Elementary Girls’ Science
Reading at Home and School
DANIELLE J. FORD, NANCY W. BRICKHOUSE, PAMELA LOTTERO-PERDUE,
JULIE KITTLESON
School of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA
Received 21 May 2004; revised 18 May 2005; accepted 15 July 2005
DOI 10.1002/sce.20139
Published online 7 February 2006 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).
ABSTRACT: Although reading is a critical part of science and science learning, it is no
longer a part of many children’s elementary science instruction. This is of concern because
girls often develop strong identities as readers, but do not develop scientific identities
with ease. In this study, we investigate girls’ science reading to know (1) if science books
were available to girls in homes and classrooms, (2) if girls were choosing to read them,
and (3) what influences their choices. Forty-five third-grade girls, 29 of their families,
and three of their teachers were interviewed to ascertain girls’ preferences among various
book genres, as well as to learn the ways in which families and teachers influence the
choices girls make. We found that girls had access to science books at school, and teachers
had strategies to encourage reading them. At home, parents encouraged reading, but were
generally less directive than teachers as to what the girls read, and underestimated their
daughters’ science-related interests. The families studied rely largely on major bookstores
as their primary source of books. Our findings suggest we need to understand better the
way gender influences girls’ engagement with science in a variety of contexts, particularly
C 2006 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Sci Ed 90:270 – 288, 2006
those in which girls exercise choice.
INTRODUCTION
Reading is an important part of science, and an important part of science learning (Norris
& Phillips, 2003; Yore, Bisanz, & Hand, 2003). Outside of school, people are far more likely
to engage in science by reading/writing/listening/talking about it than they are by engaging
in first-hand scientific investigation. The assumption that either scientists or science learners
can learn all science first-hand is an empiricist notion that distorts the role of theory in science
(Brickhouse et al., 2004). The nature of the evidence used in scientific inquiry is driven by
the models and theories scientists use. These scientific ideas are not embedded in nature to
be discovered; they are cultural accomplishments that transcend accumulated data and can
Correspondence to: Danielle J. Ford; e-mail:
[email protected]
Contract grant sponsor: National Science Foundation.
Contract grant number: HRD 0217144.
The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National
Science Foundation.
C
2006 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
271
only be engaged in by students via language (Driver et al., 1994). As Norris and Phillips
(2003) persuasively argued, science could not exist without a written language that could be
used to record and disseminate ideas in ways that to allow them to be tested, elaborated, and
at times refuted. While language-based activities such as talking have often been considered
critical in science learning, the place of written texts has been far less explored.
Too often people have associated reading in science with the use of poor quality textbooks.
Use of such books leads to reading for superficial factual content with little explanation or
understanding (Kesidou & Roseman, 2002), much less critical analysis. Textbooks typically
rely on an expository format and may not be used in ways that encourage the kinds of
dispositions and skills of inquiry that are essential to scientific literacy (Kesidou & Roseman,
2002; Newport, 1990).
Educators and writers of curriculum materials offer inquiry as an antidote to textbookbased instruction, which can result in science instruction that excludes written texts. While
models of inquiry strongly advocate the use of written text resources as a component
of scientific investigation (NRC, 1996; Schwab, 1978), classroom practice that relies on
commercially available science units may be lacking in opportunities for quality science
reading experiences for all learners (Brickhouse et al., 2004). While this state of affairs may
be harmful to all learners, it is perhaps particularly harmful for learners who do not read
science texts by choice.
The omission of written texts in science instruction may be particularly detrimental to
girls. Throughout their school lives, girls are consistently strong readers, and outperform
boys on measures of reading achievement (AAUW/NEA, 1998; Hedges & Nowell, 1995;
Young & Brozo, 2001). Without the inclusion of reading in activity-based science, classroom
science instruction may be lacking an important entry point for girls’ engagement with
scientific ideas.
The introduction of girl friendly written text experiences into inquiry-based science,
however, is not straightforward. Although girls are avid readers, their preferences in reading
consistently are for fiction (Barrs & Pidgeon, 1994). Why do girls choose not to read science
books? One reason could be simply a lack of exposure to science books. Most literacy
experiences throughout school, but especially in the early grades, are fiction experiences
(Barrs & Pidgeon, 1994; Duke, 2000; Flood & Lapp, 1990; Hedges & Nowell, 1995). As
those experiences are determined by elementary teachers (who are predominately White
women), girls’ preferences for fiction may be part of a gendered identity modeled after the
preferences of their mothers and mostly female teachers.
Another reason girls do not read informational science books may be that they are under
the impression that all science books are similar to textbooks. If the few science books
that girls are encountering are in the style of textbooks, they may find the idea of reading
science books unappealing. There are other science books, including science trade books,
that encompass a wide range of literary genres, content emphases, and writing styles.
In this study, we define science books broadly, including books with science themes that
span a range of genres and content areas. We use the classification scheme developed by
Ford (2004) to illustrate the broad categories. These include the numerous informational
books that include established scientific facts, but also books that include collections of
science experiments, books about geography and travel, books that profile scientists or the
history of science, artistic and poetic books that model nature appreciation, and fiction
books with science, nature, or animal themes. Some of these science books are much closer
to the fiction and narrative genres and life science topics reported as preferred by girls
(Adamson et al., 1998; Andre et al., 1999), including hybrid books that mix narrative and
expository genres (Smolkin & Donovan, 2004). Distinctions among these types of science
books are often lost in studies of girls’ book preferences that have primarily reported on
272
FORD ET AL.
girls’ opinions about the singular category of “science” books. A deeper investigation into
girls’ reading, with special focus on their science reading and preferences, and the ways in
which they define which books fall under the category of science, may provide insights into
the ways in which these preferences shape and are shaped by gendered reading identities
in science.
GENDERED IDENTITIES IN SCIENCE AND LITERACY
Girls’ reading in science should be understood in light of the fact that both reading and
science are gendered discourses. By discourses we mean ways of thinking, valuing, talking,
and acting that are used by communities in their activities (Gee, 1999). The way that girls
read (or do not read) and the way that they engage in science (or choose not to) are discourses
that are used by girls in forming identities, that is, in gaining the competencies that they need
in order for others to recognize them as being a particular kind of girl, belonging to certain
communities. Girls have multiple identities that they may take on in different contexts in
order to be a part of desired groups (Brickhouse, 2001).
Girls, particularly young girls, often construct identities as strong readers and writers.
Although there are many possible reasons why girls successfully construct identities as
strong readers, it seems important to understand the ways in which their successes are
consistent with the successes of their mothers and female teachers. Reading is a powerful
way in which girls become young women (Cherland, 1994). Girls and women often find
reading a pleasure. This pleasure often comes from the ways in which literature allows them
to relate to fictional characters and to understand how their lives are experienced. Girls, like
women, often cite strong preferences for reading fiction (Barrs & Pidgeon, 1994). They see
this preference modeled for them by their mothers and (mostly female) teachers (Cherland,
1994). And of course, the gendered activity of attending to the social and emotional needs
of others throughout the day is an activity that girls gain competence in at very young ages
(Dyson, 1997; Thorne, 1993).
Girls do not construct scientific identities with such ease (Brickhouse, 2001). Scientific
discourses are stereotyped as masculine, typically requiring values that are at odds with
traditional feminine values. Although young girls often do well in science in the early
grades, as they mature they often separate themselves from the sciences, particularly the
“hard” sciences such as physical science, computer science, and engineering. Girls’ loss of
interest in the sciences can be seen in the preferences of girls in their reading selections. They
may love reading, and they may love school science, but they do not typically choose to read
science. Science books are more typically regarded as “boys’ books” (Dutro, 2001– 2002).
Thus, the conflicting discourses of science and literacy led us to wonder about what
kinds of contexts may facilitate girls’ reading of science books. We are interested in how
classroom and home contexts shape girls’ reading of science books. Do the (mostly female)
models of reading that girls see in these contexts influence their own identity construction
regarding the value of science books?
We also wondered whether certain kinds of science books may be more congruent with
young girls’ gendered identities. For example, much expository text reinforces masculine
scientific values because information is presented in a style that makes it appear as though
science is very much separate from social/emotional influences. Thus, we were interested
if girls would show interest in other genres, such as informational-narrative texts, where
scientific information is presented in narrative formats, or hybrid texts, with distinct but
related narrative and expository sections.
In this study, we report on our investigation into 8 – 9-year-old girls’ science reading. We
wanted to find out (1) if science books were available to children in homes and classrooms,
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
273
(2) if girls were choosing to read the books, and (3) what influences whether or not they
read science books.
METHOD
Participants
Participants in this study are 45 third-grade girls and the family members of 29 of the girls
from 6 classrooms in a school district in the Eastern United States. Four of the classroom
teachers were participants in our study on literacy and science funded by the National Science Foundation. We invited all girls in the four classrooms to participate, and interviewed
all girls and their families who consented to be a part of this study. To obtain additional
family interviews, we solicited participants from two additional third-grade classrooms in
the same school district, and interviewed both the girls and their families. We thanked the
families for their time by giving their daughters a free science book. No classroom data
were collected from these additional classrooms.
The three schools that the girls attend serve students from suburban and urban communities. Students who live in the urban communities are bussed to the schools that are located in
the suburban areas. In these classrooms, the girls are 64% White, 24% African American, 8%
Asian/Asian American, 2% Latina, and 2% multiethnic (White/Asian American). The subset of girls whose parents were interviewed was more homogeneous than the schools from
which they are drawn. The girls whose families were interviewed were 74% White, 11%
African American, 9% Asian/Asian American, 3% Latina, and 3% multiethnic (White/Asian
American) and from suburban communities. We offered to interview families at their homes,
their child’s school, or the local McDonald’s. About half chose to be interviewed at the
school. The other half chose to be interviewed at home.
We were unable to obtain permission to interview many of the families, but especially
from the urban African American girls. Although we obviously did not have access to these
families to ask why they did not consent to the interview, we surmise that since the request
came to parents from the school, our project was perceived as part of a school that is not
integrally a part of their home communities. For prior research, we have been effective
in soliciting the participation of middle school African American girls because the girls
wanted to be a part of the project and lobbied their parents to sign the consent form. Third
graders may not have lobbied as hard or cared as much as middle schoolers. Also, the offer
for a free book may not have been seen as enough of an incentive for the hassle of arranging
time to talk to a researcher.
Data Sources
Student Interviews. The girls were interviewed individually about their preferences for
reading, and their views on science and science books in school (see Table 1 for interview
topics). Interviews were conducted in the schools, audiotaped, and lasted on average 50 min.
The interview consisted of three sections. In the first section, the girls were asked if they liked
or did not like 14 genres of books (e.g., animal books, joke books, history books, fiction),
and to provide examples of books they had read recently in the genres they liked. This
discussion lasted approximately 15 min. The second section involved questions about home
and school reading, asking girls to characterize the types and amount of reading conducted
in the home and school environments. This part of the interview lasted approximately
10 min. In the final segment of the interview, a read-aloud with the girls was conducted.
In this segment of the interview, we wished to examine girls’ preferences among different
274
FORD ET AL.
TABLE 1
Overview of the Student Interview Protocol
Interview Topic
Selected Protocol Questions
General book preferences,
genre preferences
What books do you like to read?
Do you like to read [list genres]?
Are there some books you don’t like reading?
What books do your friends read?
Do you like to read books at home? Does someone
at home read to you?
What kind of books do you read in school?
Where do you get the books you read in school?
Why do you think your teacher has you read these books?
Introduce four books
Child picks order to read
After all four are read
– which would you like to read most?
– which would your teacher want you to read?
– which would your teacher enjoy most?
Reading influences
Read aloud
subgenres (Ford, 2004) of science books. We chose four books on the same science topic
that represented four different subgenres: informational-narrative, fiction, informationalexpository, and experiment. We had the girls choose the order in which they wanted to read
from the books, then read each and discussed the girls’ reactions to the books. Follow-up
questions on their preferences were asked (see Table 1).
Family Interviews. Twenty-nine of these girls and their families participated in a second
interview (see Table 2), conducted in either the girls’ homes or at the girls’ schools. These
interviews lasted on average 40 min and were audiotaped. Present at the interviews were
the interviewer (a member of the research team), the third-grade girl, and one of her parents/guardians (occasionally both). Siblings were frequently present in the house, but were
not formally interviewed. In these interviews, major topics included the parents’ perspectives on their daughter’s reading habits and choices, the ways in which the family obtains
books, and the reading habits and interests of the other family members, such as parents
and siblings.
Teacher and Classroom Information. Additional information on school science read-
ing was provided the four classroom teachers who were engaged in a 3-year National
Science Foundation funded project on girls and science literacy. The study took place in
what could be considered exemplary settings, as science reading was the central focus of
the study. The teachers in this study were not typical. They had extensive experience and
interest in issues of literacy and science. Three of the four teachers have over 25 years of
experience, two hold National Board Certification, and one holds an Ed.D. in curriculum
and instruction, with a specialization in literacy. Though the teachers did not make any
changes to their science instruction during data collection, their awareness of the goals
of the study may have had an impact. Their classrooms were likely more science literacy
oriented than typical elementary classrooms (cf., Duke, 2000). During focused planning
discussions, the teachers examined their own practices to determine the ways in which they
encouraged girls to read science books. The teachers were also interviewed as a part of the
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
275
TABLE 2
Overview of the Family Interview Protocol
Interview Topic
Protocol Questions
Daughter’s home reading habits as
reported by parent/guardian
Does your daughter have a lot of free time to read?
When she reads, what kinds of books does
she read?
Does she usually read independently or does she
read with you or someone else in your home?
Are there some books that your daughter likes
that you do not?
Are there books that you love that you can’t
get your daughter interested in?
Where do you (or your daughter) get the books
she reads?
Who chooses the books?
Do you have time to read for pleasure?
If so, what do you read?
Do you recall reading for fun as a child? If so,
what do you remember reading?
Do you have a job in addition to parenting?
If so, do you read for work? If so, what do you
read for work?
Are there other children living in the home?
Do they enjoy reading for pleasure?
Do they read similar books as your daughter?
Obtaining books
Parent/guardian reading habits
Sibling reading habits
project to discuss their girls’ reading and science interests. The interviews lasted between
45 and 120 min. All interviews were conducted by a member of the research team and
audiotaped. The classrooms of these four teachers were observed a total of 25 times during
both science and literacy instruction. Field notes from these observations were a source of
information about science books in the classroom and school library.
Analysis
The interviews were transcribed, then analyzed using an iterative process (Cresswell,
1997) in which broad themes on the availability of text, direct and indirect influences on
girls’ reading choices, and preferences for reading by girls and their family members were
refined through close reading and re-reading of the interviews, and revision of subthemes
found in the data.
The initial coding of interviews was driven by the structure of the interviews as well as
impressions recorded in notes by the interviewers. In developing the theme of “influence,” for
example, we started with the broad categories of people and resources mentioned in relation
to reading, combined with interviewers’ notes on the potential importance in particular of
brothers and mothers in determining reading practices. Further reading of the interview data
necessitated the restructuring of the coding system into influence and availability, as both
were seen as constructing the underlying relationships between people/resources and the
girls’ reading practices. In this way, we further refined our subthemes. These themes and
subthemes are represented in Table 3. We split the data into home and school interviews;
pairs of researchers analyzed and developed major themes for each set of interviews. In
276
FORD ET AL.
TABLE 3
Emergent Themes
Theme
Subthemes
Informants
Access/availability of books
Availability of science books in home
and school
Sources of books
Reasons for reading science books
in school/at home
Interest in science books
Subgenre preferences
People/activities that influence book
reading, science book reading
Girls
Teachers
Parents
Girls
Parents
Preference/interest
Influences
Parents
Teachers
addition, each pair recorded questions to ask of the other data set in order to identify themes
that crossed the student and family data. The four researchers met to discuss all themes,
reconcile discrepant information, and check hypotheses with the other data set.
After major themes were developed, two researchers (one from each analysis pair) identified two data sources to illustrate themes. First, quotes from participants were selected
that were representative of the themes in the paper, that showed the range of responses to
various questions, or that illustrated exceptions to the general trends. Second, points within
the data that could provide quantitative support for the themes developed through iterative
readings were also identified. These points in the interviews included counts of the following responses: student and parent book sources, students’ stated book genre preferences,
parents’ reports of their daughters reading interests, and parents’ reports of their reading
interests. The two researchers tallied the responses for each of these topics to obtain the
data reported in Tables 4 – 8.
RESULTS
In this section, we discuss three major themes in girls’ science reading found across
our data sources: the availability of science books in girls’ home and school environments,
the preferences girls exhibit for books in general, and science books in particular, and the
various influences on girls’ reading choices.
Availability of Science Books
Our first question was directed toward understanding if girls have access to science books
in their day-to-day experiences. We found that science books are present in these girls’ lives
in and out of school, with varying degrees of accessibility.
School. All the girls in the study had access to science books at school, both in their
classrooms and in their school libraries. Science books available at school included: (1)
books included in teacher-organized tubs, bins, or baskets that children accessed for selfselected reading (SSR), including books linked to the science unit being taught and other
science books; (2) prominent displays of science books on a shelf, table, or chalkboard,
generally directly relevant to science instruction; and (3) school libraries, where science
books could be found on three to four sections of bookcases.
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
277
The teachers reported concerted effort to get students to read a variety of genres, including
science books. Their suggestions for supporting reading as a part of children’s science
learning focused prominently on integration of science themes into literacy activities, as the
demands on their curriculum coverage, particularly in the areas measured by high stakes
testing (i.e., reading, writing, mathematics), dominate their everyday teaching concerns. The
primary method of integration across reading and science was to include science-themed
nonfiction in the book selections available to students during free-choice reading times.
All six teachers dedicated a portion of their day (generally 30 min) to self-selected reading,
during which children read silently to themselves. During SSR, the children were allowed
restricted choices of reading materials. If students brought a book from home, they could
read that book. However, we noted from field observations that the majority of students
chose instead to read books from “tubs” or “bins” placed on their desks or in the classroom.
Each of the teachers controlled the selection of books in the bins, rotating them about
every week. Teachers selected approximately 8 – 10 books for each bin, including a range
of fiction and nonfiction genres across a variety of topical themes (e.g., holiday books near
Thanksgiving, human body books during a human body unit, and biographies of prominent
Americans during a related social studies unit). The source of their bin books was their own
trade book collections, obtained through external grants or the teachers’ own funds. These
collections were present in the classrooms, but not directly accessible to children (except
when placed in the bins). Teachers reported that they always had a selection of science
books available in the bins, and this was confirmed by field observations, where we noted
generally one to three science books in each bin for the teachers who distributed bins to
their students, or a selection of science books in a single bin for the teacher who dedicated
each of her bins to different genres.
The teachers’ science instruction used kit-based curricula, for which they had undergone
considerable district-sponsored training. These kits are commercially available and are
driven by first-hand investigations. During science instruction in the four classrooms that
were observed, we noticed isolated uses of books. For example, during a unit on earth
materials (Full Option Science System, 2000), all four study teachers read aloud books
such as Everybody Needs a Rock (Baylor, 1985) to introduce the unit, or books such as
Pebble in My Pocket (Hooper, 1996) as curricular extensions. Additional trade books on
rocks and minerals were placed prominently on the classroom chalk tray or in bins. In
contrast, during a unit on chemical tests (Science and Technology for Children, 2002), no
additional books were used.
The girls’ responses during interviews confirmed that their classroom reading selections
came primarily from their teachers. When asked where they get the books they read in
school, girls cited the bins most often (83%). Other major sources of school-read books
were home (17%), and books borrowed from the school library (15%) (see Table 4).
Home. The girls in the study whose families we interviewed had access to many different
kinds of books at home. Where do books in the girls’ homes come from? Parents reported
overwhelmingly that they get the books their daughters read from large bookstores (90%).
Other important sources include libraries (52% reported public library, 24% reported school
library, and 17% reported library without specifying) and schoolbook orders (24%) or book
fairs (7%). There was overlap in the categories; 16 families cited both bookstores and
libraries as their sources, 7 families reported bookstores as their sole source of books,
and 5 families reported libraries as their sole source of books. The availability of science
books within stores and libraries, however, varies widely. To better understand the range of
science books available at these resources, we visited the two local bookstores frequently
278
FORD ET AL.
TABLE 4
Sources for Girls’ Books. Reported in Percentages of Informants Who
Mentioned the Source
Source
School Reading
(Reported by Girls)
Home Reading
(Reported by Parents)
4
6
15
0
0
0
17
83
90
52
24
17
24
7
n/a
0
Book stores
Public library
School library
Library, unspecified
Book orders
Book fairs
Home
Teacher provided (bins)
mentioned by girls and their parents and a local public library to assess the types of science
books available (see Table 5). We counted the numbers of books in each store and the library,
dividing them into general science content areas.
In all three locations, life science topics dominate the collections. For the two bookstores,
the collections are even higher in life science titles, at the expense of physical and or/earth
science titles. The bookstores have fewer science books than libraries, the books are all only
recent or still in print publications, and they place their science books off in corners in the
children’s sections with nonstandard organization (in contrast to the library, which used the
Dewey decimal system). In the Barnes and Noble store, only three shelving sections held
books, and they were labeled “Science/Nature,” “Eyewitness,” and “Dinosaurs.” In Borders,
there were no shelf labels. Category information was found on harder to read book dividers,
including the topics “Science,” “Nature,” “Animals,” “Pets,” “Dinos,” “Transportation,” and
“Children’s Health.” One family expressed frustration at this organization:
I: Where do you get the books?
Carrie’s Mom: Borders and Barnes and Noble. . . I kind of gave her free range to find books
and she didn’t find a whole lot that interested her.
I: Sometimes it’s a little overwhelming in those stores. . . Do you go to a library?
Carrie: Yeah, we used to.
I: Do you find books there at the library?
TABLE 5
Science Books Available at Bookstores and Libraries by Science Content
Area
Science Content
Barnes and Noble
(n = 856)
Borders
(n = 469)
Public Library
(n = 1423)
Life science
Space science
Earth science
Physical science
General science
Technology
72%
10%
5%
1%
4%
8%
63%
10%
10%
2%
12%
3%
57%
11%
6%
4%
6%
15%
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
279
Carrie’s Mom: Actually we do have better luck.
Carrie: It’s easy because it has bigger shelves.
Carrie’s Mom: Much more organized.
In summary, girls do have science books available to read, but the range of books available
depends on what the girls’ teachers provide in the classroom as well as the places from which
their families get books. There were science books available in all of the classrooms, and
in all of the school libraries. Our field notes documented that on library days, children had
plenty of science books to choose from. The 52% of families that reported visits to a public
library would also have good access to science books. For the 90% of families for whom
bookstores were a source of books, there are science books present in the stores, though
they represent a narrower range of topics than found in the school setting, are a smaller
percentage of the book choices available, and are generally harder to access.
Therefore, for all the girls in this study, a range of science books are available to them,
most immediately in school, but also for those who frequent public libraries. Since we know
that girls do have science books within the range of books they are exposed to, we also
wished to see if they were given the opportunity to choose the books they read, which books
they would select.
Girls’ Preferences and Choices
During the student interviews, we asked the girls if they liked or did not like certain
genres of books (from a predetermined list of genres appropriate for third grade), and to
give us an example of a book they liked in the genre. Their responses are shown in Table 6.
We found that these third-grade girls tended to like almost everything we suggested
(at least somewhat). The major exceptions were sports, comic books, and encyclopedias.
Overall, with the exception of animal books (discussed below), the genres most frequently
cited as being liked by girls fall under the general category of fiction (funny stories, magical
stories, jokes, and mysteries). The third-grade girls in this study enjoy humor, as seen in the
TABLE 6
Girls’ Reading Genre Preferences
Genre
Animals
Funny books
Fairy tales/magic
Joke books
Mysteries
Poetry
Magazines
Biographies
Science books
History
Internet
Sports
Comic books
Encyclopedias
Percentage of Girls Reporting
That They “Like” This Genre
88
83
73
71
69
54
50
50
46
44
42
29
25
21
280
FORD ET AL.
numbers for funny books (e.g., Junie B. Jones and Captain Underpants) and joke books.
Fairy tales/magic was a popular category as well, particularly as the second Harry Potter
movie was about to be released when the interviews were conducted. A taste for mysteries
was also present. The girls’ examples within these genres represent popular children’s series
and authors: A Series of Unfortunate Events, Harry Potter, Magic Tree House, American
Girl, and Mary Kate and Ashley were mentioned more than once.
The genre most frequently liked by the girls was animals, a nonfiction genre. Eighty-eight
percent of the girls reported that they liked animal books. An intriguing result from this
preference data is the different perceptions of books about animals and books about science.
We had assumed in the interviews that girls would perceive animals as a subset of science
books. However, they made clear distinctions between the two types of books, as seen in
the strong favoring of animal books and the dislike of science books. While the genre of
“animals” can include fictional stories about anthropomorphized animals, only four of the
girls interpreted the genre in this way. The other 41 were referring to nonfiction books about
animals.
These girls clearly enjoy reading and learning about a variety of animals. While few
girls mentioned titles of books, they all mentioned animal topics that they recently had
been interested in reading. Several girls were interested in reading books about their pets,
especially cats, dogs, and horses. We noticed in field observations that one girl checked out a
stack of hamster books from the school library. Other girls were interested in learning about
wild animals, for example, “Dogs, leopards, cats, frogs, [and] lions. All kinds of lions.”
Twelve of the girls mentioned an interest in animal books even before they were prompted
by genre. We opened the interviews with the question “what kinds of books do you like to
read?” These 12 girls mentioned animal books in their initial responses. One girl declared “I
love animals. . . I have a book about baby animals.” Another girl stated that she also “loves
books about animals. I have tons. . . . I want to be anything [career wise] that includes
animals.” In contrast, science was less frequently cited as a category of books that the girls
liked to read. Only 46% of girls responded that they liked to read science books. They had
trouble naming titles in this category, most often naming specific content topics instead
(e.g., human body, rocks and minerals), or specific instances of reading in science class,
such as read-alouds led by their teacher, as the titles they “liked” to read, rather than books
they had read on their own.
In the family interviews, parents reported girls’ interests in science books as lower than
the girls themselves reported (see Table 7). In these self-reported data, we divided mentions
of animal and science books (the parents did not make this distinction). Twenty-seven
percent of parents reported that their daughters were very interested or interested in animal
books, and 21% of parents reported their daughters were interested in science books. This
TABLE 7
Girls’ Interests in Fiction, Science, and Animal Books, as Reported by Girls
and by Families. Data from the Subset of Girls Whose Families Were Also
Interviewed (n = 29)
Genre
Fiction
Animal
Science
Girls’ Reported Interest (Percentage
of Girls Indicating Interest)
Parents’ Report of Their Daughters’
Interests (Percentage of Girls)
100
86
48
100
27
21
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
281
is in contrast to 100% of parents mentioning that their daughters were interested or very
interested in fiction books.
While these data give us information about the girls’ general genre preferences, we were
also interested to find out if they favored certain science book writing styles and subgenres
over others. The emergence of enjoyment of the Magic Tree House series (fictional stories
about children who investigate science and history topics), as well as the distinction in
children’s thinking between animal and science books, supports our desire to study writing
style and genre. To investigate possible preferences within science subgenres, we conducted
a read-aloud during the student interview, asking the girls to comment on four books on the
same content, but with different subgenres (fiction, informational-narrative, informationalexpository, and experiment). The fiction book, Hill of Fire (Lewis, 1971), told the story
of a boy who witnesses the eruption of a Mexican volcano. Volcano (Branley, 1986),the
informational-expository book, included a passage about plate movements, underwater
volcanoes, and geologists. The informational-narrative text, Dear Katie, the Volcano is a
Girl (George, 1998), contrasted a granddaughter’s mythological and her grandmother’s
geological explanations of volcanic activity in Hawaii. Volcano! (Hadich, 2001) provided
experiments to model volcanic eruptions. After reading a passage from each of the books,
girls said they preferred the informational-narrative book (43%) over the experiment book
(30%), the fiction book (15%) and the informational-expository book (6%). Six percent
had no preference. In contrast, when the girls were asked which books their teachers would
want them to read, they chose the experiment book (33%) and the informational-expository
book (33%) over the informational-narrative book (13%) or the fiction book (11%).
How did the girls justify their choices? While there are many factors involved in book
preferences, the girls’ comments about their preferences reveal their attention to issues of
science ideas as well as story and illustration. One girl, who preferred the informationalnarrative text, explained her choice: “because I think this is mostly learning and fun... I like
them if they are both.” Another said, “I want to find out what happens after the grandma says
volcanoes made Hawaii, and then the girl says Pele made Hawaii.” One girl was interested
in the illustrations of “[the girl] picture in the cloud. I like that.” Students who chose the
experiment book explained that they “like doing experiments” and because “it gives me ideas
for what I can do.” One girl felt the opposite. Because she “doesn’t like doing experiments,”
she ranked this book last. Another girl did not like the experiment book because she felt it
was beneath her reading level: “It doesn’t have much writing on each page and it has a very
big space between lines.” The fictional story appealed to some girls because it was “funny
but also scary” and because “I want to find out what the mystery was.” Others considered
the fictional book too babyish. The least favorite book, the expository text, was unappealing
because it was “kind of boring. . . I already knew all the information in it,” though for a few
students the ideas, if not the text itself, were interesting. One girl was interested in learning
about “facts about the earth and minerals in there and the layers of the earth.”
In summary, the majority of girls in this study enjoy books about animals, while fewer
than half report that they like science books. Of the books read aloud during the interviews,
girls preferred the informational-narrative genre over the books they felt their teachers
would want them to read (informational-expository and experiment books). They chose
those books for both science and literary reasons. Although this study does not control
for the range of factors (e.g., reading level, stereotyped title, attractive illustrations) that
might have influenced the girls’ preference for the informational-narrative book, the girls’
reasons for their preference at least suggest what features in books might be attractive to
girls, and are in alignment with the literature that shows girls prefer fiction or narrative
genres (Cherland, 1994). What other factors might influence girls’ decision to read (or not)
science books?
282
FORD ET AL.
Influences on Girls’ Science Reading
Adult Influences. The students themselves are very aware that their teachers want them
to read, and read well, as expressed by one of the students:
Adrienne: I think that [the teacher] chooses books, specific books for us to read so we can
get the things in our head and so we can get smarter for tests...
While studies have shown that nonfiction texts are infrequently found in the classroom
setting (e.g., Duke, 2000), the teachers in this study exposed students to science books
through inclusion of books in the bins, and through read-alouds and extensions concurrent
with science investigations. They used a variety of techniques to get students, and girls in
particular, interested in reading science books. Ms. Green described the technique she calls
“blessing” of the book: “I read part of a book in class, show enthusiasm for it, then place
it in the tubs for selection. This is particularly influential on the girls, who immediately
want to read the books that I have ‘blessed.”’ These methods are used to expose children to
a variety of genres, including science informational books. However, even when teachers
give girls opportunities to read science books, and make it nearly impossible to avoid the
books altogether, they do not require students to read science books. Any girl who did not
wish to read science books would have been able to avoid them.
Parents were less attuned to genre in their emphasis on the benefits of reading. Several
parents stated that their goal was to encourage their daughters to read. One mother stated:
“as long as she is reading, I am happy.” Most of the parents (86%) allow their daughters’
preferences to guide their choices:
Rama’s father: I mean, it depends on her. Whatever she likes, seeing her interest and I’m
going to encourage her. I don’t want to impose something if I like science . . . whatever I
wanted, I got for her. Mostly like family books and story books. Wherever I go I see stuff I
get for her.
Rama’s father came from a “science family” and was interested in science himself, but
did not want to impose this on his daughter.
Some parents did try to influence their daughters to read some things or avoid others.
Parents reported that they wanted their daughters to read more: informational texts (7%
of parents); classics (14%); or specific fiction titles such as Nancy Drew or the Box Car
Children (21%). One fifth of the parents explicitly reported that they wanted their daughters
to avoid poor quality books (e.g., Power Puff Girls), easy books (e.g., Scooby Doo), or books
with mature themes (e.g., Babysitters Club). Deepah’s mother, who was a science teacher
in India, wanted her daughters to read more nonfiction books, as did Flo’s mother (though
they did not specify science books). A number of mothers wished that their daughters would
read more of the classics, including Black Beauty, Little Women, Amelia Bedelia, The Little
Princess, Little House on the Prairie, and The Secret Garden, titles that the mothers recalled
fondly from their own childhood reading experiences. This was true in the case of Niki, one
of the girls we interviewed who read mostly nonfiction. Her father, who has a doctorate in
engineering, wished that she would read more classics in addition to her voracious appetite
for nonfiction books.
We also asked parents about their own reading interests. Mothers’ reading preferences are
reported in Table 8 (we had few fathers report their reading preferences). Our data support
Cherland’s (1994) findings of women’s preferences for fiction.
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
283
TABLE 8
Mothers’ Reading Interests (Percentages Reported for Each Genre)
Genre
Percent reporting interest (n = 28)
Fiction
Magazine/newspaper
Cookbooks
Internet/email
Work brought home
Nonfiction
Bible
Do not read/no time
79
66
28
24
14
10
3
7
Sibling Influences. Throughout the family interviews, distinctions between what daugh-
ters read and what sons read emerged. For the 14 girls who had brothers (10 had only
brothers), we found that parents/daughters sometimes commented on their sons’/brothers’
interest in science books in contrast to their own interests. One of the girls in the study who
also participated in a summer science book club wrote the following dedication in her book
club journal: “Dedicated to . . . my brother, because I know he would love to read these
books.” This was not an uncommon “spontaneous” suggestion by girls or their parents that
science books would be/are enjoyed or appreciated by brothers. The presence of brothers
for some girls meant that there were science books available in the home to read, as many
of these brothers were interested in science. For example, one mother shared: “Well, we
have a lot of science books around the house, but that’s because that’s what my son enjoys. I don’t know how much the girls pick them up.” During the interview, this daughter
surprised her mother when she shared that she liked her brother’s experiment books. One
mother mentioned purchasing all of the Magic School Bus books for her 14-year-old son.
In a statement about her children’s interests, another mother aligned her interests with her
daughter and in contrast to her sons:
I: Are there some books that your daughter likes that you don’t care for?
Kathy’s mother: I don’t think so. I wouldn’t say the same thing about my boys.
Seventeen of the girls in the study had one or more sisters; 13 of these girls only had sisters.
In homes with only daughters, parents mentioned that their children tended to share books,
and in these cases, they tended not to comment on distinctions between their daughters’
reading interests.
We found two notable exceptions, however, to these trends. One daughter of a professor
of engineering was an avid reader of science books. Her parents described her as the only
one of their three daughters who was particularly interested in science reading. Another girl
read exclusively animal nonfiction or science-themed fiction, declaring in interviews and
proudly in conversations in class that she preferred nonfiction.
DISCUSSION
Our investigation of girls’ science reading at home and school shows that the girls in
this study have access to science books from a variety of sources. They are interested in
certain science books due to life science content, genre, or the information contained within
284
FORD ET AL.
them, and they do not like other science books because of content, expository writing styles,
and/or poor matches to their conceptions of their own reading ability. We found that reading
in a general sense is important to parents and teachers, and that they allow third-grade girls
considerable choice in selection of reading materials to promote and encourage girls to
read. Home is typically a setting in which the educational agenda is much more open than
at school, allowing more freedom for girls to read only what they choose to read. As we
will discuss below, we believe that while the choices we give to independently reading
girls may appear at first glance to be empowering, choice in fact allows culturally gendered
expectations to emerge. The choices that girls make are not merely individual choices so
much as they are expressions of culture and who these girls are as socially and culturally
constructed beings (Riley-Taylor, 2002).
Choice, the Free Market, and Public Libraries
Whenever we give girls choices it is important to consider the larger context in which these
choices are made and the ways in which culture influences those choices. Although there
are science books across the settings in which girls are choosing their reading materials,
these books are not prominent. Science books in the classrooms we visited are a small
percentage of the books present, and this is likely to be a higher amount than typically
found in elementary classrooms (Duke, 2000), due to the teachers’ awareness of the science
focus of our study and their interest in participating in it. Classrooms, however, are in many
ways less problematic than many bookstores.
Imagine a young girl entering a large chain bookstore. There are large signs directing
her to the children’s section. Once there, she easily finds American Girl books or books
about Mary Kate and Ashley Olsen---books clearly written for her. Many young girls would
never move beyond this spot. To leave this space to search for science books would be
a significant act of independence and resistance to adopting the social norms presented
to her.
School and public libraries, in contrast to bookstores and classrooms, include the most
diverse and high quality collections. Science books are more plentiful, of better quality,
more aligned to community needs and resources, and easier to find in the public library
than in bookstores. They are also free. Overall, we were surprised in how these families
underutilized the public library. Though the library was reported as a source for most of the
families, it was in all but three families a source secondary to bookstores.
Although public libraries have the best collections, they can only purchase books that
are produced and thus do not have a completely balanced collection. Choice is limited; the
majority of elementary science books are on life science topics. If a young girl became
interested in a science topic other than animals, she or her parents would need to exert effort
to find books to match that interest (and in some cases, they simply might not find them).
For example, supporting an interest in materials or chemicals with books would be a major
challenge.
The choice to use the public library is constrained by contemporary social/cultural
influences. The families in this study are overwhelmingly middle class, and may have
more money than time. Thus, they frequent large bookstores at least in part because of
convenience---books that are bought do not have to be returned. This purchase of convenience may disadvantage their daughters with regard to science books.
At home, where books are predominately obtained from commercial sources, girls’ reading preferences are largely left to the free marketplace of corporate bookstores that are
driven by profit. Large bookstore chains make decisions about which books to sell at a corporate level, and in many cases sell their shelf space to publishers (Hade, 2002). Although
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
285
the marketplace gives the appearance of freedom, in actuality it is not gender neutral. Books
are written and marketed in ways to appeal to one sex or the other. Balance, quality, and
community needs take a back seat to what sells. While girls may be free to choose whatever is available, this freedom may also make them more subject to behaving in ways that
are consistent with gendered stereotypes. Girls can read the less than subtle signs of the
marketplace and at this age are often more interested in conforming to gender stereotypes
than they are in challenging them. The free market certainly has no particular interest in
empowering girls to challenge gender norms.
Choice and Adult Influences
Girls’ reading practices and interests in part mirror their mothers’ and female teachers’ preferences for fiction (Cherland, 1994). In this study, we also found that the girls’
mothers, their immediate models of adult female reading, expressed their preference for
and enjoyment of fiction. Mothers in this study preferred fiction reading for themselves,
and encouraged (though never pushed) classic fiction reading for their daughters. Given
societal pressures to adopt stereotypically gendered fiction reading identities, there are few
compelling reasons for these girls to adopt a reading practice other than as a reader of
fiction.
Are there ways in which adults can help young girls construct science identities in
opposition to market influences? Our data suggest that there might be. For example, Ms.
Green’s strategy of reading a science book aloud, and then making it available for girls
to read independently seemed to be an effective way of influencing girls’ choices. This
strategy not only provides access to science books, but also provides a role model for the
girls of an adult woman reading science and enjoying it.
Our data also suggest that girls are actually more interested in science books than parents
typically know. We were struck by the discrepancy between the girls’ reporting of their
interest in science and animal books and their parents’ recollections of their daughters’
reading interests. This may indicate that parents’ expectations and assumptions about their
daughters’ interests (coupled with the mothers’ desires to share fiction with their daughters)
make it harder for them to perceive their daughters’ science interests. Or, it may be that it has
not occurred to parents to engage their daughters in science reading. In no case did a mother
report that science reading was a part of her childhood. Nor did any mother report reading
science as an adult. Given the fact that science reading, as opposed to fiction reading, is not
seen as part of the process of enculturation into womanhood, it is hardly surprising that it
is not seen as a significant component of raising young girls.
What the discrepancy may be revealing is a difference between actual reading habits (as
reported by parents) and potential reading interests, as reported by girls when we suggested
genres to them in the interviews. We think this is more an indication that girls at this age
are, for the most part, quite willing to read new things. Therefore, it may be that the window
of opportunity to get girls interested in science reading has in fact not closed by this time,
and that adults are inadvertently abdicating a potentially powerful role in exposing girls to
science reading (and other underexplored genres) to the free market, which then provides
stereotypically gendered choices.
Science and the Multiple Identities of Girls:
Conclusions and Future Directions
Identities are not singular, static characteristics of girls that are invariant across contexts.
Identities are performances that girls try out in order to be perceived by others in ways they
286
FORD ET AL.
find desirable (Brickhouse, 2001). Girls, particularly white, middle class girls, often want
to be perceived as good students and are thus willing to engage in those activities that will
facilitate the construction of this identity (Brickhouse, Schultz, & Lowery, 2000).
While girls, particularly those who have adopted good student identities (synonymous
with good reader in third grade), may readily adopt science reading practices in the classroom, at home they may enact much more conventional girl identities related to the reading
of fiction. In our interactions with girls in this study, we saw aspects of the good student
model at work, which can potentially be used to support girls’ science reading in school,
but can also make it a school activity, not a pleasurable one. Many girls were attuned to the
importance shown to science reading in interviews and in their teachers’ instruction, and eagerly adopted this stance. However, we speculate that if social studies were the focus of our
study, we would have seen the same eagerness among students to adopt social studies reading. We do not think they typically extend these reading behaviors to their home/pleasure
reading unless they are reading about animals or they are the few girls who are exceptions
to the trend. Our understanding of girls’ reading practices in home and school settings leads
us to believe that our focus in supporting girls’ reading of science should be in the encouragement of their science reading in places both inside and outside of science instruction.
The use of written texts in school science instruction must be done carefully, and take into
consideration practices of scientific text use beyond the bounds of school or practitioner
science, or it may serve only to reinforce girls’ construction of their good student identities
(Brickhouse, 2001).
In order to capitalize on girls’ interest in reading and identities as literacy/good learners,
we need to figure out a way to make science reading truly pleasurable and extend beyond
the boundaries of school. To do this we should attend to the features in books that the
girls suggest are important. The results of this study point to the possibility that narrative
may be an important means to engaging girls in science, as the girls in this study seemed
most interested in scientific information that was embedded in an engaging story. However,
this is not a straightforward finding, as our results were complicated by the girls’ sense of
the purpose for reading science books as well as their interest in the way information was
conveyed. As seen in the girls’ reports of what science books they thought their teachers
would want them to read, the school-based setting in which the read-aloud interview was
conducted may have influenced how the girls perceived science reading, shifting their focus
to their notions of reading for school purposes rather than strictly reading for enjoyment.
Their understanding of school science may be intertwined so deeply with their understanding
of reading science that they may not be able to imagine science reading outside of a schoolbased purpose of learning. This may also explain their differentiation between pleasurable
reading of animal books and the less pleasurable reading of science books. Animal books,
which focused on topics such as pets and wild animals that were distinctly not a part of the
school curriculum, are in sharp contrast to their definitions of science books, which referred
nearly directly to content covered in the third-grade curriculum. It may be that science
reading for pleasure may appeal most to girls if it is uncoupled from their perceptions of
school science.
For this reason we have begun new efforts to organize and study girls’ science book
clubs that take place outside of school and are formed around friendship groups (LotteroPerdue & Brickhouse, 2005). Our first book club for young girls was very popular with the
seven girls who participated throughout the summer of 2003. Such a venue not only has the
potential of engaging girls in science reading, but also in facilitating the critical reading of
science texts. We are currently designing and implementing a book club for mothers and
daughters to facilitate and study their ways in which the critical reading of science books
could become more a part of growing up for young girls.
ELEMENTARY GIRLS’ SCIENCE READING
287
REFERENCES
AAUW/NEA. (1998). Gender gaps: Where schools still fail our children. Washington, DC: American Association
of University Women Foundation.
Abimbola, I., & Baba, S. (1996) Misconceptions and alternative conceptions in science textbooks: The role of
teachers as filters. The American Biology Teacher, 58, 14 – 19.
Adamson, L. B., Foster, M. A., Roark, M. L., & Reed, D. B. (1998). Doing a science project: Gender differences
during childhood. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35, 845 – 857.
Andre, T., Whigham, M., Hendrickson, A., & Chambers, S. (1999). Competency beliefs, positive affect, and
gender stereotypes of elementary students and their parents about science versus other school subjects. Journal
of Research in Science Teaching, 36(6), 719 – 747.
Barrs, M., & Pidgeon, S. (1994). Reading the difference: Gender and reading in elementary classrooms. York,
ME: Stenhouse Publishers.
Brickhouse N. W. (2001). Embodying science: A feminist perspective on learning. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching, 38(3), 282 – 295.
Brickhouse, N., Ford, D., Kittleson, J., & Lottero-Perdue, P. (2003). Challenging the empiricist nature of elementary
science instruction with text. In D. Metz (Ed.), Proceedings from the 7th International Conference on the History
and Philosophy of Science in Science Teaching (pp. 180 – 186).
Brickhouse, N., Kittleson, J., & Ford, D. (2004, March). Articulating the roles of oral and written language in
science instruction. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association for Research in Science
Teaching, Vancouver, BC.
Brickhouse, N. W., Lowery, P., & Schultz, K. (2000). What kind of a girl does science? The construction of school
science identities. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(5), 421 – 458.
Cherland, M. R. (1994). Private practices: Girls reading fiction and constructing identity. Bristol, PA: Taylor &
Francis.
Cresswell, J. (1997). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five traditions. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Driver, R., Asoko, H., Leach, J., Mortimer, E., & Scott, P. (1994). Constructing scientific knowledge in the
classroom. Educational Researcher, 23,5 – 12.
Duke, N. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: The scarcity of informational texts in the first grade. Reading Research
Quarterly, 35, 202 – 223.
Dutro, E. (2001 – 2002). ”But that’s a girls’ book!” Exploring gender boundaries in children’s reading practices.
Reading Teacher, 55, 376 – 384.
Dyson, A. H. (1997). Writing superheroes: Contemporary childhood, popular culture, and classroom literacy. New
York: Teachers College Press.
Flood, J., & Lapp, D. (1990). Types of writings included in basal reading programs kindergarten through second
grade: An investigation of changes from 1983 – 1999. In J. Zutell & S. McCormick (Eds.), Literature theory
and research: Analyses from multiple paradigms (pp. 395 – 401). Chicago: National Reading Conference.
Ford, D. (2002). More than just facts: Reviewing science trade books. Horn Book Magazine, 73, 265 – 271.
Ford, D. J. (2004). Highly recommended trade books: Can they be used in inquiry science? In W. Saul (Ed.),
Border crossings: Essays on literacy and science (pp. 277 – 290). Newark, DE: IRA/NSTA Presses.
Full Option Science System (2000). Earth materials teachers guide. Nashua, NH: Delta Education.
Gee, J. P. (1999). An introduction to discourse analysis : Theory and method. London: Routledge.
Hade, D. (2002). Storyselling: Are publishers changing the way children read? The Horn Book Magazine, 78,
509 – 517.
Hedges, L., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high scoring
individuals. Science, 269, 41 – 45.
Heppler, S. (1998). Nonfiction books for children: New directions, new challenges. In R. A. Bamford & J. V.
Kristo (Eds.), Making facts come alive: Choosing quality nonfiction literature K-8 (pp. 3 – 17). Norwood, MA:
Christopher-Gordon.
Kesidou, S., & Roseman, J. E. (2002). How well do middle school science programs measure up? Findings from
Project 2061’s curriculum review. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 522 – 549.
Lottero-Perdue, P. S., & Brickhouse, N. W. (2005). Critical talk about science texts in a book club for young girls.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Dallas,
TX.
Millar, R., & Osborne, J. (Eds.). (1998). Beyond 2000: Science education for the future. London: King’s College.
National Research Council (NRC). (1996). National science education standards. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.
Newport, J. F. (1990). Elementary science texts: What’s wrong with them? The Educational Digest, 56,
68 – 69.
288
FORD ET AL.
Norris, S. P., & Phillips, L. M. (1994). Interpreting pragmatic meaning when reading popular reports of science.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31(9), 947 – 967.
Norris, S. P., & Phillips, L. M. (2003). How literacy in its fundamental sense is central to scientific literacy. Science
Education, 87, 224 – 240.
Riley-Taylor, E (2002). Ecology, spirituality, & education. New York: Peter Lang.
Schwab, J. J. (1978). Science, curriculum, and liberal education: Selected essays. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Science and Technology for Children. (2002). Chemical tests. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.
Smolkin, L., & Donovan, C. (2004). How not to get lost on The Magic School Bus: What makes high science
content read-alouds? In W. Saul (Ed.), Crossing borders in literacy and science instruction (pp. 291 – 313).
Newark, DE: IRA Press.
Thorne, B. J. (1993). Gender play: Girls and boys in school. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Yore, L. D., Bisanz, G. L., & Hand, B. M. (2003). Examining the literacy component of science literacy: 25 years
of language arts and science research. International Journal of Science Education, 25(6), 689 – 726.
Young, J. P., & Brozo, W. G. (1991). Boys will be boys, or will they? Literacy and masculinities. Reading Research
Quarterly, 36(3), 316 – 325.
CHILDREN’S LITERATURE CITED
Baylor, B. (1985). Everybody needs a rock. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Scott Foresman.
Branley, F. M. (1986). Volcanoes. New York: HarperCollins.
George, J. C. (1998). Dear Katie, the volcano is a girl. New York: Hyperion.
Hooper, M. (1996). Pebble in my pocket. New York: Viking Children’s Books.
Lewis, T. P. (1971). Hill of fire. New York: HarperTrophy.
Haduch, B. (2001). Volcano! New York: Dutton Children’s Books.