the ground layer as a result of an impact)
S. Bucklow
that has developed, and become visible, by ductile
failure ('drying cracks' in the immature paint layer).
,!
. /
/
/
Nondestructive testing
Craquelure is determined by the material properties
of the object under study. For example, in mediaeval altar frontals, craquelure can indicate whether
the seasoning of wood occurred before or after the
panel was painted [6], while in geological specimens, the pattern of surface cracks can indicate the
particular internal mode of degradation, alerting
the conservator to alterations within the fossil [7].
In this survey of paintings, misattributions (the
overlap between categories) can throw light upon
the mechanisms of crack formation. This in turn
may provide information about the materials or
methods employed in a painting.
Figures 3 and 4 are both from areas of sky in
paintings by Dutch artists. Figure 3 displays a typical seventeenth-century Dutch crack pattern
Johannes Augustus Knip, 'Jardin de
Figure 4
bagatelle, Paris ', Rijksmuseum, Amsterdam.
t
Figure 3 Frederik de Moucheron, 'Figures in an
Italian garden', National Gallery, London.
238
whereas the cracks in Figure 4 appear characteristically French.
One reason for the discrepancy between these
two paintings is suggested by their titles. Figure 3
shows the cracks associated with a painting which
uses Dutch materials and methods. The painting is
an Italianate fantasy which was almost certainly
executed in the Low Countries; indeed, it is not
even certain that Moucheron visited Italy [8].
Figure 4, on the other hand, appears from the
crack pattern to have been painted on a canvas
with a typically resilient French ground layer. This
suggests that Knip bought the pre-prepared canvas
whilst working in Paris.
Figures 5 and 6 are both from areas of sky in
paintings by Italian artists. Both of these examples
are anomalous in that subjects perceived them, and
discriminant analysis classified them, as Flemish
panels (see Figure 7).
One reason for the misattribution of these two
paintings is suggested by the identity of the artists.
Antonello da Messina is generally accredited,
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
The description and classification of craquelure
Figure 5 Antonello da Messina, 'Christ crucified',
National Gallery, London.
Figure 6 Giovanni Bellini, 'Agony in the garden',
National Gallery, London.
through his contact with Petrus Christus, with
bringing Northern painting techniques to Italy [9].
Giovanni Bellini was also influenced by Flemish
painting. In his case there is some controversy
about whether he arrived at his Northern influenced
technique independently [lo] or whether it was due
to his contact with da Messina [ll].
One could speculate whether a survey of craquelure across Bellini's oeuvre may help clarify the date
at which his method of painting became influenced
by Northern techniques. Such a survey would be
easier to undertake than any study based upon
destructive techniques such as medium analysis or
cross-sections. The interpretation of such a survey
would, however, be complicated by the observation
that, in many paintings, craquelure patterns vary
between paint passages.
As Figure 2 graphically demonstrates, the final
crack pattern is a consequence of the behaviours of
all members of the laminar structure of the painting. Figure 4 is the result of stresses within the
painting where the mechanical properties of the
ground layer predominate. In such a painting, there
is little variation in crack pattern between paint
passages. If, however, the paint and ground layers
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
Canonical Discriminant Functions
GROUP
D
-
,
Gw+Catdh
-5
MESSINA 6 BEWNI
a0194
FRENCH - Cl8
a0193
DUTCH-Cl7
'
0
G,W2
FLEMISH Cl5 / 14
2 4
-1
ITAWN-CI4Il5
n
4
4
.
2
0
2
1
-
4
FuMb1
Figure 7 Discriminant analysis of selected areas of
craquelure from the paintings by da Messina and
Bellini in relation to the four categories.
239
Figure 8 Master of Liesborn, 'Presentation in the
temple', National Gallery, London: crack pattern in
an area ofjlesh paint.
Figure 9 Master of Liesborn, 'Presentation in the
temple', National Gallery, London: crack pattern in
an area of green paint.
are of comparable strength, then some significant
variation between passages may occur. It was for
this reason that the areas of craquelure surveyed in
this study were predominantly rich in lead white.
Figures 8 and 9 are both from the same panel
from an altarpiece by the Master of Liesborn.
Figure 8 shows the crack pattern found on areas of
flesh and Figure 9 shows the pattern found in a
green garment.
One would not expect any significant variations
in the support and ground layer between adjacent
paint passages. The difference between the cracks in
the flesh and in the green garment must therefore
be due to differences in paint layer structure, pigments and/or media in those passages. The full
interpretation of the crack pattern is dependent
upon the results of other forms of analysis.
Apparent anomalies illuminate differences in
technique, either in the use of particular materials
or methods. The heuristic representation, although
simple and incomplete, is sufficiently comprehensive
to alert the observer to technical nuances in the
structure of the painting. A more complex and
complete representation of craquelure has, however,
been developed by Dr Peter Rayner and Dr
Andrew Varley [12].
240
Algorithmic representation
The above heuristic representations were generated
by humans and could be expressed in a form which
was immediately accessible to humans (the rules of
thumb). The involvement of a computer in the
study was merely as a means of validating the relationship between human response to craquelure and
technical traditions in an unambiguous and objective manner.
If the heuristic part of the research was a systematic study of the human response to craquelure,
then the algorithmic part of the research is a systematic study of the craquelure itself. For this, a
computer was required not only to classify, but also
to generate, the representation. The computational
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
The description and class@cation of craquelure
Table 7 Neural net~jorkclassification of the algoritlzmic representation of 200 patterns
Table 9 Level of success in discriminating between
individual categories using data presented in
Table 8
Neural
net
It
F1
Du
Fr
Group
totals
%
as if It
as if F1
as if D u
as if Fr
Totals
19
20
9
2
50
6
34
9
1
50
4
2
39
5
50
6
0
14
30
50
35
56
71
38
1221200=61~10/0
It
F1
83
DU
94
Fr
99
pair-wise average
It
FI
83
-
99
100
Du
Fr
Averages
94
99
-
99
100
87
87
-
92.01%)
94.11%)
93.4%)
95.4%
93 7%
methods of classification were the same as those
utilized in the heuristic part of the research.
The algorithmic representation of crack patterns
depended upon converting the digitized images of
craquelure from pixels into lines. Lines were
extracted from the images using Markov chains
[13]. The lines were then characterized as Bezier
curves [14]. An approximate model of the original
image was then recreated with Bezier curves of
known specification.
Crack patterns were represented by the statistical
parameters which summarized these collections of
Bezier curves. The parameters included data on the
length of curves. the junctions between curves. the
ends of curves, the tangents of curves within a specified range of angles to a reference direction, and
the averages and standard deviations of these measures.
Twenty such parameters were extracted from the
sets of Bezier curves and these 20 parameters
formed the basis for a representation of the crack
patterns. The classification of these algorithmic representations by a neural network is presented in
Table 7. For logistic reasons. the classification was
undertaken using 200 images derived from the same
sub-set of 40 images used by all subjects and presented in Table 1.
This compares with averages of 73 to 75% for the
unassisted sorting by experts, rule-based sorting by
novices and neural network classification of the
heuristic representation. Comparisons are offered in
the form of percentages, as opposed to more
sophisticated measures of similarity, due to the
minor differences between the tasks. The algorithmic representation would probably prove to be
more robust if tested against a wider survey.
Following the chain of reasoning outlined above,
the data were then subjected to discriminant analysis. Table 8 presents the resultant attributions.
Again. the 4 x 4 table was reduced to number of
2 x 2 tables, and the level of success in pair-wise
discrimination was calculated. These results are
summarized in Table 9.
The greatest level of success is found in the discrimination between French and all other categories
(95.4%). Next most successful is the discrimination
between Flemish and all other categories (94.1%).
In this respect, the pattern of classification of the
algorithmic and heuristic representations is the
same (98.0 and 97.7% respectively, see Table 5).
However, where the heuristic representation was
least successful for discrimination between the
Dutch and all other categories (95,7%), the algorithmic representation was least successful for discrimination between the Italian and all other
categories (92.0%).
It can be seen from Tables 6 and 7 that the
major misattributions are Italian panels classified as
Flemish: 40%) of all examples in the neural network
and 30% in discriminant analysis. Table 9 shows
that the Italian/Flemish pair-wise discrimination (at
83%) is significantly lower than pair-wise discrimination between any other combination of categories
Table 8 Discriminant analysis oj the algorithmic (87 to 100%)).
It was stated above that, for logistic reasons. the
repre~entationoj 200 patterns
algorithmic part of the research only examined the
initial sub-set of 40 photographs. These images
Discriwzinant
Group
were each sub-divided in five to create the 200
analysis
It
F1
Du Fr
totals
images studied. The classification was therefore
based upon areas of painting one fifth the area of
as if It
32
1
2
1 36
the
examples utilized in the heuristic classification
asifF1
15
49
1
0
65
exercise and averaging only 6cm2. The smaller areas
as if D u
3
0
45
10
58
and smaller sample of paintings both rendered the
as if Fr
0
0
2
39
41
Totals
50
50
50
50
165/200=82.5U/~ algorithmic classification task more vulnerable to
the 'anomalous' Bellini example, perhaps account-
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
241
Table 10 Comparison oj task durations
Task
---
Duration
-
Subject sorting 40 photographs (unassisted or rule-based sorting)
Subject classifying one photograph (unassisted or rule-based sorting)
Computer classifying 40 photographs (heuristic or algorithmic representation)
Subject heuristically representing one photograph (for classification by computer)
Computer algorithmically representing one photograph (for classification by computer)
ing for the relatively poor ItalianiFlemish discrimination.
The reason for limiting the number and size of
examples examined algorithmically was due to the
computationally intensive nature of the task.
Conversion of the digitized photographic image
into lines, segmentation of those lines into Bezier
curves and calculating statistical summaries of those
curves proved to be more time-consuming and computationally intensive than expected; this is apparent from Table 10.
As noted above. the sorting of 40 photographs is
an unrealistic task and the time taken reflects the
artificiality of the task; it represents the selection of
one solution from an enormous number (8 x 10")
of potential solutions. The assignment of a single
crack pattern to a single category by an expert is
almost instantaneous. The heuristic representation
of 528 crack patterns took the author about nine
hours. On the other hand, the algorithmic representation of 40 crack patterns took a computer several
months.
Conclusions
The heuristic and algorithmic representations, based
upon the Repertory Grid and Bezier curves respectively, produce comparable results upon classification.
The advantage of the heuristic method of representation is that it is accessible; it can be presented
informally as rules of thumb for each category. The
preceding analysis of the classification of these representations demonstrates that the eight descriptive
terms are, if not exhaustive, sufficient for a high
level of discrimination between patterns. The eight
descriptive terms are applicable to other categories
of easel paintings and discrimination between paintings from diverse traditions is possible. The same
eight descriptive terms also discriminate between
closely related traditions, and can highlight chronological and geographical trends within traditions.
Its disadvantages are that it is based upon the judgment (albeit consistent) of individuals and is a qualitative representation.
<45 minutes
c. 1 minute
< 10 seconds
< 1 minute
c. 3 days
The advantage of the algorithmic method of representation is that it is quantitative. Its quantitative
nature means that, in theory. it could be used in the
non-destructive in situ analysis of painting components such as assessment of the strength of ground
layers. Unfortunately. the present research reflects
the general finding that the characterization of
visual texture is computationally much more complex and less tractable than the characterization of
colour. However, the Bezier representation has the
potential to be fine-tuned: different parameters
could be extracted and computational routines
could be optimized. Its disadvantages are a dependence on expensive hardware and (at present) the
time taken to process images.
Another form of representation which may be
applicable to craquelure but which has not been
explored here is Fourier analysis. This method has
already been applied to the visual arts [15, 161. It
may offer an advantage of speed over the Bezier
representation, but the Fourier transform of a
craquelure pattern would not be as explicit and as
open to interrogation as Bezier curves.
The information available from craquelure is limited not only by our ability to represent it, but also
by the technical context of the painting. The discriminations alluded to above are all dependent
upon the predictable nature of the technical traditions involved. A particular painting may be typical
or atypical of a class, but we are only able to judge
the particular painting if the class itself is stable.
This is because the crack pattern is not only dependent upon the materials used in the construction of
a painting. but also upon the way in which they are
employed in the construction of the painting.
The heuristic and algorithmic methods of
representation outlined above are complementary:
qualitative and quantitative. quick and slow. etc.
The information offered by craquelure is also
complementary to that offered by other methods of
technical analysis. Methods such as medium analysis and the examination of cross-sections all focus
on a particular aspect of a particular component of
the painting and have the potential to generate
unequivocal data about that component. Analysis
of craquelure-by
whatever means-will
always
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
The description and classiJication of craquelure
produce a more equivocal result, this is a
consequence of the nature of craquelure as a
'holistic' phenomenon: it is the visible product of
the response of the whole painting to its whole
history.
Acknowledgements
The author would like to acknowledge the financial
support of the Queen Elizabeth Scholarship Trust.
Harold Wingate Foundation, Samuel H . Kress
Foundation and Girton College, Cambridge. He
would also like to thank the staff of the Fitzwilliam
Museum. Cambridge, National Gallery. Tate
Gallery. Courtauld Institute of Art and Wallace
Collection, London, Rijksmuseum, Amsterdam and
Louvre, Paris. The author expresses his gratitude to
all those individuals who acted as experimental subjects. especially Bronwyn Ormsby. He extends
thanks to Dr Peter Rayner and Dr Andrew Varley
for their contribution to the project and for permission to quote the results of their research here.
Equipment
A standard 35mm lens and SLR camera (supported
on a tripod under ambient light) were used in
the photographic survey. All statistical routines
were undertaken in SPSS for Windows, Version
6.1.3 (Professional and Advanced Statistics
Options). The neural network was emulated
within SPSS. The algorithmic representation
was undertaken on a SUN Sparc 20.
SPSS Inc., 444 N Michigan Avenue, Chicago, IL
6061 1, USA.
SUN Microsystems Inc., 2550 Garcia Avenue.
Mountain View, CA 94043, USA.
Information Science 43(2) (1992) 115-129.
4 LEACH,C.. Introduction to Statistics, Wiley,
New York (1979) 32.
5 VON BERALANFFY.
L., 'An essay on the relativity of categories'. Philosophy of Science 22
(1955) 243-263.
6 PLAHTER,U., Oldsaksamlingen, University of
Oslo, personal communication (1997).
7 COLLINS,C., Department of Earth Sciences,
University of Cambridge, personal communication (1998).
8 National Gallery General Illustrated Catalogue,
National Gallery Publications, London
(1973) 495.
9 WRIGHT.J.. 'Antonello da Messina, the origins
of his style and technique', Art History 3
(1980) 52.
10 GOFFEN,R., Giovanni Bellini, Yale University
Press, New Haven (1989) 201.
11 ROBERTSON,
G., Giovanni Bellini. Clarendon
Press. Oxford (1968) 58.
12 VARLEY.A.J., 'Statistical image analysis methods for line detection', unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Cambridge (1999).
13 GREEN,P., 'Reversible jump Markov chain
Monte Carlo computation and Bayesian
model determination', Biometrika 82 (1996)
71 1-732.
14 VARLEY,
A,, and RAYNER,
P., Bezier Modelling
of Cracks, ICIAP '97, Florence (1996).
15 DESSIPRIS, N.G.. and
SAUNDERS, D.,
'Analysing the paper texture in Van Dyck's
Antwerp sketchbook'. Computers in the
History of Art 5 (1995) 65-78.
16 HIGGINS,T., and LANG, J., 'Research into
watermarks at the British Museum'.
Conzputers in the History of Art 5 (1995)
79-86.
Author
References
1 BUCKLOW,S.L., 'The description of craquelure', Studies in Conservation 42 (1997)
129-140.
2 BUCKLOW,S.L., 'A stylometric analysis of
craquelure', Computers and the Hunzanities 31
(1998) 505-51 7.
3 LATTA.G.F., and SWIGGER,
K., 'Validation of
the Repertory Grid for modeling knowledge',
Journal of the American Society for
SPIKEBUCKLOW
holds a BSc in chemistry from the
University of Southampton (1978) MSc in artificial
intelligence from South Bank Polytechnic, London
(1988), a diploma in the conservation of easel
paintings from the Hamilton Kerr Institute,
Cambridge (1994) and PhD from the University of
Cambridge (1997). He is currently research scientist
and teacher of theory at the Hamilton Kerr
Institute. Address: Hamilton Kerr Institute,
Whittlesford, Cambridge CB2 4NE, UK.
RCsume-Cet article fournlt un contexte statistique de recherche pour dkfinir en termes dichotomiques simples
une description des craquelures. La validitk du systkme proposk, bask sur huit descripteurs, est ktablie par
rkfkrence aux resultats de diverses mkthodes de classement. Celles-ci comprennent Ie classenzent non supervisk
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244
243
des craquelures, dans des categories spCci$ques, par des sujets expPrimentPs, le mkme travail entrepris par des
sujets inexpPrimentPs assistis par des instructions, la classIJicatzon des representations des craquelures par
analogie avec un rCseau de neurones, et l'analyse discriminante. Deux types de representation sont compares:
l'une basee sur la technzque de la 'Repertory Grid: et l'autre sur les courbes de BBeziers.
Zusammenfassung-In
dem vorliegenden Papier nird versucht, auf dem Hintergrund statistischer
Forschungserkenntnisse mit ternzinologischen Fachbegriffen die Beschreibung von Krakelee zu ermoglichen.
Die Gultigkeit der vorgeschlagenen Zahl von acht Deskriptoren von Krakeleefornzen wird mit Bezug auf die
Ergebnisse verschiedener Klassi$zierungsaufgaben hergestellt. Dabei5ndet eine Zuordnung von Krakeleetypen
zu spezijischen Kategorien statt ebenso wie die Klassi$zierung von Krakeleeformen mittels eines neuronalen
Netznerkes und durch die Diskriminanzanalyse. Die rastergestiitzte Datendarstellung nird mit der auf dem
Einsatz der Bezierkurven basierenden Darstellung verglichen.
Resumen-Este articulo proporciona el context0 estadistico de investigacidn para definir terminos dicdtonzos
para la descripcidn de tipos de craquelados. La validez de un grupo propuesto de ocho slstemas de descripcidn
de craquelados se establece por referencia a 10s resultados de varios criterios de clasi$cacidn. Estos incluyen la
asignacidn de tipos de craquelado a categorias especi$cas por profesionales cualijicados y experirnentados, lo
mismo se realizd con individuos con poca experiencia ayudados por reglas tedricas de clasijicacidn, la
caracterizacidn de craquelados por 'neural network' (red neural), y por analisis discriminativo. Se comparan
dos tipos de representacidn: uno basado en la ticnica del Repertory Grid y la otra basada en las curvas de
Bezier.
Studies in Conservation 44 (1999) 233-244