SELF AND SYSTEMS
Relational Trauma and the Developing
Right Brain
An Interface of Psychoanalytic Self Psychology
and Neuroscience
Allan N. Schore
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California,
Los Angeles, David Geffen School of Medicine, Los Angeles, California, USA
Psychoanalysis, the science of unconscious processes, has recently undergone a significant transformation. Self psychology, derived from the work of Heinz Kohut, represents perhaps the most important revision of Freud’s theory as it has shifted its basic
core concepts from an intrapsychic to a relational unconscious and from a cognitive
ego to an emotion-processing self. As a result of a common interest in the essential,
rapid, bodily based, affective processes that lie beneath conscious awareness, a productive dialogue is now occurring between psychoanalysis and neuroscience. Here I
apply this interdisciplinary perspective to a deeper understanding of the nonconscious
brain/mind/body mechanisms that lie at the core of self psychology. I offer a neuropsychoanalytic conception of the development and structuralization of the self, focusing on
the experience-dependent maturation of the emotion-processing right brain in infancy.
I then articulate an interdisciplinary model of attachment trauma and pathological
dissociation, an early forming defense against overwhelming affect that is a cardinal
feature of self-psychopathologies. I end with some thoughts on the mechanism of the
psychotherapeutic change process and suggest that self psychology is, in essence, a psychology of the unique functions of the right brain and that a rapprochement between
psychoanalysis and neuroscience is now at hand.
Key words: neuropsychoanalysis; right brain; trauma; dissociation; unconscious;
attachment
Introduction
At the present time a number of scientific
and clinical disciplines are simultaneously experiencing a rapid expansion of relevant data
and even a reorganization of their underlying
theoretical concepts. Indeed, the term paradigm
shift is appearing in a number of literatures. Although current significant advances in various
technologies and the computer sciences have
catalyzed this growth spurt, an important contributor has been the rapid communication of
Address for correspondence: Allan N. Schore, 9817 Sylvia Avenue,
Northridge, CA 91324. Voice: 818-886-4368; fax: 818-349-4404.
[email protected]
information not only within but also between
disciplines. In this period of accelerated growth
of essential information about the human condition and the natural world, the transfer of
knowledge across disciplinary boundaries is occurring at a faster rate. This trend is reflected in
an increasing interest in interdisciplinary studies and in integrated models that synthesize
data generated at the interface of different scientific and clinical fields.
Within this context there exists a potential
for new and fresh solutions to certain fundamental problems, especially those concerning the essential mechanisms that lie at the
core of adaptive and maladaptive human functions. Until very recently these problems have
been studied from the unique vantage points of
Self and Systems: Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1159: 189–203 (2009).
C 2009 New York Academy of Sciences.
doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04474.x
189
190
various scientific perspectives that span the sociological, psychological, biological, and chemical domains. The overemphasis on specialization within each of these disciplines has
also fostered their isolation from one another,
which has in turn inadvertently increased an
artificial dichotomous separation between, for
example, psychology and biology, brain and
mind, mind and body, cognition and emotion.
Earlier impermeable boundaries of knowledge
between disciplines also intensified a tension
and indeed a conflict between those studying
unconscious involuntary processes and those
studying conscious voluntary processes, that is
between psychoanalysis—the science of unconscious process—and psychology—the study of
behavior.
This ambivalent relationship between psychoanalysis and the other sciences has existed
since its creation by Sigmund Freud. And yet it
is often forgotten that Freud’s early career was
in neurology and that in 1895 he wrote Project
for a Scientific Psychology, an attempt to create “a
psychology which shall be a natural science”
(Schore, 1997a). In this remarkable document
Freud used what was then known about neurophysiology and biology to begin to construct
a set of regulatory principles for psychological processes and a neuropsychological model
of brain function. Freud did not publish the
Project in his lifetime and over the course of his
career never returned to the problem of creating a model that could integrate the biological
and psychological realms. And yet he predicted
that at some point in the future “we shall have
to find a point of contact with biology” (Freud,
1913). Freud thus saw neurobiology as a discipline that could bridge the gap between biology and psychoanalysis, especially in the study
of the unconscious and its fundamental impact
on all aspects of the human experience.
Over the course of the last century, a number
of significant transformations have occurred in
Freud’s theory, although much of this work has
not transferred outside of the field. The theoretical core of psychoanalysis, almost unchanged
for most of its first century, is now undergo-
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
ing a substantial reformulation from an intrapsychic unconscious to a relational unconscious whereby the unconscious mind of one
communicates with the unconscious mind of
another. The scaffolding of clinical psychoanalysis is supported by conceptions of psychic development and structure, and it is these basic
concepts that are now being reformulated. Self
psychology, emergent from the seminal work
of Heinz Kohut, represents perhaps the most
significant updating of classical psychoanalysis
since it inception. In 1971, Kohut, trained in
neurology and then psychoanalysis, published
his classic volume The Analysis of the Self, a detailed exposition of the central role of the self in
human existence. He subsequently expanded
the theoretical framework of self psychology
in a second volume, The Restoration of the Self
(1977), and finally in How Does Analysis Cure?
(1984).
In all his clinical work and writings Kohut attempted to explore the four basic problems of
psychoanalysis that he initially addressed in his
seminal volume: how do early relational affective transactions with the social environment
facilitate the emergence of self (development of
the self ); how are these experiences internalized
into maturing self-regulating structures (structuralization of the self ); how do early deficits of
self-structure lead to later self-pathologies (psychopathogenesis); and how can a therapeutic relationship lead to a restoration of self (mechanism
of psychotherapeutic change).
Despite the fact that he was originally trained
as a neurologist, Kohut was highly ambivalent
about the incorporation of scientific data into
the core of psychoanalytic self psychology. Indeed, like Freud before him, he eschewed his
earlier neurological knowledge and attempted
to create a purely psychological model of the
unconscious systems that underlie all human
functioning. However, in the last 10 years, over
the course and since the “decade of the brain”
an interdisciplinary perspective has emerged
both within psychoanalysis and the disciplines
that border it. Because of a common interest
in the essential, rapid, bodily based, affective
Schore: Self Psychology and Neuroscience
processes that lie beneath conscious awareness,
a productive dialogue is now occurring between psychoanalysis and neuroscience. This
convergence has facilitated the emergence of
a new discipline, neuropsychoanalysis, and a
subspecialization, developmental psychoanalysis (Schore, 1997a). This discipline returns to
Freud’s attempt to create “a psychology which
shall be a natural science” by specifically focusing on the essential psychobiological role of the
unconscious in all human affect, cognition, and
behavior.
In a number of works I have suggested
that the time is right for a rapprochement between psychoanalysis and the biological sciences (Schore, 1994, 1997, 2002a,b, 2003a,b,
2005a). In this period when neuroscience is
“rediscovering the unconscious,” neuropsychoanalysis is identifying the “intrapsychic” brain
systems involved in a redefined dynamic unconscious and developmental psychoanalysis
is generating a complex model of the social–
emotional origins of the self and the early ontogeny of the biological substrate of the human
unconscious. It is now clear that Freud was correct in positing the unconscious mind develops
before the conscious and that the early development of the unconscious is equivalent to the
genesis of a self-system that operates beneath
conscious verbal levels for the rest of the life
span. I believe a deeper understanding of early
human development can never be attained by
narrowly focusing infant studies on the precursors of language, conscious thought, and voluntary behavior.
A complete model of human development
(and psychoanalysis) can only be psychobiological, not merely psychological. A deeper understanding of one of the fundamental questions of
science, why early developmental processes are
essential to the short- and long-term survival
of the organism, will not come from single or
even multiple discoveries within any one discipline (Schore, 1994). Rather, an integration
of related fields is essential to the creation of
a heuristic model of both developmental structures and functions that can accommodate and
191
interpret the data of various biological and
psychological disciplines and can freely shift
back and forth between their different levels of
analysis.
In this chapter on the integration of self psychology and neuroscience, I outline my neuropsychoanalytic work on the interpersonal
neurobiological origins of the self. I first present
a brief overview of Kohut’s concepts that represent the core of self psychology. Subsequently I
integrate interdisciplinary data in order to construct a neuropsychoanalytic conception of the
development and structuralization of the self,
focusing on the experience-dependent maturation of the early developing right brain. Then,
in a major focus of this work, I apply this developmental neuropsychoanalytic perspective to
the psychopathogenesis of severe deficits in the
self-system. Citing my work in this area, I articulate a model of the self psychology and neurobiology of early relational trauma and the
etiology of pathological dissociation, an early
forming defense that is a cardinal feature of a
number of early forming psychopathologies. I
end with some thoughts on psychotherapeutic
change and argue that the time is right for a rapprochement between psychoanalysis and neuroscience. Throughout I suggest that the “point
of contact with biology” that Freud referred
to is specifically the central role of right brain
psychobiological processes in the unconscious
regulation of affect, motivation, and cognition,
areas of intense interest to both contemporary
self psychology and neuroscience.
Self-Psychological Developmental
Models: Psychobiology
of Attachment
Perhaps Kohut’s most original and outstanding intellectual contribution was his developmental construct of selfobject. Indeed, self
psychology is built upon a fundamental developmental principle—that parents with mature
psychological organizations serve as selfobjects
that perform critical regulatory functions for
192
the infant who possesses an immature, incomplete, psychological organization. The child is
thus provided, at nonverbal levels beneath conscious awareness, with selfobject experiences
that directly effect the vitalization and structural cohesion of the self. The selfobject construct contains two important theoretical components. First, the concept of the mother–infant
pair as a self—selfobject unit emphasizes that
early development is essentially an interdependence between self and objects in a system.
This core concept was a major intellectual impetus for the expansion of the intersubjective
perspective in psychoanalysis. Indeed, Kohut’s
emphasis on the dyadic aspects of unconscious
communications shifted psychoanalysis from a
solely intrapsychic to a more balanced relational perspective. This challenged psychoanalysis to integrate the realms of a one-person psychology and a two-person psychology.
The second component of the selfobject construct is the concept of regulation. In his developmental speculations, Kohut (1971) stated
that the infant’s dyadic reciprocal regulatory
transactions with selfobjects allows for the
maintenance of his internal homeostatic equilibrium. These regulating self–selfobject experiences provide the particular intersubjective
affective experiences that evoke the emergence
and maintenance of the self (Kohut, 1984).
Siegel (1996) observes, “Kohut makes major
contributions to the understanding of emotional life, and his conceptualizations have farreaching implications for the understanding
and treatment of emotional states.” Kohut’s
idea that regulatory systems are fundamentally
involved with affect is supported in current
interdisciplinary studies that are highlighting
not just the centrality of affect but also affect
regulation.
Despite his intense interest in the early ontogeny of the self, over the course of his career
Kohut never spelled out the precise developmental details of his model nor did he attend
to the significant advances in developmental
psychology and psychoanalysis that were occurring simultaneously to his own theorizing.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
There is now agreement that current psychoanalysis is “anchored in its scientific base in
developmental psychology and in the biology
of attachment and affects” (Cooper, 1987). At
this point in time, self psychology is incorporating a broad range of current developmental
research into its theoretical model. In my own
contributions to this effort I have integrated recent advances in attachment theory into the
field (Schore, 2002a, 2003a, 2005b).
Overviewing and integrating this data, it is
now established that the essential task of the
first year of human life is the creation of a secure attachment bond of emotional communication between the infant and primary caregiver. Research now suggests “learning how
to communicate represents perhaps the most
important developmental process to take place
during infancy” (Papousek & Papousek, 1995).
Through visual-facial, auditory-prosodic, and
tactile-gestural communications, caregiver and
infant learn the rhythmic structure of the other
and modify their behavior to fit that structure, thereby co-creating a specifically fitted
interaction.
Kohut described critical episodes of “empathic mirroring” in which “The most significant relevant basic interactions between
mother and child usually lie in the visual area:
The child’s bodily display is responded to by
the gleam in the mother’s eye” (Kohut, 1971).
During bodily based affective communications
embedded in mutual gaze transactions, the psychobiologically attuned mother synchronizes
the spatiotemporal patterning of her exogenous sensory stimulation with the spontaneous
overt manifestations of the infant’s organismic rhythms. Via this contingent responsivity, the mother appraises the nonverbal expressions of her infant’s internal arousal and
affective states, regulates them, and communicates them back to the infant. To accomplish
this, the primary caregiver must successfully
modulate nonoptimal high or nonoptimal low
levels of stimulation that would induce supraheightened or extremely low levels of arousal
in the child. Secure attachment depends upon
193
Schore: Self Psychology and Neuroscience
the mother’s sensitive psychobiological attunement to the infant’s internal states of
arousal.
Importantly, research now clearly demonstrates that the primary caregiver is not always attuned and optimally mirroring, that
there are frequent moments of misattunement
in the dyad, ruptures of the attachment bond.
The disruption of attachment bonds leads to
a regulatory failure and an impaired autonomic homeostasis. Studies of “interactive repair” following dyadic misattunement (Tronick, 1989) support Kohut’s (1977) assertion
that the parental selfobject acts to “remedy
the child’s homeostatic imbalance.” In this pattern of “disruption and repair” (Beebe & Lachmann, 1994), the “good enough” caregiver who
induces a stress response through misattunement in a timely fashion reinvokes a reattunment, a regulation of the infant’s negatively
charged arousal.
In current psychobiological models, attachment is defined as the interactive regulation
of states of biological synchronicity between
and within organisms (Schore, 2000, 2003a,
2005b). The dual regulatory processes of affect synchrony that creates states of positive
arousal and interactive repair that modulates
states of negative arousal are the fundamental building blocks of attachment and its associated emotions. These interactive regulatory
mechanisms optimize the communication of
emotional states within an intimate dyad and
represent the psychobiological underpinning
of empathy, a phenomenon of intense interest to self psychology. Kohut (1977) deduced
that as a result of the empathic merger of the
child’s rudimentary psyche with the maternal
selfobject’s highly developed psychic organization, the child experiences the feeling states of
the selfobject as if they were his own. Selfobjects are thus external psychobiological regulators that facilitate the regulation of affective
experiences, and they act at nonverbal levels beneath conscious awareness in the regulation of self-esteem and the maintenance of
self-cohesiveness (Schore, 1994, 2002b).
Self-Psychological Models
of Structuralization: Links to
Interpersonal Neurobiology
A cardinal principle of self psychology dictates that, as a result of optimal self–selfobject
relational experiences, the infant becomes able
to perform the drive-regulating, adaptive, and
integrating functions that had previously been
performed by the external object. Kohut specifically posited that phase-appropriate, maternal,
optimal frustrations of the infant elicit “transmuting internalization,” the developmental
process by which selfobject function is internalized by the infant and psychological regulatory
structures are formed. Developmental data are
consonant with this, although interdisciplinary
data emphasize that not just optimal stressful
frustration but interactive repair is essential to
the formation of a structural system that can
regulate stressful affect. The formative experiences of the self are built out of internalized selfobject functions that facilitate the emergence of
more complex regulatory structures.
Recent research also support Kohut’s speculation that the infant’s regulatory transactions
with the maternal selfobject allow for maintenance of his homeostatic equilibrium. According to Ovtscharoff and Braun (2001), “The
dyadic interaction between the newborn and
the mother. . .serves as a regulator of the developing individual’s internal homeostasis. The
regulatory function of the newborn-mother interaction may be an essential promoter to ensure the normal development and maintenance
of synaptic connections during the establishment of functional brain circuits.” These researchers conclude that subtle emotion regulating attachment interactions permanently alter
the brain by establishing and maintaining developing limbic circuits (Ziabreva et al., 2003).
A large body of studies now clarifies the
developmental neurobiology of the selfobject
mechanism. In my own work I have suggested that the self-organization of the developing brain occurs in the context of a relationship with another self, another brain. More
194
specifically, the self–selfobject relationship is
embedded in infant–caregiver, right hemisphere to right hemisphere, affective, attachment communications (Schore, 1994, 2000,
2003a, 2005a). In light of the observations
that the emotion-processing human limbic system myelinates in the first year-and-a-half
(Kinney et al., 1988) and that the earlymaturing right hemisphere (Chiron et al., 1997;
Bogolepova & Malofeeva, 2001; Allman et al.,
2005; Gupta et al., 2005; Sun et al., 2005)—
which is deeply connected into the limbic
system—is undergoing a growth spurt at this
time, attachment experiences specifically impact limbic and cortical areas of the developing right cerebral hemisphere (Henry, 1993;
Schore, 1994; Siegel, 1999; Cozolino, 2002).
In very recent work on mother–infant emotional communication Lenzi et al. (in press)
offer data from a functional magnetic resonance imaging study “supporting the theory
that the right hemisphere is more involved
than the left hemisphere in emotional processing and thus, mothering.” Also confirming this model Minagawa-Kawai et al. (2009)
report a near-infrared spectroscopy study of
infant–mother attachment at 12 months and
conclude, “our results are in agreement with
that of Schore (2000) who addressed the importance of the right hemisphere in the attachment system.” Supporting Kohut’s speculations on empathic mirroring, neuroscience
researchers now conclude that developing children rely upon a “right hemisphere-mirroring
mechanism—interfacing with the limbic system that processes the meaning of observed or
imitated emotion” (Dapretto et al., 2006).
Ongoing neurobiological research on the
mother–infant intersubjective dialogue indicates, “A number of functions located within
the right hemisphere work together to aid monitoring of a baby. As well as emotion and face
processing the right hemisphere is also specialized in auditory perception, the perception
of intonation, attention, and tactile information” (Bourne & Todd, 2004). Social experiences thus facilitate the experience-dependent
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
critical period maturation of right brain systems
that process visual-facial, auditory-prosodic,
and tactile-gestural affective communications.
From infancy through all later stages of the life
span, the right hemisphere is dominant for the
nonconscious reception, expression, and communication of emotion and the cognitive and
physiological components of emotional processing (Schore, 2003a,b). With respect to empathy, a core process of self psychology, it is now
thought that “self-awareness, empathy, identification with others, and more generally intersubjective processes, are largely dependent
upon. . .right hemisphere resources, which are
the first to develop” (Decety & Chaminade,
2003).
Furthermore, the “complex psychological
regulatory structures” described by self psychology can now be located in “the right hemispheric specialization in regulating stress—
and emotion-related processes” (Sullivan &
Dufresne, 2006). Indeed, the brain’s major selfregulatory systems are located in the orbital
prefrontal areas of the right hemisphere that
undergo an anatomical maturation in postnatal
periods of mammalian development (Bradshaw
& Schore, 2007). The experience-dependent
maturation of this affect regulatory system is
thus directly related to the origin of the self
(Schore, 1994). Earlier research documented
that the development of the self and selfawareness is reflected in the ability of 2-yearolds to recognize their own visual image in a
mirror (Amsterdam, 1972). Functional magnetic resonance neuroimaging studies show
that when subjects look at an image of their
own face, activation seen in occipito–temporo–
parietal junction and the right frontal operculum (Sugiura et al., 2005), and self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network
in the right hemisphere (Uddin et al., 2005).
Indeed, a substantial amount of research
indicates that the right hemisphere is specialized for generating self-awareness and
self-recognition, and for the processing of
“self-related material” (Miller et al., 2001;
Decety & Chaminade, 2003; Fossati et al., 2004;
Schore: Self Psychology and Neuroscience
Platek et al., 2004; Feinberg & Keenan, 2005;
Perrin et al., 2005). Neuroscientists now suggest
that the essential function of the right lateralized system is to “maintain a coherent, continuous, and unified sense of self” (Devinsky,
2000). Summarizing this knowledge MolnarSzakacs and colleagues (2005) assert, “Studies
have demonstrated a special contribution of the
right hemisphere (RH) in self-related cognition,
own-body perception, self-awareness, autobiographical memory and theory of mind. Many
studies of self-face recognition have also found
a RH advantage, suggesting a special role for
the RH in processing material related to the
self.” These data clearly indicate that self psychology is in essence a psychology of the unique
functions of the right brain.
Self-Psychological Models
of Psychopathogenesis: Negative
Impact of Attachment Trauma
on the Right Brain
At the core of Kohut’s model of psychopathogenesis is the central hypothesis that
the mother’s traumatic failures of empathic
mirroring lead to enduring defects in the infant’s emerging self. Self psychology thus proposes that disturbed physiological regulation
results from primary disturbances in selfobject experiences and that a defective self and
an impaired regulatory structure lie at the
foundation of early forming psychopathologies.
Kohut (1971) highlighted the importance of
“the role of specific environmental factors (the
personality of the parents, for example; certain traumatic external events) in the genesis
of the developmental arrest,” especially when
“the mother’s responses are grossly unempathic
and unreliable. . .no transmuting internalization can take place, and the psyche. . .does not
develop the various internal functions which
re-establish narcissistic equilibrium.”
Although there is a long history of controversy within psychoanalysis, the field is now
very interested in the problem of trauma and
195
in the unique survival defenses for dealing with
early relational trauma. Laub and Auerhahn
(1993) propose that the essential experience
of trauma is a disruption of the link between
the “self” and the mothering “empathic other,”
and therefore the maternal introject, or mothering (selfobject regulatory) function, is deficient or “damaged.” They further contend “it
is the nature of trauma to elude our knowledge
because of both defence and deficit. . .trauma
overwhelms and defeats our capacity to organize it.” In line with these self-psychological
principles, current neuropsychoanalytic models now posit that, under the impact of developmental trauma, specific defensive and defective regulatory structures develop that lie at the
core of the patient’s psychopathology (Schore,
2002b).
Psychoanalysis, psychiatry, and developmental traumatology are all now converging on
dissociation, the bottom-line survival defense
against overwhelming, unbearable, emotional
experiences. Longitudinal attachment research
demonstrates an association between traumatic
childhood events and proneness to dissociation, described as “detachment from an unbearable situation,” “the escape when there is
no escape,” and “a last resort defensive strategy” (Schore, 2003b, in press). Although Kohut
never used the term dissociation, in his last book
(1984) he characterized an early interaction in
which the traumatized child “walls himself off”
from traumatizing experiences:
If the mother’s empathic ability has remained infantile, that is, if she tends to respond with panic
to the baby’s anxiety, then a deleterious chain will
be set into motion. She may chronically wall herself off from the baby, thus depriving him of the
beneficial effect of merging with her as she returns
from experiencing mild anxiety to calmness. Alternatively, she may continue to respond with panic, in
which case two negative consequences may ensue:
the mother may lay the groundwork in the child for
a lifelong propensity toward the uncurbed spreading of anxiety or other emotions, or by forcing the
child to wall himself off from such an overly intense
and thus traumatizing [experience, she] may foster
in the child an impoverished psychic organization,
the psychic organization of a person who will later
196
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
be unable to be empathic himself, to experience
human experiences, in essence, to be fully human.
What can ongoing studies in developmental psychology, affective neuroscience, and neuropsychoanalysis tell us about the neurobiology
and neuropsychology of attachment-relational
trauma and about dissociation, the mechanism
by which humans “wall themselves off” from
overwhelming emotional trauma? In this last
section I discuss interdisciplinary studies, which
indicate that experiences with a traumatizing
caregiver negatively impact the child’s attachment security, right brain maturation, and sense
of self and thereby lay the groundwork for
the use of pathological dissociation in various
self-pathologies.
Developmental Psychobiology
of Relational Trauma
During the brain growth spurt, relational, trauma-induced, arousal dysregulation
precludes the aforementioned visual-facial,
auditory-prosodic, and tactile-gestural attachment communications and alters the development of essential right brain functions. In contrast to an optimal attachment scenario, in a
growth-inhibiting relational environment the
primary caregiver induces traumatic states of
enduring negative affective arousal in the child.
This caregiver is inaccessible and reacts to her
infant’s expressions of emotions and stress inappropriately and/or rejectingly and therefore
shows minimal or unpredictable participation
in the various types of arousal-regulating processes. Instead of modulating, she induces extreme levels of stimulation and arousal, very
high in abuse and/or very low in neglect. And
because she provides no interactive repair, the
infant’s intense negative-affective states last for
long periods of time.
Studies in developmental traumatology reveal that the infant’s psychobiological reaction
to trauma comprises two separate response patterns: hyperarousal and dissociation (Schore,
2001, 2002c). In the initial hyperarousal stage,
the maternal haven of safety suddenly becomes
a source of threat, triggering a startle reaction
in the infant’s right hemisphere, the locus of
both the attachment and the fear motivational
systems. The maternal stressor activates the
hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) stress
axis, eliciting a sudden increase of the energyexpending sympathetic component of the infant’s autonomic nervous system (ANS); this results in significantly elevated heart rate, blood
pressure, and respiration, the somatic expressions of a dysregulated psychobiological state
of fear–terror. This active state of sympathetic
hyperarousal is expressed in increased secretion of corticotropin releasing factor (CRF)—
the brain’s major stress hormone. CRF regulates sympathetic catecholamine activity, creating a hypermetabolic state in the developing
brain.
But a second later forming reaction to relational trauma is dissociation in which the
child disengages from stimuli in the external world—traumatized infants are observed
to be “staring off into space with a glazed
look.” This parasympathetic dominant state
of conservation withdrawal occurs in helpless
and hopeless stressful situations in which the
individual becomes inhibited and strives to
avoid attention in order to become “unseen.”
The dissociative metabolic shutdown state is
a primary regulatory process by which the
stressed individual passively disengages in order to conserve energies, fosters survival by
the risky posture of feigning death, and allows restitution of depleted resources by immobility. In this hypometabolic state, heart
rate, blood pressure, and respiration are decreased while pain-numbing and pain-blunting
endogenous opiates are elevated. This energyconserving parasympathetic (vagal) mechanism mediates the “profound detachment” of
dissociation.
In fact there are two parasympathetic vagal systems in the brainstem medulla (Porges,
1997). The ventral vagal complex rapidly
regulates cardiac output to foster fluid engagement and disengagement with the social
environment, aspects of a secure attachment
Schore: Self Psychology and Neuroscience
bond of emotional communication. On the
other hand, activity of the dorsal vagal complex is associated with intense emotional states
and immobilization and is responsible for the
severe metabolic depression, hypoarousal, and
pain blunting of dissociation. The traumatized
infant’s sudden state switch from sympathetic
hyperarousal into parasympathetic dissociation
is described by Porges (1997) as “the sudden and rapid transition from an unsuccessful
strategy of struggling requiring massive sympathetic activation to the metabolically conservative immobilized state mimicking death associated with the dorsal vagal complex.” Whereas
the ventral vagal complex exhibits rapid and
transitory activations, the dorsal vagal nucleus
exhibits an involuntary and prolonged pattern of vagal outflow, creating lengthy “void”
states associated with pathological dissociative
detachment.
How are the dual traumatic contexts of
hyperarousal and dissociative hypoarousal expressed behaviorally within the mother–infant
dyad? Observational research demonstrates a
link between frightening maternal behavior,
dissociation, and disorganized infant attachment (Schuengel, Bakersmans-Kranenburg, &
Van IJzendoorn, 1999). Hesse and Main (1999)
observe the mother’s frightening behavior: “in
non-play contexts, stiff-legged ‘stalking’ of infant on all fours in a hunting posture; exposure of canine tooth accompanied by hissing;
deep growls directed at infant.” In recent work,
Hesse and Main (2006) document that a fear
alarm is triggered in the infant when the mother
enters a dissociative freeze state: “Here the
parent appears to have become completely unresponsive to, or even aware of, the external
surround, including the physical and verbal
behavior of their infant. . .[W]e observed one
mother who remained seated in an immobilized and uncomfortable position with her hand
in the air, blankly staring into space for 50 sec.”
Note the intergenerational transmission of not
only relational trauma but the bottom-line defense against traumatic emotional experiences,
dissociation.
197
Right Brain Pathological Dissociation
and Self-Psychological Deficits
Workers in the field of developmental traumatology now assert that the overwhelming
stress of maltreatment in childhood is associated with adverse influences on not just behavior but also on brain development (de
Bellis et al., 1999). During the intergenerational transmission of attachment trauma, the
infant is matching the rhythmic structures of the
mother’s dysregulated arousal states. This synchronization is registered in the firing patterns
of the stress-sensitive corticolimbic regions of
the right brain, dominant for coping with negative affects (Davidson et al., 1990). Describing
the essential survival functions of this lateralized system, Schutz (2005) notes “The right
hemisphere operates a distributed network for
rapid responding to danger and other urgent
problems. It preferentially processes environmental challenge, stress and pain and manages
self-protective responses such as avoidance and
escape.” The right brain is fundamentally involved in an avoidant-defensive mechanism for
coping with emotional stress, including the passive survival strategy of dissociation.
Current neurobiological data can be used to
create models of the mechanism by which attachment trauma negatively impacts the right
brain. Adamec and colleagues (2003) report
experimental data that “implicate neuroplasticity in right hemispheric limbic circuitry in
mediating long-lasting changes in negative affect following brief but severe stress.” According to Gadea et al. (2005) mild to moderate negative affective experiences activate the
right hemisphere, but an intense experience
“might interfere with right hemisphere processing, with eventual damage if some critical point is reached.” This damage is specifically hyperarousal-induced apoptotic cell death
in the hypermetabolic right brain. Thus, via
a switch into a hypoarousal, a hypometabolic
state allows for cell survival at times of intense
excitotoxic stress (Schore, 1997b, 2001, 2002c,
2003b).
198
Recall that right cortical areas and their connections with right subcortical structures are
in a critical period of growth during early human development. The massive psychobiological stress associated with attachment trauma
sets the stage for the characterological use of
right brain pathological dissociation when encountering later stressors. Converging evidence
indicates that early abuse negatively impacts
limbic system maturation, producing enduring
neurobiological alterations that underlie affective instability, inefficient stress tolerance, memory impairment, and dissociative disturbances.
In this manner, traumatic stress in childhood
leads to self-modulation of painful affect by directing attention away from internal emotional
states (Lane et al., 1997). The right brain, dominant for attention (Raz, 2004) and pain processing (Symonds et al., 2006), thus generates
dissociation, a defense by which intense negative affects associated with emotional pain are
blocked from consciousness.
Congruent with developmental and clinical
models, Spitzer et al. (2004) report a transcranial magnetic stimulation study of adults and
conclude, “In dissociation-prone individuals,
a trauma that is perceived and processed by
the right hemisphere will lead to a ‘disruption in the usually integrated functions of consciousness.’” In functional magnetic resonance
imaging research, Lanius et al. (2005) show
predominantly right hemispheric activation in
psychiatric patients while they are dissociating
and conclude that dissociation, an escape from
the overwhelming emotions associated with the
traumatic memory, can be interpreted as representing a nonverbal response to the traumatic
memory.
These studies explore the evolution of a
developmentally impaired regulatory system
and provide evidence that prefrontal cortical and limbic areas of the right hemisphere
are centrally involved in the deficits in mind
and body that are associated with a pathological dissociative response (Schore, 2002c, in
press). This right hemisphere, more so than the
left, is densely reciprocally interconnected with
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
emotion-processing limbic regions as well as
with subcortical areas that generate both the
arousal and autonomic bodily based aspect of
emotions. Sympathetic nervous system activity
is manifest in tight engagement with the external environment and high level of energy mobilization, while the parasympathetic component
drives disengagement from the external environment and uses low levels of internal energy
(Recordati, 2003). These ANS components are
uncoupled in relational trauma.
In a recent psychoanalytic formulation that
echoes Kohut’s “uncurbed spreading of anxiety or other emotions,” Bromberg (2006) links
right brain trauma to autonomic hyperarousal,
“a chaotic and terrifying flooding of affect that
can threaten to overwhelm sanity and imperil
psychological survival.” Dissociation is then automatically and immediately triggered as the
fundamental defense to the arousal dysregulation of overwhelming affective states. And in
the psychiatric literature, Nijenhuis (2000) asserts that “somatoform dissociation” is an outcome of early onset traumatization expressed
as a lack of integration of sensorimotor experiences, reactions, and functions of the individual’s self-representation. Dissociatively detached individuals are not only detached from
the environment but also from the self—their
body, their actions, and their sense of identity (Allen, Console, & Lewis, 1999). Crucian
et al. (2000) describe “a dissociation between
the emotional evaluation of an event and the
physiological reaction to that event, with the
process being dependent on intact right hemisphere function.”
Pathological dissociation thus reflects the
chronic disintegration of a right brain system
and a resultant adaptive failure of its capacity to rapidly and nonconsciously detect, process, and cope with unbearable emotional information and overwhelming survival threat.
A poorly developed right cortical–subcortical
implicit self-system is inefficient at recognizing
and processing external stimuli (exteroceptive
information coming from the relational environment) and on a moment-to-moment basis
Schore: Self Psychology and Neuroscience
integrating them with internal stimuli (interoceptive information from the body). This too
frequent failure of integration of the higher
right hemisphere with the lower right brain
induces an instant collapse of both subjectivity and intersubjectivity, even at lower levels of
interpersonal stress.
In summary, the developing brain imprints
not only the overwhelming affective states that
are at the core of attachment trauma but
also the primitive defense used against these
affects—the regulatory strategy of dissociation.
It is now established that maternal care influences both the infant’s reactivity (Menard,
Champagne, & Meaney, 2004) and the transmission of individual differences in defensive
responses (Parent et al., 2005). A large body
of psychiatric, psychological, and neurological studies supports the link between childhood trauma and pathological dissociation
(e.g., Draijer & Langeland, 1999; Macfie,
Cicchetti, & Toth, 2001; Merckelbach & Muris,
2001; Dikel, Fennell, & Gilmore, 2003; Liotti,
2004).
Conclusion: Rapprochement
between Psychoanalysis
and Neuroscience
Researchers now conclude that, because of
dissociation, elements of a trauma are not integrated into a unitary whole or an integrated
sense of self (Van der Kolk et al., 1996). The
symptomatology of pathological dissociation,
or what Kohut described as “walling oneself off” from intense, traumatizing experience,
thus represents a structural impairment and deficiency of the right brain, the locus of a “corporeal image of self” (Devinsky, 2000), affective
empathy (Schore, 1994; Decety & Chaminade,
2003), and a “sense of humanness” (Mendez
& Lim, 2004). Recall Kohut’s speculation that
early trauma acts as a growth-inhibiting environment for the developing self, one which generates “an impoverished psychic organization,”
a deficit in being empathic, and an inability “to
199
be fully human.” The self-depleting structurealtering cost of characterological dissociation is
thus a central psychopathogenetic concept of
both self psychology and neuroscience.
A central tenet of Kohut’s model of psychopathogenesis is that the long-term effects
of chronic maternal failure to provide growthfacilitating selfobject regulatory functions is the
genesis of a “developmental arrest.” Recall
the self-psychological proposal that, because of
early trauma, the developing selfobject regulatory function is deficient or “damaged.” This
development impairment can now be identified
as a maturational failure of the right brain affect regulatory system. A large body of clinical
observations and psychiatric research strongly
suggests that the most significant consequence
of early relational trauma is the child’s failure
to develop the capacity to self-regulate the intensity and duration of emotional states. The
principle that maltreatment in childhood is associated with adverse influences on brain development specifically refers to an impairment
of a higher circuit of emotion regulation on the
right side of the brain.
At the beginning of this chapter I stated that
a central area of inquiry of Kohut’s psychoanalytic theory was the problem of how the
therapeutic relationship scaffolds the “restoration of self.” Early relational trauma and the
characterological use of the right brain strategy
of pathological dissociation are common elements of the histories of severe self-pathologies
of personality disorders, a clinical population of
increasing interest to self psychology and psychotherapists in general. A large multicenter
study of adult patients with a history of early
childhood trauma reports that psychotherapy
is an essential element of the treatment of such
cases and indeed is superior to pharmacotherapy as an effective intervention (Nemeroff et al.,
2003).
Any psychotherapeutic intervention with
these patients must treat not only traumatic
symptoms but also the dissociative defense
(Bromberg, 2006). Spitzer et al.’s (2007) research shows that higher levels of dissociation
200
predict poorer outcome in patients in psychodynamic psychotherapy. These authors conclude dissociative patients have an insecure
attachment pattern negatively affecting the
therapeutic relationship and that they dissociate as a response to negative emotions arising
in psychotherapy. Clinical authors now suggest
that the treatment of traumatic dissociation is
essential to effective psychotherapy with these
patients (Spiegel, 2006; Schore, 2007).
The self-psychological focus on selfobject
regulation clearly suggests that deficits and defenses of affect and affect regulation are a primary focus of the treatment of these early
forming psychopathologies. With respect to
the mechanism of change, Kohut (1984) postulated “psychoanalysis cures by the laying
down of psychological structure.” This structure is essentially in the right brain and its
limbic emotion-regulating circuits. Studies indicate that emotional self-regulatory processes
constitute the core of psychotherapeutic approaches (Beauregard, Levesque, & Bourgouin,
2001), that the development of self-regulation
is open to change in adult life, providing a basis for what is attempted in therapy (Posner &
Rothbart, 1998), and that psychotherapy affects
clinical recovery by modulating limbic and cortical regions (Goldapple et al., 2004).
In addition to a more complex understanding of the psychotherapy change process an
integration of neuroscience and self psychology has another important potential benefit.
Psychoanalysis, neuroscience, and child psychiatry all share the well-established psychopathogenetic principle that maltreatment in childhood is associated with adverse influences
on the infant’s brain/mind/body and thereby
alters the developmental trajectory of the self
over the ensuing life span. Interdisciplinary
research that incorporates psychoanalytic self
psychology with the developmental and biological sciences can deepen our understanding
of the underlying psychoneurobiological mechanisms by which early relational trauma mediates the unconscious intergenerational transmission of the deficits in affect regulation of
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
early forming self-psychopathologies. This information may, in turn, generate more effective models of early intervention during the
brain growth spurt and thereby contribute to
the prevention of a broad range of psychiatric
disorders.
Conflicts of Interest
The author declares no conflicts of interest.
References
Adamec, R. E., Blundell, J., & Burton, P. (2003). Phosphorylated cyclic AMP response element bonding protein expression induced in the periaqueductal gray
by predator stress; its relationship to the stress experience, behavior, and limbic neural plasticity. Progress
in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 27,
1243–1267.
Allen, J. G., Console, D. A., & Lewis, L. (1999). Dissociative detachment and memory impairment: reversible
amnesia or encoding failure? Comprehensive Psychiatry,
40, 160–171.
Allman, J. M., Watson, K. K., Tetreault, N. A., & Hakeem,
A. Y. (2005). Intuition and autism: a possible role for
Von Economo neurons. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9,
367–373.
Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before
age two. Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 297–305.
Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001).
Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, RC165.
Beebe, B., & Lachmann, F. M. (1994). Representations
and internalization in infancy: Three principles of
salience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 11, 127–165.
Bogolepova, I. N., & Malofeeva, L. I. (2001). Characteristics of the development of speech motor areas 44 and
45 in the left and right hemispheres of the human
brain in early post-natal ontogenesis. Neuroscience and
Behavioral Physiology, 31, 13–18.
Bourne, V. J., & Todd, B. K. (2004). When left means
right: an explanation of the left cradling bias in terms
of right hemisphere specializations. Developmental Science, 7, 19–24.
Bradshaw, G. A., & Schore, A. N. (2007). How elephants
are opening doors: developmental neuroethology, attachment and social context. Ethology, 113, 426–436.
Bromberg, P. M. (2006). Awakening the dreamer: Clinical journeys. Analytic Press, Mahwah, NJ.
Chiron. C., Jambaque, I., Nabbout, R., Lounes, R.,
Syrota, A., & Dulac, O. (1997). The right brain
Schore: Self Psychology and Neuroscience
hemisphere is dominant in human infants. Brain,
120, 1057–1065.
Cooper, A. M. (1987). Changes in psychoanalytic ideas:
Transference interpretation. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 35, 77–98.
Cozolino, L. (2002). The neuroscience of psychotherapy. Norton, New York.
Crucian, G. P., Hughes, J. D., Barrett, A. M., Williamson,
D. J. G., Bauer, R. M., Bowres, D., et al. (2000). Emotional and physiological responses to false feedback.
Cortex, 36, 623–647.
Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A.,
Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders.
Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–31.
Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C., Senulis, J., &
Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach/withdrawal and
cerebral asymmetry: 1. Emotional expression and
brain physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 330–341.
de Bellis, M. D., Baum, A. S., Birmaher, B., Keshavan,
M. S., Eccard, C. H., Boring, A. M., et al. (1999).
Developmental traumatology Part I: Biological stress
systems. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1259–1270.
Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). When the self represents the other: A new cognitive neuroscience
view on psychological identification. Consciousness and
Cognition, 12, 577–596.
Devinsky, O. (2000). Right cerebral hemisphere dominance for a sense of corporeal and emotional self.
Epilepsy & Behavior, 1, 60–73.
Dikel, T. N., Fennell, E. B., & Gilmore, R. L. (2003). Posttraumatic stress disorder, dissociation, and sexual
abuse history in epileptic and nonepileptic seizure
patients. Epilepsy & Behavior, 4, 644–650.
Draijer, N., & Langeland, W. (1999). Childhood trauma
and perceived parental dysfunction in the etiology
of dissociative symptoms in psychiatric inpatients.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 379–38.
Feinberg, T., & Keenan, J. P. (2005). Where in the brain
is the self? Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 661–678.
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S. J., Lepage, M., Graham, S. J.,
Grady, C., Keightley, M. L., et al. (2004). Distributed
self in episodic memory: neural correlates of successful retrieval of self-encoded positive and negative
personality traits. NeuroImage, 22, 1596–1604.
Freud, S. (1913). The claims of psycho-analysis to scientific
interest. Standard Edition, 13.
Gadea, M., Gomez, C., Gonzalez-Bono, R. E., &
Salvador, A. (2005). Increased cortisol and decreased
right ear advantage (REA) in dichotic listening following a negative mood induction. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 129–138.
Goldapple, K., Segal, Z., Garson, C., Lau, M., Bieling,
201
P., Kennedy, S., et al. (2004). Modulation of corticallimbic pathways in major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 34–41.
Gupta, R. K., Hasas, K. M., Trivedi, R., Pradhan, M.,
Daqs, V., Parikh, N. A., et al. (2005). Diffusion tensor
imaging of the developing human cerebrum. Journal
of Neuroscience Research, 81, 172–178.
Henry, J. P. (1993). Psychological and physiological responses to stress: The right hemisphere and the
hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis, an inquiry into
problems of human bonding. Integrative Physiological
& Behavioral Science, 28, 369–387.
Hesse, E., & Main, M. M. (1999). Second-generation
effects of unresolved trauma in nonmaltreating
parents: dissociated, frightened, and threatening
parental behavior. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 19, 481–540.
Hesse, E., & Main, M. M. (2006). Frightened, threatening,
and dissociative parental behavior in low-risk samples: Description, discussion, and interpretations. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 309–343.
Kinney, H. C., Brody, B. A., Kloman, A. S., & Gilles,
F. H. (1988). Sequence of central nervous system
myelination in human infancy. II. Patterns of myelination in autopsied infants. Journal of Neuropathology
and Experimental Neurology, 47, 217–234.
Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. International Universities Press, New York.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. International
Universities Press, New York.
Kohut, H. (1984). How does analysis cure? University of
Chicago Press, Chicago.
Lane, R. D., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Kaszniak,
A. W. (1997). Is alexithymia the emotional equivalent
of blindsight? Biological Psychiatry, 42, 834–844.
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Bluhm, R. L., Densmore,
M., Boksman, K., Neufeld, R. W. J., et al. (2005).
Functional connectivity of dissociative responses in
posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional magnetic
resonance imaging investigation. Biological Psychiatry,
57, 873–884.
Laub, D., & Auerhahn, N. (1993). Knowing and not
knowing massive psychic trauma: Forms of traumatic memory. International Journal of Psychoanalysis,
74, 287–302.
Lenzi, D., Trentini, C., Pantano, P., Macaluso, E.,
Iacaboni, M., Lenzi, G. I., et al. (in press). Neural
basis of maternal communication and emotional expression processing during infant preverbal stage.
Cerebral Cortex.
Liotti, G. (2004). Trauma, dissociation, and disorganized
attachment: Three strands of a single braid. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Training, 41, 472–486.
Macfie, J., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2001). Dissociation
in maltreated versus nonmaltreqated preschool-age
children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25, 1253–1267.
202
Menard, J. L., Champagne, D. L., & Meaney, M. J. P.
(2004). Variations in maternal care differentially influence ‘fear’ reactivity and regional patterns of cFos
immunoreactivity in response to the shock-probe
burying test. Neuroscience, 129, 297–308.
Mendez, M. F., & Lim, G. T.H. (2004). Alterations of the
sense of “humanness” in right hemisphere predominant frontotemporal dementia patients. Cognitive and
Behavioral Neurology, 17, 133–138.
Merckelbach, H., & Muris, P. (2001). The causal link between self-reported trauma and dissociation: a critical review. Behavior Research and Therapy, 39, 245–254.
Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Matsuoka, S., Dan, I., Naoi, N.,
Nakamura, K., & Kojima, S. (2009). Prefrontal activation associated with social attachment: facialemotion recognition in mothers and infants. Cerebral
Cortex, 19, 284–292.
Miller, B. L., Seeley, W. W., Mychack, P., Rosen, H. J.,
Mena, I., & Boone, K. (2001). Neuroanatomy of
the self. Evidence from patients with frontotemporal
dementia. Neurology, 57, 817–821.
Molnar-Szakacs, I., Uddin, L. Q., & Iacoboni, M.
(2005). Right-hemisphere motor facilitation by selfdescriptive personality-trait words. European Journal
of Neuroscience, 21, 2000–2006.
Nemeroff, C. B., Heim, C., Thase, M. E., Klein, D. N.,
Rush, A. J., Schatzberg, A. F., et al. (2003). Differential responses to psychotherapy versus pharmacology in patients with chronic forms of major depression and childhood trauma. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Science USA, 100, 14293–14296.
Nijenhuis, E. R. S. (2000). Somatoform dissociation: major symptoms of dissociative disorders. Journal of
Trauma & Dissociation, 1, 7–32.
Ovtscharoff, W. Jr., & Braun, K. (2001). Maternal separation and social isolation modulates the postnatal
development of synaptic composition in the infralimbic cortex of Octodon degus. Neuroscience, 104, 33–40.
Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (1995). Intuitive parenting.
In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. II.
Ecology and biology of parenting (pp. 117–136). Erlbaum,
Hillsdale, NJ.
Parent, C., Zhang, T-Y., Caldji, C., Bagot, R., Champagne, F. A., Pruessner, J., et al. (2005). Maternal care
and individual differences in defensive responses.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 229–233.
Perrin, F., Maquet, P., Peigneux, P., Ruby, P., Degueldre,
C., Balteau, E., et al. (2005). Neural mechanisms involved in the detection of our first name: a combined
ERPs and PET study. Neuropsychologia. 43,12–19.
Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P., Gallup, G. G., & Mohamed, F.
B. (2004). Where am I? The neurological correlates
of self and other. Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 114–122.
Porges, S. W. (1997). Emotion: an evolutionary by-product
of the neural regulation of the autonomic nervous
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
system. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 807,
62–77.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention,
self-regulation, and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 353, 1915–
1927.
Raz, A. (2004). Anatomy of attentional networks. Anatomical Record, 281B, 21–36.
Recordati, G. (2003). A thermodynamic model of the
sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems.
Autonomic Neuroscience: Basic and Clinical, 103, 1–
12.
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the
self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Erlbaum,
Mahwah, NJ.
Schore, A. N. (1997a). A century after Freud’s project – Is
a rapprochement between psychoanalysis and neurobiology at hand? Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
Association, 45, 1–34.
Schore, A. N. (1997b). Early organization of the nonlinear
right brain and development of a predisposition to
psychiatric disorders. Development and Psychopathology,
9, 595–631.
Schore, A. N. (2000). Attachment and the regulation of
the right brain. Attachment & Human Development, 2,
22–41.
Schore, A. N. (2001). The effects of relational trauma
on right brain development, affect regulation, and
infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22,
201–269.
Schore, A. N. (2002a). The right brain as the neurobiological substratum of Freud’s dynamic unconscious.
In D. Scharff (Ed.), The psychoanalytic century: Freud’s
legacy for the future (pp. 61–88). The Other Press, New
York.
Schore, A. N. (2002b). Advances in neuropsychoanalysis, attachment theory, and trauma research: Implications for self psychology. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 22,
433–484.
Schore, A. N. (2002c). Dysregulation of the right brain:
a fundamental mechanism of traumatic attachment
and the psychopathogenesis of posttraumatic stress
disorder. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
36, 9–30.
Schore, A. N. (2003a). Affect regulation and the repair of the
self . Norton, New York.
Schore, A. N. (2003b). Affect dysregulation and the disorders of
the self . Norton, New York.
Schore, A. N. (2005a). A neuropsychoanalytic viewpoint.
Commentary on paper by Steven H. Knoblauch.
Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 15, 829–853.
Schore, A. N. (2005b). Attachment, affect regulation, and
the developing right brain: linking developmental
neuroscience to pediatrics. Pediatrics in Review, 26,
204–212.
Schore: Self Psychology and Neuroscience
Schore, A. N. (2007). Review of Awakening the dreamer:
clinical journeys by Philip M. Bromberg. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 17, 753–767.
Schore, A. N. (In press). Attachment trauma and the developing right brain: Origins of pathological dissociation. In P. F. Dell & J. A. O’Neil (Eds.), Dissociation
and the dissociative disorders: DSM-V and beyond. Routledge, New York.
Schuengel, C., M. J. Bakersmans-Kranenburg., & Van
IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Frightening maternal behavior linking unresolved loss and disorganized infant attachment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 54–63.
Schutz, L. E. (2005). Broad-perspective perceptual disorder of the right hemisphere. Neuropsychology Review,
15, 11–27.
Siegel, A. M. (1996). Heinz Kohut and the psychology of the self.
Routledge, London and New York.
Siegel, D. J. 1999. Developing mind: Toward a neurobiology of
interpersonal experience. Guilford, New York.
Spiegel, D. (2006). Recognizing traumatic dissociation.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 566–568.
Spitzer, C., Barnow, S., Freyberger, H. J., & Grabe, H. J.
(2007). Dissociation predicts symptom-related treatment outcome in short-term inpatient psychotherapy. Australian and New Zealand Journey of Psychiatry,
41, 682–687.
Spitzer, C., Wilert, C., Grabe, H.-J., Rizos, T., &
Freyberger, H. J. (2004). Dissociation, hemispheric
asymmetry, and dysfunction of hemispheric interaction: a transcranial magnetic approach. Journal of
Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 16, 163–169.
203
Sugiura M., Watanabe, J., Maeda, Y., Matsue, Y., Fukuda,
H., & Kawashima, R. (2005). Cortical mechanisms
of visual self-recognition. NeuroImage, 24, 143–189.
Sullivan, R. M., & Dufresne, M. M. (2006). Mesocortical
dopamine and HPA axis regulation: Role of laterality and early environment. Brain Research, 1076, 49–
59.
Sun, T., Patoine, C., Abu-Khalil, A., Visader, J., Sum,
E., Cherry, T. J., et al. (2005). Early asymmetry of
gene transcription in embryonic human left and right
cerebral cortex. Science, 308, 1794–1798.
Symonds, L. L., Gordon, N. S., Bixby, J. C., & Mande,
M. M. (2006). Right-lateralized pain processing in
the human cortex: An fMRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3823–3830.
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.
Uddin, L. Q., Kaplan, J. T., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Zaidel,
E., & Iacoboni, M. (2005). Self-face recognition activates a fromtoparietal “mirror” network in the right
hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 25, 926–935.
Van Der Kolk, B. A., Pelcovitz, D., Roth, S., Mandel, F.
S., McFarlane, A., & Herman, J. L. (1996). Dissociation, somatization, and affect dysregulation: The
complexity of adaptation to trauma. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 153, 83–93.
Ziabreva, I., Poeggel, G., Schnabel, R., & Braun, K.
(2003). Separation-induced receptor changes in the
hippocampus and amygdala of Octodon degus: influence of maternal vocalizations. Journal of Neuroscience,
23, 5329–5336.