Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev
Review
A computational neuroscience approach to schizophrenia and its onset
Edmund T. Rolls a,∗ , Gustavo Deco b,c
a
Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience, Oxford, UK
Computational Neuroscience, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain
c
Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats, Barcelona, Spain
b
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 8 April 2010
Received in revised form 12 August 2010
Accepted 2 September 2010
Keywords:
Schizophrenia
Adolescence
Computational neuroscience
Synaptic pruning
Grey matter volume
NMDA receptors
Dopamine
GABA inhibition
Attractor networks
Neural networks
Stochastic neurodynamics
Noise in the brain
Neuronal spiking
a b s t r a c t
Computational neuroscience integrate-and-fire attractor network models can be used to understand the
factors that alter the stability of cortical networks in the face of noise caused for example by neuronal
spiking times. A reduction of the firing rates of cortical neurons caused for example by reduced NMDA
receptor function (present in schizophrenia) can lead to instability of the high firing rate attractor states
that normally implement short-term memory and attention, contributing to the cognitive and negative
symptoms of schizophrenia. Reduced cortical inhibition caused by a reduction of GABA neurotransmission (present in schizophrenia) can lead to instability of the spontaneous firing states of cortical networks,
leading to a noise-induced jump to a high firing rate attractor state even in the absence of external inputs,
contributing to the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. We consider how effects occurring at the time
of late adolescence including synaptic pruning, decreases in grey matter volume, and changes in GABAmediated inhibition and dopamine may contribute to the onset in some individuals of schizophrenia at
this time.
© 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Contents
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
A top-down computational neuroscience approach to schizophrenia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
A neurodynamical hypothesis of schizophrenia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.1.
Cognitive symptoms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.2.
Negative symptoms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Positive symptoms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.3.
Schizophrenia and noise . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
The role of prefrontal cortex dopamine . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Brain changes during adolescence that could influence these processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Grey matter and synaptic changes associated with adolescence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
6.1.
GABA changes at adolescence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
6.2.
6.3.
Dopamine changes at adolescence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Stochastic neurodynamics: appendix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
The attractor framework . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
8.1.
8.2.
Energy landscape and stochasticity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
∗ Corresponding author.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (E.T. Rolls).
URL: http://www.oxcns.org (E.T. Rolls).
0149-7634/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.09.001
1645
1645
1646
1646
1647
1647
1648
1648
1648
1648
1649
1649
1650
1651
1651
1651
1652
1652
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
1. Introduction
In this paper, first we describe a computational neuroscience
approach to schizophrenia. Then we consider how brain changes
that occur in late adolescence may contribute to the onset of
schizophrenia.
The computational neuroscience approach we take involves
modeling cortical systems at the level of integrate-and-fire neurons
with synaptically activated ion channels in attractor or autoassociation networks implemented with the recurrent collateral
connections between pyramidal cells. This enables us to link from
effects expressed at synapses and ion channels, through their
effects on the spiking neuronal activity in the network and the
noise that this introduces into the system, to global effects of the
network such as the stability of short-term memory, attentional,
and decision-making systems, and thus to cognitive function,
dysfunction, and behavior. This provides a unifying approach to
many aspects of cortical function, which helps in the understanding of short-term memory, long-term memory, top-down
attention, decision-making, executive function, and the relation
between the emotional and the reasoning systems in the brain
(Deco and Rolls, 2003, 2005a, 2006; Deco et al., 2009; Rolls, 2008,
2010a,b; Rolls and Deco, 2002, 2010). This paper is the first in
which we have linked this approach to how brain changes at
the time of adolescence might influence some of these functions.
2. A top-down computational neuroscience approach to
schizophrenia
Some computational neuroscience approaches to schizophrenia
build upon single-neuron biophysics, physiology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia, and analyze their effects in neural
networks, which are then linked to the symptoms of schizophrenia (Durstewitz et al., 1999, 2000; Durstewitz and Seamans,
2008; Seamans and Yang, 2004; Winterer and Weinberger,
2004).
We have adopted a top-down approach which considers
whether generic alterations in the operation and stability of cortical circuits in different cortical areas might lead to the different
symptoms of schizophrenia (Loh et al., 2007a,b; Rolls, 2005, 2008;
Rolls et al., 2008b). Bottom-up approaches start with putative
changes at the neural level such as alterations in dopamine, and
try to understand the implications for function, which are of course
multiple, of these changes. The top-down approach complements
the bottom-up approaches, as it starts from the set of symptoms
and maps them onto a dynamical systems computational framework. The dynamical systems computational approach considers
factors that affect the stability of networks in the brain, and the
effects of noise in those networks on the stability. Because the
dynamical systems we consider can be, and are, implemented at
the level of integrate-and-fire neurons with neuronal and synaptic
dynamics that are biophysically realistic, and incorporate different classes of ion channel activated by different transmitter
receptors, effects of changes at these different levels, including
alterations in ion channels and transmitters, can be investigated
in and predicted from the model. We call this class of model
“mechanistic”, in that it describes the underlying neuronal and
subneuronal mechanisms involved in the dynamics in a biologically plausible way, so that predictions can be made about how
changes in any one part of the mechanism will affect the overall, “global”, operation of the system, measured for example by
the stability of short-term memory and attentional states. Thus
the top-down approach emphasizes how the computations in
the system perform particular functions, and then considers how
1645
possibly combinations of several neural changes can influence
the operation of the system, and how alterations of a number of possible different neural factors may be able to restore
the computational functions being performed by the neural system.
We contrast this with phenomenological models, which attempt
to capture the behavior of the system, but without regard to
whether the system could be implemented in the brain, and without any neurally plausible mechanism being modelled. An example
is the accumulator model of decision-making, in which an accumulator (linear adder) adds up each of the two (or more) inputs it
receives, adds some noise, and then considers that a decision has
been taken when some arbitrary threshold is reached in which the
sum of one of the inputs is by some amount greater than the other
(Ratcliff et al., 1999; Smith and Ratcliff, 2004; Vickers, 1979). Such
a phenomenological model does not allow any direct predictions
about how changes in ion channels, neurotransmitters, etc. alter
the operation of the system, and indeed, to make such predictions,
the phenomenological model, which may not represent realistically
the type of processing that occurs in the brain, would itself have
to be modelled in a neural system. Similar issues arise with connectionist models (Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, 1992) described
elsewhere (Rolls et al., 2008b) which also do not incorporate details
of the neurophysiological and biophysical mechanisms that contribute to the neural dynamics, and may therefore be less able to
model how changes in a number of neurobiological factors influence the operation of the system. The mechanistic approach we
adopt instead often is able to provide accounts for how important
functional properties of the system arise as emergent properties of
the system. One example is Weber’s law in decision-making (Deco
and Rolls, 2006). Another example is confidence in a decision that
has been made, which is encoded in the firing rates of the neurons in the winning attractor (Rolls and Deco, 2010; Rolls et al.,
2010a,b).
The stochastic dynamical systems approach that we utilize
(Rolls and Deco, 2010) is introduced in Section 8. The full implementation of the equations for the neuron and synaptic dynamics
is described elsewhere (Loh et al., 2007a; Rolls et al., 2008b), and
includes currents passing through voltage-dependent and hence
non-linear ion channels activated by NMDA receptors, and currents through ion channels activated by AMPA and GABA receptors.
The positive feedback in the recurrent collateral connections in the
network, the NMDA receptor non-linearity, and the non-linearity
introduced by the threshold for firing of the neurons in the system,
provide the system with non-linearities that enable it to have the
properties of an attractor network (Deco and Rolls, 2005b; Rolls
and Deco, 2010).
A feature that we have adopted from Brunel and Wang (2001)
of the approach we use is a mean-field equivalent analysis of the
network using techniques from theoretical physics. This allows
measurement of the fixed points of the system, the flow in the
system, and the operating areas in the parameter spaces that will
produce for example a stable spontaneous firing rate and also stable
high firing rates for each of the memory attractor states (depending
on the starting conditions) in a noiseless system, equivalent to a system of infinite size (Brunel and Wang, 2001; Deco and Rolls, 2006;
Loh et al., 2007a; Rolls and Deco, 2010). This enables suitable values
of for example the synaptic connection weights in the system to be
chosen. If these parameters are then used in the integrate-and-fire
version of the model, which has noise due to the approximately
Poisson spiking times of the neurons, the effects of the noise on
the operation of the system, and of alterations for example of the
different synaptic currents produced through different transmitter receptors in the system, can be investigated (Brunel and Wang,
2001; Deco and Rolls, 2006; Loh et al., 2007a; Rolls and Deco, 2010;
Rolls et al., 2008a,b).
1646
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
3. A neurodynamical hypothesis of schizophrenia
Firing rate [Hz]
The cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia include distractibility, poor attention, and the dysexecutive syndrome (Green, 1996;
Liddle, 1987; Mueser and McGurk, 2004). It has been suggested
that at the core of the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia is a
working-memory deficit characterized by a difficulty in maintaining items in short-term memory implemented in the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex (Goldman-Rakic, 1994, 1999).
Short-term memory is implemented in the prefrontal cortex
as follows. Pyramidal neurons in the cerebral cortex have a relatively high density of excitatory connections to each other within
a local area of 1–3 mm (Abeles, 1991; Braitenberg and Schütz,
1991). These local recurrent collateral excitatory connections provide a positive-feedback mechanism (which is kept under control
by GABA inhibitory interneurons) that enables a set of neurons to
maintain their activity for many seconds to implement a short-term
memory (Goldman-Rakic, 1995). Each memory is formed by the
set of the neurons in the local cortical network that were coactive
when the memory was formed, resulting in strengthened excitatory connections between that set of neurons through the process
of long-term potentiation, which is a property of these recurrent
collateral connections. When a subset of these neurons is subsequently activated, positive feedback through the strengthened
excitatory connections between the neurons results in activation
of the whole set of neurons, and so produces the completion of an
incomplete memory. Thus, in an attractor network, the state of the
network is “attracted” towards the state in which the memory was
learned; this is called an “attractor state”. An attractor network can
have many different attractor states, each consisting of a different
subset of the neurons being active; any one subset of neurons can
represent a short-term memory. The operation and properties of
attractor networks are described in Section 8 and more fully elsewhere (Amit, 1989; Hertz et al., 1991; Hopfield, 1982; Rolls, 2008;
Rolls and Deco, 2002, 2010).
Attractor networks appear to operate in the prefrontal cortex,
an area that is important in attention and short-term memory, as
shown for example by firing in the delay period of a short-term
memory task (Funahashi et al., 1989; Fuster, 1995, 2000; Fuster
and Alexander, 1971; Goldman-Rakic, 1996; Kubota and Niki, 1971;
Rolls, 2008). Short-term memory is the ability to hold information
on-line during a short time period (Fuster, 1995, 2000) and is fundamental to top-down attention in the sense that whatever requires
attention (e.g. a spatial location) has to be maintained in a shortterm memory. The short-term memory then biases competition
between the multiple bottom-up items in the stimulus input; the
result is an advantage in the neuronal competition between the
multiple inputs for the item that receives top-down bias from the
short-term memory (Deco and Rolls, 2005a; Desimone and Duncan,
1995; Rolls and Deco, 2002). The impairments of attention induced
by prefrontal cortex damage may be accounted for in large part
by an impairment in the ability to hold the object of attention stably and without distraction in the short-term memory systems in
the prefrontal cortex (Goldman-Rakic, 1996; Goldman-Rakic and
Leung, 2002; Rolls, 2008).
Specific simulations of impairments in the operation of prefrontal attractor networks can help to explain how the cognitive
symptoms of schizophrenia, including poor short-term memory,
poor ability to allocate and maintain attention, and distractibility, occur (Frith and Dolan, 1997; Loh et al., 2007a; Seidman et al.,
1994; Weinberger and Berman, 1996). Indeed, building on work by
Seamans and Yang (2004), Rolls, Loh and Deco (Loh et al., 2007a;
Rolls, 2005) have proposed that the working-memory and attentional deficits might be related to instabilities of the high-firing
Spontaneous stable
Persistent stable
50
40
30
20
10
0
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
60
Firing rate [Hz]
3.1. Cognitive symptoms
60
2.5
3
Spontaneous unstable
Persistent unstable
50
40
30
20
10
0
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
2.5
3
time [s]
Fig. 1. Example trials of integrate-and-fire attractor network simulations of shortterm memory. The average firing rate of all the neurons in the short-term memory
population S1 of neurons is shown. (Top: normal operation) On a trial in which a
recall stimulus was applied to S1 at 0–500 ms, firing continued normally until the
end of the trial in the ‘persistent’ simulation condition. On a trial on which no recall
stimulus was applied to S1, spontaneous firing (i.e. at a low rate) continued until the
end of the trial in the ‘spontaneous’ simulation condition. (Bottom: unstable operation). On this persistent condition trial, the firing decreased during the trial as the
network fell out of the attractor because of the statistical fluctuations caused by the
spiking dynamics. This type of instability is more likely if NMDA receptor activated
ion channel currents become decreased, or by other factors that decrease neuronal
excitability. This provides a model of impaired cognitive function in for example
schizophrenia. On the spontaneous condition trial, the firing increased during the
trial because of the statistical fluctuations. This type of instability is more likely if
GABA receptor activated ion channel currents become decreased, or by other factors that decrease cortical inhibition. This type of instability in which a network
jumps because of noise into a high firing rate state that is not triggered by an external input to the network contributes it is suggested to the positive symptoms of
schizophrenia. (After Rolls et al. (2008a).)
states in attractor networks in the prefrontal cortex (Fig. 1). Specifically, NMDA receptor hypofunction, which has been associated
with schizophrenia (Coyle, 2006; Coyle et al., 2003), results in
reduced currents running through NMDA receptor-activated ion
channels; this causes neurons to fire less fast, leading to shallower
basins of attraction (see Section 8) of the high firing-rate attractor states of the network (Loh et al., 2007a). The shallower basins
of attraction arise firstly because with the neurons firing less fast,
there is less positive feedback in the recurrent collateral connections between the neurons in the attractor, and this makes the
system more vulnerable to noise. The noise could be external to
the network, but an important source of noise that can destabilize the high firing rate attractor state is the random spiking times
of neurons for a given mean firing rate, which produce statistical
fluctuations by which there might due to a random set of events be
less (or more) firing in a set of neurons than average, which could
make the system fall out of a high firing rate attractor state (Rolls
and Deco, 2010). (The spike times of individual neurons are close
to being Poisson distributed.)
A second way in which reduced NMDA receptor function (or
other factors such as synaptic pruning) could decrease the depth of
the basins of attraction is by making the strengths of the synaptic
connections between the neurons in the attractor weaker, which
again reduces the positive feedback between the neurons in the
attractor, and makes the attractor state more vulnerable to noise.
These concepts are made quantitative in Section 8, Eq. (1), and in The
Noisy Brain (Rolls and Deco, 2010). Thus, the stability of the attractor
state is reduced. The result is difficulty in maintaining short-term
memory and thus attention (see Fig. 1 and also Durstewitz, 2007;
Durstewitz and Seamans, 2002). The shallower basins of attrac-
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
tion and the reduced time constant of the system caused by NMDA
receptor (NMDAR) hypofunction (Wang, 2006), in the presence of
the stochastic firing-related noise in the networks, result in distractibility, poor attention and working-memory difficulties.
The negative symptoms represent a complex of symptoms
including apathy, poor rapport, lack of spontaneity, motor retardation, disturbance of volition, blunted affect, and emotional
withdrawal and passive behavior (Liddle, 1987; Mueser and
McGurk, 2004). The negative symptoms and cognitive deficits are
highly correlated in patients with schizophrenia and their nonpsychotic relatives (Bilder et al., 2002; Delawalla et al., 2006; Jacobs
et al., 2007). Rolls, Loh and Deco propose that the negative symptoms are also related to the decreased firing rates caused by a
reduction in currents through NMDAR-activated channels, but in
brain regions that may include the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior
cingulate cortex (Loh et al., 2007a; Rolls, 2005, 2008) rather than
the prefrontal cortex. Indeed, lesions in these brain areas are well
known to produce symptoms that resemble the negative symptoms
in schizophrenia, and neuronal firing rates and BOLD activations in
these regions are correlated with reward value and pleasure (Paus,
2001; Rolls, 1999, 2005, 2006, 2008; Rolls and Grabenhorst, 2008;
Winterer et al., 2002).
This is a unifying approach to the cognitive and negative symptoms: the same reduction in NMDAR-activated channel currents
produces on the one hand, instability in high-firing-rate states in
attractor networks in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and thereby
the cognitive symptoms, and on the other hand, a reduction in
the firing rate of neurons in the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortex, leading to the negative symptoms. In addition to the reduced
emotion caused by the reduced firing rates, attractor networks may
be present in the orbitofrontal cortex that help to maintain mood
state (Rolls, 2008), and a decrease in their stability by the reduced
depth in the basins of attraction could make emotions more labile
in schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder.
3.3. Positive symptoms
The positive symptoms of schizophrenia include bizarre trains
of thoughts, hallucinations, and delusions (Liddle, 1987; Mueser
and McGurk, 2004). In contrast to the cognitive and negative symptoms, the positive symptoms generally occur intermittently during
the course of the illness, and this clinical state is called “psychosis”.
Rolls, Loh and Deco propose that owing to reduced currents through
NMDAR-activated channels, the basins of attraction of the highfiring-rate attractor states are shallow (Durstewitz, 2007; Loh et al.,
2007a; Rolls, 2005) in the temporal lobe, which includes the semantic memory networks and the auditory association cortex. Because
of the resulting statistical fluctuations in the states of the attractor networks, internal representations of thoughts and perceptions
move too freely around in the energy landscape, from thought to
weakly associated thought, leading to bizarre thoughts and associations, and to hallucinations (see Figs. 1 and 2). Such thoughts might
eventually be associated together in semantic memory, leading to
false beliefs and delusions (Rolls, 2005, 2008).
In addition, Loh et al. (2007a) propose that a reduction in GABA
interneuron efficacy in schizophrenic patients may also contribute
to the generation of positive symptoms: lower GABA-interneuron
efficacy reduces the depth of the basin of attraction of the spontaneous state, making it more likely that a high firing-rate attractor
state will emerge out of the spontaneous firing of the neurons.
This is illustrated in Fig. 1 (bottom). On the spontaneous condition trial, the firing, which should have remained low throughout
the trial as no cue was provided to start up the short-term mem-
30
Pool S1
Pool S2
25
Firing rate [Hz]
3.2. Negative symptoms
1647
20
15
10
5
0
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
time [s]
Fig. 2. Wandering between attractor states. An integrate-and-fire simulation of an
attractor network with two memories stored in it, S1 and S2. With the normal synaptic efficacies, and no initial cue in the simulation, the network would stay stably
in the spontaneous state with very little activity in the neurons in the S1 and S2
neuronal populations or pools. However, on trials of the type shown in which the
NMDA conductances were reduced by 5% and GABA by 10%, the activity moves noisily between the attractor for the spontaneous state and the two persistent states S1
and S2 by virtue of statistical fluctuations caused by the randomness of the spiking
activity. The two curves show the activity of the two selective pools S1 and S2 over
time smoothed with a 1 s sliding averaging window. (After Loh et al. (2007a).)
ory, increased during the trial because of the statistical fluctuations,
that is the spiking-related randomness in the network. This type of
instability is more likely if GABA receptor activated ion channel
currents become decreased, or by other factors that decrease cortical inhibition. This type of instability in which a network jumps
because of noise into a high firing rate state that is not triggered
by an external input to the network contributes it is suggested
to the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, including for example
hallucinations, delusions, and feelings of lack of control or being
controlled by others (Loh et al., 2007a; Rolls et al., 2008b). Empirical
evidence supports this computational proposal: markers indicating
decreased inhibition by the GABA system are found in neocortical
areas (Lewis et al., 2005) and in parts of the hippocampus (Benes,
2010). On the basis of this model, we have proposed (Loh et al.,
2007a; Rolls et al., 2008b) that treating schizophrenia patients with
D2 antagonists could increase the GABA currents (Seamans et al.,
2001; Seamans and Yang, 2004) in the networks, which would alleviate the positive symptoms by reducing the spontaneous firing
rates, which would deepen the spontaneous attractor state (see
Figs. 1 and 3). This effect of D2 antagonists leaves the persistent
attractors shallow because the high firing rates are reduced, which
may explain why the D2 antagonists do not have a major effect
on the negative and cognitive symptoms. To target negative symptoms, we have suggested that D1 agonists may help to deepen the
basin of attraction of the high-firing-rate attractor state (Loh et al.,
2007a; Rolls et al., 2008b). This two-dimensional approach allows
us to address the specific characteristics of the psychotic (positive)
symptoms which appear in episodes, in contrast to the negative
and cognitive symptoms which typically persist over time.
When both NMDA and GABA are reduced one might think
that these two counterbalancing effects (excitatory and inhibitory)
would cancel each other out. However, this is not the case:
modeling these conditions showed that the stability of both the
spontaneous and the high-firing-rate states is reduced (Loh et al.,
2007a) (see also Brunel and Wang, 2001; Durstewitz and Seamans,
2002). Indeed, under these conditions, the network wandered
freely between the two short-term memory (high firing-rate) states
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
1648
5. The role of prefrontal cortex dopamine
external input
ei
wij
yj
h = dendritic
i activation
y = output firing
i
output
Fig. 3. The architecture of an autoassociation or attractor network (see Section 8.1).
in the network and the spontaneous state (Fig. 2). We relate this pattern to the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, in which both the
basins of attraction of the spontaneous and high-firing-rate states
are shallow, and the system jumps, helped by the statistical fluctuations, between the different attractor states and the spontaneous
state (Fig. 2) (Loh et al., 2007a).
4. Schizophrenia and noise
The changes in the integrate-and fire model we have just
described produced by alterations in the activation of synaptically
activated ion channels can be interpreted in terms of a reduced
signal-to-noise ratio. In the computational models, the stability is
defined as the proportion of trials on which the correct short term
memory (or signal) is maintained until the end of the trial, and the
signal-to-noise ratio can be measured by the mean squared divided
by the variance of the synaptic currents over the whole trial period
(Loh et al., 2007a,b).
Three possible mechanisms for a decreased signal-to-noise
ratio as reflected in reduced stability are highlighted by the
computational models as follows, and may be relevant to the
decreased signal-to-noise ratio described below in schizophrenia.
First, reduced NMDAR-activated synaptic currents will reduce the
firing rates of neurons, and this will decrease the depth of the basins
of attraction of cortical attractor states, making them less stable
in the face of spiking-related and other noise in the brain, and
of distracting stimuli in the world. Second, the reduced contribution of NMDAR-activated current will reduce the time constant of
the whole attractor network, also making the attractor states less
stable in the face of noise and distracting stimuli. Third, reduced
GABAR-mediated currents may reduce the stability of the spontaneous state, and the resulting noise will cause the system to jump
into a high-firing-rate attractor state, as described above (Loh et al.,
2007a,b).
A way to link the signal-to-noise ratio measure from models
with experimental data is to use the trial-by-trial variability with
experimental measures. With this approach, there is some evidence
for decreased signal-to-noise ratio in schizophrenia, in studies in
which the variability of EEG and functional neuroimaging data in
attentional tasks is measured (Rolls et al., 2008b; Winterer et al.,
2004, 2000).
With the computational approach described, links can be made
to help understand the role of dopamine in schizophrenia (Rolls et
al., 2008b). Dopamine, acting through D1 receptors, is important
in maintaining high firing activity in prefrontal cortical networks
involved in short-term memory and attention (Brozoski et al., 1979;
Sawaguchi and Goldman-Rakic, 1991, 1994; Sawaguchi et al., 1988,
1990), with an optimal level being important (Goldman-Rakic et
al., 2000; Vijayraghavan et al., 2007; Williams and Goldman-Rakic,
1995). Impairments in this system may contribute to the cognitive
deficits in schizophrenia.
While D1 receptor activation increases NMDA and GABA
receptor-activated ion channel conductances (a D1-receptordominated state), D2-receptor stimulation tends to have the
opposite effect (a D2-receptor-dominated state) (Seamans et al.,
2001; Seamans and Yang, 2004). On the basis of these findings, Seamans and Yang (2004) and others (Castner et al., 2000;
Durstewitz, 2007; Durstewitz et al., 2000; Seamans and Yang,
2004) predict that D1 agonists and D2 antagonists should help to
treat schizophrenic symptoms by increasing NMDAR- and GABAR-activated synaptic conductances. Indeed, an important effect of
D2 antagonists relates to the increase in GABA-R activated currents
(Seamans et al., 2001; Seamans and Yang, 2004), which would make
the spontaneous firing state more stable and may help to control
the positive symptoms of schizophrenia (Rolls et al., 2008b).
6. Brain changes during adolescence that could influence
these processes
There are marked changes in emotion, cognition, and behavior
at the time of adolescence (Steinberg, 2005).
In relation to the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia, there
has always been the fact that schizophrenia is a condition that often
has its onset in the late teens or twenties (Lewis and Levitt, 2002),
and it has been suggested that there could be a link in this respect
to changes in NMDA and related receptor functions that are related
to aging (Rolls, 2008; Rolls and Deco, 2010). In particular, working
memory shows a steady decline with age from the 20 s to the 70 s
(Borella et al., 2008; Johnson et al., 2010; Swanson, 1999), and it
may be the case that by the late teens or early twenties NMDA
and related receptor systems (including dopamine) may be less
efficacious than when younger, so that the cognitive symptoms of
schizophrenia are more likely to occur at this age than earlier.
In this section, we examine some of the changes that occur in the
brain during (late) adolescence, and address whether there is evidence that the changes that occur about this time, if interacting with
other causal factors, could contribute to the onset of schizophrenia
when it occurs in late adolescence and the early twenties. The computational points made next are clear, but the extent to which the
changes in the brain lead them to apply is an area where further
investigation is needed.
6.1. Grey matter and synaptic changes associated with
adolescence
In the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the
density of excitatory synapses decreases by 40–50% during adolescence, and this occurs without a major change in synaptic strength
(Gonzalez-Burgos et al., 2008). Structural neuroimaging studies in
humans demonstrate that adolescence is associated with a substantial decrease in cortical grey matter thickness (Giedd et al., 1999;
Gogtay and Thompson, 2010), a decrease usually interpreted to
result from the massive and increased synaptic pruning occurring
during adolescence and early adulthood (Gogtay et al., 2004; Rakic,
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
1996; Rakic et al., 1994). Indeed, the total volume of grey matter
increases across the cortex prior to puberty, reaching a peak somewhere in the early to-mid pubertal period after which there is a
post-pubertal decline.
It is noted that a decreased density of dendritic spines (a marker
of excitatory synaptic inputs to pyramidal neurons) in the DLPFC
of individuals with schizophrenia (Garey et al., 1998; Glantz and
Lewis, 2000), is consistent with reduced excitatory drive in DLPFC
circuits in schizophrenia. In addition, convergent lines of evidence
indicate that schizophrenia might be associated with reduced
excitatory synaptic neurotransmission through NMDA (N-methyld-aspartate) glutamate receptors (Coyle, 2006; Coyle et al., 2003).
Further, the loss of excitatory synapses is even greater at adolescence in those diagnosed with schizophrenia, amounting to 60%
(Bennett, 2009; Glantz et al., 2007; Glantz and Lewis, 2000).
What are the functional and computational implications of this
synaptic pruning and decrease in grey matter volume at the time
of adolescence? A first possible computational implication is that
neurons in the prefrontal cortex become less excitable, as they
have fewer excitatory synapses. A decrease in the firing rates of
these neurons could decrease the stability of cortical attractor networks that implement short-term memory, and attention. This
could result, if added to other effects, in a tendency of some individuals at about the time of adolescence to show the cognitive
changes of schizophrenia. Similarly, if the same pruning of excitatory synapses occurred in the orbitofrontal cortex and the firing
rates of the neurons were lower as a result, this could result (in
otherwise predisposed individuals) in some of the negative symptoms of schizophrenia (decreased emotionality and motivation)
appearing at about this time. Whether changes in GABA compensate for the decreased excitatory drive to cortical pyramidal cells
in adolescence is considered in Section 6.2. The argument here is
that the massive and increased synaptic pruning in late adolescence/the early twenties (Gogtay et al., 2004; Rakic, 1996; Rakic et
al., 1994) may be sufficiently large to reduce the stability of attractor
networks involved in working memory, especially with the larger
reduction in excitatory synapses found in schizophrenics (Bennett,
2009; Glantz et al., 2007; Glantz and Lewis, 2000). (Although synaptic pruning starts earlier in development, we do not know whether
the synapses being pruned before and during early adolescence are
computationally efficacious in these attractor networks (i.e. are on
the right neurons to support attractors, and with the associatively
increased synaptic strength required to support discrete attractor
states (Rolls, 2008); and the synaptic pruning rate does become
larger in late adolescence and the early twenties (Gogtay et al.,
2004; Rakic, 1996; Rakic et al., 1994).)
A second computational implication of a reduced number of
excitatory connections onto each neuron arises from the fact that
the number of different memories that can be stored in an associative network is proportional to the number of associatively
modifiable excitatory connections onto each neuron (Rolls, 2008;
Rolls and Treves, 1998). This applies to both pattern association
networks used to associate one stimulus with another, for example
a visual stimulus with the sight of food (Rolls and Treves, 1990);
and to autoassociation or attractor networks used to store a memory as a set of events, and later recall the whole memory from any
part (Treves and Rolls, 1991). The number of different memories
that can be stored in both types of associative network is of the
order of the number of associatively modifiable connections onto
each neuron if sparse representations are used (Rolls and Treves,
1990; Treves and Rolls, 1991). This number will be in the order of
10,000 for a network in a small, local, region of the neocortex (Rolls,
2008). The implication of a reduction in the number of associatively modifiable synapses onto each neuron will thus be a reduced
memory capacity, and, particularly important in the present context, more interference between different memories (Rolls, 2008).
1649
This increased interference will tend to impair the distinctiveness
and stability of any one memory by altering the shape of the energy
landscape, and making the system more sensitive to the effects of
noise generated by the Poisson nature of the spiking of neurons
(Rolls, 2008; Rolls and Deco, 2010). This will for example tend to
impair short-term memory and attention, and increase distractibility. A reduced number of synapses onto each neuron could, by the
time of late adolescence, be sufficient to have therefore an impact
on for example working memory, and this in individuals predisposed to schizophrenia could contribute to an account of why the
cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia may become apparent in late
adolescence or the early twenties.
6.2. GABA changes at adolescence
Any change during adolescence in the excitatory input to cortical pyramidal cells could be compensated by changes in the
GABA inhibitory interneurons that receive from the pyramidal cells
and provide negative feedback to them. No significant changes in
the density of inhibitory synapses are observed in the neocortex
during adolescence (De Felipe et al., 1997; Rakic et al., 1986). However, the efficacy of GABAergic transmission may change during
adolescence because during this developmental period substantial
changes occur in the levels of expression of GABAA receptors, GABA
transporters, and parvalbumin, an interneuron-specific calcium
binding protein (Lewis et al., 2004). Whether or not GABA-mediated
transmission can regulate excitatory synaptic pruning during adolescence remains to be investigated.
In schizophrenia, a deficiency in signalling through the
TrkB neurotrophin receptor leads to reduced GABA (gammaaminobutyric acid) synthesis in the parvalbumin-containing
subpopulation of inhibitory GABA neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of individuals with schizophrenia. Despite both preand post-synaptic compensatory responses, there is a resulting
decrease in the perisomatic inhibition of pyramidal neurons (Lewis
et al., 2005). Moreover, markers indicating decreased inhibition by
the GABA system were found in other neocortical areas (Lewis et
al., 2005) and in parts of the hippocampus (Benes, 2010), of individuals with schizophrenia. As described in Section 3.3, a decrease
in inhibition could decrease the stability of the spontaneous firing
state of cortical attractor networks, and might least to spurious high
firing rate states, which could contribute to the positive symptoms
of schizophrenia.
6.3. Dopamine changes at adolescence
In primates, cortical and subcortical tissue concentrations of
dopamine (DA) are increased during adolescence compared to
childhood and adulthood (Goldman-Rakic and Brown, 1982; Irwin
et al., 1994). In addition, DA innervation of the frontal cortex peaks
during adolescence relative to childhood and adulthood, specifically in cortical layer III, which contains pyramidal cells responsible
for cortico-cortical information processing (Rosenberg and Lewis,
1995). D1 and D2 receptor densities appear to decrease from adolescence to adulthood in both nonhuman primate and human
cortex and subcortical regions, though peaks in receptor density
occur in childhood (Jucaite et al., 2010; Lidow and Rakic, 1992;
Seeman et al., 1987). Thus, cortical and subcortical regions undergo
specific increases in DA concentrations and innervation during
adolescence, with receptor levels decreasing from peaks achieved
during childhood (Wahlstrom et al., 2010). It has been noted elsewhere that the ratio of D1 to D2 receptor mediated effects may
be a relevant factor, with D1 receptor activation increasing NMDA
and GABA conductances, and D2-receptor stimulation tending to
have the opposite effect (Rolls et al., 2008b; Seamans et al., 2001;
Seamans and Yang, 2004).
1650
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
It has been suggested that increases in risk-taking and emotional
lability at the time of adolescence could be related to heightened
DA efficacy (Wahlstrom et al., 2010). According to this approach, DA
underlies a behavioral activation system that modulates incentivemotivated approach behavior (Depue and Collins, 1999). This
system promotes reward-seeking through activity in limbic, striatal, and frontal networks. It is suggested that the increase occurs
via a tonic increase in DA availability which impacts both subcortical (limbic and striatal) and cortical (prefrontal) circuits, and
produces over-activation of incentive motivation (Wahlstrom et al.,
2010).
If there are impairments in the DA system that contribute to the
cognitive deficits in schizophrenia (Goldman-Rakic, 1999; Rolls et
al., 2008b), then it is possible that the decrease in D1 receptor binding from adolescence to adulthood in normal subjects (Jucaite et al.,
2010) may have the effect of reducing the stability of cortical attractors (by reducing the firing rate of the neurons in part by reducing
NMDA receptor mediated synaptic conductances) involved in cognitive functions in the prefrontal cortex such as short-term memory
and attention. This decrease could be a factor that contributes to
cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia tending to appear in late
adolescence or early adulthood, by contributing to the instability
of a system at that time that may already be less stable in those
predisposed to have schizophrenia. Decreased affect, which could
be related to decreased effects of rewards and punishers in the
orbitofrontal cortex and related systems (Rolls, 2005), which may
be modulated by dopamine, may appear at about the same time, if
D1 receptors become reduced.
As noted in Sections 3.3 and 6.2, the use of D2 blockers in
schizophrenia may not relate to these cognitive and emotional systems, but instead to increasing the activity of GABA systems which
may help to control the positive symptoms (Rolls et al., 2008b).
7. Conclusions
We have reviewed a stochastic neurodynamics approach to
schizophrenia, which suggests that:
A reduced depth in the basins of attraction of cortical attractor states destabilizes the activity at the network level due to the
statistical fluctuations caused by the stochastic spiking of neurons.
A decrease in the NMDA receptor conductances, present in
schizophrenia, which reduces the depth of the attractor basins,
decreases the stability of short-term memory states and increases
distractibility. The effects produced decrease the signal-to-noise
ratio of the networks.
The cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia such as distractibility,
working memory deficits or poor attention could be caused by this
instability of attractor states in prefrontal cortical networks.
A reduction of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex, producing
reduced dopamine D1 receptor activation in schizophrenia, acting at least in part by reducing NMDA receptor-activated synaptic
currents, can produce similar effects.
A reduction of NMDA receptor-activated synaptic conductances,
present in schizophrenia, produces lower firing rates in neurons,
and in the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex could account
for the negative symptoms including a reduction of emotions.
Decreasing the GABA as well as the NMDA conductances produces not only switches between the attractor states, but also
jumps from spontaneous activity into one of the attractors. The
spontaneous state of firing is less stable when GABA efficacy is
reduced because there is less inhibition. We relate this to the positive symptoms of schizophrenia including delusions, paranoia, and
hallucinations, which may arise because the basins of attraction are
shallow and there is instability in temporal lobe semantic memory
networks, leading thoughts to move too freely round the attrac-
tor energy landscape. The instability that accounts for the positive
symptoms we argue is due to less stability of the spontaneous
(unstimulated) state of firing due to reduced GABA inhibition which
leads to entry into a high firing state in the absence of a relevant
stimulus; and is due also to less stability of the high firing rate
states due to reduced NMDA receptor mediated neuronal activation so that a high firing rate state moves too freely into other
high firing rate attractor states, or back to the spontaneous firing
state.
This approach shows how any factors that reduce cortical
excitability and any factors that reduce cortical inhibition can be
interpreted in terms of the stochastic neurodynamics of cortical
systems. This opens a way to interpret effects, possibly not yet
discovered, that reduce cortical excitation, and/or reduce cortical
inhibition, on the stability of cortical networks involved in cognitive functions. This approach also opens up new ways to explore
combinations of treatments that by influencing these two sources
of instability in cortical networks, altered excitation and altered
inhibition, might ameliorate the instabilities, and improve functioning.
The approach described thus enables links to be made from
factors that modulate currents in synapses, or the excitability of
cortical neurons, or alterations in cortical inhibition, to the effects
that these will have on the global function performed by a network,
to implement for example cognitive processes such as short-term
memory and attention.
In terms of some of the brain changes that are prominent at the
time of adolescence, the following points can be made.
First, and this includes changes yet to be discovered or fully
understood, one way in which the changes can be interpreted is
in terms of their effects on the stochastic neurodynamics of the
cerebral cortex. As we have seen, factors that decrease cortical
excitability may produce cognitive changes such as alterations of
short-term memory, attention, or executive function because of
instability of the high firing rate attractor states. Further, factors
that decrease cortical inhibition may produce cognitive changes
because of instability of the low firing rate attractor states, which
might be provoked by noise caused for example by the stochastic
neuronal firing to lead to spurious states not produced by external
inputs or requirements.
Second, in relation to the evidence that the synaptic pruning, and the reduction in grey matter volume, that normally take
place during adolescence are especially marked in those who
develop schizophrenia, this could lead to reduced cortical excitability, which would lead to cognitive and negative symptoms. This
may provide an understanding of why schizophrenia has a tendency to become evident in late adolescence or the early twenties,
as this is a time when cortical pruning is taking place especially
rapidly, and even more rapidly in those diagnosed with schizophrenia.
Third, in so far as reduced NMDAR function is related to
schizophrenia (Coyle, 2006), we might predict that this reduction
would become especially evident in late adolescence especially in
those who develop schizophrenia.
Fourth, given that the efficacy of GABAergic transmission may
decrease during adolescence because of changes in the levels of
expression of GABAA receptors, GABA transporters, and parvalbumin (an interneuron-specific calcium binding protein) (Lewis et al.,
2004), and that reduced GABAergic inhibition of pyramidal neurons is associated with schizophrenia (Lewis et al., 2005), we might
expect the stability of the spontaneous firing states to be reduced,
and the positive symptoms to become evident at about this time.
Fifth, given that dopamine levels are high in adolescence compared to adulthood (Section 6.3), and that reduced dopamine partly
by reducing NMDA currents may decrease cortical excitability and
contribute to the cognitive and negative symptoms of schizophre-
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
1651
nia (Section 3), it would be interesting to investigate whether
dopamine levels or efficacy in those who become schizophrenic are
low relative to the average especially at the time of adolescence.
8. Stochastic neurodynamics: appendix
8.1. The attractor framework
The attractor framework is important in many aspects of neural
dynamics, including the systems that implement short-term memory, long-term memory, attention, and decision-making (Rolls,
2008; Rolls and Deco, 2002). In an attractor network of interconnected neurons, a memory pattern (or set of active neurons)
can be stored by synaptic modification and activated by external inputs. When a retrieval cue is applied, the system moves
towards one of the stored patterns, thus implementing memory
retrieval. The system can implement memory retrieval in this way
from an incomplete retrieval cue, implementing “completion”. The
retrieved pattern can be stably maintained by the system even
after the input ceases (Hertz et al., 1991; Hopfield, 1982; Rolls,
2008). These patterns could correspond to memories, perceptual
representations or thoughts. If several input cues are presented
simultaneously, the non-linear dynamics results in one of the
attractor states winning, and this implements a realistic model of
decision-making in the brain (Deco and Rolls, 2006; Rolls and Deco,
2010; Wang, 2002, 2008). In all these dynamical processes, noise,
in part due to the random spiking times of individual neurons (for
a given mean rate), can make the processes in a finite-sized system
stochastic, making the stability of the system, memory retrieval,
and decision-making probabilistic, which, as we have shown elsewhere, can be advantageous (Rolls and Deco, 2010).
The architecture of an attractor or autoassociation network is
as follows (see Fig. 3): external inputs ei activate the neurons in
the network, and produce firing yi , where i refers to the i’th neuron. The neurons are connected by recurrent collateral synapses wij ,
where j refers to the j’th synapse on a neuron. By these synapses
an input pattern on ei is associated with itself, and thus the network is referred to as an autoassociation network. Because there is
positive feedback via the recurrent collateral connections, the network can sustain persistent firing. These synaptic connections are
assumed to build up by an associative (Hebbian) learning mechanism (Hebb, 1949; Rolls, 2008) (according to which the more two
neurons are simultaneously active the stronger the neural connection becomes). The inhibitory interneurons are not shown. They
receive inputs from the pyramidal cells, and make negative feedback connections onto the pyramidal cells to control their activity.
The recall state (which could be used to implement short-term
memory, or memory recall) in an attractor network can be thought
of as the local minimum in an energy landscape (Hopfield, 1982),
where the energy would be defined as
E=−
1
2
wij (yi − < y >)(yj − < y >)
(1)
i,j
where yi is the firing of neuron i, and <y> indicates the average
firing rate. The intuition here is that if both yi and yj are above their
average rates, and are exciting each other through a strong synapse,
then the firing will tend to be stable and maintained, resulting in a
low energy state that is stable.
Autoassociation attractor systems have two types of stable fixed
point: a spontaneous state with a low firing rate, and one or more
persistent states with high firing rates in which the neurons keep
firing. Each one of the high firing rate attractor states can implement
a different memory. When the system is moved to a position in
the space by an external retrieval cue stimulus, it will move to the
closest stable attractor state. The area in the space within which
Fig. 4. (a) The neuronal components of an integrate-and-fire attractor network simulation (see Section 8.2). (b) The energy landscape of an integrate-and-fire attractor
network simulation (see Section 8.2).
the system will move to a stable attractor state is called its basin of
attraction.
8.2. Energy landscape and stochasticity
Realistic attractor network architectures of the cerebral cortex are typically implemented by integrate-and-fire neurons and
realistic synaptic dynamics (see Fig. 4a; Brunel and Wang, 2001;
Deco and Rolls, 2006; Rolls and Deco, 2010). The integrate-and-fire
model describes the subthreshold membrane potential, which is
influenced by synaptic currents:
Cm
dV (t)
= −gm (V (t) − VL ) − Isyn (t)
dt
N
Isyn (t) = gs (V (t) − VE )
wij sj (t)
j=1
dsj (t)
dt
=−
dsj (t)
+
ı(t − tjk )
k
where Cm is the membrane capacitance, gm the leak conductance,
V(t) the membrane potential of the neuron, VL the resting potential,
and Isyn (t) the incoming synaptic currents. Here we just write one
synaptic current Isyn (t), where gs is the synaptic conductance, VE
the current source, wij the synaptic weights, and sj (t) the fractions
of open synaptic channels. The synaptic variable sj (t) is described
by an exponential decay with time constant and influenced by
the incoming spikes k. When the threshold membrane potential
Vthr is reached, the neuron is set to the reset potential Vreset at
which it is kept for a refractory period tref and the action potential
is propagated to the other neurons. These networks can maintain
a stable pattern of firing of a subset of neurons, which are strongly
interconnected.
The attractor dynamics can be pictured by effective energy landscapes, which indicate the basin of attraction by valleys, and the
attractor states or fixed points by the bottom of the valleys (see
Fig. 4b). The stability of an attractor is characterized by the aver-
1652
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
age time in which the system stays in the basin of attraction under
the influence of noise, which provokes transitions to other attractor states. Noise results from the interplay between the Poissonian
character of the spikes (i.e. the times at which each spike of a
neurons occurs is random) and the finite-size effect due to the limited numbers of neurons in the network. In fact, the number of
spikes emitted in a time interval dt by the network is a Poisson
variable with mean and variance Nr(t) dt, where N is the number of neurons in the network and r(t) the averaged firing rate
at time t. The estimate of r(t), is then a stochastic process
rN (t),
∼ r(t) + r(t)/N ,
well described in the limit of large Nr by rN (t) =
where
is Gaussian white noise with zero mean and unit variance, and r(t) is the probability of emitting a spike per unit time
in the infinite network. Such finite-N fluctuations, which affect the
global activity rN (t), are felt coherently by all neurons in the network.
Two factors determine the stability. First, if the depths of the
attractors are shallow (as in the left compared to the right valley
in Fig. 4b), less force is needed to move a ball from the shallow
valley to the next. Second, a high level of noise increases the likelihood that the system will jump over an energy boundary from one
state to another. We envision that the brain, as a dynamical system,
has characteristics of such an attractor system, including statistical
fluctuations (Rolls, 2008; Rolls and Deco, 2010).
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the Oxford McDonnell Centre for
Cognitive Neuroscience, and by the Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience. We acknowledge contributions by a number
of colleagues to some of the work described, and the citations are
provided.
References
Abeles, M., 1991. Corticonics—Neural Circuits of the Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge
University Press, New York.
Amit, D.J., 1989. Modeling Brain Function. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Benes, F.M., 2010. Amygdalocortical circuitry in schizophrenia: from circuits to
molecules. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 239–257.
Bennett, M.R., 2009. Synapse formation and regression in the cortex during adolescence and in schizophrenia. The Medical Journal of Australia 190, S14–S16.
Bilder, R.M., Goldman, R.S., Volavka, J., Czobor, P., Hoptman, M., Sheitman, B.,
Lindenmayer, J.P., Citrome, L., McEvoy, J., Kunz, M., Chakos, M., Cooper, T.B.,
Horowitz, T.L., Lieberman, J.A., 2002. Neurocognitive effects of clozapine, olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol in patients with chronic schizophrenia or
schizoaffective disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 1018–1028.
Borella, E., Carretti, B., De Beni, R., 2008. Working memory and inhibition across the
adult life-span. Acta Psychologica 128, 33–44.
Braitenberg, V., Schütz, A., 1991. Anatomy of the Cortex. Springer-Verlag, Berlin.
Brozoski, T.J., Brown, R.M., Rosvold, H.E., Goldman, P.S., 1979. Cognitive deficit
caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey.
Science 205, 929–932.
Brunel, N., Wang, X.J., 2001. Effects of neuromodulation in a cortical network model
of object working memory dominated by recurrent inhibition. Journal of Computational Neuroscience 11, 63–85.
Castner, S.A., Williams, G.V., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 2000. Reversal of antipsychoticinduced working memory deficits by short-term dopamine D1 receptor
stimulation. Science 287, 2020–2022.
Cohen, J.D., Servan-Schreiber, D., 1992. Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review 99,
45–77.
Coyle, J.T., 2006. Glutamate and schizophrenia: beyond the dopamine hypothesis.
Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology 26, 365–384.
Coyle, J.T., Tsai, G., Goff, D., 2003. Converging evidence of NMDA receptor hypofunction in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Annals of the New York Academy
of Sciences 1003, 318–327.
De Felipe, J., Marco, P., Fairen, A., Jones, E.G., 1997. Inhibitory synaptogenesis in
mouse somatosensory cortex. Cerebral Cortex 7, 619–634.
Deco, G., Rolls, E.T., 2003. Attention and working memory: a dynamical model of
neuronal activity in the prefrontal cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience 18,
2374–2390.
Deco, G., Rolls, E.T., 2005a. Attention, short-term memory, and action selection: a
unifying theory. Progress in Neurobiology 76, 236–256.
Deco, G., Rolls, E.T., 2005b. Neurodynamics of biased competition and co-operation
for attention: a model with spiking neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 94,
295–313.
Deco, G., Rolls, E.T., 2006. Decision-making and Weber’s Law: a neurophysiological
model. European Journal of Neuroscience 24, 901–916.
Deco, G., Rolls, E.T., Romo, R., 2009. Stochastic dynamics as a principle of brain
function. Progress in Neurobiology 88, 1–16.
Delawalla, Z., Barch, D.M., Fisher Eastep, J.L., Thomason, E.S., Hanewinkel, M.J.,
Thompson, P.A., Csernansky, J.G., 2006. Factors mediating cognitive deficits and
psychopathology among siblings of individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 32, 525–537.
Depue, R.A., Collins, P.F., 1999. Neurobiology of the structure of personality:
dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. The Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 22, 491–517, discussion 518–569.
Desimone, R., Duncan, J., 1995. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.
Annual Review of Neuroscience 18, 193–222.
Durstewitz, D., 2007. Dopaminergic modulation of prefrontal cortex network
dynamics. In: Tseng, K.-Y., Atzori, M. (Eds.), Monoaminergic Modulation of Cortical Excitability. Springer, New York, pp. 217–234.
Durstewitz, D., Kelc, M., Gunturkun, O., 1999. A neurocomputational theory of the
dopaminergic modulation of working memory functions. Journal of Neuroscience 19, 2807–2822.
Durstewitz, D., Seamans, J.K., 2002. The computational role of dopamine D1 receptors in working memory. Neural Networks 15, 561–572.
Durstewitz, D., Seamans, J.K., 2008. The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex
dopamine function with relevance to COMT genotypes and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 64, 739–749.
Durstewitz, D., Seamans, J.K., Sejnowski, T.J., 2000. Dopamine-mediated stabilization of delay-period activity in a network model of prefrontal cortex. Journal of
Neurophysiology 83, 1733–1750.
Frith, C., Dolan, R.J., 1997. Brain mechanisms associated with top-down processes
in perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 352,
1221–1230.
Funahashi, S., Bruce, C.J., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1989. Mnemonic coding of visual
space in monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 61,
331–349.
Fuster, J.M., 1995. Memory in the Cerebral Cortex. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Fuster, J.M., 2000. Executive frontal functions. Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung 133, 66–70.
Fuster, J.M., Alexander, G.E., 1971. Neuron activity related to short-term memory.
Science 173, 652–654.
Garey, L.J., Ong, W.Y., Patel, T.S., Kanani, M., Davis, A., Mortimer, A.M., Barnes, T.R.,
Hirsch, S.R., 1998. Reduced dendritic spine density on cerebral cortical pyramidal
neurons in schizophrenia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry
65, 446–453.
Giedd, J.N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N.O., Castellanos, F.X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A.,
Paus, T., Evans, A.C., Rapoport, J.L., 1999. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience 2,
861–863.
Glantz, L.A., Gilmore, J.H., Hamer, R.M., Lieberman, J.A., Jarskog, L.F., 2007. Synaptophysin and postsynaptic density protein 95 in the human prefrontal cortex from
mid-gestation into early adulthood. Neuroscience 149, 582–591.
Glantz, L.A., Lewis, D.A., 2000. Decreased dendritic spine density on prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 57,
65–73.
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A.C., Nugent
3rd, T.F., Herman, D.H., Clasen, L.S., Toga, A.W., Rapoport, J.L., Thompson, P.M.,
2004. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood
through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America 101, 8174–8179.
Gogtay, N., Thompson, P.M., 2010. Mapping gray matter development: implications for typical development and vulnerability to psychopathology. Brain and
Cognition 72, 6–15.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1994. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. The
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 6, 348–357.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1995. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron 14, 477–485.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1996. The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional
architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 351, 1445–1453.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1999. The physiological approach: functional architecture of
working memory and disordered cognition in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 46, 650–661.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., Brown, R.M., 1982. Postnatal development of monoamine content and synthesis in the cerebral cortex of rhesus monkeys. Brain Research 256,
339–349.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., Leung, H.-C., 2002. Functional architecture of the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex in monkeys and humans. In: Stuss, D.T., Knight, R.T. (Eds.), Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 85–95.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., Muly 3rd, E.C., Williams, G.V., 2000. D(1) receptors in prefrontal
cells and circuits. Brain Research 31, 295–301.
Gonzalez-Burgos, G., Kroener, S., Zaitsev, A.V., Povysheva, N.V., Krimer, L.S., Barrionuevo, G., Lewis, D.A., 2008. Functional maturation of excitatory synapses
in layer 3 pyramidal neurons during postnatal development of the primate
prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 18, 626–637.
Green, M.F., 1996. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits
in schizophrenia? American Journal of Psychiatry 153, 321–330.
E.T. Rolls, G. Deco / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1644–1653
Hebb, D.O., 1949. The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. Wiley,
New York.
Hertz, J., Krogh, A., Palmer, R.G., 1991. An Introduction to the Theory of Neural
Computation. Addison-Wesley, Wokingham.
Hopfield, J.J., 1982. Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective
computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the
United States of America 79, 2554–2558.
Irwin, I., DeLanney, L.E., McNeill, T., Chan, P., Forno, L.S., Murphy Jr., G.M., Di Monte,
D.A., Sandy, M.S., Langston, J.W., 1994. Aging and the nigrostriatal dopamine
system: a non-human primate study. Neurodegeneration 3, 251–265.
Jacobs, J., Kahana, M.J., Ekstrom, A.D., Fried, I., 2007. Brain oscillations control timing
of single-neuron activity in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 3839–3844.
Johnson, W., Logie, R.H., Brockmole, J.R., 2010. Working memory tasks differ in factor
structure across age cohorts: Implications for dedifferentiation. Intelligence 38,
513–528.
Jucaite, A., Forssberg, H., Karlsson, P., Halldin, C., Farde, L., 2010. Age-related reduction in dopamine D1 receptors in the human brain: from late childhood to
adulthood, a positron emission tomography study. Neuroscience 167, 104–110.
Kubota, K., Niki, H., 1971. Prefrontal cortical unit activity and delayed alternation
performance in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology 34, 337–347.
Lewis, D.A., Cruz, D., Eggan, S., Erickson, S., 2004. Postnatal development of prefrontal inhibitory circuits and the pathophysiology of cognitive dysfunction in
schizophrenia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021, 64–76.
Lewis, D.A., Hashimoto, T., Volk, D.W., 2005. Cortical inhibitory neurons and
schizophrenia. Nature Reviews 6, 312–324.
Lewis, D.A., Levitt, P., 2002. Schizophrenia as a disorder of neurodevelopment.
Annual Review of Neuroscience 25, 409–432.
Liddle, P.F., 1987. The symptoms of chronic schizophrenia. A re-examination of the
positive-negative dichotomy. The British Journal of Psychiatry 151, 145–151.
Lidow, M.S., Rakic, P., 1992. Scheduling of monoaminergic neurotransmitter receptor
expression in the primate neocortex during postnatal development. Cerebral
Cortex 2, 401–416.
Loh, M., Rolls, E.T., Deco, G., 2007a. A dynamical systems hypothesis of schizophrenia.
PLoS Computational Biology 3, e228, 210.1371/journal.pcbi.0030228.
Loh, M., Rolls, E.T., Deco, G., 2007b. Statistical fluctuations in attractor networks
related to schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry 40, S78–S84.
Mueser, K.T., McGurk, S.R., 2004. Schizophrenia. Lancet 363, 2063–2072.
Paus, T., 2001. Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and
cognition interface. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2, 417–424.
Rakic, P., 1996. Development of the cerebral cortex in human and nonhuman primates. In: Lewis, M.D. (Ed.), Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Williams and
Wilkins, Baltimore, MD, pp. 9–30.
Rakic, P., Bourgeois, J.P., Eckenhoff, M.F., Zecevic, N., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1986. Concurrent overproduction of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral
cortex. Science 232, 232–235.
Rakic, P., Bourgeois, J.P., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1994. Synaptic development of the
cerebral cortex: implications for learning, memory, and mental illness. Progress
in Brain Research 102, 227–243.
Ratcliff, R., Van Zandt, T., McKoon, G., 1999. Connectionist and diffusion models of
reaction time. Psychological Reviews 106, 261–300.
Rolls, E.T., 1999. The Brain and Emotion. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Rolls, E.T., 2005. Emotion Explained. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Rolls, E.T., 2006. The neurophysiology and functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. In:
Zald, D.H., Rauch, S.L. (Eds.), The Orbitofrontal Cortex. Oxford University Press,
Oxford, pp. 95–124.
Rolls, E.T., 2008. Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making: A Unifying Computational Neuroscience Approach. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Rolls, E.T., 2010a. A computational theory of episodic memory formation in the
hippocampus. Behavioural Brain Research 205, 180–196.
Rolls, E.T., 2010b. Noise in the brain, decision-making, determinism, free will, and
consciousness. In: Perry, E., Collerton, D., Ashton, H., Lebeau, F. (Eds.), New Horizons in the Neuroscience of Consciousness. John Benjamin, Amsterdam.
Rolls, E.T., Deco, G., 2002. Computational Neuroscience of Vision. Oxford University
Press, Oxford.
Rolls, E.T., Deco, G., 2010. The Noisy Brain: Stochastic Dynamics as a Principle of
Brain Function. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Rolls, E.T., Grabenhorst, F., 2008. The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect
to decision-making. Progress in Neurobiology 86, 216–244.
Rolls, E.T., Grabenhorst, F., Deco, G., 2010a. Choice, difficulty, and confidence in the
brain. NeuroImage 53, 694–706.
Rolls, E.T., Grabenhorst, F., Deco, G., 2010b. Decision-making, errors, and confidence
in the brain. Journal of Neurophysiology. Electronic publication 1 September.
Rolls, E.T., Loh, M., Deco, G., 2008a. An attractor hypothesis of obsessive-compulsive
disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience 28, 782–793.
1653
Rolls, E.T., Loh, M., Deco, G., Winterer, G., 2008b. Computational models of
schizophrenia and dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience 9, 696–709.
Rolls, E.T., Treves, A., 1990. The relative advantages of sparse versus distributed
encoding for associative neuronal networks in the brain. Network (Bristol, England) 1, 407–421.
Rolls, E.T., Treves, A., 1998. Neural Networks and Brain Function. Oxford University
Press, Oxford.
Rosenberg, D.R., Lewis, D.A., 1995. Postnatal maturation of the dopaminergic
innervation of monkey prefrontal and motor cortices: a tyrosine hydroxylase
immunohistochemical analysis. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 358,
383–400.
Sawaguchi, T., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1991. D1 dopamine receptors in prefrontal cortex: involvement in working memory. Science 251, 947–950.
Sawaguchi, T., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1994. The role of D1-dopamine receptor in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into the prefrontal cortex
of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor delayed-response task. Journal of
Neurophysiology 71, 515–528.
Sawaguchi, T., Matsumura, M., Kubota, K., 1988. Dopamine enhances the neuronal
activity of spatial short-term memory task in the primate prefrontal cortex.
Neuroscience Research 5, 465–473.
Sawaguchi, T., Matsumura, M., Kubota, K., 1990. Catecholaminergic effects on neuronal activity related to a delayed response task in monkey prefrontal cortex.
Journal of Neurophysiology 63, 1385–1400.
Seamans, J.K., Durstewitz, D., Christie, B.R., Stevens, C.F., Sejnowski, T.J., 2001.
Dopamine D1/D5 receptor modulation of excitatory synaptic inputs to layer V
prefrontal cortex neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America 98, 301–306.
Seamans, J.K., Yang, C.R., 2004. The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine
modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Progress in Neurobiology 74, 1–58.
Seeman, P., Bzowej, N.H., Guan, H.C., Bergeron, C., Becker, L.E., Reynolds, G.P., Bird,
E.D., Riederer, P., Jellinger, K., Watanabe, S., et al., 1987. Human brain dopamine
receptors in children and aging adults. Synapse (New York, NY) 1, 399–404.
Seidman, L.J., Yurgelun-Todd, D., Kremen, W.S., Woods, B.T., Goldstein, J.M., Faraone,
S.V., Tsuang, M.T., 1994. Relationship of prefrontal and temporal lobe MRI measures to neuropsychological performance in chronic schizophrenia. Biological
Psychiatry 35, 235–246.
Smith, P.L., Ratcliff, R., 2004. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions.
Trends in Neurosciences 27, 161–168.
Steinberg, L., 2005. Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences 9, 69–74.
Swanson, H.L., 1999. What develops in working memory? A life span perspective.
Developmental Psychology 35, 986–1000.
Treves, A., Rolls, E.T., 1991. What determines the capacity of autoassociative memories in the brain? Network (Bristol, England) 2, 371–397.
Vickers, D., 1979. Decision Processes in Visual Perception. Academic Press, New York.
Vijayraghavan, S., Wang, M., Birnbaum, S.G., Williams, G.V., Arnsten, A.F., 2007.
Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in
working memory. Nature Neuroscience 10, 376–384.
Wahlstrom, D., White, T., Luciana, M., 2010. Neurobehavioral evidence for changes in
dopamine system activity during adolescence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews 34, 631–648.
Wang, X.J., 2002. Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical
circuits. Neuron 36, 955–968.
Wang, X.J., 2006. Toward a prefrontal microcircuit model for cognitive deficits in
schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry 39 (Suppl. 1), S80–S87.
Wang, X.J., 2008. Decision making in recurrent neuronal circuits. Neuron 60,
215–234.
Weinberger, D.R., Berman, K.F., 1996. Prefrontal function in schizophrenia: confounds and controversies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
London 351, 1495–1503.
Williams, G.V., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1995. Modulation of memory fields by
dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex. Nature 376, 572–575.
Winterer, G., Adams, C.M., Jones, D.W., Knutson, B., 2002. Volition to action—an
event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage 17, 851–858.
Winterer, G., Coppola, R., Goldberg, T.E., Egan, M.F., Jones, D.W., Sanchez, C.E., Weinberger, D.R., 2004. Prefrontal broadband noise, working memory, and genetic
risk for schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 490–500.
Winterer, G., Weinberger, D.R., 2004. Genes, dopamine and cortical signal-to-noise
ratio in schizophrenia. Trends in Neurosciences 27, 683–690.
Winterer, G., Ziller, M., Dorn, H., Frick, K., Mulert, C., Wuebben, Y., Herrmann, W.M.,
Coppola, R., 2000. Schizophrenia: reduced signal-to-noise ratio and impaired
phase-locking during information processing. Clinical Neurophysiology 111,
837–849.