The Psychological Record, 2008, 58, 623–640
aN eXPeRiMeNTal TesT oF a cogNiTiVe deFusioN
eXeRcise: coPiNg WiTh NegaTiVe aNd
PosiTiVe selF-sTaTeMeNTs
Hilary-Anne Healy, Yvonne Barnes-Holmes,
Dermot Barnes-Holmes, and Claire Keogh
National University of Ireland, Maynooth
Carmen Luciano
University of Almeria, Spain
Kelly Wilson
University of Mississippi
This study investigated the impact of defusion on a nonclinical sample ( n = 60)
in the context of negative (e.g., “I am a bad person”) and positive (e.g., “I am
whole”) self-statements. Participants were assigned to one of three experimental
conditions (Pro-Defusion, Anti-Defusion, and Neutral) that manipulated instructions about the impact of a defusion strategy. Defusion was also manipulated
through the visual presentation of the self-statements, with each presented in
three formats (Normal, Defused, Abnormal). Participants rated each self-statement for comfort, believability, and willingness. Although the instructions did
not affect ratings, negative statements presented in the defused format decreased discomfort and increased willingness and believability relative to the
nondefused statements. The findings suggest using defusion strategies in coping
with negative psychological content.
In acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT: Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson,
1999), an acceptance-based therapeutic regime, clients are encouraged to make
willing contact with aversive psychological content. In order to achieve this
therapeutic aim, ACT interventions are often bolstered with techniques that
facilitate what is commonly referred to as cognitive defusion (Bach & Hayes,
2002; Hayes, Strosahl, et al., 1999). From an ACT perspective, clients are frequently “fused” with painful or negatively evaluated psychological content,
Research for this paper was funded in part by the Irish Research Council for the Humanities
and the Social Sciences, through a Government of Ireland Scholarship. The authors thank two
anonymous reviewers for their help and constructive comments on revisions to the manuscript.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Yvonne Barnes-Holmes,
Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland, Maynooth, Maynooth, Co. Kildare,
Ireland. E-mail:
[email protected]
624
HEALY ET AL.
and defusion strategies create a “de-fused” perspective that permits greater
behavioral flexibility.
Defusion
Defusion is a difficult concept to define in simple language. In clinical
terms, clients are described as being “fused” with their thoughts when they believe that their thoughts say something important about who they are. For example, if they frequently had the thought “I am a horrible person,” they might
easily begin to have additional and more convincing thoughts like “I really am
a horrible person and nobody could ever love me.” ACT clinicians encourage
clients to “defuse” from their negative content by viewing their thoughts as
just thoughts, rather than considering them to have meaning. In this way, defusion involves a change in perspective that permits clients to see themselves
as more than simply the sum of their thoughts and personal evaluations. In
order to achieve this change in perspective, ACT therapists also employ techniques that focus on a process referred to as “self-as-context” (e.g., the Floating
Leaf Exercise). This technique establishes a broader sense of self within which
thoughts and internal events have less importance (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, &
Roche, 2001). In more technical terms, the process of defusion has also been
defined as the disruption of existing verbal functions of language such that
the “ongoing process of framing events relationally is evident in the moment”
(Hayes, Strosahl, et al., 1999, p. 74). This process is thought to result in a breaking down of existing problematic verbal relations, or at least appears to permit
the person to see the relations without having to act in accordance with them.
Some aspects of defusion and self-as-context resemble, or are derived from,
practices within cognitive research such as those that establish a distanced
self-perspective (e.g., Kross, Ayduk, & Mischel, 2005) and those that involve
mindfulness (e.g., Teasdale, Segal, Williams, Ridgeway, Soulsby et al, 2000). But
Titchener’s (1916) rapid word-repetition technique (e.g., “milk, milk, milk”) is
perhaps the most well-known method for facilitating defusion. Within ACT, for
example, clients might be encouraged to repeat negative content words rapidly
(e.g., by saying “stupid, stupid, stupid”). The therapeutic aim of such a strategy
is that during the repetition exercise the semantic functions, or meaning of the
word, will be significantly reduced. Indeed, clients report that toward the end
of the exercise they experienced the word simply as a strange sound (Hayes,
Strosahl, et al., 1999, p. 154). The therapist then uses this experience to highlight that the client’s negative content is also purely verbal and not a reflection
of reality.
Empirical Evidence for Defusion
Although cognitive defusion strategies are frequently used within an ACTbased clinical context, the empirical evidence to support their efficacy is relatively limited. For example, indirect evidence for the utility of defusion may
be derived from chronic pain analog studies involving the Cold Presser Task
(Hayes, Bisset, et al., 1999; Takahashi, Muto, Tada, & Sugiyama, 2002). These
studies have indicated that acceptance-based interventions that rely heavily on
defusion strategies increase participants’ pain tolerance. Some support for the
utility of defusion can also be derived from ACT intervention research. For example, Bach & Hayes (2002) reported positive clinical outcomes for psychosis
COGNITIVE DEFUSION, NEGATIVE SELF-STATEMENTS
625
with a brief ACT intervention in which defusion was a central component.
Specifically, clients were encouraged to notice thoughts rather than to believe
them. They engaged in a common defusion exercise in which they were instructed to “take their minds for a walk.” Indeed, the researchers argued that
the 50% reduction in rehospitalization rates for the ACT group could be attributed to a reduction in the believability (i.e., defusion) of their psychotic psychological content. However, since defusion in this case was only one aspect of a
complete ACT package, the specific role of defusion is difficult to decipher.
More direct evidence for defusion was generated by the first empirical investigation of the technique when Titchener’s (1916) word-repetition exercise
was recently used in the context of negatively evaluated self-referential content
(Masuda, Hayes, Sackett, & Twohig, 2004). Specifically, undergraduate students
were asked to generate two self-relevant negative thoughts that they found
particularly disturbing (e.g., “I am too fat”) and then to restate each thought in
a single word (e.g., “fat”). Participants then rated the levels of discomfort and
believability associated with each word. In Experiment 1, the researchers compared the defusion strategy (i.e., word repetition) with a distraction technique
(i.e., reading an unrelated article). In Experiment 2, defusion was compared
with thought control (e.g., participants could use breathing, positive self-talk,
or positive imagery). The results from both experiments indicated that the
defusion rationale produced the largest reductions in the believability of the
negative self-relevant words, as well as in the levels of discomfort the words
produced.
The Current Study
The current study was designed to test an alternative defusion technique
to the one Masuda et al. (2004) used. The purpose was to ascertain if the reduction in believability was unique to the exercise used in the previous study, or
if the same findings would be obtained across defusion strategies more generally. Rather than using word repetition, we presented a diffusion technique to
undergraduate participants that ACT clinicians often use and is functionally
similar to those described by Hayes, Strosahl, et al. (1999). Specifically, the
phrase “I am having the thought that” was used to create a sense of cognitive
distance between a participant and negative self-referential statements. For example, the effects of the statement “I am a bad person” (Normal presentation)
were then compared with the effects produced by the same statement when
prefixed with the defusion phrase (“I am having the thought that I am a bad
person”: Defused presentation). Similar to word repetition, the purpose of the
prefix was to highlight the subsequent statement as simply a thought and not
a fact.
In the current study, participants’ reactions to negative and positive selfstatements were measured with and without the defusion prefix, and with
three self-report Likert-type rating scales that assessed levels of discomfort,
believability, and willingness associated with the target statements. The three
self-report scales were chosen on the basis of their inclusion in previous ACT
research (e.g., Hayes, Bisset, et al., 1999; Levitt, Brown, Orsillo, & Barlow, 2004;
Masuda et al., 2004). However, because the defusion prefix changed the length
of the statement and might also be seen as creating an unusual verbalization,
the self-statements were also prefixed with a phrase that matched the defusion
statement in length and was equally unusual (i.e., “I have a wooden chair and
626
HEALY ET AL.
. . .”: Abnormal presentation). This provided a comparison statement of similar
length that was also unusual but did not have the same level of defusion as the
cognitive distancing statements.
In the current study, we hypothesized that the defusion statements would
likely impact directly upon the explicit ratings associated with the negative selfstatements presented in the three formats (Normal, Abnormal, and Defused).
Specifically, we hypothesized that the negative statements in the defused presentation format would likely decrease believability to see or say the statements, but would increase willingness, relative to the normal and abnormal
statements. The believability prediction was consistent with ACT findings, in
which the change of one’s perspective about one’s psychological content, by
indicating that the person is greater than the sum of the content, reduces the
believability of the content (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). In other words, if I know that the thought “I am a horrible person” is only a thought,
then I will be aware that the thought can say nothing about who I am as a real
person. As a result, the thought will be less believable. Put simply, the believability of thoughts results from their potential to be viewed as having more
meaning than they need to have (i.e., by saying something about who you are
as a person). When this potential is reduced in the context of particular psychological content, that content, by definition, will be less believable. In the research by Masuda et al. (2004), the defusion exercise reduced the believability
of the targeted psychological content.
The willingness prediction was also consistent with ACT; it simply asserted that if you are defused from psychological content such that it says nothing about who you are, you do not need to be unwilling to have it. In other
words, if the content has no power, then you need not be afraid of what it can
do to you. Although willingness had not been targeted directly by Masuda et
al. (2004), other sources of empirical evidence have indicated that acceptancebased strategies increase willingness (Levitt et al., 2004). We therefore predicted that the defusion presentation format here would increase the willingness
of participants to see or read the self-statements.
In the current research, we initially made no clear predictions about the
impact of defusion on psychological discomfort. From an ACT perspective, discomfort is not targeted directly because one can still engage in valued action
when levels of discomfort are high. Furthermore, because individuals generally
have little or no control over their emotional states, ACT encourages clients
not to focus upon these as a potential source of behavior change. According
to this view then, discomfort may increase, decrease, or remain unchanged.
Specifically, one might argue that increasing acceptance of content might increase discomfort because one is more willing to make experiential contact
with it. Alternatively, one might predict that discomfort would decrease when
one realizes that thoughts associated with emotion are only thoughts, and
as such have limited control or impact on who you really are. Interestingly,
Masuda et al. (2004) reported that their defusion technique resulted in decreased discomfort. Taken together then, we had no clear initial prediction
about what would happen to the participants’ levels of discomfort as a result
of the defusion technique.
Traditionally, within a therapeutic context, cognitive defusion exercises
have been employed as coping strategies for negative thoughts and feelings. Thus, we made no clear predictions about the impact of defusion on
the positive self-statements. These statements were simply included here as
COGNITIVE DEFUSION, NEGATIVE SELF-STATEMENTS
627
an experimental control to determine if the emotional impact of defusion depended on psychologically engaging with negative thoughts and feelings, or
if the defusion effect simply reflected the process of distancing oneself from
one’s thoughts in general. Overall, the inclusion of the positive self-statements
would allow a fuller understanding of the manner in which defusion works.
In order to control for demand compliance effects (Fernandez & Turk,
1994; Kanter, Kohlenberg, & Loftus, 2004), participants in the current study
were also divided according to the types of explicit instructions they received
about the utility of the defusion technique employed here. Each instruction
stated that previous research had shown that the defusion prefix (1) increased
(Pro-Defusion Condition); (2) decreased (Anti-Defusion Condition); or (3) had
no effect upon (Neutral Condition) the emotional impact of self-statements. We
hypothesized that although the defusion-relevant instructions might impact
differentially upon participants’ ratings of discomfort, believability, and willingness, the defusion prefix would still outperform the Normal and Abnormal
statements.
An important caveat to the current research involves emphasizing that the
primary aim of the work was to simply study the impact of a common ACTbased defusion technique, particularly on individuals’ emotional reactions to
negative self-statements. The research, therefore, was not designed to determine what defusion is or exactly how it works. With such limited existing evidence of the utility of defusion per se, an important starting point for research
in this area seemed to be to simply identify defusion techniques, isolate them
as much as possible, and determine whether or not they produce positive and
predictable outcomes. Hence, the current work may be described as analog research in which some aspect of clinical practice is isolated and presented to a
nonclinical sample in a controlled experimental environment. As a result, we
anticipated that the current findings would contribute more to our faith in defusion rather than our understanding of it.
Method
Participants
A total of 60 participants (31 female, 29 male) with an age range of 18–
57 years volunteered for the study (mode = 21). All participants were undergraduate students and were recruited through Faculty announcements in the
Department of Psychology. None had prior exposure to similar experimental
procedures. Participants were divided into three groups, each composed of 20
students that differed only in the nature of the defusion-relevant instructions
they received (Pro-Defusion, Anti-Defusion, and Neutral).
Materials
The 10 negative statements used in the study were rated the most negative
from a larger pool, rated from 0 (extremely negative) to 20 (extremely positive);
none of the 32 independent raters was thereafter recruited as an experimental
participant. The mean ratings for the 10 negative statements ranged from 1.45
for “I am a failure” to 3.23 for “No one will ever love me.” Table 1 presents all
10 negative self-statements and their mean ratings.
628
HEALY ET AL.
Table 1
Mean Ratings of the 10 Negative Self-Statements
Employed in the Current Study
I am a failure.
My life is pointless.
I am stupid.
I am a bad person.
I make a mess of everything.
I am broken.
I am ugly.
I am helpless.
Sometimes I wish I wasn’t me.
No-one will ever love me
1.45
2.39
2.54
2.66
2.69
2.71
2.85
3.07
3.21
3.23
The 10 positive statements used here were selected in a similar manner
by the same 32 independent raters. The mean ratings for the 10 positive statements ranged from 18.66 (“I love life”) to 16.37 (“I am whole”). Table 2 presents
all 10 self-statements and their mean ratings.
Table 2
Mean ratings of the 10 Positive Self-Statements Employed in the Current Study
I love life.
I know that I am loved.
I am happy with who I am.
There is so much that I can do with my life.
There is so much for me to be happy about.
I am part of a beautiful world.
When things go wrong I know that I will always have friends.
I have no problems that can’t be solved.
I am proud of myself.
I am whole.
18.65
17.79
17.77
17.48
17.41
17.35
16.92
16.73
16.68
16.37
Measures
Each participant first completed three self-report questionnaires. These
were composed of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ-37, see Hayes
et al., 2004); the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II, see Beck, Steer, & Brown,
1996); and the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI Form Y-2, see Spielberger,
Gorsuch, Luschene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983). The assignment of participants to
experimental conditions was randomized and did not depend upon their scores
on the questionnaires. The three measures were simply included as a means
of determining any differences between the three experimental groups (ProDefusion, Anti-Defusion, and Neutral) that might influence their performance
during the experiment. For example, participants who score high in acceptance
COGNITIVE DEFUSION, NEGATIVE SELF-STATEMENTS
629
(i.e., low in avoidance) on the AAQ might respond more readily to defusion than
those who score low in acceptance (high in avoidance). Furthermore, low AAQ
responders may be less willing to engage with the negative self-statements,
and this might considerably undermine their full participation in the study.
Experiential Avoidance
The AAQ 37-item version is a self-report measure of an individual’s general
level of emotional avoidance. Individuals are asked to rate the truth of each statement as it applies to themselves, on a scale of 1 (never true) to 7 (always true).
Low scores on the AAQ indicate high avoidance and low acceptance, whereas high
scores indicate low avoidance and high acceptance. Although the AAQ 37-item
is a relatively new measure, initial research on similar versions of the AAQ indicate good psychometric properties, with good evidence of convergent, criterionrelated, and construct validity (see Bond & Bunce, 2003; Hayes et al., 2004).
Depression
The BDI-II consists of 21 groups of four statements (all scored from 0 to 3).
Individuals are asked to select one statement from each group that best describes how they have been feeling during the previous two weeks. The BDI-II
is scored according to four categories: 0–13: Minimal Depression; 14–19: Mild;
20–28: Moderate; and 29–63: Severe. The BDI-II is a widely used measure of
depression and has good, well-established psychometric properties. The scales
indicate good internal consistency (Cronbach coefficient alpha = 0.93 for college students) and correlate well with several clinical assessment ratings of
depression (Beck et al., 1996).
Trait Anxiety
The STAI Form Y-2 is used to assess levels of trait anxiety in both clinical and nonclinical populations. It is composed of 20 self-statements that ask
individuals to rate how they feel in general, on a scale of 1 (almost never) to
4 (almost always). The STAI Form Y is a valid measure of anxiety indicating
good test-retest reliability for a sample of college students (0.73–0.86) and
high internal consistency (Cronbach coefficient alpha is 0.90). The STAI Form
Y is also reliable and correlates with other established anxiety measures (see
Spielberger, et al., 1983).
Procedure
This study used a design of 3 × 3 × 2 mixed-between-within participants
with instructions (about the impact of defusion as a coping strategy) as the
between-participant variable (Pro-Defusion, Anti-Defusion, and Neutral), and
the visual presentation format of the self-statements (Normal, Defused, and
Abnormal), and statement-type (negative and positive) as within-participant
variables. Participants completed each aspect of the experiment independently, while the experimenter remained seated outside the experimental room. All
participants were seated on a standard plastic office swivel chair (rather than
a wooden chair). Each individual took approximately 90 minutes to complete
the study.
630
HEALY ET AL.
Phase 1: Psychological Measures
Participants completed the AAQ, the BDI-II, and the STAI. The psychological measures were presented in this order and in written format in a booklet.
Phase 2: Defusion-related Instructions
Prior to Phase 1, each participant was randomly assigned to one of three
groups (Pro-Defusion, Anti-Defusion, and Neutral). The defusion-related instructions were presented in written format, in a booklet the experimenter
provided. Each instruction booklet differed as to the specific instructions pertaining to the emotional impact of defusion. Participants in the Pro-Defusion
Group received instructions supporting a defusion rationale. That is, they were
instructed that adding the prefix “I am having the thought that” to a negative
self-statement decreases its emotional impact, thereby making the statement
easier to cope with. In contrast, participants in the Anti-Defusion Group were
given an instruction undermining a defusion strategy. The instruction stated
that the prefix “I am having the thought that” increases the emotional impact
of negative self-statements, thereby making them harder to cope with. The
Neutral Group simply functioned as an experimental control. Participants in
this group were instructed that the prefix would have no effect on one’s emotional reaction to negative self-statements. Having read the defusion-related
instructions, participants were required to indicate they had understood the
instructions by selecting the correct answer from a multiple choice question.
Phase 3: Automated Presentation and Ratings of Self-Statements
In each of the three groups, participants were exposed to an identical automated procedure that was used to deliver a number of independent computer
trials. Each computer trial presented 1 of the 10 negative self-statements or
1 of the 10 positive statements on the screen. In addition, each negative and
positive self-statement was presented in three different presentation formats
(Normal, Abnormal, and Defused). Thus, each participant was presented with
a total of 60 single trials (i.e., 10 negative statements presented in three formats and 10 positive statements presented in three formats). In the Normal
presentation format, each self-statement appeared without a prefix (e.g., “I am
a bad person”). In the Defused presentation, each statement was prefixed with
“I am having the thought that” (e.g., “I am having the thought that I am a bad
person”). In Abnormal presentation, each statement was prefixed with “I have
a wooden chair and” (e.g., “I have a wooden chair and I am a bad person”).
Each experimental trial commenced with the instruction to read the following single self-statement carefully and to think about it. On each of the 60 independent trials, one of the negative or positive statements in one of the three
formats appeared on the screen for 6 s. Immediately after the presentation of
each self-statement, a new screen automatically appeared with three rating
scales. Participants were asked to provide three ratings, as follows: (1) “Rate
the extent to which you felt comfortable or uncomfortable reading and thinking about the previous statement” (Comfort); (2) “Rate the extent to which you
found the previous statement believable” (Believability); and (3) “Rate the extent to which you were willing to read and think about the previous statement”
(Willingness). All three rating scales ranged from 0 (extremely comfortable;
extremely believable; and extremely willing) to 100 (extremely uncomfortable;
COGNITIVE DEFUSION, NEGATIVE SELF-STATEMENTS
631
extremely unbelievable; and extremely unwilling). Participants rated their reactions to the statements by using the computer’s mouse to drag a pointer along
each of the three scales. The trial ended when the participant clicked on an
end-of-trial button; this cleared the screen and after a 1 s. inter-trial interval,
the next trial commenced immediately (i.e., the next self-statement appeared
on the screen). Each of the 10 negative and 10 positive self-statements, in their
three formats, was presented once in random order for each participant. After
the last trial (60), a message appeared on screen asking the participant to report to the experimenter.
Phase 4: Postexperimental Measures
After the 60 trials, participants completed two rating scales that asked
(1) “To what extent did you read the statements and ratings and answer them
honestly?” (honesty) and (2) “To what extent, do you think your ratings have
been affected by the instructions you received at the beginning of the study?”
(control of defusion-related instructions). Participants provided each rating on
a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (Not at All) to 7 (Entirely). Participants
were then thanked and debriefed.
Results
Psychological Measures
Table 3 presents the mean (and standard deviation) ratings for each of the
psychological measures (AAQ, BDI-II, and STAI) for each group of participants.
In each case, the measures were broadly similar across groups. Indeed, three
separate one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) for each measure revealed no
significant differences (AAQ 37-item [p = 0.67]; BDI [p = 0.25]; STAI (Y2) [p =
0.84]).
Table 3
Means (and Standard Deviations) for Psychological Measures for Each
Instruction Group
AAQ-37
Pro-Defusion
Anti-Defusion
Neutral
160.95 (17.37)
158.25 (12.54)
163.25 (21.81)
BDI-II
7.25 (4.87)
6.10 (5.77)
8.90 (5.26)
STAI-Y2
36.45 (8.42)
37.25 (11.15)
38.40 (11.31)
Adherence Measures
High ratings were obtained on the honesty measure across all three groups
(Pro-Defusion: M = 6.20, SD = 0.61; Anti-Defusion: M = 5.95, SD = 0.99; and
Neutral: M = 5.90, SD = 0.85). Furthermore, a one-way ANOVA revealed no significant differences between groups, F(2, 57) = 0.74, p = 0.48. These findings
indicated that participants on the whole had responded honestly to the selfstatements.
In contrast, the mean ratings on adherence to the defusion-related instructions appeared to be relatively low for each of the three groups (Pro-Defusion:
HEALY ET AL.
632
M = 3.40, SD = 1.90; Anti-Defusion: M = 2.95, SD = 1.98; and Neutral: M = 3.90,
SD = 2.07). The results from a one-way ANOVA revealed no significant difference between groups in this regard, F(2, 57) = 1.14, p = 0.33. Although these
findings suggested that the defusion-related instructions did not greatly influence the participants, perhaps a more important result was of no betweengroup differences.
Self-Report Measures
For the purposes of analysis, the 30 negative and 30 positive self-statements
were divided according to the three presentation formats: Normal, Abnormal,
and Defused. The three ratings (comfort, believability, and willingness) for
each of the three statement-types, summed across the 10 trials for each participant, ranged from 0 to 1000. The data obtained for the negative and positive self-statements were analyzed separately and are presented in separate
sections below.
Negative Self-Statements
The mean overall scores were calculated for the negative self-statements
for each of the three ratings, across participants, for each of the three instruction groups and divided by the three statement-types. In the interests of clarity,
the three sets of means are referred to as negative comfort, negative willingness, and negative believability. Table 4 presents the means and standard deviations for each of these ratings, calculated across participants, for each of
the three instruction groups divided by the three statement-types.
Table 4
Means (and Standard Deviations) for Discomfort, Willingness, and Believability
Ratings of the Three Statement Types for the Three Conditions for Negative
Self-Statements
Statement type Pro-Defusion group Anti-Defusion group
Comfort ratings
Neutral group
Normal
Abnormal
Defused
326.80 (293.18)
465.65 (322.41)
284.70 (261.68)
421.50 (275.46)
219.90 (220.62)
393.70 (297.62)
Willingness ratings
390.95 (268.59)
412.75 (293.69)
323.05 (232.14)
Normal
Abnormal
Defused
347.85 (336.91)
453.40 (314.58)
346.95 (325.22)
433.65 (255.67)
245.30 (259.04)
369.65 (281.98)
Believability ratings
481.80 (266.26)
505.75 (286.82)
400.25 (262.42)
Normal
Abnormal
Defused
849.250 (99.66)
835.05 (145.51)
630.80 (199.70)
837.10 (132.21)
817.25 (149.89)
726.00 (127.79)
709.50 (264.92)
706.35 (259.01)
625.90 (270.13)
Note. Lower scores indicate greater comfort, greater willingness, and greater
believability.
The descriptive statistics indicated that within each group, the Defused
presentation format, relative to Normal and Abnormal, produced lower levels of
discomfort, higher levels of willingness, but—contrary to our prediction—higher
COGNITIVE DEFUSION, NEGATIVE SELF-STATEMENTS
633
levels of believability. Across-group comparisons indicated lowest levels of discomfort for Pro-Defusion and highest for Anti-Defusion. For willingness, ProDefusion produced greatest willingness and Neutral lowest. For believability, the
Anti-Defusion group produced the highest levels; the differences between ProDefusion and Neutral were unsystematic. The statistical analyses conducted for
each of the three types of ratings are discussed separately below.
Comfort ratings. A 3 × 3 mixed repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on the comfort ratings, with group (Pro-Defusion, Anti-Defusion, and
Neutral) as the between-participant variable, and presentation format (Normal,
Abnormal, and Defused) as the within-participant variable. A significant main
effect was identified for presentation format, F(2, 57) = 19.05, p < 0.0001, ηp2 =
0.25, but not for group (p = 0.21); and the interaction was also nonsignificant
(p = 0.17). Post hoc (Scheffe) tests revealed a significant difference when comparing Defusion with Normal (p < 0.0001) and with Abnormal (p = 0.0001) presentation formats but not between Normal and Abnormal. In short, defusion
significantly decreased discomfort relative to the other two presentation formats, and this effect was not significantly modulated by the defusion-related
instructions (see Table 4).
Willingness ratings. Another 3 × 3 ANOVA was conducted on the willingness ratings. Similar to the comfort measure, a significant main effect for the
presentation format was obtained, F (2, 57) = 20.91, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.26; but
once again, no other significant effects were found for group (p = 0.23) or interaction (p = 0.74). Scheffe post hoc tests revealed the same pattern as for comfort (Defusion vs. Normal, p < 0.0001; Defusion vs. Abnormal, p < 0.0001; Normal
vs. Abnormal, not significant). In effect, defusion significantly increased willingness relative to the other two presentation formats, with no modulation by
the defusion-related instructions (see Table 4).
Believability ratings. The results of a third 3 × 3 ANOVA on the believability
data also revealed a significant main effect for presentation format, F (2, 57) =
44.30, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.43, and no effect for group (p = 0.12). However, a significant interaction was obtained between format and group, F(4, 114) = 4.18,
p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.12. In order to examine this effect, three separate one-way repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted, one for each group. The ANOVA for
the Pro-Defusion group was significant, F(2, 19) = 24.19, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.56,
and Sheffe post hoc tests indicated that believability was greater for Defusion
relative to both Normal and Abnormal presentation formats (p < 0.0001, p <
0.0001, respectively), with no significant difference between the latter (p =
0.92). The Anti-Defusion ANOVA was also significant, F(2, 19) = 6.33, p = 0.004,
ηp2 = 0.25;
.25;
25; and post hoc tests revealed greater believability for Defusion relative to Normal and Abnormal (p = 0.0125, p = 0.0168, respectively), with no
significant difference between the latter (p = 0.99). The final ANOVA for the
Neutral group was also significant, F(2, 19) = 16.98, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.47, with
the post hoc tests again revealing greater believability for Defusion relative to
Normal and Abnormal (p < 0.0001, p = 0.0003, respectively), but no significant
difference between the latter (p = 0.62). In summary, and contrary to predictions, Defusion significantly increased believability relative to the Normal and
Abnormal presentation formats for each of the three groups (see Table 4).
Positive Self-Statements
The mean overall scores were calculated for the positive self-statements for
each of the three ratings, across participants, for each of the three instruction
HEALY ET AL.
634
groups and divided by the three statement-types. In the interests of clarity, the
three sets of means are referred to as positive comfort, positive willingness,
and positive believability. Table 5 presents the means and standard deviations
for each of the ratings, calculated across participants, for each of the three instruction groups divided by the three statement-types.
Table 5
Means (and Standard Deviations) for Discomfort, Willingness, and Believability
Ratings of the Three Statement Types for the Three Conditions for Positive SelfStatements
Statement Type Pro-Defusion group Anti-Defusion group
Neutral group
Comfort ratings
Normal
Abnormal
Defused
112.55 (101.09)
141.40 (126.61)
122.15 (117.04)
189.90 (155.74)
119.40 (137.72)
195.50 (147.08)
Willingness ratings
158.25 (153.05)
220.55 (173.78)
166.95 (142.43)
Normal
Abnormal
Defused
125.05 (118.75)
172.25 (144.99)
162.15 (178.62)
227.95 (184.08)
135.15 (157.85)
190.85 (154.00)
Believability ratings
216.95 (175.51)
285.10 (198.43)
219.25 (158.75)
Normal
Abnormal
Defused
196.80 (140.65)
249.35 (191.89)
218.95 (204.13)
227.75 (156.34)
278.60 (125.09)
258.40 (156.77)
196.30 (143.53)
301.75 (190.77)
246.50 (149.09)
Note. Lower scores indicate greater comfort, greater willingness, and greater
believability.
The descriptive statistics indicated that within each group, the Defused
presentation format produced medium levels of willingness and believability
relative to the Normal and Abnormal formats, with the Normal format producing the highest level for each measure. The Normal format also produced
the highest level of comfort; however, the differences between Abnormal and
Defused failed to reveal the same pattern as the other two measures (i.e., willingness and believability). Across group comparisons indicated highest levels of comfort for Pro-Defusion, whereas the differences between Neutral and
Anti-Defusion groups were unsystematic. The Pro-Defusion group also produced the highest levels of willingness, with the Neutral group producing the
lowest levels of willingness. For believability, across group differences were
overall unsystematic, although there is some indication that the Pro-Defusion
instruction overall produced the highest levels of believability. The differences
between believability ratings for the remaining instruction groups appeared
random.
Comfort ratings. A 3 × 3 mixed repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on the comfort ratings, with defusion-related instruction group as the
between-participant variable and statement presentation format as the within-participant variable. A significant main effect was identified for statement
presentation format, F (2, 57) = 6.64, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.10, but not for instruction group (p = 0.25). Furthermore, there was no significant interaction effect
(p = 0.07). Post hoc (Scheffe) tests revealed a significant difference when comparing Normal and Abnormal statements (p = 0.002), with Normal statements
COGNITIVE DEFUSION, NEGATIVE SELF-STATEMENTS
635
rated as more comfortable than Abnormal statements. However, no significant
difference was obtained when comparing Defusion with Normal (p = 0.12) and
with Abnormal (p = 0.31) presentation formats. Taken together, these results
suggest that the Defused presentation format had little impact on the comfort
ratings relative to the other two presentation formats. These findings were not
significantly modulated by the defusion-related instructions (see Table 5).
Willingness ratings. Another 3 × 3 mixed repeated measures ANOVA was
conducted on the willingness ratings. This analysis revealed a significant main
effect for statement presentation format, F(2, 57) = 9.54, p = 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.14.
However, the main effect for defusion-related instruction group was nonsignificant (p = 0.13), as was the interaction effect (p = 0.76). Post hoc (Scheffe) tests
revealed a significant difference between Normal and Abnormal (p = 0.0004)
and between Defusion and Abnormal (p = 0.005) presentation format, with
participants being more willing to experience statements in the Normal and
Defused presentation format and least willing to experience statements presented in the Abnormal format. The difference between Normal and Defusion
statements was nonsignificant (p = 0.73). Thus, the Defused format and the
Normal format appeared to impact on participants’ willingness to experience
the positive self-statements in a similar manner. Once again, these findings
were not modulated by the defusion-related instructions (see Table 5).
Believability ratings. A 3 × 3 mixed repeated measures ANOVA was also
conducted on the positive believability ratings. Similar to both the positive
comfort and positive willingness data, the analysis of the believability ratings
revealed a significant main effect for statement presentation format, F (2, 57) =
9.79, p = 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.15, but the effects for instruction group and the interaction effects were both nonsignificant (p = 0.76, p = 0.62, respectively). Similar
to the comfort ratings, post hoc (Scheffe) tests revealed a significant difference between Normal and Abnormal statements only (p = 0.0001), with Normal
statements rated as more believable than Abnormal statements. No significant
differences were obtained when comparing Defusion with Normal (p = 0.10)
and with Abnormal (p = 0.09) presentation formats.
In short, the Defused presentation format had little impact on the believability ratings relative to the other two presentation formats, and these findings
could not be attributed to the defusion-related instructions (see Table 5).
In summary, a different pattern of responding was observed for the negative relative to the positive self-statements. With respect to the negative selfstatements, the Defused presentation format produced significantly higher
levels of comfort (i.e., reduced discomfort), willingness, and believability
relative to the Normal and Abnormal presentation formats. In contrast, the
Defused presentation format had relatively little impact on the emotional ratings of the positive self-statements and overall did not significantly differ from
the Normal or Abnormal presentation formats.
Discussion
The current study primarily investigated the impact of cognitive defusion
on participants’ self-reported levels of discomfort, willingness, and believability in the context of negative self-statements that appeared randomly on
a computer screen. For all participants, the impact of the negative statements
presented in the Defused form (relative to Normal and Abnormal presentations) was consistent with experimental hypotheses in that they increased
636
HEALY ET AL.
participants’ willingness to read and think about them. Although we had not
made firm predictions about the potential impact of the defused presentations on participant discomfort associated with the negative statements,
the data indicated that discomfort decreased in that context but not when
the same statements were presented as Normal or Abnormal. In contrast,
we had made firm predictions that believability of the negative statements
would also be reduced by defusion, but the findings were inconsistent with
these predictions and indicated that believability of the statements actually
increased as a result of defusion. The results for the discomfort ratings here
are consistent with those reported by Masuda et al. (2004), but the believability data are not (Masuda et al. reported decreases in believability). The
current research was the first empirical analysis of the impact of defusion on
experiential willingness.
In attempting to explain the discrepancy between the current and previous findings on believability of the negative self-statements, it might be argued that the target process of defusion did not actually occur here. However,
this seems unlikely, because increases in willingness are consistent with a
defusion effect. In other words, defusion should increase willingness to experience negative thoughts and feelings: if the content is perceived to be
meaningless (i.e., if you are fully defused), then there should be no reason to
avoid it.
Consequently, the believability measure probably did not operate here in
the way we had anticipated. In retrospect, the believability findings obtained
in the current study seem highly likely. Consider first that participants were
required to respond to the discomfort and willingness scales by responding
to the whole defusion statement (e.g., how uncomfortable does the statement
“I am having the thought that I am a bad person” make you feel?). It seems
likely, therefore, that participants responded to the believability of the defusion statements in the same way (e.g., how believable is it that you are having
the thought that you are a bad person?). In effect, the increased believability
ratings for the defused statements indicated that the participants believed
they were indeed having that thought, rather than indicating that the thought
was true. In this sense, therefore, the increased believability ratings could be
seen as evidence for a defusion effect as specified by ACT, especially when
taken in the context of decreased discomfort and increased willingness for
the same set of statements. Although we can only speculate about whether
participants here were responding to the self-statement per se or to the fully
defused statement, the latter is a possibility and in any case raises an interesting empirical issue for future research.
The differences in believability across the two studies may also be accounted for by the fact that different defusion exercises may operate in different ways. Specifically, Masuda et al. (2004) employed a semantic satiation
exercise (i.e., word repetition) as opposed to the more cognitive distancing
exercise employed here. Although both types of exercise altered believability
in different ways, believability did change, and thus one can reasonably argue that changes in believability are an important mechanism for the impact
of defusion. However, whether on any instance believability should increase
or decrease perhaps depends on how believability is explicitly assessed. The
current research, therefore, highlights the need for future research on how
best to assess the truth or meaning of an individual’s psychological content.
The current study manipulated instructions on the putative effects of
COGNITIVE DEFUSION, NEGATIVE SELF-STATEMENTS
637
defusion in order to compare the relative utility of defusion instructions
against more active experiential defusion through the visual presentation
of the statements. Across all three defusion-related instruction groups, the
Defusion presentation format continued to produce positive effects (relative
to the other two formats) in terms of comfort, believability, and willingness,
whereas the instructions overall on defusion appeared to have little or no
impact. The primary aim of this minor manipulation was to begin to compare the impact of defusion-based instructions versus exercises, because in
ACT, defusion is rarely instructed and more often is delivered via experiential
work. Indeed, the findings recorded here provided some very preliminary evidence that the least visual experiential contact with the defused statements
generated greater defusion than instructions.
One issue that may be raised about the current manipulation of defusion instructions is that all three types of instructions actually contained
the defusion prefix (“I’m having the thought that”). And it might be argued
that this contaminated the potential impact of the prefix when presented
with the statements in the Defused presentation format. Although this possibility remains, the data indicated that the instructions were of little impact, and so the observed defusion effects were not likely to depend upon
them. Indeed, what is remarkable is that the observed defusion effects on
the ratings occurred for participants who had been instructed that defusion has either no impact, or that it is counter-productive in the context
of negative self-statements. Furthermore, our experimental target here was
to compare defusion instructions versus experiential contact. Nonetheless,
one avenue for future research might involve an alternative instruction manipulation that targeted all three types of presentation format and would
provide greater clarity on the potential interactions between instructions
and presentation.
In effect, the results suggest the superiority of experientially engaging
with the statements over simple Pro-Defusion instructions. Indeed, the data
here do not indicate that the provision of Pro-Defusion instructions actually enhanced the effect of the Defused presentation format, relative to the
other two types of instruction. Although clinical concerns over the relative
utility of instructions versus experiential exercises are commonplace (e.g.,
Eifert & Forsyth, 2005, p. 124), the current research contributes to this debate
with empirical support for experiential contact over instructions, at least in
the context of defusion. However, it is important to note that the defusion
instructions provided here were not presented as a full therapeutic intervention; nor were they designed to be, and, as such, an entirely different
preparation may be necessary to fully explore this issue.
The low adherence ratings for the defusion-related instructions are entirely consistent with their lack of impact on the ratings. If participants in
all three groups had not attended well to the information on defusion, they
were probably not influenced by this subsequently. However, it was not the
case that participants did not understand these instructions, because on the
instruction booklet, they had been asked to tick a box to indicate that they
had fully understood and all participants had done so. It also remains possible that participants interpreted the adherence question (presented at the
end of the experiment) as a reference to their adherence to the main instructions on the presentations of the statements and the ratings. However, this
seems unlikely because the strong changes in ratings suggested no lack of
638
HEALY ET AL.
adherence during this part of the experiment, whereas the limited impact of
the defusion instructions does imply lack of adherence at that earlier point.
Although instructions encouraging participants to attend fully to them are
difficult to provide, while at the same time avoiding them doing any experiential exercises, the current study, at the very least, highlights the importance
of including adherence measures.
The current research suggests that defusion is less psychologically active
when used in conjunction with the positive self-statements relative to the
negative self-statements. Specifically, although statements presented in the
Defused format were significantly different from the Normal and Abnormal
forms for the negative self-statements, the same effect was not recorded for
the positive statements. Indeed, positive statements presented in the Defused
format did not significantly differ from the other presentation formats, suggesting that the defusion effect did not occur here. The absence of any change
in levels of believability about the Defused presentation format further suggests the absence of a defusion effect with the positive self-statements if we
assume that changes in levels of believability are a useful measure of defusion-related change (e.g., Masuda et al., 2004). These differential outcomes
are consistent with our original hypotheses, and indeed with ACT, in suggesting the greater impact of defusion on psychological content that is perhaps
more susceptible to emotional avoidance.
However, it could be argued that participants simply failed to experientially
engage with the positive self-statements, but did so with the more challenging
negative statements. This may be particularly so because all the defusionbased instructions contained a negative, rather than a positive, self-statement.
Put simply, the instructions manipulation may have oriented participants toward the negative statements (similar to the previous possibility that the instructions also oriented participants toward the defusion prefix). Yet again
this seems unlikely because of the significant differences between the positive
Normal and Abnormal presentation formats and because of the general lack of
impact of the instructions. Indeed, the inclusion of a positive statement within
the instructions would not likely have generated different results.
In summary, the research presented here provides empirical evidence of
the utility of cognitive defusion in reducing the negative emotional impact of
psychologically painful content and encouraging willingness to experience it.
These findings are consistent with the clinical assumptions of ACT (Hayes,
Strosahl, et al., 1999), and they provide indirect evidence that the positive
outcomes reported for ACT result from its strong emphasis on defusion
(e.g., Gifford et al., 2004; Gutierrez, Luciano, & Fink, 2004; Twohig, Hayes, &
Masuda, 2006). At the very least, the evidence here supports the view that defusion functions by changing an individual’s levels of discomfort, believability, and willingness associated with painful content, rather than by altering
the content directly. The clinical implications of the work suggest that defusion should be delivered through experiential exercises rather than solely by
instructions, and that the success of defusion techniques may be measured
by assessing concurrent changes in discomfort, believability, and willingness.
Although the process of defusion itself remains obscure, the current research
offers further insight into how it can be measured and how it appears to operate and, as such, makes a useful contribution to further research on the
topic and more insightful clinical practice.
COGNITIVE DEFUSION, NEGATIVE SELF-STATEMENTS
639
References
BACH, P., & HAYES, S. C. (2002). The use of acceptance and commitment
therapy to prevent the rehospitalization of psychotic patients: a
randomized control trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
70, 1129–1139.
BECK, A. T., STEER, R., & BROWN, G. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory: Second
Edition. Psychological Corporation.
BOND, F. W., & BUNCE, D. (2003). The role of acceptance and job control in
mental health, job satisfaction and work performance. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 88, 1057–1067.
EIFER, G. H., & FORSYTH, J. P. (2005). Acceptance and commitment therapy
for anxiety disorders: a practitioner’s guide to using mindfulness,
acceptance, and values-based behavior change strategies. U.S.A.: New
Harbinger Publications.
FERNANDEZ, E., & TURK, D. C. (1994). Demand characteristics underlying
ratings of sensory versus affective components of pain. Journal of
Behavioural Medicine, 17, 375–390.
GIFFORD, E. V., KOHLENBERG, B. S., HAYES, S. C., ANTONUCCIO, D. O.,
PIASECKI, M. M., RASMUSSEN-HALL, M. L. ET AL. (2004). Acceptance-
based treatment for smoking cessation. Behavior Therapy, 35, 689–705.
GUTIERREZ, O., LUCIANO, C., RODRIGUEZ, M., & FINK, B. C. (2004).
Comparison between an acceptance-based and a cognitive-control-based
protocol for coping with pain. Behavior Therapy, 35, 767–784.
HAYES, S. C., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & ROCHE, B. (2001). Relational frame
theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New
York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
HAYES, S. C., BISSET, R. T., KORN, Z., ZETTLE, R. D., ROSENFARD, I. S.,
COOPER, L. D. ET AL. (1999). The impact of acceptance versus control
rationales on pain tolerance. The Psychological Record, 49, 33– 47.
HAYES, S. C., LUOMA, J. B., BOND, F. W., MASUDA, A., & LILLIS, J. (2006).
Acceptance and commitment therapy: model, processes and outcomes.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25.
HAYES, S. C., STROSAHL, K. D., & WILSON, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and
commitment therapy: an experiential approach to behavior change. New
York: Guilford Press.
HAYES, S. C., STROSAHL, K., WILSON, K. G., BISSET, R. T., PISTORELLO, J.,
TOARMINO, D. ET AL. (2004). Experiential avoidance: a preliminary test
of a working model. The Psychological Record, 54, 553–578.
KANTER, J. W., KOHLENBERG, R. J., & LOFTUS, E. F. (2004). Experimental and
psychotherapeutic demand characteristics and the cognitive therapy
rationale: an analogue study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28,
229–239.
KROSS, E., AYDUK, O., & MISCHEL, W. (2005). When asking “why” does not
hurt: distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative
emotions. Psychological Science, 16, 709–715.
LEVITT, J. T., BROWN, T. A., ORSILLO, S. M., & BARLOW, D. H. (2004). The
effects of acceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjective and
psychophysiological response to carbon dioxide challenge in patients
with panic disorder. Behavior Therapy, 35, 747–766.
640
HEALY ET AL.
MASUDA, A., HAYES, S. C., SACKETT, C. F., & TWOHIG, M. P. (2004). Cognitive
defusion and self-relevant negative thoughts: examining the impact of a
ninety year old technique. Behavior Research and Therapy 42, 477– 485.
SPIELBERGER, C. D., GORSUCH, R. L., LUSCHENE, R., VAGG, P. R., & JACOBS, G.
A. (1983). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Mind Garden Inc.
TAKAHASHI, M., MUTO, T., TADA, M., & SUGIYAMA, M. (2002). Acceptance
rationale and increasing pain-tolerance: acceptance-based and FEARbased practice. Japanese Journal of Behavior Therapy, 28, 35– 46.
TEASDALE, J. D., SEGAL, Z. V., WILLIAMS, J. M. G., RIDGEWAY, V. A.,
SOULSBY, J. M., LAU, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in
major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615– 623.
TITCHENER, E. B. (1916). A text-book of psychology. New York: MacMillan.
TWOHIG, M.P., HAYES, S. C., & MASUDA, A. (2006). Increasing willingness
to experience obsessions: acceptance and commitment therapy as a
treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy, 37, 3–13.