Personality and Social Psychology
Review
http://psr.sagepub.com/
The Dynamics of Self-Evaluation
Robin R. Vallacher, Andrzej Nowak, Michael Froehlich and Matthew Rockloff
Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2002 6: 370
DOI: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0604_11
The online version of this article can be found at:
http://psr.sagepub.com/content/6/4/370
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Society for Personality and Social Psychology
Additional services and information for Personality and Social Psychology Review can be found at:
Email Alerts: http://psr.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
Subscriptions: http://psr.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Citations: http://psr.sagepub.com/content/6/4/370.refs.html
>> Version of Record - Nov 1, 2002
What is This?
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
Copyright 0 2002 by
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Personality and Social Psychology Review
2002, Vol. 6, No. 4, 370-379
The Dynamics of Self-Evaluation
Robin R. Vallacher
Department of Psychology
Florida Atlantic University
Andrzej Nowak
Department of Psychology
University of Warsaw
Michael Froehlich
Oregon Research Institute
Matthew Rockloff
Department ofPsychology
Central Queensland University
We conceptualize self-concept as a self-organizing dynamical system and investigate
implications ofthisperspectivefor the dynamic andfixed-point attractor tendencies of
self-evaluative thought. Participants who differed in self-concept valence (self-esteem)
and coherence (self-certainty, self-stability) engaged in verbal self-reflection for several minutes, then used a computer mouse to track the moment-to-moment self-evaluation expressed in their recorded narrative. Prior to self-reflection, participants recalled
positive or negative past actions (positive vs. negative priming), or did not recall past
actions (no priming). Priming affected overall self-evaluation (i.e., greatest positivity
under positive priming), but only early in the narrative. The effects ofself-concept, in
contrast, became stronger over time. Self esteem affected overall self-evaluation,
whereas self-certainty and self-stability affected the dynamic properties (e.g., rate of
movement between self-evaluative states) and attractor tendencies ofself-evaluation.
Discussion centers on the interplay between structure anddynamics in the self-system.
self-structure-most notably, global criteria of
self-evaluation-are considered central to one's subjective well-being and the quality of one's interpersonal relations. These functions suggest that the
self-structure plays a prominent, if not decisive, role in
shaping the spontaneous flow of cognitive elements
during self-reflective thought and in dictating how the
stream of self-reflective thought accommodates incoming information and other factors capable of biasing self-perception in some fashion. The precise nature
of the interplay between structural properties of the
self and the dynamics of self-reflection, however, is
largely unexplored. Accordingly, our aim in this article
is to consider how key properties of self-structure are
manifest in the stream of thought when people think
about themselves.
The self is arguably the largest and most accessible
structure in the cognitive system. It encompasses virtually every facet of one's experience, from the nuances
of physical appearance to personal aspirations and
self-perceived traits, and it is never more than a
stranger's glance from being activated. In recognition
of its size and ease of accessibility, the self is commonly imbued with substantial importance as well.
Specific elements of self-understanding (e.g., goals,
values, obsessions) are said to provide platforms for
action and self-regulation, and basic properties of
Preparation of this article was supported by NSF Grant SBR
95-11657 to the first two authors. The research was conducted as a
Master's Thesis by Michael Froehlich under the direction of Robin
R. Vallacher. We thank Eric Rudich and Kim Sussman for their assistance in data collection, Wojciech Borkowski and Tom Monson for
their assistance in statistical analyses, and Eliot Smith, Steve Read,
and an anonymous reviewer for their constructive comments on an
earlier version of this article. Preliminary data were presented at
"Preconference on Personality," Society for Personality and Social
Psychology, San Antonio, TX, in February, 2001.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Robin R. Vallacher, Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton,
FL 33431. E-mail:
[email protected]
Coherence in Self-Evaluation
Our theoretical rationale is derived from a model we
recently developed to depict the emergence of global
properties (e.g., self-esteem, self-certainty) in self-un-
370
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
SELF-EVALUATION DYNAMICS
derstanding as a result of internal integration processes
(Nowak, Vallacher, Tesser, & Borkowski, 2000). The
model assumes that the self-structure can be conceptualized as a complex system composed of cognitive elements representing specific pieces of self-relevant information. These elements are diverse by many criteria
(e.g., episodic memories, self-perceived skills, physical features) but all can be scaled with respect to the
common parameter of evaluation. This information invariably runs the gamut of possible self-evaluation,
from memories of socialfaux pas and the awareness of
personal shortcomings to memorable accomplishments and personal values. Everyone's self-structure,
in other words, consists of cognitive elements that are
diverse with respect to valence.
The elements of self-structure are not static in valence or passive, but rather influence each other in order to achieve a common evaluation. In this process, an
element that is evaluatively incongruent with neighboring (thematically related) elements may change its
valence or, conversely, change the valence of its neighbors, so as to establish evaluative coherence with the
related elements. The self-perception of "clumsiness,"
for example, may take on positive valence in the context of other self-perceived qualities that collectively
convey an image of oneself as a charming, absent-minded professor. As a result of this press for integration, specific subsets of self-relevant information
become increasingly coherent with respect to a shared
evaluation. By the same token, the self-structure becomes differentiated, with different sets of elements
stabilizing on different values of self-evaluation. A
person may have a coherent and positive view of him or
herself as a professor, for example, and an equally coherent but negative view of him or herself as an athlete.
Using computer simulations with a cellular automata
platform, Nowak et al. (2000) demonstrated a tendency
for self-relevant information to become differentiated
into evaluatively coherent substructures (cf. Showers,
1992, 1995), each corresponding to a distinct aspect of
self-perception (e.g., social roles, areas of competence). The emergence of evaluative differentiation in
self-structure is portrayed in Figure 1.
There is reason to suspect variability in the extent to
which self-structure achieves differentiation. First of
all, the valence of some elements may be fairly resistant to change. In Figure 1, this feature is represented
by the relative height of the elements; the higher the element, the greater the resistance to change. Elements
that are defined in relatively high- as opposed to
low-level terms, for example, are correspondingly
more stable and resistant to redefinition (cf. Vallacher
& Wegner, 1987). "Making a critical comment," for instance, can be identified at higher level in either positive or negative terms (e.g., "being constructive" vs.
"being rude"), but these higher-level identities are
themselves difficult to re-identify in ways that change
Figure 1. The Emergence of Coherence in Self-Evaluation. Light
gray cells denote positively valenced elements of self-relevant information; dark gray cells denote negatively valenced elements of
self-relevant information. Resistance to change in valence is represented by the height of cells (higher cells have greater resistance
and thus are more stable in valence). The top figure represents a
self-system with a random configuration of positive and negative
elements. The bottom figure represents a self-system that has become differentiated as a result of self-organization.
their valence. Even a lower-level element, however,
may have a fixed valence that is resistant to change if
salient social feedback or unequivocal social norms
dictate how the element is evaluated. Driving while intoxicated, for example, is a fairly basic depiction of action, but one that is difficult to identify at higher level
in other than negative terms. Second, the press for
evaluative integration may vary across different regions of self-structure, and those regions associated
with weaker press may achieve less coherence as a result (Nowak et al., 2000). Private as opposed to public
aspects of the self, for example, may be associated with
a relatively weak press for integration because of diminished self-presentation concerns. For these reasons, then, specific regions of self-structure may vary
with respect to their evaluative coherence. Conceivably, in fact, an entire self-structure could be devoid of
any regions with high coherence.
Coherence and Self-Reflection
Dynamics
We hypothesize that evaluative coherence plays a
primary role in shaping the dynamic properties of
self-evaluation. When attention is directed toward a
relatively coherent region of self-structure, self-evalu-
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
371
VALLACHER, NOWAK, FROEHLICH, & ROCKLOFF
ation should show correspondingly low variability. Because the majority of elements in such regions share a
common valence, their activation will produce relatively univalent self-relevant thoughts. When attention
is directed toward a relatively incoherent region, on the
other hand, the elements are mutually inconsistent in
valence, so their activation should promote correspondingly high variability in self-evaluation.
Beyond dictating self-evaluation within a region of
self-structure, coherence also plays a role in generating
the flow of self-evaluative thought across regions of
self-structure. We suggest that the temporal trajectory of
self-evaluation can be framed in terms of the system's
tendency to maximize its evaluative coherence. In this
view, each region of self-structure is characterized by a
specific value of a coherence function. The greater the
evaluative consistency among elements within a given
region, the higher the value ofthe coherence function for
that region. In an unperturbed system, the flow of
self-reflective thought tends to move from regions with
low coherence values toward those with higher values.
Over time, then, self-evaluative thinking in an unperturbed system should evolve toward regions characterized by relatively coherent self-relevant thoughts (i.e.,
thoughts that share a common valence). Even if the particular thoughts and images that arise in the course of
self-reflection are highly disparate with respect to content, their shared valence ensures that self-evaluation
will remain relatively stable over time.
The notion of attractor is useful in capturing this relationship between structure and dynamism. An attractor corresponds to a state toward which a system
evolves asymptotically over time. A system at its attractor state tends to remain at rest if unperturbed by
external influences, and will return to this state if
slightly perturbed by such influences. The strength of
an attractor is reflected in its basin of attraction (i.e.,
the number and variety of alternative states of the system that evolve toward it) and in its resistance to external influence (i.e., the strength of perturbation required
to move the system out of the basin of attraction to an
alternative attractor). With respect to self-reflection, an
attractor corresponds to self-relevant thoughts that are
evaluatively coherent and provide mutual support for
one another.' The stream of consciousness tends to
evolve toward such thoughts, and once they are activated, subsequent self-reflection tends to be resistant
to alternative thoughts and incoming information of a
conflicting nature. In a region of self-structure with
'Because our primary concern in the present context is the relative stability of self-evaluative states, we consider only the notion of
fixed-point attractor, which is equivalent to the notion of stable equilibrium in many psychological theories (see Nowak, Vallacher, &
Zochowski, 2002). Other types of attractors (i.e., periodic, quasi-periodic, chaotic) have been identified in dynamical systems, however,
and these may be relevant to various psychological phenomena (e.g.,
Vallacher & Nowak, 1997).
372
low evaluative coherence (i.e., a weak attractor), in
contrast, self-evaluation is characterized by inconsistency and is easily perturbed by incoming information.
Over time, the stream of thought will move away from
such regions (i.e., weak attractors) to areas of
self-structure characterized by higher evaluative coherence (i.e., strong attractors).
This depiction has implications for individual differences in self-evaluation tendencies. Because the
self-structure tends to be become differentiated into
distinct regions of evaluatively coherent elements
(Nowak et al., 2000), a person is likely to have more
than one attractor for the flow of self-evaluative
thought. Thus, someone with high self-esteem can converge on regions of self-structure that convey a relatively coherent but negative view of him or herself, just
as someone else with lower self-esteem can stabilize to
some extent on regions comprised of fairly coherent
positive self-relevant thoughts. What determines a person's characteristic level of self-esteem, then, is the relative coherence of positive and negative regions in his
or her self-structure. Stated differently, a person's level
of self-esteem is determined by those regions of
self-structure with maximal coherence. A person with
mediocre talents and a mixed record of accomplishment in most areas of life, for example, might nonetheless have high self-esteem if his or her relatively few
self-perceived assets form a coherent and positive
self-view. By the same token, someone with low
self-esteem may have a wide variety of talents and positive attributes, but if these elements do not form coherent regions, the person may not be able to sustain
thoughts concerning those elements.
As noted earlier, self-structures can vary in their
overall degrees of coherence. Despite the press for integration, some people may have relatively weak coherence, with neither positive nor negative self-relevant
thoughts providing an attractor for self-reflection. Because the local interactions in each region do not provide
sufficient mutual support to stabilize the system,
self-evaluation is characterized by changes over time
and heightened susceptibility to external influences.
Such influences (e.g., social feedback) may push the
system to one attractor, but the inconsistent influences
of neighboring elements in that region quickly dislodge
the system and restore its volatility. The incoherence, instability, and susceptibility to external influence associated with this type of self-structure are likely experienced as self-concept uncertainty (e.g., Campbell et al.,
1996; Kernis, 1993; Pelham, 1991; Vallacher, 1980).
Overview of Research Strategy
Assessment of the hypothesized link between structure and dynamics in self-reflection calls for sampling
the stream of thought with a fairly high degree of tem-
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
SELF-EVALUATION DYNAMICS
poral resolution. To do so, we recently adapted the
mouse paradigm employed in previous research on the
intrinsic dynamics of social judgment (Vallacher &
Nowak, 1994, 1997; Vallacher, Nowak, & Kaufman,
1994). In this approach, participants use a computer
mouse to control the position and movement of a cursor on a computer screen. Two symbols are presented
on the screen: a small circle positioned in the middle of
the screen that represents the target ofjudgment and an
arrow showing the current position of the cursor. Participants are instructed to position the cursor with respect to the circle to express their current feelings
about the target, with less distance indicating more
positive feelings. By tracking the position of the cursor
over time, then, it is possible to track the pattern of
changes in participants' feelings about the target.
This approach has been modified to enable insight
into the dynamics of self-evaluation. Participants are instructed to describe themselves by speaking into a microphone, and their self-description narratives are
stored as wave files or audiotapes. They are encouraged
to describe themselves as fully and completely as possible with respect to personality traits, goals, plans, relationships, or whatever else comes to mind. Upon completion of the recording, participants listen to it and use
the computer mouse procedure to indicate on a moment-to-moment basis the positivity versus negativity
of their self-descriptive comments. Specifically, they
position the cursor close to the circle if the momentary
self-description conveys positive feelings about themselves and position the cursor distant from the circle if
the self-description conveys negative self-relevant feelings. By tracking the cursor's position several times a
second, it is possible to chart a trajectory of self-evaluation for participants' stream-of-thought narrative and to
derive dynamic properties from the trajectory, such as
the variability in self-evaluation over time and the rate of
change between self-evaluative states.
Preliminary Research
Procedure
In our initial investigation, we recruited 69 undergraduates (23 men, 46 women) for a two-part study.
The first part consisted of several mass testing sessions, at which participants completed several self-report instruments, including a set designed to assess
global properties of self-concept. Self-esteem was assessed with a test developed by Rosenberg (1965).
Using Likert-type scales, participants expressed their
agreement vs. disagreement with 10 statements tapping a sense of personal worth (e.g., "I feel I have a
number of good qualities"). Self-concept stability was
assessed with an additional 4 items developed by
Rosenberg that measure agreement vs. disagreement
with statements tapping aspects of temporal consistency in self-concept (e.g., "Some days I have a very
good opinion of myself; other days I have a poor opinion of myself," "I have noticed that my ideas about myself seem to change very quickly"). To measure
self-concept certainty, we asked participants to indicate how certain vs. uncertain they were of their standing with respect to 20 common personality traits (e.g.,
sincerity, sociability, independence). These ratings
were made on 7-point response scales, anchored by not
at all certain and very certain.
Several days later, participants returned individually to take part in the second part, an ostensibly unrelated experiment involving memory and judgment.
Some participants were asked to think about 5 past actions that reflected positively on themselves (positive
priming); other participants were asked to think about
5 past actions that reflected negatively on themselves
(negative priming). In both conditions, participants entered brief (one- or two-sentence) descriptions of each
act into a computer. The remaining participants were
not asked to think of past actions (no priming). Participants then were instructed to describe themselves by
speaking into a microphone positioned next to the
computer. They were encouraged to say whatever
came into their minds as they thought about themselves, although they were advised not to mention their
names or other information that might identify them.
We assured them that their audiotapes would be treated
as confidential material and erased upon completion of
the study. After answering questions about this procedure, they were left alone for up to five minutes to create the audiotape.
Upon completion of their self-descriptive narratives, participants listened to the audiotape and indicated how positive vs. negative their self-description
was by using the modified mouse procedure (described
earlier). Thus, they used a computer mouse to control
the cursor's position vis a vis a small target circle in the
middle of the screen to indicate the moment-to-moment valence conveyed in their self-descriptive narrative. The less the distance from the target circle, the
more positive the momentary content of the narrative.
By tracking changes in cursor position, then, we could
capture the moment-to-moment self-evaluation conveyed in participants' stream of thought protocols. The
recordings ranged from I1/2 min to the allowable 5 min
(M = 2 min, 52 sec), so the length of the corresponding
mouse trajectories varied in the same manner.
Dependent measures
Responses to the 10 self-esteem items formed an internally consistent scale (cc = .87) and were averaged to
yield a measure of global self-esteem. The stability
items from the Rosenberg questionnaire were also
highly intercorrelated (average r = .68) and thus were
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
373
VALLACHER, NOWAK, FROEHLICH, & ROCKLOFF
averaged to general an overall measure of self-stability
(ca = .8 1). The certainty ratings also formed a highly reliable scale (ox = .83) and were averaged to yield an
overall measure of self-certainty. Self-esteem was reliably correlated with self-certainty (r = .50), in line with
previous research (e.g., Baumgardner, 1990; Campbell
et al., 1996; Kernis, 1993; Vallacher, 1978), and both
variables were reliably correlated with self-stability (r
= .40 for self-esteem, .47 for self-certainty).
From participants' mouse trajectories, we derived
the following measures: distance from the target circle
(in pixels), variability (standard deviation of distance),
speed (pixels per 0.1 s), acceleration (change in the
number ofpixels traversed in 0.1 s), and time at rest (sec.
without mouse movement). Distance provides a measure of self-evaluation, with greater distance indicating
lower self-evaluation. Variability reflects difference in
self-evaluative states-the greater the variability, the
greater the spread in self-evaluation. Speed and acceleration both reflect volatility in self-evaluation (i.e., rate
of change and instability in rate of change). Because
these measures have similar meaning and were highly
correlated, r(69) = .79, p < .001, they were averaged to
create a measure of dynamism. Time at rest is a direct
measure of the tendency for the self-system to settle on
an attractor state for self-evaluation. To assess positive
and negative attractor tendencies, time at rest was calculated separately for positions close to the target (positive
self-evaluation) and positions distant from the target
(negative self-evaluation). The close-far distinction was
determined by dividing in half the total distance in each
participant's mouse trajectory and computing the
amount of rest time in each half. To determine if the dynamic properties of participants' self-narratives
changed over time, we divided the mouse trajectories
into 3 equal time intervals, and for each one we computed the mouse-derived variables (distance, variability, dynamism, rest-close, and rest-far).
Hypotheses
Our theoretical rationale holds that everyone's
self-structure consists of both positive and negative
self-relevant information, and that there is a tendency
for these elements to become organized into
evaluatively coherent regions. Presumably, everyone
can think about themselves in either positive or negative terms if these respective regions are activated. We
predicted, therefore, that in comparison to the absence
of priming, positive priming would promote relatively
positive self-evaluation (i.e., relatively small distance
from the target circle), whereas negative priming
would promote relatively negative self-evaluation (relatively large distance).
Because priming only temporarily activates certain
regions of self-structure, however, it should have a correspondingly short-lived effect on the stream of
374
self-evaluation. As the activation induced by priming
dissipates, participants' self-evaluative tendencies
should evolve over time toward their dominant attractors. The effect of priming on momentary self-evaluation (distance), then, should be most apparent early in
participants' narratives (i.e., the 1st time period),
whereas the association between individual variation
in self-esteem and self-evaluation should become increasingly apparent at later points in their narratives
(i.e., the 2nd and 3rd time periods). Thus, we expected
participants with higher levels of self-esteem to show
more positivity (less distance) in their momentary
self-evaluation than those with lower levels of self-esteem, with the strength of this association growing over
the three time periods. Because systems tend to rest in
their attractor states, we also expected to observe a relationship between self-esteem and the rest-close and
rest-far measures. In line with the positivity bias (cf.
Taylor & Brown, 1988; Tesser, 1988), participants in
general were expected to spend more time at rest close
to the circle than far from the circle, but this asymmetry
in positive vs. negative attractor tendencies was expected to be greater for those with relatively high as opposed to relatively low self-esteem. Because an attractor represents a state to which the system converges
over time, we anticipated that the effects of self-esteem
on asymmetry in rest-close vs. rest-far would become
stronger from the 1st to the 3rd time period.
Predictions concerning variability and dynamism in
self-evaluation centered on the certainty and stability
of participant's self-concept. Compared to highly certain or stable participants, those with lower certainty or
stability were expected to display a greater range of
self-evaluative states and to evince more frequent and
erratic changes in self-evaluation during self-reflection. Because self-certainty and self-stability are theoretically distinct constructs, however, they may display
correspondingly distinct relationships with variability
and dynamism. It could be argued, for example, that
self-uncertainty reflects conflicting self-evaluative attractors, whereas self-instability represents a
self-structure that lacks any strong attractors for
self-evaluation. If so, self-concept uncertainty should
be associated with relatively high self-evaluation disparity (i.e., high variability), whereas self-concept instability should be associated with frequent and erratic
changes in self-evaluation (i.e., high dynamism) during self-reflection. Because self-evaluation evolves
over time to its attractor states, these relationships were
expected to become more pronounced from the 1st to
the 3rd time period.
Results
We performed analyses of variance (ANOVAs) to
assess the effects of priming and time period on the
mouse-derived variables (distance, variability, dyna-
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
SELF-EVALUATION DYNAMICS
Momentary Self-Evaluation by Self-Esteem and rime
120
104-8
100
_.
,
80
S
41-1
* Low Serf esteern
* High Self-esteem
60
v
i8
C
40
20
2nd
lst
Time
Period
Figure 2. Momentary Self-evaluation (Distance) by Self-Esteem and Time
mism, rest-close vs. far). Correlational analyses were
performed to assess the relationship between individual variation in global properties of the self (self-esteem, self-stability, self-certainty) and the mouse-derived variables.
Momentary Self-Evaluation. A priming x time
period ANOVA performed on distance revealed a marginally reliable effect for priming, F(2,66) = 2.73, p <
.07. Negative priming tended to promote greater distance (M = 109.6) than did no priming (M = 75.6) and
positive priming (M = 74.1). With the data blocked on
time period, results revealed a reliable priming effect
for only the lst time period, F(2,66) = 3.85, p < .03.
Negative priming promoted greater distance early in
participants' self-narratives (M = 108.2) than did no
priming (M = 64.0) and positive priming (M = 61.9). So
although inducing participants' to think about negative
acts on their part rendered negative aspects of themselves salient, this effect was fairly short-lived. In line
with our hypotheses, then, priming tended to create
only temporary attracting tendencies for participants'
stream of self-reflection.
Correlational analyses, meanwhile, demonstrated a
reliable relationship between self-esteem and distance,
r(69) = -.24, p < .05, with low self-esteem participants
displaying more negative self-evaluation than high
self-esteem participants. To determine if participants'
self-narratives became more polarized over time in line
with their predominant attractors, we performed the
correlational analyses separately for each time period.
Results revealed that the association between self-esteem and distance was marginally reliable in the 1st time
period, r(57) = -.23, p < .09, and became progressively
more reliable over time: r(57) = -.28, p <.03, for the 2nd
time period, r(57) = -.34,p < .01, for the 3rd time period.
(These analyses were replicated with self-certainty and
self-stability each substituted for self-esteem. The results failed to demonstrate any clear pattern of association between these variables and distance.)
This effect is illustrated in Figure 2, which depicts
the self-evaluative tendencies of high and low self-esteem participants at each time period. To create high
and low self-esteem groups, we divided the distribution of self-esteem scores into the upper and lower
40%. In line with the correlational data, the results of
ANOVAs revealed that the difference between low
and high self-esteem participants became larger and
increasingly reliable over time: F(1,55) = 2.92, p <
.09, for the 1st time period, F(1,55) = 4.80, p < .03,
for the 2nd time period, and F(1,55) = 7.16, p < .01,
for the 3rd time period (see Fig. 2). Unlike the effect
of priming, then, the effect of self-esteem became
more pronounced as participants reflected on themselves, a pattern that is consistent with convergence
on a
stable attractor state.
test
Attractors for Self-Evaluation.2 To
whether individual variation in self-esteem is associated with differential self-evaluative attractor tenden-
2Johnson and Nowak (2002) used recently developed software
(Nowak & Vallacher, 2002) to identify fixed-point attractors in time
series data collected on bipolar depressive individuals. In principle,
this software is applicable to the data generated by the mouse procedure employed in the preliminary research. Because of specific properties of mouse-generated data (e.g., vastly different ranges of mouse
movements across participants), however, the method utilized by
Johnson and Nowak (2002) requires specific refinements before it
can be reliably applied to the data obtained in the preliminary research reported in the present article. Work currently in progress is
addressed toward generalization of this method to diverse types of
time-series data (Nowak, Vallacher, Tesser, & Voss, 2002).
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
375
VALLACHER, NOWAK, FROEHLICH, & ROCKLOFF
Equabriumn
by SeLl-Estm andnm
so
I..
I
..
it
*M LWa|
-9-EOW
SQ v:$00£-
.,
2
b.1
C1.
Thmu Pwiod
Figure 3. Equilibrium Asymmetry (Rest close-far) by Self-Esteem and Time
cies, correlations were computed between self-esteem
and the rest time measures. For participants' self-narrative as a whole, self-esteem was reliably correlated
with rest-close, r(69) = .25, p < .05, but not with
rest-far, r(69) = -.01, ns. The association with
rest-close was not reliable at the 1st time period, r(69) =
.1 6, ns, achieved marginal reliability at the 2nd time period, r(69) = .21, p < .08, and became highly reliable
during the 3rd time period, r(69) = .30, p < .01. We also
computed correlations between self-esteem and the
difference between rest-close and rest-far. For self-narratives as a whole, results revealed that the higher the
self-esteem, the greater the difference in time spent at
positive vs. negative attractor states (i.e., rest-closerest-far), r(69) = .27, p < .05. This association was not
reliable during the 1st and 2nd time periods, r(69) = .08
and .15, ns, respectively, but was highly reliable during
the 3rd time period, r(69) = .31, p < .01. (Correlational
analyses were also performed for self-certainty and
self-stability. The results did not demonstrate reliable
associations between these properties of the self and
attractor tendencies.)
Figure 3 illustrates the relationship between self-esteem and convergence on differential self-evaluative attractor states. We performed ANOVAs comparing the
difference between rest-close and rest-far for high and
low self-esteem participants (based on the upper and
lower 40% of self-esteem scores). High and low self-esteem participants did not reliably differ in the respective
asymmetry of positive and negative attractors for the
first two time periods, F< 1 in both cases. During the 3rd
time period, however, high self-esteem participants displayed reliably greater asymmetry in positive vs. negative attractor tendencies, F( 1,55) = 6.1 8,p < .01 (see Fig.
3). All participants demonstrated greater stability in momentary self-evaluation when their thoughts entered a
376
region of self-structure corresponding to positive as opposed to negative self-evaluation, but over time this
differential attractor tendency became greater for high
than for low self-esteem participants.
Self-Evaluation Variability. To test whether the
dynamics of self-structure can be described as a progression from regions of relatively low coherence to regions of relatively high coherence, we performed a
prime x time period ANOVA on self-evaluation variability, with time period as the repeated measure. Results revealed only a reliable effect for time period,
F(1,66) = 4.88, p < 03. Decomposition of this effect
revealed that variability did not reliably differ between
the Ist and 2nd time periods, t(68) = -1.59, ns, but that
variability was reliably lower during the 3rd time period
(M=60.3) than during the 2nd time periods (M=71.4),
t(68) = 2.21, p < .03. In line with our rationale, then,
participants' self-evaluation tendency converged over
time on relatively coherent self-evaluative states.
Correlational analyses were also performed to assess whether uncertainty and/or instability in self-concept were associated with divergent self-evaluative
states (i.e., high variability) in the course of self-reflection. Results revealed that higher self-certainty was associated with a narrower range of self-evaluation for
participants' self-narrative as a whole, r(69) = -.27, p <
.05, and for the 1st and 3rd time periods, r(69) = -.27
and -.23, p < .05 (for the 2nd time period, r(69) = -.19,
p < .10). Results also revealed that higher self-stability
was associated with a relatively narrow range of
self-evaluation for the self-narrative as a whole, r(69) =
-.26, p < .05, and for the 2nd time period, r(69) = -.23, p
< .05 (for the 1st and 3rd time periods, r(69) = .14 and
-.16, ns). All participants displayed movement be.
tween different self-evaluative states in their stream of
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
SELF-EVALUATION DYNAMICS
thought narratives, but the difference between the most
extreme positive and negative states was reliably
greater for participants with conflicting or unstable assessments of themselves. (Although self-esteem was
associated with momentary self-evaluation and attractor tendencies, this property of the self was not reliably
correlated with variability in self-evaluation.)
performed
Self-Evaluation Dynamism. We
correlational analyses to assess whether uncertainty
and/or instability in self-concept promoted rapid and
erratic changes in self-evaluation during self-reflection. Results revealed that self-stability was reliably
correlated with dynamism for the self-narrative as a
whole, r(69) = -.32, p < .01, and for the 2nd time period
of the narrative, r(69) = -.35,p < .01 (for the 1st and 3rd
time periods, r(69) = .01, ns, and -.20, p < 10). Thus,
participants who indicated that their feelings about
themselves sometimes change from day to day were
also more likely than their stable counterparts to demonstrate changes in self-evaluation on a considerably
shorter time scale. Lacking a clear anchor for self-evaluation, unstable participants produce a stream of
self-reflection that fails to converge on a stable attractor, but rather continually shifts among different
self-evaluative states. Self-certainty was not associated
with dynamism in these analyses, r(69) < -.18, ns, in
all cases. (When the analyses were repeated with
self-esteem substituted for self-stability, no reliable
correlations were observed.)
Conclusions, Implications, and Future
Iterations
The results of this preliminary research confirmed
our general expectation that global properties of
self-structure are meaningfully and systematically related to basic features of self-evaluative thought.
Self-esteem, which represents global positivity in
self-structure, shaped the evaluative tone of self-reflection, with higher levels of self-esteem promoting correspondingly positive self-relevant thoughts in subjects'
self-reflection narratives. Self-certainty and self-stability, which reflect the coherence of self-structure, were
manifest in the dynamic properties of self-reflection.
Both uncertainty and instability were associated with
heightened variability in self-evaluation, although at
different points in participants' stream of self-reflective
thought, whereas self-instability tended to promote
rapid and erratic changes in self-evaluation over time.
The evaluative tone of participants' self-reflection
was also affected by the priming of positive versus
negative self-relevant information. Participants who
were induced to recall past acts that reflected positively or negatively on themselves tended to express
correspondingly valenced thoughts about themselves
when subsequently asked to reflect on themselves.
This effect, however, proved to be transitory, shaping
self-reflective thought only during the early portions
of participants' self-narratives. In contrast, the effect
of self-esteem became stronger the longer participants reflected on themselves. This effect was observed not only for self-evaluation per se (i.e., less
distance for higher self-esteem participants), but also
for the relative time spent at positive versus negative
attractor states. All participants tended to stabilize
more on positive than on negative self-relevant
thoughts, but by the end of participants' self-narratives, this asymmetry was reliably greater for those
with higher levels of self-esteem. In terms of our theoretical rationale, the stream of self-reflection tended
to converge on a self-evaluative state representing
maximal coherence. For participants with high
self-esteem, positive self-evaluation provided a coherent state and hence a stable attractor. For participants with low self-esteem, negative self-evaluation
was relatively coherent and thus provided a relatively
stable attractor that competed with their tendency to
converge on positive self-evaluation.
The asymmetry in positive versus negative coherence has implications for people's response to external
influence. When evaluatively inconsistent information
(e.g., social feedback) enters a region with high coherence, it is likely to be undermined or changed in valence by the combined influence of other elements in
that region (Nowak et al., 2000). If a person has an
evaluatively consistent view of him or herself as a good
parent, for example, the reminder that he or she forgot
about a parent-teacher conference may have only a
temporary effect on his or her self-image in this domain. As the person thinks about the event in the context of myriad sources of evidence painting a more flattering picture, the isolated incident diminishes in its
relative impact and may even be reinterpreted in positive terms (e.g., a sign of how hard he or she works to
maintain a quality life for his or her children). The
same reasoning holds for a self-structure with coherent
regions of negatively valenced elements. Inconsistent
(i.e., positive) information about the self may have
only a short-lived impact as the mutual influences
among elements in the negative substructure reinforce
one another and thereby diminish the impact of the
new information. In short, a greater amount of
evaluatively inconsistent information may be required
to disrupt self-evaluation with respect to a coherent as
opposed to an incoherent attractor state.
This reasoning resonates with contemporary research regarding self-concept and reactions to self-relevant feedback. The work on self-verification, for instance, has established that people tend to resist social
feedback in a given domain that doesn't confirm their
self-conception (cf. Swann, 1990). If people lack confidence in their self-view, however, they demonstrate
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
377
VALLACHER, NOWAK, FROEHLICH, & ROCKLOFF
less resistance to inconsistent feedback and may embrace it as informative about themselves (e.g., Swann
& Ely, 1984). In similar fashion, when people are induced to think about the specific elements comprising
a region of self-structure rather than the region's global
representation, they demonstrate vulnerability to social feedback regarding the purported implications of
these elements for self-understanding (Wegner,
Vallacher, Kiersted, & Dizadji, 1986). The research on
threatened egotism, meanwhile, has shown that people
with high self-esteem react defensively-even violently when exposed to feedback that questions their
self-image in domains about which they harbor private
doubts (Baumeister, Smart, & Bowden, 1996).
Each of these empirical generalizations is consistent with the rationale concerning coherence and
self-evaluation stability. Thus, for regions of self-structure that are experienced as certain, unitary, and free
from doubt, inconsistent information is readily dismissed with little or no disruption to the person's predominant self-view. But for regions that are experienced as uncertain, fragmented, or otherwise open to
interpretation, inconsistent information represents a
threat and prompts either acceptance or highly defensive and active opposition. We recognize, of course,
that the empirical generalizations described earlier
were developed in the context of different assumptions
and purported mechanisms. Hence, establishing that
people's response to incoming information is mediated
primarily by the relative coherence of the region of
self-structure at issue constitutes an agenda for future
empirical research.
Despite the press for evaluative coherence, people
vary in the extent to which they develop strong attractors for self-evaluation. The lack of coherent
self-evaluative states is likely to be experienced in phenomenal terms as uncertainty or instability in self-evaluation (e.g., Kernis, 1993; Vallacher, 1980). By this
reasoning, uncertain people should be particularly vulnerable to social feedback regarding any region of their
self-structure (e.g., Vallacher & Wegner, 1989). They
may feel negative about themselves after an interaction
with someone who is critical, for example, only to rebound to a positive view when subsequently exposed to
a flattering assessment from someone else. Such malleability in response to social feedback is consistent
with the absence of a single dominant attractor for
self-evaluative thinking. In the absence of social feedback or other sources of incoming information, an uncertain person should demonstrate sustained dynamism in self-evaluation over time as conflicting
elements are activated in the stream of thought. In contrast, the stream of self-reflection for a person with a
high degree of self-concept certainty should display
relatively little variability in self-evaluation over time,
because the elements that achieve activation provide
mutual support for one another and thus function as an
378
attractor for self-reflective thought. In accord with this
reasoning, uncertain and unstable participants in the
preliminary research moved between relatively divergent self-evaluative states, none of which were sufficiently coherent to stabilize participants' assessments
of themselves, and unstable participants did so in a relatively rapid and erratic manner.
The inability to stabilize on a coherent region of
self-structure would seem to pose a serious challenge
for self-regulation. As noted by Carver and Scheier
(2002), self-regulation involves maintaining congruence between one's behavior and a desired state or goal.
To the extent that regulatory goals reflect regions of
self-structure (e.g., specific identities or self-images),
self-regulation is facilitated by coherence in such regions. Lacking coherence, self-regulation with respect
to these aspects ofthe self is likely to be erratic and to falter when obstacles to goal attainment are encountered.
For individuals whose self-structure is largely devoid of
coherent regions, then, action is likely to reflect momentary impulse, external influence, and opportunity rather
than commitment, purpose, and other criteria of
self-regulation. It remains for future research, ofcourse,
to subject this reasoning to empirical test (see, however,
Johnson & Nowak, 2002).
Evaluation is not the only basis for organization in
the self-system. Although everyone presumably experiences a press for evaluative integration in self-understanding (cf. Nowak et al., 2000), this press coexists
with a tendency to integrate self-relevant information
in terms of social roles, personal concerns and interests, and other aspects of content. Indeed, this basis for
self-structure has been investigated empirically (e.g.,
Linville, 1985) and has been forwarded as an integrative principle that can outweigh the tendency to organize self-relevant information on the basis of evaluation (cf. Showers, 1992, 1995). When integrated in this
fashion, a region of self-structure may consist of elements that are evaluatively incongruent and thus give
rise to self-reflective thought that changes notably over
time in valence.
Conceivably, uncertain or unstable participants in
the preliminary research had self-structures that were
organized in terms of content rather than evaluation. If
so, their dynamism and lack of attractor tendencies with
respect to evaluation may have masked temporal stability with respect to content-based self-understanding. An
unstable person, for example, might focus on his or her
academic qualities, exhausting the available self-relevant information in this domain-negative as well as
positive-before moving on to another content-defined
self-aspect. Of course, the fact that this person's sense of
self is experienced as incoherent (i.e., uncertain or unstable) attests to the importance of achieving evaluative
integration in self-structure. Nonetheless, the possibility that people with an uncertain or unstable self-concept demonstrate relative stability in terms of content
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
SELF-EVALUATION DYNAMICS
during self-reflection warrants further investigation.
Research underway in ourrespective labs (United States
and Poland) is intended to shed light on this issue.
The preliminary research emphasized fixed-point
attractors for self-evaluation. The stream of self-reflective thought, however, may conform to temporal patterns other than convergence on a stable state. Indeed,
the work on nonlinear dynamical systems has shown
that a fixed-point attractor represents only one of several possible temporal patterns for many phenomena
(cf. Schuster, 1984), including social psychological
processes (cf. Arrow, McGrath, & Berdahl, 2000;
Nowak & Vallacher, 1998). A system may display a
sustained pattern of periodic evolution, for example,
with a macro-level property of the system (e.g., evaluation in social judgment) moving between (among) two
(or more) states in a regular or quasi-regular manner.
These states effectively operate as "repellors" (Nowak,
Lewenstein, & Tarkowski, 1994)-as the system approaches one of the states, it is repelled and moves toward another state, and so on, creating a periodic temporal pattern. A system's macro-level property can also
display chaotic evolution, such that the system appears
to move randomly and erratically among a variety of
states. Far from being random, however, a chaotic attractor represents the (deterministic) interaction of system elements, often very few in number. We are currently developing software to identify and distinguish
among fixed-point, periodic, and chaotic attractors in
time-series data (Nowak & Vallacher, 2002). In subsequent iterations of the stream of self-reflection paradigm, this method will be used to provide insight into
the various temporal trajectories of self-evaluation and
their relation to self-structure.
References
Arrow, H., McGrath, J. E., & Berdahl, J. L. (2000). Small groups as
complex systems. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Bowden, J. M. (1996). Relation of
threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of
self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33.
Baumgardner, A. H. (1990). To know oneself is to like oneself:
Self-certainty and self-affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 58, 1062-1072.
Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L.
F., & Lehman, D. R. (1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and cultural boundaries. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 141-156.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Control processes and self-organization as complementary principles underlying behavior.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 304-315.
Johnson, S. L., & Nowak, A. (2002). Identifying equilibrium tendencies in bipolar depression. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 6, 380-387.
Kernis, M. H. (1993). The roles of stability and level of self-esteem
in
psychological functioning. In R. F. Baumeister (Eds.),
Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 167-182). New
York: Plenum.
Linville, P. W. (1985). Self-complexity and affective extremity:
Don't put all your eggs in one cognitive basket. Social Cognition, 3, 94-120.
Nowak, A., Lewenstein. M., & Tarkowski, W. (1994). Repellor neural networks. Physical Review E, 48, 1491-1498.
Nowak, A., & Vallacher, R. R. (1998). Dynamical social psychology.
New York: Guilford.
Nowak, A., & Vallacher, R. R. (2002). Identifying attractors in
time-series data. Unpublished research data.
Nowak, A., Vallacher, R. R., Tesser, A., & Borkowski, W. (2000).
Society of self: The emergence of collective properties in
self-structure. Psychological Review, 107, 39-61.
Nowak, A., Vallacher, R. R., Tesser, A., & Voss, R. (2002). Equilibrium dynamics: The maintenance of stable states in psychological systems. Manuscript in preparation.
Nowak, A., Vallacher, R. R., & Zochowski, M. (2002). The emergence of personality: Personality stability through interpersonal
synchronization. In D. Cervone & W. Mischel (Eds.), Advances
in personality science (Vol. 1, pp. 292-331). New York:
Guilford Publications.
Pelham, B. W. (1991). On confidence and consequences: The certainty and importance of self-knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology; 60, 518-530.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Schuster, H. G. (1984). Deterministic chaos. Vienna: Physik Verlag.
Showers, C. J. (1992). Compartmentalization of positive and negative self-knowledge: Keeping bad apples out of the bunch. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1036-1049.
Showers, C. J. (1995). The evaluative organization of self-knowledge: Origins, processes, and implications for self-esteem. In
M. H. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem (pp.
101-120). New York: Plenum.
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1990). To be adored or to be known? The interplay
of self-enhancement and self-verification. In E. T. Higgins & R.
M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition
(Vol. 2, pp. 408-448). New York: Guilford.
Swann, W. B., & Ely, R. J. (1984). A battle of wills: Self-verification
versus behavioral confirmation. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 46, 1287-1302.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological
Bulletin, 103, 193-210.
Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
socialpsychology(Vol. 21, pp. 181-227). New York: Academic.
Vallacher, R. R. (1978). Objective self awareness and the perception
of others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 63-67.
Vallacher, R. R. (1980). An introduction to self theory. In D. M.
Wegner & R. R. Vallacher (Eds.), The self in social psychology
(pp. 3-30). New York: Oxford University Press.
Vallacher, R. R. & Nowak, A. (1994). The stream of social judgment.
In R. R. Vallacher & A. Nowak (Eds.), Dynamical systems in
social psychology (pp. 251-277). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Vallacher, R. R. & Nowak, A. (1997). The emergence of dynamical
social psychology. Psychological Inquiry. 4, 73-99.
Vallacher, R. R., Nowak, A., & Kaufman, J. (1994). Intrinsic dynamics of social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 20-34.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think
they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3-15.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal
agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 57, 660-671.
Wegner, D. M., Vallacher, R. R., Kiersted, G., & Dizadji, D. (1986).
Action identification in the emergence of social behavior. Social Cognition, 4, 18-38.
379
Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at FLORIDA ATLANTIC UNIV on October 31, 2011
View publication stats