Musical experiences in the play of a 16-month-old child
Vanessa Stansall
Centre for Research in Early Childhood (MA Student)
Birmingham, UK
[email protected]
Abstract
This small study was undertaken as part of an MA in Early Childhood Music Education. A case
study of a 16-month-old child’s musical experiences through play in the home, it is informed
by theoretical perspectives on child development and learning, musical learning and
musical childhoods. The data were gathered through naturalistic observations of the child in
his home and recorded in the form of video snapshots in time. Information about the child’s
background was gained through a short conversation with his parents. Of the video taken
over five visits with the family, three short vignettes were chosen for analysis. These vignettes
were then transcribed as illustrative detailed descriptions. Vignette 1 examines the
affordances of sound makers and how these influence a child’s music making. Vignette 2
considers the aural and physical environment and the role recorded music can play in affect
regulation. Vignette 3 shows an episode with a musical toy and discusses the affordances of
such toys, their capacity for multimodal use, and their role in parent-child interaction. The
study identified implications for practice, some of the limitations of working in the home, and
areas of interest for future research.
Keywords
Musical childhoods, parents, play, affect regulation, technology, interaction,
Introduction
This small-scale, qualitative case study investigated the musical experiences
in the play of a 16-month-old child, George.
Research with young children has more usually been focused on laboratory
studies with younger infants or studies in nursery settings with children from
three years old and accordingly there is less research with children of
George’s age. This study aims to make a small contribution to the research
on music in daily life (see, for example, Young and Gillen, 2007 and Young,
2008).
George’s parents are Lola and Joe. George attends nursery Monday to
Wednesday and is at home with Joe on Thursdays and Lola on Fridays.
George’s mobility has developed rapidly over the last few months and the
family increasingly spend time in the park and the garden where George can
enjoy the space and permission to move around. They still attend certain
parent and child classes, including a music group that Lola takes George to
on Fridays. Musical toys as well as drums and a recorder are available to
George at home. The radio is often on at home and in the car, usually BBC
Radio 1 or BBC Radio 2.
262
This paper is based on an assignment carried out as part an MA in Early
Childhood Music Education and includes some selected perspectives from
the original paper.
Research design
The data were collected through naturalistic observations of the child in his
home over five visits in the spring of 2016 and recorded in video form. The
footage collected was edited and three short vignettes selected for analysis.
These vignettes were then transcribed as illustrative detailed descriptions.
The video was recorded on a mobile phone. George is used to adults in his
home using mobile phones and it was hoped this would minimise any
tendency to ‘perform’ for the camera, compared with a video camera. An
additional consideration was whether George’s parents might try to set up
specific musical situations in the home and it was made clear to them this
was neither necessary nor desirable for the study. George was too young to
sign a consent form and so the written consent of his parents was gained.
George’s body language and facial expressions were monitored throughout
however for indications of his ongoing assent or signs of distress. See Street
(2007) for an exploration of ethical issues and the rights of young children in
research in naturalistic settings.
All names have been changed to protect the family’s identity.
Vignette 1
George is sitting on the floor holding a spoon in his left hand. He puts it inside
the cup next to him and strikes the base eight times. He looks inside the cup
and strikes again six times. He picks up the cup and tries to drink from it. The
spoon falls out. He puts it back inside and strikes eight more times, this time
moving the spoon between two opposite sides of the inside of the cup. He
makes a scooping motion and puts the spoon in his mouth. He hits the floor
with the spoon 10 times singing ‘da da’ with the first two strikes, the two notes
at the same pitch. He lifts the cup to his mouth and sings an open vowel
sound. Holding the spoon between thumb and index finger he returns it to
the cup, singing ‘mmm’ through a rising interval and striking the back of the
spoon against the inner surface of the cup, this time with a wiping, slightly
swinging motion. He places his hands on the floor and sings another rising
vowel sound. He kicks his right leg three times to shuffle forward towards the
step and sings ‘eek eek eek’. He positions himself on the step singing ‘mmmm’, the second note slightly lower than the first, then repeats this while he
stands up and walks off towards the bin.
A Piagetian view of George’s play points to the sensorimotor, exploratory
nature of his striking gestures (Pound, 2012) and musical developmental
models such as the Spiral of Musical Development (Swanwick & Tillman, 1986)
have also pointed to the sensory, experimental nature of music-making in
children of this age. The sensorimotor moments in George’s play seem to
263
alternate with units of pretend play, solitary play (Parten, 1932 cited in
Tarnowski, 1999) or object play (Hughes & Melville, 2002). Marsh and Young
(2006) however argue that such categories of play do not recognise the
social aspects or real-life contexts of play and that far from being random
and exploratory, children’s music-making is complex and structured.
In a study with older children in a preschool, Young (2003) showed how
structures in space relating to instrumental affordances and structures in time
work generatively and organizationally in young children’s music-making.
One such structure of time is clustering (Young, 2003, p. 54), where identical
movements are repeated in a group followed by a pause, characteristically
four to ten repetitions; such a pattern provides a fitting description of
George’s rhythmic groupings. With regards to space and the shape of the
cup, George’s initial striking motif with the spoon reoccurs, now moving back
and forth in a straight line between the opposite faces of the inside of the
cup, and then another time, going back & forth by swinging around the
curve of the cup and wiping the inner surface. In this way the motif is
transformed, creating a sequence. Does the swinging action dictate the
pathway round the curve of the cup or does following that pathway give rise
to the swinging action? In this particular example they seem inextricably
linked.
Young (2012) also suggests that the physical environment gives structure to
children’s music and George incorporates other parts of his physical
environment into his music-making, striking the floor and later the washing
machine. For the adults of the house, only the soles of their feet are in
contact with the floor; George however spends much time lying and sitting
on the floor, it is much closer to him, and features more in his field of vision.
After the clip finished, George extended his striking motif further, using the
spoon in the larger circular space of the washing machine drum in what
could be interpreted as a sound-gesture transfer (Delalande & Cornara,
2010) or potential schema (Pound, 2012)
Vignette 2
It is morning and the family are preparing to leave for a holiday. Lola tells me
that George ‘has been a bit fractious today’. They are in the kitchen and
George starts slamming a cupboard door. Lola puts a song on. When
George hears the music he turns his face away from the cupboard, listens
and starts to bounce, bending his legs with his left arm out in front of him. He
looks at Lola, who asks ‘are you having a boogie?’ in an encouraging tone.
He lifts his left arm higher and continues to bounce, legs and arm moving
together. He claps his hands together then returns his left arm to the air. He
bounces for a few bars before again clapping his hands together then
returning his left arm to the air. He starts to rock from left to right and then
moves to the other side of the kitchen. Here he puts his left arm in the air
again and tilts from left to right. He pauses to look at another adult in the
room, a friend of his parents, who is watching and smiling. He lifts his arm
264
back up, and rocks again. He approaches the other adult and then returns
to his space. He turns to look at me and then at Lola. He walks towards the
door, pauses and rocks back and forth for a few seconds, before turning
back into the kitchen.
Music had been playing all morning via an online streaming service; Lola
deliberately selected this song and she told me afterwards that it is a song
that Joe loves and which he has danced to with George since he was born.
In choosing this song deliberately to try to change George’s mood, we see
how Lola uses the recorded music as a form of affect regulation. Much of
the research on the use of music for affect regulation in infants has focused
on parental singing (See Dissanayake, 2000, Trehub, 2004; and Trevarthen,
1999). The predetermined nature of recorded music of course limits
opportunities for the parent-child dyad to dictate the pace of turn-taking
and for the sonic content to be shaped in response to the infant, but other
factors mentioned by Trehub, Ghazban, and Corbeil (2015) as being
important in affect regulation, such as moving, smiling and touching need
not be precluded when using recorded music.
Young and Gillen (2007) describe different levels of involvement by family
members in listening to recorded music as parents guide and support their
children’s participation. In George’s family, Joe has previously supported
George’s participation by dancing with him in his arms, and now that George
is older and dances independently, Lola continues to support his
participation by noticing his dancing and commenting on it encouragingly.
The music may also act as a form of affect regulation for Lola herself, possibly
relaxing her to see George happy or reminding her of meaningful moments
for her family.
George moves through the room, alternating between dancing on the spot
and walking to a new place. This pattern concludes when he reaches the
end of the kitchen at the start of the hallway. Here, instead of continuing on
the same trajectory, he turns back into the kitchen. Hancock and Gillen
(2007) in their study of two-and-a-half-year-old girls in the home found that
the shape and quality of space elicited different movements from children.
The hallway here is darker, narrower, a transitional space and perhaps is not
as attractive an option to George for dancing.
Vignette 3
George is sitting on Lola’s lap while she talks to a friend. He is playing with a
toy with a number of buttons, each of which plays a different animal noise.
He presses the ‘wolf’ button and hearing the howl that is played, copies it
with his voice, lifting his right arm as he does so. This gains Lola’s attention and
she asks ‘Did you do the wolf noise? Can you do it again?’, pressing the
button for him. He copies the sound again, this time lifting the toy into the air
with both hands as he does it. Lola laughs and imitates him, and so George
presses the button again, making his own sound immediately rather than
265
waiting for the toy to finish its sound first. Lola presses more of the buttons.
When she presses the duck sound, George lifts his arm into the air and starts
to sing a falling melodic pattern, two long notes during which he holds his
hand in the air and three short notes accompanied by waves of his arm. Lola
presses more of the buttons. When she presses the dog button George replies
with a ‘he he he’, bouncing himself on each sound he makes. Lola presses
the wolf button and again George replies with a howl, this time lifting his left
arm into the air. Lola comments that she thinks the arm action is George’s
representation of an elephant. They press more buttons. On pressing the lion
button, Lola comments ‘that one’s from Row, Row, Row, Your Boat. Do you
want to do Row, Row, Row?’. George replies by singing three long notes,
vowel sounds with a soft ‘r’ at the beginning, the melody falling in pitch over
the three notes. Lola sings Row, Row, Row, and George turns to face her on
the last line. She sings a second verse, and after the final words ‘don’t forget
to roar’ plays the lion sound from the toy and makes a roaring sound herself,
to which George turns and laughs. She starts a third verse. On the second
line George turns to look at her, smiling. After the words ‘don’t forget to
squeak’ she presses the mouse button and makes a squeaking sound,
moving her face towards his neck. She sings another verse, this time with the
final word ‘shiver’. She wraps her arm round him, making a shivering motion
that shakes them both.
Lola’s view was that George had listened to the howling wolf sound and, as
the two sounds have similar contours, interpreted it as an elephant’s
trumpeting, George’s arm movement representing the elephant’s trunk. She
noted that George had recently seen an elephant statue in a shop and
made the same noise and gesture. George’s interpretation of the wolf howl
as an elephant trumpet may also be related to the sound quality of the toy.
Young and Gillen note the prevalence of “anodyne, thin, neutral timbres”
(2007, p.24) in toys and the lack of a distinctive timbre may make this sound
more open to interpretation.
In a study on the affordances of early childhood technological activities Carr
(2000) suggests three factors: transparency, challenge, and accessibility, the
last of which refers to the toy’s potential for social participation and
collaboration. An initial reading might suggest this toy’s accessibility is low;
only one person can press a button and only one button can be pressed at a
time. Social participation in technology-enhanced toys is a matter of some
debate and Young (2007) notes the polarization of opinion regarding
childhood use of technology.
Bergen et al. (2010) suggest that play with technology-enhanced toys in fact
presents favourable conditions for adults to scaffold language, as the
context is one of the child’s interests. Tamis-LeMonda, Kuchirko, and Tafuro
(2013) propose that in a dyadic process of object exploration parents are
more likely to use referential than regulatory language, providing the input
multimodally to facilitate word learning. In naming the various animals, Lola
266
provides referential language input and this is indeed presented
multimodally, including also sound, facial expression and gesture.
Young and Gillen (2007) suggest that technology-enhanced toys do not
displace parents’ musical participation with their children but supplement it.
This toy encourages Lola and George’s object exploration and shared
attention and inspires their musical participation; the lion sound acts as a
trigger, a reminder of a song they have shared together. Lola asks George if
he wants to sing it and he responds positively by singing the song.
Each verse of Row, Row, Row is melodically identical, the repetition of verses
creating a sense of expectation. Dissanayake (2012) describes how as infants
get older they enjoy fun, silliness and divergence from expectation; in this
song the divergence, and therein the fun, comes from the lyrical substitutions
of place (shore, river and so on) animal (lion, mouse and so on), and
accompanying noise. These noises represent the climax of each verse, in
contrast with the original ‘life is but a dream’ verse which ends with a
dimuendo and/or rallentando, more in line with Papoušek’s (1996)
description of common structural features of early interactional games.
The multimodality of these interactional games is important as it allows for
songs to be repeated or varied depending on the infant’s signals of interest
(Papoušek, 1996) and for non-verbal participants to contribute to the building
up of an interaction (Fantasia et al., 2014). As Lola sings she also makes eye
contact with George, strokes his hair and kisses him, and creates actions to
represent the different animals. Each verse includes more physical contact
than the last, starting with just singing the first verse, then adding the sound
effect in the second verse, adding an ‘eeek’ and nuzzling George’s neck in
the third verse, and in the fourth verse adding a shivering sound and
enveloping George in the hug it appears the song has been working
towards.
Conclusion
The study presented some methodological challenges. Some of the more
interesting episodes were captured when George was left to play freely
without any adult encouragement; these most frequently occurred when the
adults were distracted and engaged in conversation, though this made
those same videos difficult to use as the conversation obscured the audio
recording of George’s music.
In vignette 2, George turns around to face each of the adults in the open
kitchen space; I would tentatively suggest that he may have been seeking or
expecting some further interaction from one of them and that had this been
forthcoming he may have demonstrated more extended engagement. It is
possible that my presence was a factor in why this didn’t happen; perhaps
Lola felt unable to intrude on what was being recorded or had unconsciously
also taken on a role as ‘observer’.
267
Despite these challenges this study has provided an opportunity to examine
the rich and varied musical world in which George lives. His music-making is
structurally complex, he listens to a range of music for which he has tastes
and preferences, and spends time with adults in musical interactions where
he is an active agent.
An understanding of these competencies and experiences is important for
practice with such young children. The song to which George dances is
perhaps not one that might be typically considered ‘music for children’ but it
has a special meaning for Joe and for George who, through his smiles and
dancing and apparent change in mood, seems to enjoy the song. On the
basis of his play with the cup and washing machine George’s interests could
lead him to, for example, find new ways of playing with a drum that exploit its
internal cylindrical space; ways of playing which would risk being
extinguished by a focus on an adult-constructed ‘correct’ way to play the
drum.
From this study a number of areas emerge where further research would be
of interest, particularly around the effect of developments in technology on
musical life in the home and more specifically how musical affect regulation
works beyond parental singing and what the role of recorded music is in that,
and how technological devices play a role in participation in parent-child
interactions.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I would like to thank my supervisors Dr Alison Street and Dr Jessica Pitt for their advice and to
thank Julian Knight for his support. Many thanks to George, Lola and Joe for participating in
this study.
References
Bergen, D., Hutchinson, K., Nolan, J.T., & Weber, D. (2010). Effects of infant-parent play with a
technology-enhanced toy: Affordance-related actions and communicative
interactions. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 24(1), 1-17.
Carr, M. (2000). Technological affordance, social practice and learning narratives in an early
childhood setting. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 10(1), 6180.
Delalande, F., & Cornara, S. (2010). Sound explorations from the ages of 10 to 37 months: the
ontogenesis of musical conducts. Music Education Research, 12(3), 257-268.
Dissanayake, E. (2000). Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother-infant
interaction. The origins of music, 389-410.
Dissanayake, E. (2012). The earliest narratives were musical. Research Studies in Music
Education, 34(1), 3-14.
Fantasia, V., Fasulo, A., Costall, A., & López, B. (2014). Changing the game: exploring infants'
participation in early play routines. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 522.
Hancock, R., & Gillen, J. (2007). Safe places in domestic spaces: Two-year-olds at play in their
homes. Children's Geographies, 5(4), 337-351.
Hughes, B., & Melville, S. (1996) A Playworker's Taxonomy of Play Tips. PlayEducation.
268
Marsh, K., & Young, S. (2006) Musical play. In G. McPherson (Ed.), The child as musician: A
handbook of musical development (pp. 289-310). New York: Oxford University Press.
Papoušek, M. (1996). Intuitive parenting: A hidden source of musical stimulation in infancy. In
I. Deliege and J. Sloboda (Eds.), Musical beginnings: Origins and development of
musical competence (pp.88-112). New York: Oxford University Press.
Pound, L. (2012). How children learn: From Montessori to Vygotsky - educational theories and
approaches made easy: Vol 1. Luton: Andrews UK Limited.
Street, A. (2007). ‘They won’t let you in’ Ethical issues in a study of maternal singing in
naturalistic settings. Paper presented at EuNET Music Educators and Researchers of
Young Children: Practising Research, Researching Practice (pp. 26-28).
Swanwick, K., & Tillman, J. (1986). The sequence of musical development: A study of
children's composition. British journal of music education, 3(03), 305-339.
Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Kuchirko, Y., & Tafuro, L. (2013). From action to interaction: Infant
object exploration and mothers' contingent responsiveness. IEEE Transactions on
Autonomous Mental Development, 5(3), 202-209.Tarnowski, S.M. (1999). Musical play
and young children. Music Educators Journal, 86(1), 26-29.
Trehub, S. E. (2004). Music in infancy. In J. W. Flohr (Ed.), Musical lives of young children (pp.
24-29). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Trehub, S.E., Ghazban, N., & Corbeil, M. (2015). Musical affect regulation in infancy. Annals of
the New York Academy of Sciences, 1337(1), 186-192.
Trevarthen, C. (1999). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human
psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae scientiae, 3(1_suppl), 155-215.
Young, S. (2003). Time–space structuring in spontaneous play on educational percussion
instruments among three-and four-year-olds. British Journal of Music Education, 20(01),
45-59.
Young, S. (2007). Digital technologies, young children, and music education practice. In K.
Smithrim, & R. Upitis (Eds.), Listen to their voices: Research and practice in early
childhood music (330-344). Waterloo, ON: Canadian Music Educators’ Association.
Young, S. (2008). Lullaby light shows: everyday musical experience among under-two-yearolds. International Journal of Music Education, 26(1), 33-46.
Young, S. (2013). Musical Childhoods: A survey of contemporary research. In O. Saracho, &
B. Spodek (Eds.), Handbook of Research on the Education of Young Children (3, pp.
250-264). Abingdon: Routledge.
Young, S., & Gillen, J. (2007). Toward a revised understanding of young children's musical
activities: Reflections from the" day in the life" project. Current Musicology, (84), 7-27.
269