ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION
Sex Differences in Brain Aging
A Quantitative Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study
C. Edward Coffey, MD; Joseph F. Lucke, PhD; Judith A. Saxton, PhD; Graham Ratcliff, DPhil;
Lori Jo Unitas; Brenda Billig; R. Nick Bryan, MD, PhD
Background: Little is known about the effect of sex on
age-related changes in brain structure.
Methods: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of
the brain was performed in 330 elderly (age range, 66-96
years) volunteers living independently in the community, all of whom were participants in the Cardiovascular Health Study. Blinded measurements of global and regional brain size were made from T1-weighted axial images
by means of computer-assisted edge detection and trace
methods. High measurement reliabilities were obtained.
occipital region area. Main effects of age were observed
for all the remaining brain regions examined (cerebral
hemisphere volume, frontal region area, temporoparietal region area, lateral ventricular volume, and third
ventricle volume), but these effects were similar in men
and women. Asymmetries in brain structures were not
affected by aging in either sex.
Conclusions: Our results are generally consistent with
Results: Age-specific changes in brain size were signifi-
the few published studies on sex differences in brain aging and suggest that, for at least some structures, aging
effects may be more apparent in men than women. The
neurobiological bases and functional correlates of these
sex differences require further investigation.
cantly greater in men than women for the peripheral (sulcal) cerebrospinal fluid volume, the lateral (sylvian) fissure cerebrospinal fluid volume, and the parieto-
Arch Neurol. 1998;55:169-179
B
From the Departments of
Psychiatry and Neurology,
Henry Ford Health System,
Detroit, Mich (Dr Coffey);
Allegheny-Singer Research
Institute, Allegheny University
of the Health Sciences
(Dr Lucke and Mss Unitas and
Billig), and Western
Psychiatric Institute and Clinic,
University of Pittsburgh
Medical Center (Drs Saxton
and Ratcliff), Pittsburgh, Pa;
and Department of Radiology,
Johns Hopkins University,
Baltimore, Md
(Dr Bryan).
OTH POSTMORTEM (reviewed
by Powers1) and in vivo imaging (reviewed by Coffey2) studies have demonstrated that advancing age in
humans is generally associated with decreased brain tissue size and increased
brain cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) volume.
Although sex differences have been described in the size, symmetry, and function of several brain structures,3-8 only a
small number of imaging studies have examined the effects of sex on brain aging
in nonpatient samples of living humans
(Table 1).9-27 While the findings have
been inconsistent, a few investigators have
reported sex differences in the effects of
age on some brain structures, and in most
cases males showed greater aging changes
than females.14,17,18,22,25,26 These studies are
somewhat difficult to compare, however,
given differences in subject samples
(eg, sample size, age range, exclusion
criteria), imaging and data acquisition
protocols (eg, computed tomography vs
magnetic resonance [MR] imaging), mea-
surement technique, and statistical analyses (Table 1).
The present study used quantitative
MR imaging morphometry to examine the
effects of sex on age-related changes in the
size of regional brain matter and CSF
spaces in a large sample of elderly volunteers living independently in the community. We tested the hypothesis that such
changes would be more dramatic in men
than in women.
RESULTS
The regional cerebral measures are
shown in Table 2. Results are discussed
first for measures that showed an effect
of sex on age-related changes in volume
(ie, an age3sex interaction), and then
for measures that showed only a main
effect of age without an interaction.
AGE 3 SEX INTERACTION
Significant age3sex interactions were
found for the peripheral CSF volume, the
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
169
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
SUBJECTS AND METHODS
SUBJECTS
Subjects were selected from among participants in the Cardiovascular Health Study (CHS), an ongoing multicenter,
population-based observational study of 5888 volunteers
65 years and older, including 2495 men and 3393 women.28,29 The major goal of the CHS is to identify risk factors
related to the development and course of coronary heart
disease and stroke in individuals living independently in
the community. After providing informed consent, subjects undergo extensive clinical evaluation (home interview and physical examination) and laboratory testing (including brain MR imaging [see below]) at baseline, and
annual follow-up assessment. Additional details of the CHS
have been published.28
A detailed description of subject recruitment for the
CHS has been published.29,30 For the present study, we identified from the CHS cohort a sample of 500 subjects recruited from 2 CHS sites (Pittsburgh, Pa, and Hagerstown, Md) who gave written consent to participate in an
ancillary investigation of cognitive functioning and aging
(these data will be the subject of a future report). All available subjects from these 2 sites in whom brain MR imaging was performed within 1 year of this cognitive testing
were screened for inclusion in this study. We subsequently excluded from this cohort a total of 170 subjects
for 1 or more of the following reasons: not right-handed
(subjects were determined to be right-handed if they used
their right hand to write, throw a ball, and brush their
teeth31); lifetime history of any psychiatric illness or of any
illness or injury referable to the brain (per the CHS clinical evaluation described earlier); incomplete cognitive test
data; incomplete MR imaging data (eg, scan artifact, missing slices); or MR images with structural abnormalities (cortical infarct, n=5; hydrocephalus, n=1; tumor, n=1; and
markedly thickened calvarium, n=1).32,33
The final sample consisted of 330 subjects, 129 men
and 201 women, ranging in age from 66 to 96 years
(Table 2). Our subjects were similar to the CHS population as a whole with regard to age (CHS mean ± SD, 72.77
± 5.61 years), sex distribution (CHS, 59% female), and education level (CHS mean ± SD, 12.35 ± 3.10 years). Of the
subjects, 244 (74%) were taking medications for 1 or more
of the following medical conditions: hypertension or ischemic heart disease (74 men [57.4%]; 111 women [55.2%]),
peptic ulcer disease (18 men [13.9%]; 21 women [10.4%]),
osteoarthritis (14 men [10.9%]; 35 women [17.4%]), hypercholesterolemia (9 men [6.9%]; 30 women [14.9%]),
hypothyroidism (4 men [3.1%]; 20 women [10%]), infection (9 men [7.0%]; 14 women [7.0%]), diabetes mellitus
(oral agent: 6 men [4.7%]; 4 women [2.0%]; insulin: 8 men
[6.2%]; 3 women [1.5%]), postmenopausal hormone replacement (16 women [8.0%]), gout (8 men [6.2%]; 1
woman [0.5%]), chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (2
men [1.6%]; 8 women [4.0%]), benign prostatic hypertrophy (5 men [3.9%]), breast cancer in remission (3 women
[1.5%]), and hyperthyroidism (1 man [0.8%]). No subject was taking medication known to affect brain size (eg,
corticosteroids). Additional subject characteristics are given
in Table 2.
BRAIN MR IMAGING TECHNIQUE
As noted earlier, brain MR imaging was performed in all
subjects as a result of their participation in the CHS. The
standardized CHS brain MR imaging acquisition protocol has been previously described.34 Magnetic resonance
imaging was performed on either a 1.5-T scanner (General Electric, Milwaukee, Wis) (n=248) or a 0.35-T scanner (Toshiba) (n=82) at 1 of 2 CHS field centers (Pittsburgh and Hagerstown, respectively). Head position was
oriented in the scanner and was stabilized during the
scanning procedure by the use of Velcro straps and foam
head supports. To establish slice orientation, the first
scanning sequence consisted of a T 1-weighted sagittal
series (repetition time [TR], 500 milliseconds; echo time
[TE], 20 milliseconds; thickness, 5 mm; gap, 0 mm; and
matrix, 1283256) centered at the midline to define the
anterior commissure–posterior commissure (AC/PC)
line. Then a second series of proton-density (TR, 3000
milliseconds; TE, 30 milliseconds; flow compensated)
and T2-weighted (TR, 3000 milliseconds; TE, 100 milliseconds; flow compensated) images was obtained (thickness, 5 mm; gap, 0 mm; matrix, 2563192; number of
excitations, one-half [1 on the 0.35-T scanner]), oriented parallel to the AC/PC line, and extending from the
vertex to the skull base. A third series consisting of T1weighted (TR, 500 milliseconds; TE, 20 milliseconds)
axial images was then obtained (thickness, 5 mm; gap, 0
mm; matrix, 2563192; number of excitations, 1), oriented parallel to the AC/PC line, and extending from
vertex to skull base. Images were stored on 9-track magnetic tape.
IMAGE ANALYSIS AND BRAIN MORPHOMETRY
For the present study, the brain images were transferred
from magnetic tape to read/write magneto-optical disks. Data
were analyzed on a workstation (Power Mac 8100, Apple,
Cupertino, Calif) with high-resolution color graphic monitor. The measurements of regional brain size were made
on the recalled T1-weighted axial images by 1 of 2 trained
technicians blinded to all subject characteristics. Window
center settings were first standardized to ensure precision
in boundary detection.35 Structures were identified with the
help of brain and MR imaging atlases36,37 and then measured with a combination of computer-assisted edge detection and manual tracing, using graphic analysis software (MedVision, Imnet/Evergreen Technologies, Castine,
Me). The area (in square centimeters) within the outline
was calculated automatically; volume (in milliliters) was
determined by multiplying the area by the slice thickness
and summing over the multiple slices in which the structure appeared (described later).
The following regions were defined for volume measurement.
Intracranial volume (IV) was defined by the internal
surface of the diploe16 and measured in every slice between the vertex and the superior border of the midbrain
(approximately 12-15 slices per subject were measured).
Intracranial size could not be reliably measured inferior to
this level because of the presence of structures such as the
globes and sinuses. As such, this measure is an underestimate of the true total intracranial volume. There was no
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
170
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
significant correlation between age and intracranial volume.
Cerebral hemisphere volume was measured in every slice
between the vertex and the skull base (approximately 18-20
slices per subject). Ventricular volumes were excluded from
this measurement.
Lateral ventricle volume was measured in each slice on
which lateral ventricles were present. We also measured
the various subregions of the lateral ventricles, including
the body, the frontal horns, the posterior horns, and the
temporal horns.
Third ventricle volume was measured in each slice beginning at the level of the foramen of Monro and extending inferiorly to the superior border of the midbrain (approximately 3-4 slices per subject).
Peripheral (sulcal) CSF volume was a calculated value
derived by subtracting the cerebral hemisphere and ventricular volumes from the intracranial volume, for each slice
on which intracranial volume was measured. As such, this
measure is an underestimate of the true total peripheral CSF
volume.
Lateral (sylvian) fissure CSF volume provided an indirect estimate of atrophy of the temporal lobe, as well as of
the frontal and parietal lobes. The lateral fissures were measured in each slice on which they were present, beginning
at the level of the foramen of Monro. When the lateral fissure communicated freely with the peripheral CSF, the anterior boundary of the fissure was defined by a horizontal
line connecting the anterior tip of the temporal lobe to the
medial temporal region.
It was not possible to reliably subdivide the cerebral
hemisphere into its various lobes (ie, frontal, temporal,
parieto-occipital) because of difficulties in establishing
boundaries for such subregions in the axial plane of orientation.2 Nevertheless, a regional brain morphometric
analysis was possible on 1 of our axial slices. For this
analysis, we followed the method of Pearlson et al38 and
chose a T1-weighted axial slice that passed through both
the pineal gland and foramen of Monro (hereafter designated the “region-of-interest [ROI] slice”). This slice is
approximately 1 slice above the AC/PC line and is especially suited to subregional analysis because it contains
both gray and white matter, it is not dominated by CSF
spaces, and it contains anatomical regions believed to be
associated with performance on a number of neuropsychological tests. 38 Using the boundary definitions of
Pearlson et al,38 the following 4 subregions were defined
for area measurement on the ROI slice (ventricular areas
were excluded from all regions) (Figure).
Frontal region area: The posterior border of this region was defined by a horizontal line intersecting the anteriormost aspect of the lateral ventricles.
Temporoparietal region area: This region was the area
situated between the frontal lobes anteriorly and the parietooccipital lobes posteriorly, and was bordered medially by
the internal capsule.
Parieto-occipital region area: The anterior border of this
region was defined by a horizontal line intersecting the anterior atria of the ventricles.
Intracranial area (IA): This area was defined by the inner surface of the diploe (per above).
Extensive reliability studies of our measurement techniques have indicated that area/volume measurements
of these regions are highly reliable.16 On the basis of a
randomly selected sample of 10 brains from the current
study, intraclass correlation coefficients for interrater
reliability of the 2 raters ranged from 0.85 (for small
regions such as the third ventricle) to 0.99 (for large
regions such as the cerebral hemisphere). Intraclass correlation coefficients for intrarater reliability ranged from
0.84 to 0.99.
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Preliminary Analysis
By exploratory methods, the data were examined for outliers and extreme values by means of box plots and normal quantile-quantile plots. Transformations of the outcome variables—in particular, cube root transformations
for the volume data, square root transformations for the
ROI data, and logarithmic transformations for both—
were reviewed. These analyses demonstrated no need for
transformation.
Regressions, using the full model given below, were conducted on untransformed and logarithmically transformed
outcome variables. The residuals from these regressions were
examined by means of deviation plots and normal quantilequantile plots, again to assess whether the outcome variables needed transformation. The results of these analyses also
indicated that the untransformed data best fit the assumptions of normal-theory linear regression.
Our analysis treated intracranial size as a covariate. An
alternative approach is to use percentage size based on the
ratio of brain structure size to intracranial size. We rejected this approach for 2 reasons. First, the ratio approach implicitly assumes that brain size is perfectly correlated with intracranial size. Although the 2 are highly
correlated, we found the assumption of perfect correlation untenable. Second, the ratio approach creates outcome variables that are necessarily bounded between 0 and
1. Such variables may have distributions poorly suited for
linear regression analysis.
Regression Analyses
The outcome variables consisted of the cerebral volumes,
the left2right differences for the relevant cerebral volumes, the cerebral areas from the ROI slice, and the leftright differences for these cerebral areas. There were 4 predictor variables. The first predictor in the regression equation
was either IV (for the 2 sets of volume data) or IA (for the
2 sets of area data), as appropriate. The second predictor
was sex, with the effect coded as 1 for men and −1 for
women. The third was age, centered at 75 years (roughly
the mean age of the sample) to eliminate colinearity
with the age3sex interaction. The fourth predictor was
the age3sex interaction, created by multiplying the
(centered) age variable by the sex variable.
The regression models were the same for all outcome
variables. Each outcome variable was first regressed against
the full model consisting of IV (or IA, as appropriate),
sex, age, and age3sex, using the hierarchical method
in the order given. In this approach, the significance of a
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
171
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Continued on next page
predictor is adjusted for all predictors preceding it
in the list, but not adjusted for any predictors following it. In all tests, the significance level was set at
.05. If age 3 sex was found significant, the full model
was accepted, regardless of the significance values of
any of the preceding predictors, and testing was
stopped. If age 3 sex was not significant, then it was
eliminated from the equation and the regression was
run again. If age was significant, then this model was
accepted and testing stopped. Otherwise, age was
eliminated and the regression was run again. These
iterations were repeated until a significant effect was
found or no predictors were left. The regression coefficients from the final accepted model were then used
to interpret the results.
lateral fissure CSF volume, and the parieto-occipital region area. For each of these regions, men showed greater
age-related changes than did women. Table 3 illustrates these interactions for persons with an average intracranial size, from ages 65 to 95 years. For the peripheral CSF volume, the regression coefficient was 2.11 for
men but only 0.06 for women (P,.03). At age 65 years,
men had a mean peripheral CSF volume about 5.70 mL
smaller than that of women, but at age 95 years, men had
a mean peripheral CSF volume about 55.67 mL larger
than that of women (Table 3). For the lateral fissure CSF
volume, the regression coefficient was 0.23 for men but
only 0.10 for women (P,.04). At age 65 years, men had
a mean lateral fissure volume about 0.80 mL larger than
that of women, but at age 95 years, this difference increased to 4.86 mL (Table 3). For the parieto-occipital
region area, the regression coefficient was −0.31 for men
but only −0.09 for women (P,.03). At age 65 years, men
had a mean parieto-occipital region area about 2.15 cm2
larger than that of women, but at age 95 years, men had
a mean parieto-occipital region area about 4.54 cm2
smaller than that of women (Table 3).
AGE MAIN EFFECTS
Age was significantly related to each of the remaining brain
matter and CSF regions measured. Increased age was associated with decreased cerebral hemisphere volume (coefficient = −2.79, P,.001), frontal region area (coefficient = −0.13, P,.001), and temporoparietal region area
(coefficient = −0.13, P,.001). Increased age was also associated with increased volumes of the lateral ventricles
(coefficient = 0.95, P,.001) and the third ventricle (coefficient = 0.05, P,.001).
REGIONAL CEREBRAL ASYMMETRIES
To examine potential laterality differences in the effects
of sex on age-related changes in regional cerebral size,
left − right differences were analyzed by means of the same
hierarchical regression model described above. There were
no age 3 sex interactions and no main effects of age or
sex for any of the regions. A main effect was found for
intracranial area, but for the frontal region only. Increas-
ing intracranial area was associated with an increased left
− right difference in frontal region area (coefficient =
−0.01, P,.01), a result of greater increases of the right
side than of the left.
COMMENT
We found that age-specific changes in brain size were significantly greater in men than women for the peripheral
(sulcal) CSF volume, the lateral (sylvian) fissure CSF volume, and the parieto-occipital region area. Main effects
of age were observed for all the remaining brain regions
examined, but these effects were similar in men and
women. Asymmetries in brain structures were not affected by aging in either sex. Our blinded measures of
these brain regions were highly reliable, and our estimates of their age-specific sizes agree closely with previous reports, including those that used more sophisticated voxel-by-voxel techniques.2 Our results shed light
on some of the conflicting findings in the literature (discussed later) and extend these observations to a large
sample of elderly persons living independently in the community.
METHODOLOGICAL LIMITATIONS
Our findings are subject to certain potential limitations.
Although cross-sectional studies of age effects allow for
relatively efficient and rapid acquisition of large amounts
of data, they are subject to secular effects, such as birth
cohort. This effect refers to the possibility that brain size,
like cranial size, may exhibit systematic changes over successive birth cohorts in the general population. If such
trends actually exist in the population at large and if they
are not secondary to secular trends associated with correlates such as cranial size (in the present study, cranial
size was not correlated with age), then an assessment of
the true effects of aging per se on brain volume will require longitudinal investigation.
A second issue relates to the health status of our subjects. First, our sample represents a group that may be
somewhat healthier than the entire population because
of selection criteria for the CHS and the current study.30
As such, our findings may not be applicable to the entire population of seniors. Second, there is heterogeneity of health status within our subjects, in that 26% were
also free of major systemic illness while 74% had at least
some mild physical disease, corresponding to the distinction between successful and usual aging.39 Such differences in health status could account for differences in
brain aging, and indeed systemic disease such as hypertension has been found to be associated with changes in
brain structure.40,41 The prevalence of this condition was
generally similar among the men and women in our study,
however. Furthermore, studies in subjects free of major
medical illness have reported sex differences in agerelated changes in brain structure similar to our present
findings.22,25 Still, it is possible that sex differences in the
prevalence of systemic diseases may account for some of
the sex differences observed in structural brain aging.
The measurements of regional brain size in our study
are subject to certain limitations. First, because of
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
172
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table 1. Imaging Studies of Sex Differences in Human Brain Aging*
Source, y
Imaging and
Measurement Technique
Subjects
9
Findings
Grant et al, 1987
64 healthy volunteers
18-64 y old
25 M; 39 F
No history of neurologic disease;
psychiatric history not reported
Handedness not specified
MR imaging (0.15 T)
Mathematically derived estimate
of ventricular volume from
signal intensity measurements
made on single sagittal slice
(No. of raters not specified)
Age associated with increased
lateral ventricular volume in M
but not F; however, apparent
gender difference not tested
statistically
Effects on asymmetries not reported
No control for size of brain or head
Condon et al,10 1988
40 volunteers
20-60 y old
20 M; 20 F
No additional details provided
MR imaging (0.15 T)
Volume measurement (2 raters)
derived from computer-assisted
pixel segmentation of
contiguous sagittal slices
(variable slice thickness and
number)
Age negatively correlated with ratio
of total brain volume to IV in M
but not F; however, correlations
not statistically compared
Yoshi et al,11 1988
58 healthy volunteers
21-81 y old
29 M; 29 F
Neurologic and psychiatric histories
not reported
Handedness not specified
MR imaging (1.0 T)
Blinded global ratings (4-point
scales) of cortical atrophy and
lateral ventricular enlargement
from inversion recovery films
(axial slices [No. unspecified], 10
mm thick, 3-mm interscan gap)
Mathematically derived estimate
of brain volume from inversion
recovery films, based on
planimetric area measurement
made on single slice (10 mm
thick) at level of foramen of
Monro
No. of raters and rater reliabilities
not specified
Age correlated with ratings of
cortical atrophy and lateral
ventricular enlargement (M and F)
Laterality effects not reported
No correlation between age and
brain volume, in either M or F
Krishnan et al,12 1990
39 healthy volunteers
24-79 y old
17 M; 22 F
No evidence of major medical,
neurologic, or psychiatric illness
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Blinded stereological
measurement (1 of 2 raters) of
axial slices (variable number, 5
mm thick, 2.5-mm interscan
gap) from intermediate and
T2-weighted films
Age negatively correlated with total
caudate volume (M and F)
Caudate volume was less in
subjects older than 50 y (n=22)
No adjustments for cranial size
Effects on asymmetries not reported
Doraiswamy et al,13 1991
36 healthy volunteers (overlap with
subjects in Krishnan et al12 and
McDonald et al15)
26-79 y old
16 M; 20 F
No evidence of major medical,
neurologic, or psychiatric illness
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Area measurement of T2-weighted
midsagittal image (5 mm thick)
using computer-assisted trace
method
Rater reliabilities not reported
Age negatively correlated with
corpus callosum area in M but
not F
No adjustments for cranial size
Gur et al,14 1991
69 healthy volunteers
18-80 y old
34 M; 35 F
No neurologic or psychiatric illness
66 dextrals; 3 sinistrals
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Volume measurements (any 2 of 4
raters) derived from
segmentation technique based
on 2-feature pixel classification
of multiple spin-echo axial
images (5 mm thick,
contiguous)
Older ($55 y) subjects had smaller
whole brain volumes (M=F),
larger total CSF volume (M.F),
larger ratio of ventricular CSF
volume to IV (M=F), and larger
ratio of sulcal CSF volume to IV
(M.F)
Effects of age on ratio of ventricular
CSF volume to IV were asymmetric
(L.R) in M but not in F
McDonald et al,15 1991
36 healthy volunteers (subjects also
included in Krishnan et al12)
24-79 y old
13 M; 23 F
No evidence of major medical,
neurologic, or psychiatric illness
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Same as Krishnan et al12
Age negatively correlated with total
putamen volume (M=F;
left=right), but no adjustments
for cranial size
(continued)
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
173
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table 1. Imaging Studies of Sex Differences in Human Brain Aging* (cont)
Imaging and
Measurement Technique
Findings
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Adjusting for IV, age associated with
Volume measurements (1 of 3 raters
decreasing total volumes of cerebral
with established reliabilities) using
hemispheres (0.23%/y), frontal lobes
computer-assisted trace method of
(0.55%/y), temporal lobes (0.28%/y), and
T1-weighted coronal images (n=30-35,
amygdala-hippocampal complex (0.30%/y)
5 mm thick, contiguous)
(M=F for all regions)
Blinded clinical ratings (5-point scale)
Adjusting for IV, age associated with
of cortical atrophy and of lateral
increased volumes of third (2.8%/y) and
ventricular enlargement from films
lateral (3.2%/y) ventricles
(average score of 2 experienced
Increasing age associated with increasing
raters)
odds (8.9%/y) of “cortical atrophy,” from
0.08 at age 40 y to 2.82 at age 80 y (M=F)
Age associated with increased odds (7.7%/y)
of at least mild lateral ventricular
enlargement, from 0.10 at age 40 y to 2.22
at age 80 y (M=F)
No lateralized effects
Source, y
Coffey et al,16 1992
Subjects
76 healthy volunteers
36-91 y old
25 M; 51 F
No lifetime evidence of neurologic or
psychiatric illness
All right-handed
Kaye et al,17 1992
107 healthy volunteers
64 M (21-90 y old)
43 F (23-88 y old)
No major medical, neurologic,
or psychiatric illness
Handedness not specified
Golomb et al,18 1993
154 healthy elderly volunteers
CT (n=51); MR imaging (1.5 T) (n=81);
55-88 y old
both CT and MR imaging (n=22)
73 M; 81 F
Blinded ratings (4-point scale) of
No evidence of active medical, neurologic,
hippocampal atrophy as defined by
or psychiatric illness
dilation of transverse choroidal
Handedness not specified
fissure on films
Interrater reliabilites established,
but No. of raters not reported
Subjects with hippocampal atrophy (rating of
2 or greater in either hemisphere; n=50)
were significantly older than those without
atrophy
More M (41%) than F (25%) had
hippocampal atrophy
Effects on asymmetries not reported
Raz et al,19 1993
29 healthy volunteers
18-78 y old
17 M; 12 F
No major medical, neurologic,
or psychiatric illness
Handedness not specified
MR imaging (0.30 T)
Blinded volume measurements (digital
planimetry) from films of T1-weighted
and proton density images in sagittal
and coronal planes
Good rater reliabilities, but No. of raters
not specified
After controlling for head size, age associated
with increased lateral ventricular volume
(M=F) and decreased visual cortex volume
(F.M); age not associated with volumes of
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior
cingulate gyrus, prefrontal white matter,
hippocampal formation, postcentral gyrus,
inferior parietal lobule, or parietal white
matter
Sullivan et al,20 1993
114 healthy volunteers
21-82 y old (mean±SD,
51.2±17.7 y)
84 M; 30 F
No history of major medical, neurologic,
or psychiatric illness
90% right-handed
CT
Volume measurements derived from
computer-assisted segmentation
technique
Axial slices (n=10), 10 mm thick
Adjusting for head size, age correlated with
total ventricular volume, third ventricular
volume, and CSF volume in sylvian fissure
and in vertex, frontal, and parieto-occipital
sulci (M=F for all regions)
Effects on asymmetries not reported
Christiansen et al,21 1994
142 healthy volunteers
21-80 y old
78 M; 64 F
No major medical neurologic illness
Psychiatric history and handedness not
specified
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Volume measurements using manual
tracing of T2-weighted axial images
(4 mm thick, 4-mm interscan gap)
No additional details provided
Age associated with increased lateral ventricle
volume in M (134%) and F (66%), but
these apparent gender differences were not
statistically compared
Cowell et al,22 1994
130 healthy volunteers
18-80 y old
70 M; 60 F
No major medical, neurologic,
or psychiatric illness
All right-handed
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Ratio of frontal lobe to IV smaller in M .40 y
Volume measurements using
of age than in younger M; no such group
combination of computer-assisted
difference seen in F; R.L asymmetry of
trace method and pixel segmentation
frontal lobe to IV larger in older F than
of 3-dimensional images reconstructed
younger F; no such group difference
from T2-weighted axial images (5 mm
observed in M
thick, no gap)
Ratio of temporal lobe to IV also smaller in M
Good rater reliabilities, but “blindness”
.40 y old than in younger M; no such
not specified
group difference seen in F; no lateralized
effects
CT
Volume measurement derived from
computer-assisted segmentation
technique (ASI-II program)
Axial slices, 10 mm thick, 7-mm
interscan gap
Age associated with increased ratio of
ventricular volume to IV (about
20%/decade) (M=F); precipitous increases
observed beginning in fifth decade in M
and in sixth decade in F
Effects on asymmetries not reported
(continued)
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
174
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table 1. Imaging Studies of Sex Differences in Human Brain Aging* (cont)
Source, y
Imaging and
Measurement Technique
Subjects
Findings
Ratio of the remaining brain volume to
IV smaller in older than younger
subjects (M=F); no lateralized effects
Blatter et al,23 1995
194 healthy volunteers
16-65 y old
89 M; 105 F
No history (by questionnaire) of any
neurologic or psychiatric illness
95% right-handed
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Volume measurements derived from
semiautomated pixel segmentation
and trace methods, of intermediate
and T2-weighted axial images (5 mm
thick, 2-mm gap)
High rater reliabilities (blinded status?)
Adjusting for IV, age associated with
decreased total brain matter volume
and gray matter volume (F only), but
not white matter volume; increased
subarachnoid CSF volume; and
increased lateral and third ventricular
volumes, but not fourth ventricular
volume; except for gray matter
volume, correlations tended to be
higher for M than F, but apparent
differences not analyzed
Effects on asymmetries not reported
Parashos et al,24 1995
80 healthy volunteers (overlap with
subjects in Coffey et al16)
30-91 y old
28 M; 52 F
No lifetime history of neurologic or
psychiatric illness
All right-handed
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Blinded area measurements using
computer-assisted trace method of
T1-weighted midsagittal image (5 mm
thick), made by single rater with
established rater reliabilities
Adjusting for IV, increasing age
associated with smaller total and
regional callosal areas, especially of
anterior regions (M=F)
Murphy et al,25 1996
69 healthy volunteers
35 M (mean±SD age, 44±23 y)
34 F (50±21 y)
No major medical or psychiatric illness
All right-handed
MR imaging (0.5 and 1.5 T)
Blinded volume measurements using
computer-assisted segmentation and
trace method of contiguous coronal
images (5-6 mm thick)
No. of raters not specified
Relative to “young” subjects (age, 20-35
y), “old” subjects (60-85 y) had
smaller brain matter volume ratios of
cerebellum to IV (M=F), cerebrum to
IV (M.F), frontal lobe to IV (M.F),
temporal lobe to IV (M.F), parietal
lobe to IV (F.M), parieto-occipital
lobe to IV (M=F), parahippocampal
gyrus to IV (M=F), amygdala to IV
(M=F), hippocampus to IV (F.M),
thalamus to IV (M=F), lenticular
nucleus to IV (M=F), and caudate to
IV (M=F); old subjects also had larger
lateral ventricular (M=F), third
ventricular (F.M), and peripheral
CSF to IV (M=F) ratios
For frontal lobe, right side decreased
more than left with age in M, but in F
left side decreased more than right
Raz et al,26 1997
148 healthy volunteers
18-77 y old
66 M (mean±SD age, 47.39±18.07 y)
82 F (45.72±16.48 y)
No major medical, neurologic, or
psychiatric illness
Handedness not specified
MR imaging (1.5 T)
Blinded volume measurements (digital
planimetry) from films of T1-weighted
reformatted coronal images (1.3 mm
thick, contiguous)
Good rater reliabilities among 8 raters
Adjusted for height, age significantly
related to smaller volumes of whole
brain (M=F), prefrontal gray matter
(M=F), inferior temporal cortex
(M.F), fusiform gyrus (M=F),
hippocampal formation (M=F),
primary somatosensory cortex (M=F),
superior parietal cortex (M=F),
prefrontal white matter (M=F), and
superior parietal white matter (M=F)
No age effects found for anterior
cingulate cortex, parahippocampal
cortex, primary motor cortex, inferior
parietal cortex, visual cortex, and
precentral, postcentral, and inferior
parietal white matter
No lateralized age effects
Yue et al,27 1997
1488 healthy elderly volunteers from
CHS
65-80+ y old
No. M and F not specified
Handedness not specified
No major medical or neurologic illness
(psychiatric illness not reported)
MR imaging (0.35 or 1.5 T)
Blinded ratings (10-point scales) of
sulcal prominence and ventricular
size from T1-weighted axial images
Good to excellent rater reliabilities, but
No. of raters not specified
Age associated with sulcal prominence
and ventricular enlargement (M=F)
*MR indicates magnetic resonance; IV, intracranial volume; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid; CT, computed tomography; and CHS, Cardiovascular Health Study.
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
175
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table 2. Subject Characteristics and Regional Brain Size as a Function of Sex*
Subject characteristics
Age, y†
Education, y
WAIS-R Vocabulary
MR imaging intracranial volume, mL
Brain matter size
Cerebral hemispheres, mL
Total
Left
Right
Left 2 right
Frontal lobe region, cm2
Total
Left
Right
Left 2 right
Temporoparietal region, cm2
Total
Left
Right
Left 2 right
Parieto-occipital region, cm2
Total
Left
Right
Left 2 right
CSF volume, mL
Peripheral CSF
Lateral fissures
Total
Left
Right
Left 2 right
Lateral ventricles
Total
Left
Right
Left 2 right
Third ventricle
All Subjects
(N=330)
Men
(n=129)
Women
(n=201)
74.98±5.09
12.88±3.00
46.66±14.19
940.83±99.79
75.38±5.60
13.12±3.25
46.33±15.22
1000.22±96.22
74.72±4.72
12.73±2.82
46.98±13.60
902.72±81.90
945.33±99.72
474.58±50.10
470.76±50.30
3.82±11.58
996.60±100.22
500.81±49.46
495.79±51.43
5.01±11.86
912.44±84.55
457.74±42.83
454.69±42.48
3.05±11.36
37.68±4.57
18.57±2.33
19.11±2.45
−0.54±1.40
38.68±4.84
19.00±2.45
19.69±2.63
−0.69±1.55
37.03±4.27
18.29±2.21
18.74±2.25
−0.44±1.29
26.03±3.57
12.87±1.86
13.06±1.97
−0.18±1.48
25.63±3.69
12.73±2.05
12.90±1.94
−0.17±1.52
26.13±3.44
12.97±1.74
13.16±1.99
−0.19±1.45
59.68±5.86
30.20±3.14
29.48±3.05
0.71±2.0
61.77±6.43
31.29±3.42
30.48±3.28
0.81±1.87
58.33±5.04
29.49±2.74
28.84±2.71
0.65±2.08
211.75±50.90
235.28±49.74
196.64±45.74
9.73±3.30
4.70±1.64
5.03±1.92
−0.32±1.37
11.21±3.58
5.36±1.79
5.85±2.11
−0.49±1.57
8.78±2.72
4.28±1.40
4.50±1.58
−0.22±1.22
29.10±19.32
15.16±10.45
13.93±9.18
1.23±3.71
2.29±0.90
35.19±20.35
18.33±11.11
16.86±9.53
1.47±3.87
2.63±0.96
25.19±17.60
13.13±9.49
12.06±8.45
1.08±3.61
2.04±0.79
*Data are mean±SD. WAIS-R indicates Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised; MR, magnetic resonance; and CSF, cerebrospinal fluid.
†There was no significant correlation between age and MR imaging intracranial volume.
Intracranial Area
Frontal Area
Temporoparietal Area
Parieto-occipital Area
Typical T1-weighted axial brain magnetic resonance image at the level of the
foramen of Monro, demonstrating the subdivisions of the region of interest
slice.
limitations inherent in the CHS MR imaging acquisition
protocol, our analyses of brain size were restricted to the
axial plane (3-dimensional reconstruction was not possible without dramatic loss of resolution). The axial plane
does not permit optimal boundary delineation of many brain
regions, and as such our anatomic definitions were arbitrary and frequently underrepresentative of the true size
of the structure. In particular, our estimates of regional brain
size were based on single-slice area measurements (the ROI
slice) rather than multislice volume measures, which are
more valid estimates of true brain size.2 Second, accurate
delineation of regional boundaries can be affected by several sources of technical error, including improper window center settings, magnetic field inhomogeneity (resulting in spatial distortion of objects and object pixel
nonuniformity), and differences in MR imaging technical
variables.35 The effects of these variables were minimized
in this study by use of a set of procedures that has been
shown to optimize the accuracy of MR imaging size mea-
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
176
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table 3. Effect of Sex on Age-Specific Changes in Regional Brain Size*
Age, y
Peripheral CSF volume, mL
Men
Women
Lateral fissure CSF volume, mL
Men
Women
Parieto-occipital region area, cm2
Men
Women
65
70
75
80
85
90
95
199.36
205.06
209.90
205.37
220.43
205.68
230.97
205.98
241.50
206.29
252.03
206.59
262.57
206.90
8.70
7.90
9.86
8.38
11.02
8.87
12.19
9.36
13.35
9.85
14.51
10.34
15.68
10.82
62.79
60.64
61.23
60.18
59.66
59.74
58.10
59.29
56.54
58.84
54.97
58.39
53.40
57.94
*Data are for persons with an average intracranial size. CSF indicates cerebrospinal fluid.
surements.16,35 Third, field strength differences between the
2 scanners could affect estimates of brain size.2 To test
whether such differences could have confounded the relations between brain size and the predictor variables, scanner assignment was entered as a covariate in the regression analyses (entered after sex). Scanner assignment was
not confounded with any of the age 3 sex interactions or
the age main effects.
SEX EFFECTS ON AGE-SPECIFIC
CEREBRAL ATROPHY
We found that the age-related increase in peripheral CSF
volume, a marker of cortical atrophy, was significantly
greater in elderly men than women. For example, from
ages 65 to 95 years, men (of average IV) had an increase
in peripheral CSF volume of approximately 32% compared with less than a 1% increase in women (Table 3).
Gur et al14 also found that the ratio of sulcal CSF volume to IV was greater for elderly (55 years and older)
subjects and for men. Similarly, Blatter et al23 found higher
correlations between age and “subarachnoid” CSF volume (adjusted for IV) in men (r=0.653) than in women
(r=0.545), although these correlations were not statistically compared. Other studies that examined peripheral CSF volume have found no sex effects on agerelated increases.20,25,26
We found no sex differences in the age-related decrease in cerebral hemisphere volume (ie, there was no
age 3 sex interaction) and no age effects (neither main
nor interaction with sex) on the left − right difference in
cerebral hemisphere volume. Similar negative findings
have been reported.11,14,16,19,20,25,26 Although Condon et al10
found that men, but not women, exhibited a significant
correlation between age and the ratio of total brain volume to IV, these correlations were not statistically compared. Similarly, Blatter et al23 observed higher correlations in men than women between age and the ratio of
total brain volume to IV (r=−0.675 vs r=−0.539, respectively), but again these correlations were not statistically compared. Murphy et al25 reported that men had a
significantly greater age-related decrease in the ratio of
cerebral hemisphere volume to IV than did women.
Our finding of a significant sex effect on the agerelated increase in peripheral CSF volume, in the ab-
sence of a sex effect on age-related volume loss of cerebral hemisphere brain matter, is consistent with the
observations of Gur et al.14 Taken together, these reports suggest that while peripheral CSF volume may show
a greater age-related increase in men than women (likely
as a result of cortical atrophy), such sex differences in
cortical atrophy may not be apparent statistically when
the cortex is averaged in with a relatively larger structure, such as the cerebral hemisphere. We are not aware
of any studies that have examined sex effects on agerelated tissue loss in the cortex per se.
SEX EFFECTS ON AGE-SPECIFIC DIFFERENCES
IN REGIONAL BRAIN SIZE
We found that the age-associated increase in lateral fissure CSF volume, a marker of frontotemporal (and, to a
lesser extent, parietal) atrophy, was significantly greater
in men than women. For example, from ages 65 to 95
years, men (of average IV) had an increase in lateral fissure volume of approximately 80%, while women had
an increase of only approximately 37% (Table 3). Although Sullivan et al20 found no sex differences in the
age-related increase in sylvian fissure volume, they used
computed tomographic scanning and relatively thicker
brain slices (10 mm).
In contrast to the results with lateral fissure volume, we found no sex effects on the age-related decrease in temporoparietal region area or frontal region
area. Thus, in our study, lateral fissure CSF volume was
a more revealing marker of atrophy in these regions than
was their direct measurement from the ROI slice. The literature is conflicting with regard to the effects of sex on
age-related changes in temporal lobe size (Table 1). Cowell et al22 and Murphy et al25 found that men exhibited
greater age-related decreases in the ratio of temporal lobe
volume to IV than did women. Similarly, Golomb et al18
found that age-related hippocampal atrophy was more
common in men than women, and Raz et al26 observed
greater age-related inferior temporal volume loss in men
than women. In contrast, Murphy et al25 actually observed greater temporal lobe atrophy in women than men.
Despite differences in which sex is more affected, the published results suggest that sex may impact the agerelated volume loss of the temporal lobe region. These
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
177
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
findings may provide a neuroanatomical substrate for the
sex differences noted earlier in age-related verbal memory
impairment.42-44
The literature is also conflicting with regard to the
effects of sex on age-related changes in frontal lobe size
(Table 1). Cowell et al22 and Murphy et al25 both observed greater age-related frontal lobe volume loss in men
than in women. In contrast, others have found no sex
effects.16,19-22,26 These discrepant results may reflect differences between studies in samples and brain measurement techniques (ie, quantitative vs qualitative measures, area measures from a single slice vs volume
measures from multiple slices).
Our analysis of left − right difference in temporoparietal and frontal region areas showed no age effect (neither main effect nor interaction with sex). Similar negative results have been reported.16,20,22,25,26 In contrast,
Cowell et al22 observed that the right greater than left
asymmetry of frontal lobe volume to IV was larger in older
women than in younger women, whereas in men no such
group differences were seen.
We found that age-related decreases in parietooccipital region area were greater for men than women—
for example, from ages 65 to 95 years, men (of average
IA) lost approximately 15% of their parieto-occipital lobe
area, while women lost only 4% (Table 3). Using a somewhat different definition of this brain region, Cowell et
al22 did not find any sex effect on the age-related decrease in the ratio of the posterior cerebral hemisphere
volume to IV. Murphy et al25 likewise found no sex differences in the age-related decrease in parieto-occipital
region volume to IV, although they actually observed
worse atrophy in women for the ratio of parietal lobe volume to IV. Similarly, Raz et al19 reported that women exhibited greater age-related volume loss in the visual cortex than did men. These widely divergent findings indicate
a need for additional research. Our analysis of left−right
difference in parieto-occipital lobe area disclosed no age
effect (neither main effect nor interaction with sex). Similar negative results have been reported.16,20,22,25
With regard to ventricular volumes, we found no
sex effects on the age-related increase in lateral ventricular CSF volume or third ventricular CSF volume. Similar negative findings have been reported by the majority
of studies that have examined the lateral ventricles11,14,16,17,19,20,25,27 or the third ventricle.14,16,17,20 Since
age-related ventricular enlargement is presumed to occur as a result of shrinkage of periventricular brain matter, our results are also consistent with other studies that
found no effect of sex on the age-related volume loss of
structures that form the borders of the lateral ventricles
(ie, the caudate nuclei)12,25 or the third ventricle (ie, the
thalamus).25 In contrast, Grant et al9 reported that men,
but not women, exhibited a significant age-related increase in lateral ventricular volume, although this apparent sex difference was not tested. Likewise, Blatter et
al23observed higher correlations in men than women between age and lateral ventricle volume (adjusted for IV)
(r=0.444 vs r=0.218, respectively) and between age and
third ventricle volume (adjusted for IV) (r=0.634 vs
r=0.406, respectively), but again these correlations were
not statistically compared. Kaye et al17 reported that the
precipitous age-related increases in lateral ventricular volume began about a decade earlier in men than women.
Finally, Murphy et al25 found that women actually had a
greater age-related increase in the ratio of third ventricle volume to IV than did men.
Our analysis of left − right difference in lateral ventricle volume showed no age effect (neither main effect
nor interaction with sex). Similar negative findings were
noted by Murphy et al25 for the ratio of right − left lateral ventricle volume to IV. However, Gur et al14 found
that the ratio of ventricular CSF volume to IV was more
pronounced in the left hemisphere than in the right, a
difference they attributed primarily to elderly men.
In summary, brain morphologic characteristics in
humans appear to be sensitive to the effects of both age
and sex, and converging data suggest that these 2 variables may interact over the life span to influence brain
size. These data should provide a useful context within
which to interpret changes in regional brain structure associated with “abnormal” aging. The neurobiological bases
for these sex differences in brain aging are not known.
Neuroendocrinological differences between sexes have
been proposed as a possible explanation given that gonadal corticosteroids affect brain development and aging, and that age affects both the function and regional
distribution of androgen and estrogen systems in the
brain.22 Still, most studies of human brain aging at the
cellular level have not examined sex effects.
The behavioral effects in humans of these sex differences in brain aging are likewise unknown. These findings may provide a neuroanatomical substrate for the sexually dimorphic effects of age on cerebral blood flow and
metabolism,3-5,8 and it is possible that sex differences in
brain aging could interact with a superimposed pathological process to produce sex differences in brain disorders such as Alzheimer disease.25 In this regard, sex differences in brain aging are consistent with observed sex
differences in some aspects of cognitive aging.42-44 Correlative neuropsychological investigations are currently
under way in our laboratory to determine the potential
functional significance of differences between the sexes
in brain aging.
Accepted for publication July 21, 1997.
This study was supported in part by the AlleghenySinger Research Institute, Pittsburgh, Pa, the Mental Illness Research Association, Detroit, Mich, and the National
Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md (grant MH 46643).
We acknowledge the assistance of Theresa Cunningham, Lorrie Cain, Mike Dormnod, Sandy Giconi, Bonnie
Lind, Linda Wilkins, and John Yee, MA.
Reprints: C. Edward Coffey, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Henry Ford Health System, 1 Ford Pl, Detroit, MI
48202 (e-mail:
[email protected]).
REFERENCES
1. Powers RE. Neurobiology of aging. In: Coffey CE, Cummings JL, eds. Textbook
of Geriatric Neuropsychiatry. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press Inc;
1994:35-69.
2. Coffey CE. Anatomic imaging of the aging human brain: computed tomography
and magnetic resonance imaging. In: Coffey CE, Cummings JL, eds. Textbook
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
178
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
23.
of Geriatric Neuropsychiatry. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press
Inc; 1994:159-194.
Gur RC, Gur RE, Obrist W, Skolnick B, Reivich M. Age and regional cerebral blood
flow at rest and during cognitive activation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:617621.
Gur RE, Gur RC. Gender differences in regional cerebral blood flow. Schizophr
Bull. 1990;16:247-254.
Rodriguez G, Warkentin S, Risberg J, Rosadini G. Sex differences in regional blood
flow. J Cereb Blow Flow Metab. 1988;8:783-789.
Schlaepfer TE, Harris GJ, Tien AY, et al. Structural differences in the cerebral
cortex of healthy female and male subjects: a magnetic resonance imaging study.
Psychiatry Res. 1995;61:129-135.
Witelson SF. Cognitive neuroanatomy: a new era. Neurology. 1992;42:709-713.
Shaw T, Moriel K, Meyer J, et al. Cerebral blood flow changes in benign aging
and cerebrovascular disease. Neurology. 1984;34:855-862.
Grant R, Condon B, Lawrence A, et al. Human cranial CSF volumes measured by
MRI: sex and age influences. Magn Reson Imaging. 1987;5:465-468.
Condon B, Grant R, Hadley D, Lawrence A. Brain and intracranial cavity volumes: in vivo determination by MRI. Acta Neurol Scand. 1988;78:387-393.
Yoshi F, Barker WW, Chang JY, et al. Sensitivity of cerebral glucose metabolism
to age, gender, brain volume, brain atrophy and cerebrovascular risk factors.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1988;8:654-661.
Krishnan KR, McDonald WM, Doraiswamy PM, et al. In vivo stereological assessment of caudate volume in man: effect of normal aging. Life Sci. 1990;47:
1325-1329.
Doraiswamy PM, Figiel GS, Husain MM, et al. Aging of the human corpus callosum: magnetic resonance in normal volunteers. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1991;3:392-397.
Gur RC, Mozley PD, Resnick S, et al. Gender differences in age effect on brain
atrophy measured by magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
1991;88:2845-2849.
McDonald WM, Husain M, Doraiswamy PM, et al. A magnetic resonance image
study of age-related changes in human putamen nuclei. Neuroreport. 1991;2:
41-44.
Coffey CE, Wilkinson WE, Parashos IA, et al. Quantitative cerebral anatomy of
the aging human brain: a cross-sectional study using magnetic resonance imaging. Neurology. 1992;42:527-536.
Kaye JA, DeCarli CD, Luxenberg JS, Rapoport SI. The significance of agerelated enlargement of the cerebral ventricles in healthy men and women measured by quantitative computed x-ray tomography. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40:
225-231.
Golomb J, deLeon MJ, Kluger A, et al. Hippocampal atrophy in normal aging:
an association with recent memory impairment. Arch Neurol. 1993;50:967973.
Raz N, Torres IJ, Spencer WD, Acker JD. Pathoclysis in aging human cerebral
cortex: evidence from in vivo MRI morphometry. Psychobiology. 1993;21:151160.
Sullivan EV, Shear PK, Mathalon DH, et al. Greater abnormalities of brain cerebrospinal fluid volumes in younger than in older patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1993;50:359-373.
Christiansen P, Larsson HBW, Thomsen C, et al. Age dependent white matter
lesions and brain volume changes in healthy volunteers. Acta Radiol. 1994;35:
117-122.
Cowell PE, Turetsky BI, Gur RC, et al. Sex differences in aging of the human frontal and temporal lobes. J Neurosci. 1994;14:4748-4756.
Blatter DD, Bigler ED, Gale SD, et al. Quantitative volumetric analysis of brain
MR: normative database spanning 5 decades of life. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol.
1995;16:241-251.
24. Parashos IA, Wilkinson WE, Coffey CE. Magnetic resonance imaging of the corpus callosum: predictors of size in normal adults. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1995;7:35-41.
25. Murphy DGM, DeCarli C, McIntosh AR, et al. Sex differences in human brain morphometry and metabolism: an in vivo quantitative magnetic resonance imaging
and positron emission tomography study on the effect of aging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:585-594.
26. Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D, et al. Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex
observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb
Cortex. 1997;7:268-282.
27. Yue NC, Arnold AM, Longstreth WT, et al. Sulcal, ventricular, and white matter
changes at MR imaging in the aging brain: data from the Cardiovascular Health
Study. Neuroradiology. 1997;202:33-39.
28 Fried LP, Borhani NO, Enright P, et al. The Cardiovascular Health Study: design
and rationale. Ann Epidemiol. 1991;3:263-276.
29. Longstreth WT, Manolio TA, Arnold A, et al. Clinical correlates of white matter
findings on cranial mangetic resonance imaging of 3301 elderly people. Stroke.
1996;27:1274-1282.
30. Tell GS, Fried LP, Hermanson B, et al. Recruitment of adults 65 years and older
as participants in the Cardiovascular Health Study. Ann Epidemiol. 1993;3:311317.
31. Newcombe FG, Ratcliff GG, Carrivick PJ, et al. Hand preference and IQ in a group
of Oxfordshire villages. Ann Hum Biol. 1975;2:235-242.
32. Yue NC, Longstreth WT Jr, Elster AD, et al. Clinically serious abnormalities found
incidentally at MR imaging of the brain: data from the Cardiovascular Health Study.
Radiology. 1997;202:41-46.
33. Bryan RN, Wells SW, Miller TJ, et al. Infarctlike lesions in the brain: prevalence
and anatomic characteristics at MR imaging of the elderly—data from the Cardiovascular Health Study. Radiology. 1997;202:47-54.
34. Manolio TA, Kronmal RA, Burke GL, et al. Magnetic resonance abnormalities and
cardiovascular disease in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Stroke.
1994;25:318-327.
35. Jack CR, Gehring DC, Sharbrough FW, et al. Temporal lobe measurement from
MR images: accuracy and left-right asymmetry in normal persons. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1988;12:21-29.
36. DeArmand SJ, Fusce MM, Dewey MM. Structure of the Human Brain. 2nd ed.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1976.
37. Daniels DL, Haughton VM, Naidich TP. Cranial and Spinal MRI: An Atlas and Guide.
New York, NY: Raven Press; 1987.
38. Pearlson GD, Rabins PV, Kim WS, et al. Structural brain CT changes and cognitive deficits in elderly depressives with and without reversible dementia (pseudodementia). Psychol Bull. 1989;3:573-584.
39. Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Human aging: usual and successful. Science. 1987;237:
143-149.
40. Salerno JA, Murphy DGM, Horwitz B, et al. Brain atrophy in hypertension: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Hypertension. 1992;20:340-348.
41. Schmidt R, Fazekas F, Koch M, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging cerebral abnormalities and neuropsychologic test performance in elderly hypertensive subjects. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:905-910.
42. Zelinski EM, Gilewski MJ, Schaie KW. Individual differences in cross-sectional
and 3-year longitudinal memory performance across the adult life span. Psychol Aging. 1993;8:176-186.
43. West RL, Crook TH, Barron KL. Everyday memory performances across the life
span: effects of age and noncognitive individual differences. Psychol Aging. 1992;
7:72-82.
44. Wiederholt WC, Cahn D, Butters N, et al. Effects of age, gender and education on
selected neuropsychological tests in an elderly community cohort. J Am Gerontol Soc. 1993;41:639-647.
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, FEB 1998
179
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
20. Alpherts WCJ, Aldenkamp AP. Computerized neuropsychological assessment of
cognitive functioning in children with epilepsy. Epilepsia. 1990;31:S35-S40.
21. Hänninen H, Lindström K. Behavioral Test Battery for Toxic Psychological Studies. Helsinki, Finland; Institute of Occupational Health; 1979.
22. Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman RA. MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer’s dementia and normal aging. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1987;8:421-426.
23. Könönen M, Partanen JV. Blocking of EEG alpha activity during visual performance in healthy adults: a quantitative study. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993;84:164-166.
24. Gasser T, Bächer P, Möcks J. Transformations towards the normal distribution
of broad band spectral parameters of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1981;53:119-124.
25. Norman G, Streiner D. Biostatistics: The Bare Essentials. St Louis, Mo: Mosby–
Year Book Inc; 1994:66-67.
26. Newman MF, Wolman R, Kanchuger M, et al. Multicenter preoperative stroke
risk index for patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Circulation. 1996;94(suppl 2):II-74–II-80.
27. Savageau JA, Stanton BA, Jenkins CD, Klein MD. Neuropsychological dysfunction
following elective cardiac operation. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg. 1982;84:585-594.
28. Moody DM. Is there a role for MR in the development of safer cardiac surgery?
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1996;17:213-215.
29. Libman RB, Wirkowski E, Neystat M, Barr W, Gelb S, Graver M. Stroke associated with cardiac surgery: determinants, timing, and stroke subtypes. Arch Neurol. 1996;54:83-87.
30. Moody DM, Bell MA, Challa VR, Johnston WE, Prough DS. Brain microemboli
during cardiac surgery or aortography. Ann Neurol. 1990;28:477-486.
31. Gerraty RP, Bowser DN, Infeld B, Mitchell PJ, Davis SM. Microemboli during carotid angiography: association with stroke risk factors or subsequent magnetic
resonance imaging changes? Stroke. 1996;27:1543-1547.
32. Newman S. The incidence and nature of neuropsychological morbidity following cardiac surgery. Perfusion. 1989;4:93-100.
33. Vingerhoets G, Jannes C, De Soete G, Van Nooten G. Prospective evaluation of
verbal memory performance after cardiopulmonary bypass surgery. J Clin Exp
Neuropsychol. 1996;18:187-196.
34. Vingerhoets G, De Soete G, Jannes C. Relationship between emotional variables
and cognitive test performance before and after open-heart surgery. Clin Neuropsychol. 1995;9:198-202.
35. Schmidt R, Fazekas F, Offenbacher H, et al. Brain magnetic resonance imaging
in coronary artery bypass grafts: a pre- and postoperative assessment. Neurology. 1993;43:775-778.
36. Toner I, Peden CJ, Hamid SK, Newman S, Taylor KM, Smith PLC. Magnetic
resonance imaging and neuropsychological changes after coronary artery
bypass graft surgery: preliminary findings. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol.1994;6:
163-169.
37. Simonson TM, Yuh WTC, Hindman BJ, Embrey RP, Halloran JI, Behrendt DM.
Contrast MR of the brain after high-perfusion cardiopulmonary bypass. AJNR
Am J Neuroradiol. 1994;15:3-7.
38. Moody DM, Brown WR, Challa VR, Stump DA, Reboussin DM, Legault C. Brain
microemboli associated with cardiopulmonary bypass: a histologic and magnetic resonance imaging study. Ann Thorac Surg. 1995;59:1304-1307.
39. Vanninen E, Vanninen R, Äikiä M, Tulla H, Könönen M, Koivisto K, Partanen J,
Partanen K, Hippeläinen M, Kuikka J. Frequency of carotid endarterectomyrelated subclinical cerebral complications. Cerebrovasc Dis. 1996;6:272-280.
40. Gilman S. Cerebral disorders after open-heart operations. N Engl J Med. 1965;
272:489-498.
41. Mills SA. Cerebral injury and cardiac operations. Ann Thorac Surg. 1993;56
(suppl):S86-S91.
42. Moody DM. A new role for radiologists in the development of cardiac surgery.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1991;12:815-816.
Correction
Error in Text. In the article entitled “Sex Differences in Brain Aging: A Quantitative Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study,” published in the February issue
of the ARCHIVES (1998;55:169-179), the coefficient for increased age associated with increased volumes of the lateral ventricles was incorrectly stated on
page 172 in the paragraph subtitled “Age Main Effects,” last sentence. The sentence should have read as follows: “Increased age was also associated with increased volumes of the lateral ventricles (coefficient=0.88, P,.001) and the
third ventricle (coefficient=0.05, P,.001).”
ARCH NEUROL / VOL 55, MAY 1998
627
©1998 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.